ChapterPDF Available

The Objective Benefits of Subjective Well-Being

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The aim of this paper is to survey the “hard” evidence on the effects of subjective well-being. In doing so, we complement the evidence on the determinants of well-being by showing that human well-being also affects outcomes of interest such as health, income, and social behavior. Generally, we observe a dynamic relationship between happiness and other important aspects of our lives, with influence running in both directions.
Content may be subject to copyright.
The Objective Benefits of Subjective Well-Being*
Jan-Emmanuel De Neve
University College London and Centre for Economic Performance (LSE)
Ed Diener
University of Illinois and The Gallup Organization
Louis Tay
Purdue University
Cody Xuereb
Centre for Economic Performance (LSE)
6 August 2013
De Neve, J.-E., Diener, E., Tay, L., & Xuereb, C. (2013) The objective benefits of
subjective well-being. In Helliwell, J., Layard, R., & Sachs, J., eds. World Happiness
Report 2013. New York: UN Sustainable Development Solutions Network.
* Corresponding author: Jan-Emmanuel De Neve (email: j.de-neve@lse.ac.uk). The authors thank Claire
Bulger, John Helliwell, and Richard Layard for very helpful comments and guidance. This article was
prepared, in part, as a contribution to the research undertaken for the forthcoming New Development
Paradigm (NDP) report of the Royal Government of Bhutan. Financial support from the LSE Centre for
Economic Performance, Emirates Competitiveness Council, Earth Institute (Columbia University), UK
Department for Work and Pensions, and National Institute on Aging/NIH Grant RO1-AG040640 is
gratefully acknowledged.
2
I. Introduction
The aim of this paper is to survey the “hard” evidence on the effects of subjective
well-being. In doing so, we complement the evidence on the determinants of well-being
by showing that human well-being also affects outcomes of interest such as health,
income, and social behavior. Generally, we observe a dynamic relationship between
happiness and other important aspects of our lives, with influence running in both
directions.
Although happiness is considered here as a means rather than an end in itself
we do not imply that normative arguments for raising well-being are insufficient to make
the case for well-being. However, a better understanding of the objective benefits of
raising happiness may also help to put happiness more center-stage in policy-making and
to refine policy evaluation.
In the following sections we review the growing literature on the objective benefits
of happiness across the major life domains categorized into (i) health & longevity; (ii)
income, productivity, & organizational behavior; and (iii) individual & social behavior.
Scientific research increasingly points to specific ways in which happiness generates
tangible benefits. The experience of well-being encourages individuals to pursue goals that
are capacity-building to meet future challenges. At the physiological level, positive
emotions have been found to improve immune, cardiovascular, and endocrine functioning.
In contrast, negative emotions are detrimental to these processes. Table 1 below
summarizes and categorizes the literature on the effects of subjective well-being.
Although high subjective well-being tends to help people function better, it is of
course not a cure-all. Happy people do get sick and do lose friends. Not all happy people
are productive workers. Happiness is like any other factor that aids health and functioning;
with all other things being equal, it is likely (but not guaranteed) to help. It is important to
emphasize that research does not prescribe extreme bliss but, rather, tentative evidence
suggests that a moderate degree of happiness tends to be “optimal” for the effects
surveyed in this paper.
Before concluding this paper we also discuss how happiness may lead to better life
outcomes and what its role may be in human evolution. There is initial evidence about the
processes that mediate between happiness and its beneficial outcomes. For instance,
positive feelings bolster the immune system and lead to fewer cardiovascular problems,
whereas anxiety and depression are linked to poorer health behaviors and problematical
physiological indicators such as inflammation. Thus, a causal impact of happiness on
health and longevity can be understood with the mediating mechanisms that are now being
uncovered. Research in the field of neuroscience provides further prospects for new
scientific insights on mediating pathways between happiness and traits or outcomes of
interest.
It naturally follows from this survey that it is important to balance economic
measures of societal progress with measures of subjective well-being and to ensure that
economic progress leads to broad improvements across life domains, not just greater
economic capacity. Given the tangible benefits to individuals and societies of moderately
high well-being, it is ever more urgent that we act to effectively put well-being at the heart
of policy and generate the conditions that allow everyone to flourish.
Table 1: Summary of the objective benefits of subjective wellbeing
!
Benefits(
Evidence!
!"#$%&'('
)*+,"-.%/'
0"123"1'.+4$#55#%.*+'
6578*-"1'3#81.*-#932$#8'&"#$%&:'
.552+"'('"+1*38.+"'9/9%"59'
)*;"8"1'8.9<'*4'&"#8%'1.9"#9":'
9%8*<"'('9293"7%.=.$.%/'%*'
.+4"3%.*+'
>8#3%.3.+,',**1'&"#$%&'="&#-.*89'''
?7""1'*4'8"3*-"8/'
?28-.-#$'('$*+,"-.%/'
Adversity*and*stress*in*childhood*is*associated*with*higher*inflammation*later*in*
life.1**
Positive* emotions* help* cardiovascular,* immune* and* endocrine* systems,2*
including*heart*rate*variability.3*Evidence*suggests*a*causal*link*between*positive*
feelings* and* reduced* inflammatory,* cardiovascular* and* neuroendocrine*
problems.4*
Positive* affect* is* associated* with* lower* rates* of* stroke* and* heart* disease* and*
susceptibility*to*viral*infection.5*
High* subjective* wellDbeing* is*linked* to* healthier* eatin g,* likelihood* of* smoking,*
exercise,*and*weight.6*
Positive* emotions* can* undo* harmful* physiological* effects* by* speeding* up*
recovery.7**
Happier*individuals*tend*to*live* longer*and*have*a* lower* risk*of*mortality,*even*
after*controlling*for*relevant*factors.8*
6+3*5":'
>8*123%.-.%/'('
@8,#+.A#%.*+#$'
B"&#-.*8'
6+38"#9"1'78*123%.-.%/'
>""8C8#%"1'('4.+#+3.#$'
7"84*85#+3"'
0"123"1'#=9"+%"".95'
D8"#%.-.%/'('3*,+.%.-"'4$"E.=.$.%/'
D**7"8#%.*+'('3*$$#=*8#%.*+'
!.,&"8'.+3*5"'
@8,#+.A#%.*+#$'7"84*85#+3"'
Individuals* with* induced* positive* emotions* were* more* productive* in* an*
experimental*setting.9'
Happy*workers*were*more* likely*to*be* rated*highly*by*supervisors*and*in*terms*
of*financial*performance.10'
Happiness*can*increase*curiosity,*creativity,*and*motivation*among*employees.11'
Happy* individuals* are* more* likely* to* engage* collaboratively* and* cooperatively*
during*negotiations.12'
WellDbeing* is* positively* associated* with* individual* earnings.13 *Longitudinal*
evidence*suggests* that* happiness*at*one*point* in* time* predicts* future*earnings,*
even*after*controlling*for*confounding*factors.14'
Greater* satisfaction* among* employees* tends* to* predict* organizationDlevel*
productivity*and*performance,*e.g.*revenue,*sales*and*profits.15'
6+1.-.12#$'('
?*3.#$'B"&#-.*8'
)*+,"8C%"85'%.5"'78"4"8"+3"9'
#+1'1"$#/"1',8#%.4.3#%.*+'
In*experiments,* individuals*with* higher* wellDbeing*and*positive*affect* are* more*
willing* to* forego* a*smaller* benefit* in* the* moment* in* order* to* obtain* a* larger*
Reduced'consumption'&'
increased'savings'
Employment'
Reduced'riskCtaking'
ProCsocial'behavior'(e.g.,'
donating'money'and'
volunteering)'
Sociability,'social'relationships'&'
networks'
benefit* in* the* future.16*Happier* individuals* may* be* better* able* to* purse* longD
term* goals* despite* shortDterm* costs* due* to* a* greater* ability* to* delay*
gratification.17'
Longitudinal* studies* find* evidence* that* happier* individuals* tend* to* spend* less*
and* save* more,* take* more* time* when* making* decisions* and* have* higher*
perceived*life*expectancies.18'
Survey* evidence* shows* the* probability* of* reDemployment* within* one* year* is*
higher*among*individuals*who*are*happier.19'
The* prevalence* of* seatDbelt* usage* and* the* likelihood* of* being* involved* in* an*
automobile* accident* were* both* linked* to* life* satisfaction* in* a*survey* of* over*
300,000*US*households.20'
Individuals* who* report* higher* subjective* wellDbeing* donate* more* time,* money,*
and*blood*to*others.21'
WellDbeing* increases* interest* in* social* activities* leading* to* more*and* higher*
quality*interactions.22*Positive* moods* also* lead* to* more* engagement* in* social*
activities.23 *The* happinessDsocial* interaction* link* is* found* across* different*
cultures*and*can*lead*to*the*transmission*of*happiness*across*social*networks.24(
Note: Further detail on each study cited in the table is included in the relevant sections of this paper.
II. Benefits of happiness
i. Happiness on health and longevity
There are many factors that influence health, such as having strong social
support, and practicing good health behaviors, such as exercising and not smoking.
Although being happy is only one of those factors, it is an important one. This is
because higher levels of subjective well-being can both directly and indirectly
influence health. Below we review the up-to-date research on whether happy people
experience better health.25
Happiness and unhappiness have been directly associated with physiological
processes underlying health and disease. For example, Kubzansky and colleagues find
that adversity and stress in childhood predict elevated markers of inflammation a few
years later. 26 And chronic inflammation that occurs over years can harm the
cardiovascular system. Cohen et al. (2003) found that positive emotions were
associated with stronger immune system responses to infection. Bhattacharyya et al.
(2008) found that positive feelings were associated with healthier levels of heart rate
variability. Negative emotions harm cardiovascular, immune, and endocrine systems
in humans, whereas positive emotions appear to help them.27 Levels of subjective
well-being influence health, with positive levels helping health and negative levels
harming it. Through an accumulation of studies, we are beginning to understand not
just that subjective well-being influences health, but how this occurs.
Because subjective well-being influences physiological processes underlying
health and disease, it is predictive of lower rates of cardiovascular disease and quicker
recovery. For example, positive affect is associated with lower rates of strokes in
senior citizens.28 Davidson et al. (2010) found in a prospective longitudinal study that
those without positive feelings were at a higher risk for heart disease than those with
some positive feelings, who in turn had higher levels of heart disease than those with
moderate positive feelings. Stress can even hinder wound healing after an injury.29
One indirect route from happiness to health is that individuals who are high in
subjective well-being are more likely to practice good health behaviors and practices.
Blanchflower et al. (2012) found that happier individuals have a healthier diet, eating
more fruits and vegetables. Ashton and Stepney (1982) reported that neurotic
individuals, people who are prone to more stress, are more likely to smoke. Pettay
(2008) found that college students high in life satisfaction were more likely to be a
healthy weight, exercise, and eat healthy foods. Schneider et al. (2009) found that
happier adolescents, as assessed by brain scans of the left prefrontal area, showed a
more positive response to moderate exercise. Garg et al. (2007) found that people put
in a sad mood as part of an experiment were more likely to eat tasty but fattening
foods, such as buttered popcorn, rather than a healthy fruit.
Using a large sample representative of the USA, Strine and her colleagues
(2008a & b) found that depressed individuals are more likely to be obese and twice as
likely to smoke, and parallel results were found for those with very high in anxiety.
Lack of exercise was associated with depression, and excessive drinking of alcohol
was associated with anxiety. Grant et al. (2009) found, in a large sample across 21
nations, that higher life satisfaction was associated across regions with a greater
likelihood of exercising and a lower likelihood of smoking. Kubzansky et al. (2012)
found that distressed adolescents are more likely to be overweight. Thus, not only is
there a direct biological path from happiness to healthier bodily systems, but
unhappiness is also associated with destructive behaviors that can exacerbate health
problems.
Another indirect effect of happiness, as will be described more fully in a next
section, is that higher happiness can lead to more positive and fulfilling social
relationships. And having these relationships promotes health.30 For instance, the
experience of prolonged stress can lead to poor health, but the presence of supportive
friends and family can help individuals during this time. In contrast, lonely
individuals experience worse health .31
An important concern with these research findings is that healthier people may
be happier because of their good health, and not the other way around. While this may
be true for some reported findings, scientific studies also show support for a causal
link going from happiness to health. Research findings have established a link from
happiness to better physiological functioning. Ong (2010) and Steptoe et al. (2009)
review various possible explanations for the effects of positive feelings on health.
Steptoe et al. (2005) found among middle-aged men and women that those high in
positive feelings had reduced inflammatory, cardiovascular, and neuroendocrine
problems. For instance, happiness was associated with a lower ambulatory heart rate
and with lower cortisol output across the day. Similarly, Rasmussen et al. (2009)
found that optimism predicted future health outcomes such as mortality, immune
function, and cancer outcomes, controlling for factors such as demographics, health,
and negative feelings. Boehm and her colleagues found that optimism and positive
emotions protect against cardiovascular disease and also predict slower disease
progression.32 They discovered that those with positive moods were more often
engaged in positive health behaviors, such as exercising and eating a nutritious diet.
Furthermore, positive feelings were associated with beneficial biological markers,
such as lower blood fat and blood pressure, and a healthier body mass index. These
associations held even controlling for level of negative moods.
Another piece of evidence supporting happiness causing good health is that
positive emotions can undo the ill-effects of negative emotions on health. Negative
emotions generate increased cardiovascular activity and redistribute blood flow to
specific skeletal muscles. It has been shown that positive emotions can undo harmful
physiological effects by speeding physiological recovery to desirable levels.33
Diener and Chan (2011) reviewed eight types of evidence that point to a
causal connection going from subjective well-being to health and longevity. They
reviewed longitudinal studies with adults, animal experiments, experiments in which
participants' moods are manipulated and biomarkers are assessed, natural quasi-
experiments, and studies in which moods and biomarkers are tracked together over
time in natural settings. Diener and Chan (2011) concluded that the evidence
overwhelmingly points to positive feelings being causally related to health.
Happiness on average leads not only to better health, but also to a longer life.
Danner et al. (2001) found that happier nuns lived about 10 years longer than their
less happy colleagues. Because the nuns all had similar diets, housing, and living
conditions, and the happiness measure was collected at a very early age many decades
before death (at age 22 on average), the study suggests a causal relation between
positive moods and longevity. In another study, Pressman and Cohen (2012) found
that psychologists who used aroused positive words (e.g., lively, vigorous) in their
autobiographies lived longer. In a longitudinal study of individuals 40 years old and
older, Wiest et al. (2011) found that both life satisfaction and positive feelings
predicted mortality, controlling for socio-economic status variables. Conversely, Russ
et al. (2012) reviewed 10 cohort studies and found that psychological distress
predicted all-cause mortality, as well as cardiovascular and cancer deaths. Russ et al.
found that even mild levels of psychological distress led to increased risk of mortality,
controlling for a number of possible confounding factors. Whereas risk of death from
cardiovascular diseases or external causes, such as accidents, was significant even at
lower levels of distress, cancer death was only related to high levels of distress. Bush
et al. (2001) found that even mild depression increased the risk of mortality after
people had experienced a heart attack.
A systematic review by Chida and Steptoe (2008) on happiness and future
mortality in longitudinal studies showed that happiness lowered the risk of mortality
in both healthy and diseased populations, even when initial health and other factors
were controlled. Moreover, the experience of positive emotions predicted mortality
over and above negative emotions, showing that the effects of subjective well-being
go beyond the absence of negativity. Therefore, not only do negative emotions predict
mortality, but positive emotions predict longevity. One reason this may be so, besides
the toll that cardiovascular and immune diseases take on unhappy people, is that stress
might lead to more rapid ageing. Epel et al. (2004) found shorter telomeres (the
endcaps protecting DNA) in women who had significant stress in their lives. Because
DNA must replicate with fidelity for an individual to remain healthy over the decades
of life, and because the telomeres protect our DNA during replication, the reduction
of telomeres due to stress leads to more rapid aging when a person chronically
experiences unhappiness.
In a large representative sample of elderly people in the UK, Steptoe and
Wardle (2011) found that higher levels of positive affect were significantly associated
with a higher probability of survival in the five years following the survey. The study
divided respondents into three groups based on the positive affect they reported over a
24-hour period and then compared their mortality rates over a five-year period
following the survey. Mortality rates among respondents in the highest positive affect
group were reduced by 35% on average relative to those in the lowest positive affect
group. This rate was robust even when controlling for demographic factors as well as
health behaviors, self-reported health, and other conditions. Those in the high and
medium positive affect groups had death rates of 3.6% and 4.6%, respectively,
compared to 7.3% for the low positive affect group. Figure 1 below shows the
differences in survival rates among the three groups in the follow-up period.
Figure 1: Proportion of individuals surviving by level of positive affect in an
analysis of the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing
High-Positive Affect group
Medium-Positive Affect group
Low-Positive Affect group
Notes: Figure from Steptoe and Wardle (2011). “Survival from affect assessment” is measured in
months from initial interview where positive affect levels where reported. The English Longitudinal
Survey of Ageing is a representative sample of older men and women living in England. Positive affect
reported on a single day by individuals between 52 and 79 years old were used. Values are adjusted for
age and sex. Respondents with the highest third of reported positive affect were 34% less likely to die
over the period studied than those in the lowest positive affect group after controlling for demographic
and health factors. Those in the high and medium positive affect groups had death rates of 3.6% and
4.6%, respectively, compared to 7.3% for the low positive affect group.
Primate studies also point to happiness affecting longevity. Weiss et al. (2011)
found that orangutans who were rated as happier by their caretakers lived longer.
Indeed, the difference between the apes that were one standard-deviation above
versus below the mean in happiness was 11 years. Because these apes often live about
50 years in captivity, happiness accounted for a very large increase in longevity.
Research on the role of happiness in human evolution (a topic explored in
more depth below) finds a relationship between well-being and successful
reproduction. A recent review by Diener et al. (2012) highlighted the evidence linking
positive mood to the frequency of sexual intercourse and fertility. For example,
Rasmussen et al. (2009) found that pregnant women who were more optimistic tended
to miscarry less frequently and have babies of a healthy weight.
The positive benefits of subjective well-being on health at the individual level
generalize to more aggregate levels. Lawless and Lucas (2011) found that places with
higher life satisfaction had greater life expectancies, with lower levels of mortality
from heart disease, homicide, liver disease, diabetes, and cancer. Similarly,
Blanchflower and Oswald (2008) found that higher levels of national well-being were
related to lower levels of national hypertension in a sample of 16 nations.
Blanchflower and Oswald (2008) also found that regions in the United Kingdom
reporting more stress also had higher rates of blood pressure. Moum (1996) found that
low subjective well-being is both a short- and long-term predictor of suicide, and
uncovered similar findings in a 20-year study. Across 32 nations, it was found that
experiencing higher life satisfaction and happiness was related to lower suicide
rates.34 These findings suggest that links between happiness and health outcomes are
not simply relative in nature as they persist in aggregate and cross-national studies.
Happiness can therefore influence health outcomes for both individual citizens and
entire societies.
There is also evidence that negative affect can worsen health, even making
illness more likely. For example, depressed people are substantially more likely to
have cardiovascular problems, such as heart disease and strokes. Rugulies (2000)
found in a review of 11 studies that depressed feelings predict coronary heart disease
and that clinical levels of depression predict even more strongly. Similarly, when a
person is angry and hostile they are more likely to suffer from coronary heart
disease.35 Depression is associated with unhealthy physiological processes, such as
inflammation,36 which is believed to be connected to the development of heart
diseases. Antidepressant medications can lower inflammation. A review by Zorrilla et
al. (2001) found that stress is related to a weaker immune system. Studies on fertility
provide yet more evidence on how negative emotions can be detrimental to healthy
functioning. Fertility is lower among depressed women.37 An unhappy pregnancy is
more likely to lead to a premature and low birth weight child.38 However, as
discussed above, the effect of negative affect is not a mirror image of that observed
for positive affect. In a study of susceptibility to developing a cold, Cohen et al (2003)
found that individuals with positive emotional styles had greater resistance to the
virus when controlling for other factors, whereas negative emotions were not
associated with resistance. This suggests that positive and negative affect may impact
on health through different pathways but further study is needed to understand this
interaction.
ii. Happiness on income, productivity, and organizational behavior
The experience of happiness is beneficial to workplace success because it
promotes workplace productivity, creativity, and cooperation. There are several
reasons why this is the case. The experience of positive feelings motivates people to
succeed at work and to persist with efforts to attain their goals. As discussed above,
individuals who are happier are more likely to be healthy and will, in turn, tend to be
more productive (in part, simply because happier and healthier individuals will take
fewer sick days). In addition, individuals who are happier better integrate information
leading to new ideas, which leads to creativity and innovation. Finally, individuals
who are happier tend to have better social relations. In the context of work this leads
to greater cooperation among coworkers and with customers.
10
Oswald et al. (2012) investigated how positive feelings influence productivity
in an experimental setting. In an experiment involving piece-rate pay for research
participants across a number of days, the economists found that those who were put in
a positive mood had a greater quantity of work output (about 10-12%), but no less
quality of output. Those performing the task at low and medium levels of productivity
were helped most by being put in a good mood. As part of that same research, Oswald
et al. (2012) also found that a bad mood induced by family illness or bereavement had
a detrimental impact on productivity.
Employees who are high in subjective well-being are more likely to achieve
more while at work. Peterson et al. (2011) found that happy workers – optimistic and
hopeful, resilient and high in self-efficacy – were more likely to be high in supervisor-
rated performance and in financial performance. Conversely, whereas positive
feelings reduce absenteeism from work, negative feelings increase absenteeism as
well as turnover.39
Happiness has also been shown to enhance curiosity and creativity. Foremost,
positive feelings are associated with curiosity and creativity.40 Leitzel (2001) found
that happy people are more likely to feel energetic and interested in doing things, as
well as scoring higher on measures of curiosity. Further, there is a large experimental
research literature showing that people put in a good mood tend to be more original,
creative, and show greater cognitive flexibility.41 Both Amabile et al (2005) and
George and Zhou (2007) found that workers are more creative when they experience
positive moods. Indeed, two recent meta-analyses of experimental and non-
experimental studies showed that although the strength of effects depend on the
context and motivational focus, happiness is related to and generates creativity.42
A major reason for the success of happy individuals and organizations is that
they experience on average more positive social relationships. Research clearly shows
that happy workers are more cooperative and collaborative in negotiations than
unhappy ones. In general, positive emotions boost cooperative and collaborative
behavior in negotiations rather than withdrawal or competition.43 Individuals who are
in a positive mood are more willing to make concessions during negotiations.44
Through cooperation, they reach a better joint solution in negotiations.45 Individuals
in a positive mood are more likely to make cooperative choices in a prisoner’s
dilemma game as well.46 People in a positive mood are also more likely to show
cohesion with their group. Recent experimental studies have shown that positive
emotions lead to trust and cooperation when specific conditions are met.47 Overall,
happiness leads to cooperation and collaboration in the workplace, particularly so in
situations involving negotiation.
On the other hand, negative emotions in the workplace, especially chronic or
intense ones, can be very detrimental to the organization. For example, Felps et al.
(2006) found that a single negative individual in a work unit often brings down the
morale and functioning of the entire group.
One indicator of the subjective well-being of employees is job satisfaction.48
A quantitative review found that job satisfaction is a key predictor of job
performance, showing that happy employees are better performers in their
11
workplace. 49 To establish a causal relation, a meta-analysis of panel data
demonstrated that job satisfaction predicted future performance, but performance did
not predict future job satisfaction.50 Erdogan et al. (2012) reviewed the research
showing that individuals with higher life satisfaction are more likely to have higher
levels of career satisfaction, lower turnover intentions, and higher organizational
commitment.
In line with the notion that happier workers are better workers, higher well-
being is also shown to be associated with higher income51 and future income.52 De
Neve & Oswald (2012) used a large US representative panel study to show that
adolescents and young adults who report higher life satisfaction or positive affect
grew up to earn significantly higher levels of income later in life. They used siblings
as comparison controls, and also accounted for factors such as intelligence and health,
as well as the human capacity to imagine later socioeconomic outcomes and
anticipate the resulting feelings in current well-being (see Figure 2 below). Thus, to
date, four longitudinal studies have systematically found that happiness at one point in
time predicts higher future income, controlling for relevant factors such as
intelligence, parental income, and even a sizable part of any genetic predispositions.53
Figure 2: Longitudinal relationship between subjective well-being during
adolescence and young adulthood (ages 16, 18 and 22) and later earnings (at age
29)
Notes: Figure from De Neve & Oswald (2012). The bars represent the response categories for positive
affect (at ages 16 and 18) and life satisfaction (at age 22), from lowest to highest levels, and relate this
to the mean income for the respondents in each category at age 29. Across the sample, the mean
income at age 29 was $34,632. Large samples were observed for each category (N=14,867 for positive
affect at age 16, N=11,253 for positive affect at age 18 and N=12,415 for life satisfaction at age 22). A
margin of error (i.e. 2 Standard Errors) is included around each estimate.
Subjective well-being brings about greater success at the organizational level
as well. Bockerman and Ilmakunnas (2012) found that job satisfaction predicts the
productivity of manufacturing plants. Harter et al. (2010) found in a longitudinal
study of 10 large organizations that worker engagement makes a difference to
productivity. Work units in which employees were satisfied and otherwise felt highly
engaged with their work led to improvements in the bottom line, measured in terms of
income. The effect sizes of the well-being measures remain rel-
atively stable between Table 1 (full panel) and Table 2 (sibling
panel). The lower signicance levels in Table 2 are presumably
due to the reduced number of observations in the sibling panel as
well as having accounted for family xed effects.
Tables S7 and S8 present results for an individual xed-effects
model and a Granger causality analysis that use the available
information on earnings in 2001 (age 22). Both model speci-
cations obtain highly signicant results for the effect of lagged
subjective well-being on earnings. However, we do not lend these
results full credence, given that earnings at age 22 may not yet ac-
curately represent individual income and also because these panel
data allow for only one time interval, and the exogeneity assumption
necessary for panel data models is unlikely to be satised.
If income is indeed endogenous to happiness, it becomes im-
portant to study how happiness comes to inuence a persons
income. Table 3 presents results for our investigation into po-
tential mediating pathways. These univariate SobelGoodman
Table 1. Earnings equations: Linear regression models of log income at age 29 (2008) on lagged
subjective well-being and covariates
Independent variable
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
Coeff. Pvalue Coeff. Pvalue Coeff. Pvalue Coeff. Pvalue
Positive affect (1994) 0.033 0.002 0.014 0.314
Positive affect (1996) 0.044 0.000 0.030 0.037
Life satisfaction (2001) 0.051 0.000 0.047 0.000
Positive affect (2008) 0.092 0.000 0.089 0.000 0.086 0.000 0.074 0.000
Male 0.149 0.000 0.146 0.000 0.147 0.000 0.145 0.000
Age 0.089 0.000 0.084 0.000 0.088 0.000 0.085 0.000
College 0.210 0.000 0.216 0.000 0.212 0.000 0.201 0.000
IQ 0.038 0.001 0.037 0.004 0.043 0.000 0.030 0.022
Medication 0.013 0.174 0.013 0.253 0.013 0.197 0.014 0.211
Height 0.036 0.008 0.039 0.011 0.035 0.011 0.037 0.016
Self-esteem (1994) 0.057 0.000 0.041 0.003
Self-esteem (1996) 0.044 0.001 0.015 0.306
Self-esteem (2001) 0.054 0.000 0.030 0.017
Black 0.068 0.000 0.064 0.000 0.063 0.000 0.065 0.000
Hispanic 0.056 0.000 0.065 0.000 0.051 0.000 0.064 0.000
Asian 0.062 0.000 0.063 0.000 0.061 0.010 0.064 0.000
Intercept 10.15 0.000 10.14 0.000 10.15 0.000 10.14 0.000
N11,080 8,620 11,086 8,585
R
2
0.12 0.12 0.13 0.13
Variable coefcients (Coeff.) are standardized, and Pvalues are presented. Variable denitions are in
Table S2.
Fig. 1. The longitudinal relationship between subjective well-being (at ages 16, 18, and 22) and later earnings (at age 29). Response categories for positive
affect (at ages 16 and 18) and life satisfaction (at age 22) are presented in relationship with their respective mean income levels at about age 29. Mean income
across the sample is $34,632 at age 29. N=14,867 for positive affect at age 16, N=11,253 for positive affect at age 18, and N=12,415 for life satisfaction at
age 22. The original positive affect variable categories are reshaped to a 5-point scale for ease of comparison. Error bars (2 SEs) are shown.
De Neve and Oswald PNAS
|
December 4, 2012
|
vol. 109
|
no. 49
|
19955
ECONOMIC
SCIENCES
12
revenue, sales, and profit.54 On the other hand, reverse causality going from company
success to employee satisfaction was weaker. An analysis of the “100 Best
Companies to Work For in America” revealed that they increased more in equity
value compared to the industry benchmarks. The resulting higher returns were about
3% per year.
The study by Harter and his colleagues (2010), based on 2,178 work units in
10 large companies, found that engaged and satisfied workers led to greater revenue,
sales, and profits. The two factors that mediated the relation between employee
engagement and the performance outcomes were customer loyalty and employee
retention. It makes intuitive sense that customers would prefer to interact with
positive employees and thus frequent the business. Employee retention is a large
challenge for modern companies both because it is expensive to replace employees,
especially highly skilled ones, and because more senior employees have more
experience on the job. Thus, it is not surprising that employee engagement, resulting
in customer loyalty and employee retention, accounted for 10% of the variability in
the productivity of the corporations studied.
iii. Happiness on individual and social behavior
Subjective well-being has an impact on individual behavior and decision-
making. Happiness and positive affect have been identified as determinants of
economic behavior ranging from consumption and savings to time preferences and
risk-taking. Research in psychology and economics suggests this may occur through
improved integration of information and broadened focus of attention in happier
individuals. 55 Thus, happier individuals may be better able to evaluate the
implications of decisions with short and long term trade-offs, resulting in decisions
that reflect greater self-control and appropriate risk-taking.
Well-being can influence how individuals evaluate outcomes that may occur
in the present or future a concept known in economics as time preference, or
discounting. In survey and experimental evidence, Ifcher and Zarghamee (2011a)
found that subjective well-being and positive affect were associated with less
preference for consumption in the present relative to the future. Using a randomized
assignment experiment, they observed that among the group where greater positive
affect was induced, participants were less likely to discount future payments, i.e. they
were more likely to give up a smaller payment in the current period to receive a larger
payment at a later point in time. This implies that individuals with greater positive
affect may be more able to exercise self-control or delay gratification (i.e. foregoing
smaller short term benefits in order to receive greater benefits in the future or to avoid
longer term costs). Happy individuals are motivated to pursue long-term goals despite
short-term costs.56 Fry (1975) found that children placed in a happy mood better
resisted temptation. Additionally, Lerner and Weber (2012) found in lab experiments
that inducing sadness among participants led to a greater discounting of future
rewards than those in a neutral state. Moreover, lack of self-control is also related to
over-consumption, obesity, and financial decisions, suggesting that changes in well-
being may influence their prevalence.57
Greater self-control and longer-term time preferences among happier people
13
have been linked to consumption and saving behavior. Guven (2012) analyzed two
representative longitudinal household surveys in the Netherlands and Germany to
estimate the causal relationship (if any) between happiness and consumption and
saving behaviors. The regression results found that happier people were more likely to
save more and consume less than others. Further, happier people had different
expectations about the future than those less happy. These individuals were more
optimistic about the future, took more time when making decisions, and had higher
perceived life expectancies (i.e. moving from “neither happy or unhappy” to “happy”
was associated with 1.1 year increase in perceived life expectancy).58 Thus, happier
individuals may be more forward-thinking and willing to consider the long-term
implications of decisions taken in the present, leading to “better” decisions for
themselves and society.
The probability of being re-employed has also been linked to individual
happiness. Among individuals recently entering unemployment in Germany, Krause
(2012) found a statistically significant positive relationship between job seekers with
higher than average well-being and the probability of re-employment within a year.
Additionally, these individuals were more likely to enter into self-employment,
suggesting a link between happiness and entrepreneurship. Interestingly, the effect of
happiness on re-employment decreased at the extremes, indicating that an “optimal”
level of happiness may exist.
Research on individual risk-taking provides evidence of a relationship
between happiness and risk-related behavior. According to economic theory, happier
individuals have more to lose from engaging in risky behavior that may carry the risk
of injury or death. Happier individuals should therefore be more willing to engage in
activities that reduce risk. Goudie et al. (forthcoming) found that seatbelt use and not
being involved in a motor vehicle accident were both more likely among those with
higher subjective well-being (see Figure 3 with respect to seatbelt use). In a
representative sample of 313,354 US households, the authors estimated that
individuals who reported being “very satisfied” with life were 5.3% more likely to
always wear a seatbelt in the survey, even after controlling for potentially
confounding factors. When Goudie et al. (forthcoming) looked at the probability of
motor vehicle accidents, they found that individuals with higher levels of life
satisfaction were less likely to be involved in an accident several years later59. While
these statistical analyses cannot fully rule out the possibility of reverse causality, the
results are robust to including a number of confounding variables and provide strong
evidence for a positive relationship between happiness and risk-avoiding behavior.
14
Figure 3: Frequency of seatbelt use by subjective well-being in a US
representative sample
Notes: Figure from Goudie et al. (forthcoming). Data is from the Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance
System, a random-digit telephone survey in the US, N=313,354. Pearson’s chi-squared statistic =
3,242, p-value < 2.2 x 10-16. Cross-tabulation figures indicate that subjective well-being and seatbelt
use are strongly correlated but this does not account for other factors that may explain this relationship.
Goudie et al. (2013) use regression analysis to control for other potentially confounding factors and
find the association is robust to these controls. Individuals who report they are “very satisfied” with life
are 5.3% more likely to state they always wear a seatbelt. The authors also find that subjective well-
being at the time of the survey is statistically significantly associated with a lower probability of having
a motor vehicle accident several years later (even after controlling for confounding factors).
Research studies also indicate a powerful link between high subjective well-
being and social behavior, such as being a better friend, colleague, neighbor, and
citizen. People who are in a positive mood see others more inclusively and
sympathetically. For example, they are less biased against other ethnic groups.60
Nelson (2009) found that people in a positive mood induction condition, as compared
to neutral and negative mood conditions, showed greater compassion, perspective
taking, and sympathy for a person experiencing distress.
Individuals who report high subjective well-being give more to their
communities — in both time and money. Morrison et al. (2012) found that both life
satisfaction and positive feelings predicted reports of donating money to charity,
helping a stranger, and volunteering activities. Oishi et al. (2007) found that happier
people volunteer more. Aknin et al. (2013) found in a study of 136 countries that
prosocial uses of money by happy people generalized across regions of the world.
However, further research is underway to clarify the causal relationship between
prosocial spending and happiness. Priller and Shupp (2011) found slightly higher
rates of blood donation, as well as monetary giving to charity, among happier
individuals. They also found that those who were satisfied with their incomes were
more likely to donate money to worthy causes.
Do happy moods cause the helping behavior and good citizenship? It is a
consistent finding in social psychology experiments that when people are induced into
a good mood, by various means, they are more likely to help others.61 These
experimental studies in which people who are put into a good mood and compared to
those in a neutral mood leave little doubt that happier feelings generally tend to
increase helping. The fact that people give both more time and money when they are
put into a positive mood in an experiment indicates that being happy raises prosocial
Proportion of individuals
Increasing SWB
Very satisfied
Satisfied
Dissatisfied
Very Dissatisfied
0.0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1.0
Always Nearly always Sometimes Seldom Never
Seatbelt use:
Sample size n=3295
n=13524
n=152021
n=144514
FIGURE 1
FREQUENCY OF SEATBELT USE CROSS-TABULATED BY SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING (SWB).
EACH CATEGORY CONTAINS AT LEAST 101 INDIVIDUALS.PEARSONSCHI-SQUARED
STATISTIC IS 3242 (P-VAL U E p<2.21016).
25
15
behavior.62 Aknin et al. (2012) suggest that the relation between mood and helping is
circular as shown in Figure 4. When people are in a good mood they tend to help
others; helping others in turn fosters a good mood. Thus, friends, family, neighbors,
and the society as a whole tend to profit from happy people because these individuals
are more likely to be helpful to others.
Figure 4: Model of positive feedback loop between prosocial spending and
happiness
Notes: Figure from Aknin, Dunn and Norton (2012). The model posits that prosocial spending
promotes happiness and, in turn, happiness improves the probability of future prosocial spending.
Having supportive relationships boosts subjective well-being, but having high
subjective well-being in turn leads to better social relationships.63 Thus, good
relationships both cause happiness and are caused by it. Two major reasons why
happiness benefits social relationships are because happiness increases a person’s
level of sociability and also improves the quality of social interactions. Happier
people have a larger quantity and better quality of friendships and family
relationships.64
Frequent positive emotions create a tendency in people to be more sociable. In
a laboratory experiment people placed in a positive mood expressed greater interest in
social and prosocial activities compared to those in a neutral condition, whereas those
placed in a negative mood indicated lower interest in social activities.65 This pattern
was replicated in a second study that found an interest in social and prosocial
activities among those in a good mood. People who were placed in a good mood
expected social activities to be more rewarding than those not placed in a good mood.
Similarly, other experimental studies have demonstrated that inducing happiness, in
contrast to sadness, makes people more likely to express liking for others they meet
for the first time.66 On the other hand, the absence of positive feelings is accompanied
by feeling bored, unsociable, uninterested in things, slowed down, and unenergetic,
reflecting a lack of active involvement with the environment and other people.67 It has
also been shown that depressed individuals cause others to react in a negative
manner.68 This can lead to unwillingness to have future interactions with those who
have low happiness.
The links between positive moods and sociability are not just in terms of
feeling sociable, but translate into actual behavior. Cunningham (1988a) discovered
that people in an induced positive mood condition compared to a negative mood
2 Method
Fifty-one individuals (51% female, M
age
=20.3) on the University of British Columbia
campus were asked to recall and describe in as much detail as possible the last time they
spent either $20 or $100 on either themselves or someone else. That is, participants were
randomly assigned to one of four recall conditions in a 2 (spending amount: $20 vs. $100)
92 (purchase target: self vs. someone else) design. Recall instructions were designed to
elicit vivid reminiscence and modeled on those used by Strack et al. (1985). After
describing this memory, participants reported their happiness on the Subjective Happiness
Scale (a=.86; Lyubomirsky and Lepper 1999).
Next, participants were offered a choice between two monetary windfall amounts ($5
vs. $20) and two different ways to spend this money. These four spending options were
selected to map on to the conditions used in previous research (Dunn et al. 2008), enabling
comparison across studies. Consistent with this previous research, participants were told
they could spend the money on (1) a bill, expense, or gift for themselves (personal
spending) or (2) on a gift for someone else or donation to charity (prosocial spending).
Participants were told that they should choose whichever option would make them the
happiest and that once their decision was made, they would receive the appropriate amount
of money with a reminder of the spending guidelines. Importantly, this procedure was
conducted in an anonymous fashion to mitigate social desirability concerns by giving
participants cue cards labeled A, B, C, or D on one side with a condition description on the
other side (Fig. 2). Participants were informed that all research assistants were unaware of
which condition assignment corresponded to each letter, so the research assistants would
never know which spending experience they had selected. Participants were given several
minutes to read the spending descriptions on the cue cards and choose their spending
experience. After deciding, participants informed the research assistant of their choice
using the appropriate letter (A, B, C, or D) and then received an envelope with the windfall
amount and a spending direction reminder.
1
Fig. 1 Model of positive
feedback loop between prosocial
spending and happiness. Recalled
acts of prosocial spending lead to
higher levels of happiness.
Happiness, in turn, increases the
likelihood of engaging in future
acts of prosocial spending
1
Although this procedure was carefully designed to minimize social desirability concerns, it is conceivable
that some participants who selected the prosocial spending option actually intended to spend the money on
themselves. This possibility is unlikely given that previous work has shown that the majority of participants
are quite willing to express a preference for spending money on themselves, even when social desirability
concerns are higher (Dunn et al. 2008). In addition, research suggests that making an initial commitment to
engage in prosocial behavior does lead to longer term prosocial behaviour (Nelson and Norton 2005).
Happiness Runs in a Circular Motion 349
123
16
condition were more talkative. Mehl et al. (2010) monitored people's everyday
conversations for four days and assessed happiness through both self-reports and
informant reports. They found that happy participants spent about 25% less time alone
and about 70% more time talking when they were with others. Furthermore, the happy
participants engaged in less small talk and more substantive conversations compared
to their unhappy peers.
Recent evidence shows the happiness-relationship link occurs across cultures.
Lucas et al. (2000) found that across the world positive feelings were associated with
tendencies for affiliation, dominance, venturesomeness, and social interaction.
Similarly, a world survey of 123 nations found that the experience of positive feelings
was strongly related to good social relationships across different socio-cultural
regions.69
Happy people are not just more sociable; they also experience higher-quality
social relationships. Kazdin et al. (1982) found that children put in a positive mood
showed greater social skills and confidence in social behavior than those not put in a
good mood. Boehm and Lyubomirsky (2008) reviewed evidence showing that happy
people tend to be more popular and likable. One study showed that reports of better
interaction quality were not merely a function of the happy person’s perceptions, but
that observers similarly rated happier individuals as having better interactions with
strangers.70
Happiness has the potential to generate positive snowball effects in society.
Research has shown that people who are happier are likely to bring happiness to those
around them, resulting in networks of happier individuals. It was found that happiness
extends up to three degrees of separation, and longitudinal models show that
individuals who are surrounded by happy people are likely to become happier in the
future.71
Happiness can also have effects on the long-term quality of relationships.
Luhmann et al. (2013) found that unmarried people high in life satisfaction are more
likely to get married in the following years and less likely to get separated or divorced
if they get married. Conversely, Stutzer and Frey (2006) found low life satisfaction
prior to courtship predicted later dissolution of the marriage.
Depression, which is characterized by low or absent positive feelings, creates
problems in social relationships such as divorce, limited social support, and distancing
from one’s neighbors.72 Even minor depression results in problems in social relations,
such as higher rates of divorce.73 Even those recovering from depression show
impairments in the social and occupational domains.74 In addition, clinical depression
interferes with executive functioning, which is a hallmark of human’s special adaptive
abilities. For example, Fossati et al. (2002) review evidence indicating that depressed
individuals suffer deficits in problem solving and planning. Snyder (2012) reviewed
extensive evidence showing that depressed people suffer substantially from broad
impairments in executive functions, such as planning, with strong effect sizes varying
from 0.32 to 0.97.
In sum, there is substantial evidence connecting positive moods to higher
sociability and better quality of social relationships, and the opposite is the case for
17
negative moods and depression. Happier people enjoy the company of others, and
find that interacting with people is more rewarding compared to less happy
individuals. Others in turn enjoy interacting with happy individuals. Those high in
subjective well-being thus have more rewarding and stable social relationships.
III. Moderation, mediation, and the evolutionary role of happiness
Although happy people and societies have a number of advantages, this does
not imply that high subjective well-being is a panacea for everything. To illustrate,
happiness can facilitate good health but is not a guarantee of it. Happy individuals
may die at a young age. However, on average they will live longer. We can make
statements about the effects of average happiness using the notion of ceteris paribus
(i.e. assuming “all other things being equal”) because in particular cases there will be
other factors that override the influence of high subjective well-being.
Not every study has found positive benefits for long-term happiness. A few
studies find no differences between happier and less happy individuals, and the rare
study has shown opposite effects. This is common in research because of sampling,
methodology, and other differences between studies. Nonetheless, reviews that
summarize results across studies have virtually always shown benefits for high
subjective well-being. One reason for the few null findings is that happiness will not
show its value in all samples and contexts. For instance, for young adults there might
be no differences in health or longevity due to happiness because young adults very
rarely die and mostly have healthy bodies. The results of happiness and unhappiness
become more manifest as adults age. Similarly, one would not be surprised if
happiness did not reduce divorce in a nation where divorce is virtually nonexistent.
Another caution about the conclusion that happiness is desirable is that people
do not need to be constantly euphoric or ecstatic. Happy people most of the time feel
merely pleasant a mild positive state. Only occasionally do happy people feel
intensely positive. Oishi et al. (2007) found that although the happiest individuals did
very well in social relationships, the moderately happy not 100% satisfied often
did the best in achievement domains. There is evidence that frequent high-arousal
emotions could be harmful to health.75 Krause (2012) shows that re-employment
prospects actually decreased for those with extreme levels of happiness. Furthermore,
in a randomized lab experiment, Ifcher and Zarghamee (2011b) found that positive
affect increased overconfidence among participants in the treatment group. Thus,
extremely high happiness is not a recipe for extremely effective functioning, and in
fact, moderate happiness can be more helpful.
It is important to note that happy people also occasionally feel unhappy, and
this is not necessarily undesirable. Gruber et al. (2011) and Forgas (2007), as well as
others, have shown that in some situations negative emotions can help people to
respond more effectively. Thus, happiness does not mean a complete absence of
negative feelings. The happy person, however, does not feel chronic negative
feelings; he or she experiences negative feelings only occasionally, not frequently,
and in appropriate situations.
18
An important question that is receiving increasing attention is how well-being
and positive emotions may influence life outcomes. This is an emerging area of
research with important contributions from psychology and neuroscience. The
pathways leading from happiness to the life outcomes discussed in this paper can
either be direct or be subject to moderation and/or mediation by other variables that
influence the effect that subjective well-being may have on a trait or outcome of
interest. Our discussion here is mostly on mediating pathways that may carry some
part of the influence of happiness onto the outcome of interest and thus help explain
the relationship. One branch of thinking in psychology posits that positive emotions
broaden cognitive capacity and attention, allowing individuals to engage in the
behaviors and build the skills associated with better health, productivity, and social
interaction.76 Evidence from lab experiments provides initial backing for this theory.
For example, Fredrickson and Branigan (2005) found that participants where positive
emotions were induced showed greater scope of cognition and attention in
psychological tests.
Studies focusing on neurological processes also support this approach and
provide evidence for a connection between well-being and brain structure.
Experiments using brain imaging to monitor participants’ neurological processes have
reported that positive affect is associated with activity in a part of the brain that is
associated with “exploratory modes of thought and behavior.”77 Further, Schmitz et
al. (2009) found that affect can also alter neurological processing of visual stimuli
specifically, positive affect led to a widening of individuals’ field of vision. Small-
scale trials of the effect of mindfulness training, a type of meditation that has been
linked to improved well-being in psychological studies, have also been shown to
increase grey matter in parts of the brain that are believed to regulate cognition and
emotion.78 Happiness may therefore be linked to neurological and cognitive processes
that influence human behavior and particularly to behaviors that require broader and
more integrative thinking (e.g. considering benefits over a longer time period or
helping others).
In a promising new development in the study of mediating pathways between
subjective well-being and health outcomes, Fredrickson and colleagues (2013)
provide preliminary evidence for different epigenetic dynamics as a result of varying
levels and types of happiness. The authors find that varied states of well-being
influence gene expression with particular relevance to genotypes underlying the
immune system. Although the study is small-scale and is mostly interested in the
epigenetic effects of different types of well-being (hedonic and eudaimonic well-
being) it opens a promising new direction in the study of how happiness may
influence health outcomes.
In their study of happiness in young adulthood and earnings later in life, De
Neve & Oswald (2012) shed light on the potential pathways between happiness and
income in a longitudinal survey. Their mediation tests reveal a direct effect as well as
indirect effects that carry the influence from happiness to income. Significant
mediating pathways include obtaining a college degree and a job, higher degrees of
optimism and extraversion, and less neuroticism.79
Given the increasing evidence for a strong connection between happiness and
behavior, a handful of studies have started to investigate the role of well-being in
19
human evolution. Happiness is argued to play a role in promoting evolutionary
success in two possible ways: (1) the experience of happiness acts as a reward for
behaviors that increase the likelihood of evolutionary success (e.g. survival,
reproduction, resource accumulation, etc.); or, alternatively, (2) given that happiness
is beneficial to survival and other important life outcomes (such as those discussed
throughout this paper), it has persisted as an evolutionary characteristic.
Happiness as a reward mechanism for evolutionarily-advantageous behaviors
has been explored in psychological and neurological research. A review of laboratory
experiments by Wise (2004) highlighted the critical role dopamine plays in the
neurological learning processes that embed how the brain anticipates reward and
prompts action to obtain this reward. For example, Wise (2004) discusses a study
where mice whose dopamine production is impaired are less able to undertake
previously learned tasks to receive a reward (e.g. pressing a certain lever to receive
food). Psychologists have argued elsewhere that positive affect and dopamine levels
are connected.80 They hypothesize that the positive affect feedback from goal-directed
behavior and the associated dopamine production are crucial to understanding how
humans “learn” what behaviors and habits promote evolutionary success. This fits
with other evolutionary theories that suggest the pursuit and experience of happiness
incentivizes and increases the probability of successfully engaging in behaviors that
improve health, productivity, and reproduction.81
Diener et al. (forthcoming) find that in a globally representative sample, 70%
of respondents reported enjoying much of the previous day.82 The fact that happiness
is a relatively common human trait can be considered indicative of its important role
in evolutionary fitness. The authors also review the evidence that “positive mood
offset,” or the presence of positive mood in a neutral state, is associated with
characteristics, such as longevity, material and social resource accumulation, and
fertility, that have allowed humans to propagate successfully.
IV. Conclusion
Existing scientific evidence indicates that subjective well-being has an
objective impact across a broad range of behavioral traits and life outcomes, and does
not simply follow from them. In fact, we observe the existence of a dynamic
relationship between happiness and other important aspects of our lives with effects
running in both directions. Experimental research in which moods and emotions are
induced in some participants and their actions are compared to a control group show
that positive moods lead to creativity, sociability, altruism, and beneficial
physiological patterns. Levels of subjective well-being are found to predict future
health, mortality, productivity, and income, controlling statistically for other possible
determinants. For example, young people who are less happy many years before they
meet their future spouse later show higher rates of divorce compared to their happier
peers. Furthermore, predictions in the other direction, from conditions to subjective
well-being (that is, conditions influencing happiness) are also positive, helping to
create feedback loops that may raise the longer-term happiness effects.
Although high subjective well-being tends to help people function better, it is
of course not magic or a cure-all. Happy people do get sick and do lose friends. Not
all happy people are productive workers. Happiness is like any other factor that aids
20
health and functioning—all other things being equal it is likely (but not guaranteed) to
help. Needless to say that many other factors such as personality, intelligence, and
social capital are also important for good functioning.
It is important to emphasize that research does not prescribe extreme bliss but,
rather, tentative evidence suggests that a moderate degree of happiness tends to be
“optimal” for the effects surveyed in this paper. Thus, a desirable level of happiness
would imply feeling mildly to moderately positive most of the time, with occasional
negative emotions in appropriate situations.
There is initial evidence about the processes that mediate between happiness
and beneficial outcomes. For instance, happiness is associated with greater
cooperation, motivation, and creativity, which in turn are instrumental to success in
business, and in life as a whole. Conversely, depression creates problems, such as
illness and quitting one’s job more frequently, that all lead to less success in the
workplace. Similarly, positive feelings harness the immune system and lead to fewer
cardiovascular problems, whereas anxiety and depression are linked to poorer health
behaviors and problematical physiological indicators, such as inflammation. Thus, a
causal mechanism of happiness on health and longevity can be understood with the
mediating mechanisms that are now being uncovered. Research in the field of
neuroscience provides further prospects for new scientific insights on mediating
pathways between happiness to behavioral traits and socio-economic outcomes of
interest.
It naturally follows from this survey that it is important to balance economic
measures of societal progress with measures of subjective well-being, to ensure that
economic progress leads to broad improvements across life domains, not just greater
economic capacity. By assessing subjective well-being as well as economic variables,
a society can gauge whether overall net progress is positive in terms of raising human
well-being. Diener et al. (2009) detail the case for national accounts of well-being.
Most arguments for putting happiness more center-stage in policy-making have been
normative in nature; happiness is what would appear to matter most to most people.
The aim of this paper is to complement and inform the normative reasoning with a
survey of the “hard” evidence on the benefits of subjective well-being across
outcomes of importance, such as health, income, and social behavior. A better
understanding of the objective benefits of raising happiness may help in estimating
the potential impact of making happiness more central in policy-making and in
enhancing policy evaluation by informing cost-benefit analyses. Indeed, an argument
could be constructed that raising subjective well-being leads to positive externalities
or spillover effects across a number of policy domains, ranging from health to traffic
safety. Given the tangible benefits to individuals and societies of moderately high
well-being, it is imperative that we act to effectively put well-being at the heart of
policy and generate the conditions that allow everyone to flourish.
Bibliography
Aknin, L. B., Dunn, E. W., & Norton, M. I. (2012). Happiness runs in a circular
motion: Evidence for a positive feedback loop between prosocial spending and
happiness. Journal of Happiness Studies, 13, 347-355.
Aknin, L. B., Barrington-Leigh, C. P., Dunn, E. W., Helliwell, J. F., Biswas-Diener,
R., Kemeza, I., Nyende, P., Ashton-James, C. E., & Norton, M. I. (2013).
Prosocial spending and well-being: Cross-cultural evidence for a
psychological universal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104,
635-652.
Amabile, T. M., Barsade, S. G., Mueller, J. S., & Staw, B. (2005). Affect and
creativity at work. Administrative Science Quarterly, 50, 367-403.
Anik, L., Aknin, L. B., Norton, M. I. & Dunn, E. W. (August 6, 2009). Feeling good
about giving: The benefits (and costs) of self-interested charitable behavior.
Harvard Business School Marketing Unit Working Paper No. 10-012.
Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1444831 or
http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1444831
Appleton, A. A. Buka, S. L., McCormick, M. C., Koenen, K. C., Loucks, E. B.,
Gilman, S. E., & Kubzansky, L. D. (2011). Emotional functioning at age 7
years is associated with C-reactive protein in middle adulthood.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 73, 295-303.
Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, A. U. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of
positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review, 106, 529-
550.
Ashby, F. G., Valentin, V. V., & Turken, U. (2002). The effects of positive affect and
arousal on working memory and executive attention. In S. Moore & M.
Oaksford (Eds.), Emotional cognition: From brain to behaviour (pp. 245-287).
Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Ashton, H., & Stepney, R. (1982). Smoking: Psychology and pharmacology. London:
Tavistock Publications.
Aspinwall, L. G. (1998). Rethinking the role of positive affect in self-regulation.
Motivation and Emotion, 22, 1-32.
Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta-analysis of 25 years of
mood-creativity research: Hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus?
Psychological Bulletin, 134, 779-806.
Baron, R. A. (1987). Interviewer's moods and reactions to job applicants: The
influence of affective states on applied social judgments. Journal of Applied
Social Psychology, 17, 911-926.
22
Baron, R. A. (1990). Environmentally induced positive affect: Its impact on self-
efficacy, task performance, negotiation, and conflict. Journal of Applied
Social Psychology, 20, 368-384.
Baron, R. A., Fortin, S. P., Frei, R. L., Hauver, L. A., & Shack, M. L. (1990).
Reducing organizational conflict: The role of socially-induced positive affect.
The International Journal of Conflict Management, 1, 133-152
Baron, R. A., Rea, M. S., & Daniels, S. G. (1992). Effects of indoor lighting
(illuminance and spectral distribution) on the performance of cognitive tasks
and interpersonal behaviors: The potential mediating role of positive affect.
Motivation and Emotion, 16, 1-33.
Barsade, S. (2002). The ripple effect: Emotional contagion and its influence on group
behavior. Administrative Science Quarterly, 47, 644-675.
Beck, D. A. & Koenig, H. G. (1996). Minor depression: A review of the literature.
International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 26, 177-209.
Berry, D. S., & Hansen, J. S. (1996). Positive affect, negative affect, and social
interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 796-809.
Bhattacharyya, M. R., Whitehead, D. L., Rakhit, R., & Steptoe, A. (2008). Depressed
mood, positive affect, and heart rate variability in patients with suspected
coronary artery disease. Psychosomatic Medicine, 70, 1020-1027.
Blanchflower, D. G., & Oswald, A. J. (2008). Hypertension and happiness across
nations. Journal of Health Economics, 27, 218-233.
Blanchflower, D. G., Oswald, A. J., & Stewart-Brown, S. (2012). Is psychological
well-being linked to the consumption of fruit and vegetables? Social
Indicators Research, in press.
Bockerman, P., & Ilmakunnas, P. (2012). The job-satisfaction-productivity nexus: A
study using matched survey and register data. Industrial & Labor Relations
Review, 65, 244-262.
Boehm, J. K., & Kubzansky, L. D. (2012). The heart's content: The association
between positive psychological well-being and cardiovascular health.
Psychological Bulletin, 138, 655-691.
Boehm, J. K., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Does happiness lead to career success?
Journal of Career Assessment, 16, 101-116.
Boehm, J. K., Peterson, C., Kivimaki, M., & Kubzansky, L. (2011). A prospective
study of positive psychological well-being and coronary heart disease. Health
Psychology, 30, 259-267.
23
Bray, I., & Gunnell, D. (2006). Suicide rates, life satisfaction and happiness as
markers for population mental health. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric
Epidemiology, 41, 333-337.
Bush, D. E., Ziegelstein, R. C., Tayvack, M., Richter, D., Zahalsky, H., & Fauerbach,
J. A. (2001). Even minimal symptoms of depression increase mortality risk
after acute myocardial infarction. American Journal of Psychiatry, 15, 337-
341.
Cacioppo, J. T., & Patrick, W. (2008). Loneliness: Human nature and the need for
social connection. New York: W. W. Norton.
Carlson, M., Charlin, V., & Miller, N. (1988). Positive mood and helping behavior: A
test of six hypotheses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 211-
229.
Carnevale, P. J. (2008). Positive effect and decision frame in negotiation. Group
Decision and Negotiation, 17, 51-63.
Carnevale, P. J. D., & Isen, A.M. (1986). The influence of positive affect and visual
access on the discovery of integrative solutions in bilateral negotiations.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 37, 1-13.
Chida, Y., & Steptoe, A. (2008). Positive psychological well-being and mortality: A
quantitative review of prospective observational studies. Psychosomatic
Medicine, 70, 741-756.
Christian, L. M., Graham, J. E., Padgett, D. A., Glaser, R., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K.
(2006). Stress and wound healing. Neuroimmunomodulation, 13, 337-346.
Cohen, S., Doyle, W. J., Turner, R. B., Alper, C. M., & Skoner, D. P. (2003).
Emotional style and susceptibility to the common cold. Psychosomatic
Medicine, 65, 652-657.
Cohen, S., & Pressman, S. D. (2006). Positive affect and health. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 15, 122-125.
Coyne, J. C. (1976). Depression and the response of others. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 85, 186-193.
Cunningham, M. R. (1988a). Does happiness mean friendliness? Induced mood and
heterosexual self-disclosure. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14,
283-297.
Cunningham, M. R. (1988b). What do you do when you're happy or blue? Mood,
expectancies, and behavioral interest. Motivation and Emotion, 12, 309-330.
Danner, D. D., Snowdon, D. A., & Friesen, W. V. (2001). Positive emotions in early
life and longevity: Findings from the nun study. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 80, 804-813.
24
Davidson, K. W., Mostofsky, E., & Whang, W. (2010). Don’t worry, be happy:
Positive affect and reduced 10-year incident coronary heart disease: The
Canadian Nova Scotia Health Survey. European Heart Journal, 31, 1065-
1070.
Davis, M. A. (2009). Understanding the relationship between mood and creativity: A
meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 108, 25-38.
De Neve, J.-E., & Oswald, A. J. (2012). Estimating the influence of life satisfaction
and positive affect on later income using sibling fixed effects. PNAS:
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109:49, 19953-19958.
De Neve, J.-E. (2011) Functional polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) in the serotonin
transporter gene is associated with subjective well-being: evidence from a
U.S. nationally representative sample. Journal of Human Genetics, 56, 456-
459.
De Neve, J.-E., Christakis, N. A., Fowler, J. H., & Frey, B. S. (2012). Genes,
economics, and happiness. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and
Economics, 5(4), 193-211.
Diener, E., & Chan, M. (2011). Happy people live longer: Subjective well-being
contributes to health and longevity. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-
being, 3, 1-43.
Diener, E., & Diener, C. (1996). Most people are happy. Psychological Science, 7,
181-185.
Diener, E., Lucas, R., Schimmack, U., & Helliwell, J. (2009). Well-being for public
policy. New York: Oxford University Press.
Diener, E., Nickerson, C., Lucas, R. E., & Sandvik, E. (2002). Dispositional affect
and job outcomes. Social Indicators Research, 59, 229-259.
Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Suh, E. (2012). Positive emotion offset was essential to
human evolutionary success. Paper submitted for publication, University of
Illinois.
Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Very happy people. Psychological Science,
13, 81-84.
Dinan, T. G. (2009). Inflammatory markers in depression. Current Opinion in
Psychiatry, 22, 32-36.
Edmans, A. (2011). Does the stock market fully value intangibles? Employee
satisfaction and equity prices. Journal of Financial Economics, 101, 621-640.
25
Edmans, A. (2012). The link between job satisfaction and firm value, with
implications for corporate social responsibility. Academy of Management
Perspectives, in press.
Edwards, J. R., & Cooper, C. L. (1988). The impacts of positive psychological states
on physical health: A review and theoretical framework. Social Science and
Medicine, 12, 1447-1459.
Epel, E. S., Blackburn, E. H., Liu, J., Dhabhar, F. S., Adler, N. E., Morrow, J. D. et al.
(2004). Accelerated telomere shortening in response to life stress. PNAS:
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 101, 17323–17324.
Erdogan, B., Bauer, T. N., Truxillo, D. M., & Mansfield, L. R. (2012). Whistle while
you work: A review of the life satisfaction literature. Journal of Management,
38, 1038-1083.
Estrada, C. A., Isen, A. M., & Young, M. J. (1997). Positive affect facilitates
integration of information and decreases anchoring in reasoning among
physicians. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 72, 117-
135
Felps, W., Mitchell, T. R., & Byington, E. (2006). How, when, and why bad apples
spoil the barrel: Negative group members and dysfunctional groups. Research
in Organizational Behavior: An Annual Review Series of Analytic Essays and
Critical Reviews, 27, 175-222.
Field, T., Diego, M., & Hernandez-Reif, M. (2006). Prenatal depression effects on the
future of the newborn: A review. Infant Behavior and Development, 29, 445-
455.
Field, T., Diego, M., Hernandez-Reif, M., Deeds, O., Holder, V., Schanberg, S., &
Kuhn, C. (2009). Depressed pregnant black women have a greater incidence
of prematurity and low birthweight outcomes. Infant Behavior and
Development, 32, 10-16.
Forgas, J. P. (1998). On feeling good and getting your way: Mood effects on
negotiator cognition and bargaining strategies. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 74, 565-577.
Forgas, J. P. (2007). When sad is better than happy: Negative affect can improve the
quality and effectiveness of persuasive messages and social influence. Journal
of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 513-528.
Fossati, P. Ergis, A. M., & Allilaire, J. F. (2002). Executivie functioning in unipolar
depression: A review. L’Encephale, 28, 97-107.
Fowler, J. H., & Christakis, N. A. (2008). Dynamic spread of happiness in a large
social network: Longitudinal analysis over 20 years in the Framingham Heart
Study. British Medical Journal, 337, a2338.
26
Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General
Psychology, 2, 300-319.
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The
Broaden-and-Build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56,
218-226.
Fredrickson, B.L., & Branigan, C. (2005) Positive emotions broaden the scope of
attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 313-332.
Fredrickson, B. L., & Levenson, R. W. (1998). Positive emotions speed recovery
from the cardiovascular sequelae of negative emotions. Cognition and
Emotion, 12, 191-220.
Fredrickson, B. L., Mancuso, R. A., Branigan, C., & Tugade, M. M. (2000). The
undoing effect of positive emotions. Motivation and Emotion, 24, 237-258.
Fredrickson, B. L., Grewen, K. M., Coffey, K. A., Algoe, S. B., Firestine, A. M.,
Arevalo, J. M. G., Ma, J., & Cole, S. W. (2013). A functional genomic
perspective on human well-being. PNAS: Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, doi:10.1073/pnas.1305419110.
Fry, P. S. (1975). Affect and resistance to temptation. Developmental Psychology, 11,
466-472.
Garg, N., Wansink, B., & Inman, J. J. (2007). The influence of incidental affect on co
nsumers’ food intake. Journal of Marketing, 71, 194!206.
George, J. M., & Zhou, J. (2007). Dual tuning in a supportive context: Joint
contributions of positive mood, negative mood, and supervisory behaviors to
employee creativity. Management Journal, 50, 605-622.
Gotlib, I. H., & Hammen, C. L. (2009) (Eds.). Handbook of depression. New York:
Guilford.
Gotz, A. A., Martin, W., & Volker, S. (2008). Psychosocial maternal stress during
pregnancy: Effects on reproduction for F0 and F1 generation laboratory rats.
Physiology and Behavior, 93, 1055-1060.
Goudie, R., Mukherjee, S., De Neve, J.-E., Oswald, A. J., & Wu, S. (forthcoming).
Happiness as a driver of risk-avoiding behavior: A conceptual framework with
an application to seatbelt wearing and automobile accidents. Economica.
Graham, L. & Oswald, A.J. (2010) Hedonic capital, adaption and resilience. Journal
of Economic Behavior & Organization, 76(2), 372-384.
Graham, C., Eggers, A., & Sandip, S. (2004). Does happiness pay? An exploration
based on panel data from Russia. Journal of Economic Behavior and
Organization, 55, 319-342.
27
Grant, N., Wardle, J, & Steptoe, A. (2009). The relationship between life satisfaction
and health behaviour: A cross-cultural analysis of young adults. International
Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 16, 259-268.
Green, J. D., Sedikides, C., A., S. J., Wood, J. V., & Forzano, L. B. (2003). Happy
mood decreases self-focused attention. British Journal of Social Psychology,
42, 147-157.
Gruber, J., Mauss, I. B., & Tamir, M. (2011) A dark side of happiness: How, when,
and why happiness is not always good. Perspectives on Psychological
Science, 6, 222-233.
Guven, C. (2012). Reversing the question: Does happiness affect consumption and
savings behavior? Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(4), 701-717.
Harter, J. K., Schmidt, F. L., Asplund, J. W., Killham, E. A., & Agrawal, S. (2010).
Causal impact of employee work perceptions on the bottom line of
organizations. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 378-389.
Hertel, G., Neuhof, J., Theuer, T., & Kerr, N. L. (2000). Mood effects on cooperation
in small groups: Does positive mood simply lead to more cooperation?
Cognition and Emotion, 2000, 441-472.
Hölzela, B.K., Carmodyc, J., Vangela M., Congletona, C., Yerramsettia, S.M., Garda,
T., & Lazara, S.W. (2011). Mindfulness practice leads to increases in regional
brain gray matter density. Psychiatry Research, 191(1), 36-43.
Howell, R. T., Kern, M. L., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2007). Health benefits: Meta-
analytically determining the impact of well-being on objective health
outcomes. Health Psychology Review, 1, 83-136.
Ilies, R., Scott, B. A., & Judge, T. A. (2006). The interactive effects of personal traits
and experienced states on intraindividual patterns of citizenship behavior.
Academy of Management, 49, 561-575.
Inglehart, R., Foa, R., Peterson, C., & Welzel, C. (2008). Development, freedom, and
rising happiness: A global perspective (1981-2007). Perspectives on
Psychological Science, 3, 264-285.
Ifcher, J., & Zarghamee, H. (2011a). Happiness and Time Preference: The Effect of
Positive Affect in a Random Assignment Experiment. American Economic
Review, 101(7), 3109-3129.
Ifcher, J. & Zarghamee, H. (2011b). Positive Affect and Overconfidence: A
Laboratory Investigation. SCU Leavey School of Business Research Paper
No. 11-02
Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates
creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Positive Psychology, 52,
1122-1131.
28
Isen, A. M., Rosenzweig, A. S., & Young, M. J. (1991). The influence of positive
affect on clinical problem solving. Medical Decision Making, 11, 221-227.
Johnson, K. J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2005). “We all look the same to me:” Positive
emotions eliminate the own-race bias in face recognition. Psychological
Science, 16, 875-881.
Jovanovic, V., & Brdaric, D. (2012). Did curiosity kill the cat? Evidence from
subjective well-being of adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences,
52, 380-384.
Judge, T. A., & Kinger, R. (2007). Job satisfaction: Subjective well-being at work. In
M. Eid & R. Larsen (Eds.), The science of subjective well-being. New York:
Guilford Publications.
Judge, T. A., Piccolo, R. F., Podsakoff, N. P., Shaw, J. C., & Rich, B. L. (2010). The
relationship between pay and job satisfaction: A meta-analysis of the
literature. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 77, 157-167.
Judge, T. A., Scott, B. A., & Ilies, R. (2006). Hostility, job attitudes, and workplace
deviance: Test of a multilevel model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 126-
138.
Judge, T. A., Thoreson, C. J., Bono, J. E., & Patton, G. K. (2001). The job
satisfaction-job performance relationship: A qualitative and quantitative
review. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 376-407.
Kazdin, A. E., Esveldt-Dawson, K., & Matson, J. L. (1982). Changes in children's
social skills performance as a function of preassessment experiences. Journal
of Clinical Child Psychology, 11, 243-248.
Kessler, R. C. (2012). The costs of depression. The Psychiatric Clinics of North
America, 35, 1-14.
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., McGuire, L., Robles, T. F., & Glaser, R. (2002). Emotions,
morbidity, and mortality: New perspectives from psyhoneuroimmunology.
Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 83–107.
Koivumaa-Honkanen, H., Honkanen, R., Viinamaki, H., Heikkila, K., Kaprio, J., &
Koskenvuo, M. (2001). Life satisfaction and suicide: A 20-year follow-up
study. American Journal Psychiatry, 158, 433-439.
Kok, B. E., Coffey, K. A., Cohn, M. A., Catalino, L. I., Vacharkulksemsuk, T., Algoe,
S. B., Brantley, M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2013). How positive emotions build
physical health: perceived positive social connections account for the upward
spiral between positive emotions and vagal tone. Psychological Science, doi:
10.1177/0956797612470827.
29
Krause, Annabelle (2012). Don't worry, be happy? Happiness and reemployment.
Discussion Paper Series, Forschungsinstitut zur Zukunft der Arbeit, No. 7107
Kubzansky, L. D., Gilthorpe, M. S., & Goodman, E. (2012). A prospective study of
psychological distress and weight status in adolescents/young adults. Annals
of Behavioral Medicine, 43, 219-228.
Lawler, E. J., Thye, S. R., & Yoon, J. (2000). Emotion and group cohesion in
productive exchange. American Journal of Sociology 106, 616-657.
Lawless, N. M. & Lucas, R. E. (2011). Predictors of regional well-being: A county
level analysis. Social Indicator Research, 101, 341-357.
Lee, K., & Allen, N. J. (2002). Organizational citizenship behavior and workplace
deviance: The role of affect and cognitions. Journal of Applied Psychology,
87, 131-142.
Leitzel, J. D. (2000). A confirmatory factor analytic investigation of the tripartite
model of depression and anxiety in high school adolescents. Dissertation
Abstracts International, 2000-95024-186 (Dissertation Number AA19976359).
Lerner, J. S., Li, Y., & Weber, E. (2013). The financial cost of sadness. Psychological
Science 24, 72-79.
Lount, J. R. B. (2010). The impact of positive mood on trust in interpersonal and
intergroup interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98,
420-433.
Lucas, R. E., Diener, E., Grob, A., Suh, E. M., & Shao, L. (2000). Cross-cultural
evidence for the fundamental features of extraversion. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 79, 452-468.
Luechinger, S. (2009). Valuing air quality using the life satisfaction approach. The
Economic Journal, 119, 482-515.
Luhmann, M., Lucas, R. E., Eid, M., & Diener, E. (2013). The prospective effect of
life satisfaction on life events. Social Psychological and Personality Science,
4, 39-45.
Lyubomirsky, S., King, L. A., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent positive
affect: Does happiness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 803-855.
Lyubomirsky, S. & Boehm, J.K. (2010). Human Motives, Happiness, and the Puzzle
of Parenthood: Commentary on Kenrick et al. Perspectives on Psychology
Science 5(3), 327-334.
Marks, G. N., & Fleming, N. (1999). Influences and consequences of well-being
among Australian young people: 1980-1995. Social Indicators Research, 46,
301-323.
30
Mehl, M. R., Vazire, S., Holleran, S. E., & Clark, C. S. (2010). Eavesdropping on
happiness: Well-being is related to having less small talk and more substantive
conversations. Psychological Science, 21, 539-541.
Morrison, M., Tay, L., & Diener, E. (2012). Subjective well-being across the lifespan
worldwide. Paper submitted for publication, University of Western Ontario.
Moum, T. (1996). Subjective well-being as a short- and long-term predictor of suicide
in the general population. Paper presented at the World Conference in Quality
of Life, University of Northern British Columbia, Prince George, Canada.
August 22-25.
Myers, D. G. (2000). The funds, friends, and faith of happy people. American
Psychologist, 55, 56-67.
Neggers, Y., Goldenberg, R., Cliver, S. & Hauth, J. (2006). The relationship between
psychosocial profile, health practices, and pregnancy outcomes. Acta
Obstetricia et Gynecologica Scandinavica, 85, 277-285.
Nelson, D. W. (2009). Feeling good and open-minded: The impact of positive affect
on cross cultural empathic responding. Journal of Positive Psychology, 4, 53-
63.
Oishi, S., Diener, E., & Lucas, R. (2007). The optimum level of well-being: Can
people be too happy? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 346–360.
Ong, A. D. (2010). Pathways linking positive emotion and health in later life. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 358-362.
Organ, D. W., & Ryan, K. (1995). A meta-analytic review of attitudinal and
dispositional predictors of organizational citizenship behavior. Personnel
Psychology, 48, 775-802.
Orr, S. T., & Miller A. (1995). Maternal depressive symptoms and the risk of poor
pregnancy outcome. Epidemiologic Reviews, 17, 165-171.
Ostir, G. V., Markides, K. S., Peek, M. K., & Goodwin, J. S. (2001). The association
between emotional well-being and the incidence of stroke in older adults.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 63, 210-215.
Oswald, A. J., Proto, E., & Sgroi, D. (2012). Happiness and productivity. University
of Warwick, Manuscript submitted for publication.
Pelled, L. H., & Xin, K. R. (1999). Down and out: An investigation of the relationship
between mood and employee withdrawal behavior. Journal of Management,
25, 875-895.
Peterson, S. J., Luthans, F., Avolio, B. J., Walumbwa, F. O., & Zhang, Z. (2011).
Psychological capital and employee performance: A latent growth modeling
approach. Personnel Psychology, 64, 427-450.
31
Pettay, R. S. (2008). Health behaviors and life satisfaction in college students.
Doctoral Dissertation, Kansas State University.
Pressman, S. D., & Cohen, S. (2005). Does positive affect influence health?
Psychological Bulletin, 131, 925–971.
Pressman, S. D., & Cohen, S. (2012). Positive emotion word use and longevity in
famous deceased psychologists. Health Psychology, 31, 297-305.
Priller, E., & Schupp, J. (2011). Social and economic characteristics of financial and
blood donors in Germany. DIW Economic Bulletin, 6, 23-30.
Rasmussen, H. N, Scheier, M. F, & Greenhouse, J. B. (2009). Optimism and physical
health: A review. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 37, 239-256.
Rietveld, C. A., Cesarini, D., Benjamin, D. J., Koellinger, P. D., De Neve, J.-E.,
Tiemeier, H., Johannesson, M., Magnusson, P., Pedersen, N. L., Krueger, R.
F., & Bartels, M. (2013). Molecular genetics and subjective well-being.
PNAS: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110: 24, 9692-9697.
Riketta, M. (2008). The causal relation between job attitudes and performance: A
meta-analysis of panel studies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 472-481.
Romera, I., Perez, V., Menchon, J. M., Delgado-Cohen, H., Polavieja, P., & Gilaberte,
I. (2010). Social and occupational functioning impairment in patients in partial
versus complete remission of a major depressive disorder episode. A six-
month prospective epidemiological study. European Psychiatry, 25, 58-65.
Rugulies, R. (2002). Depression as a predictor for coronary heart disease: A review
and meta-analysis. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 23, 51-61.
Russ. T. C., Stamatakis, E., Hamer, M., Starr, J. M., Kivimaki, M., & Batty, G. D.
(2012). Association between psychological distress and mortality: Individual
participant pooled analysis of 10 prospective cohort studies. British Medical
Journal, 31, 345.
Schmitz, T.W., De Rosa, E., & Anderson, A.K. (2009). Opposing influences of
affective state valence on visual cortical encoding. Journal of Neuroscience,
29, 7199-7207.
Schneider, M., Graham, D., Grant, A., King, P., & Cooper, D. (2009). Regional brain
activation and affective response to physical activity among healthy
adolescents. Biological Psychology, 82, 246-252.
Smith, T. W., Glazer, K., Ruiz, J. M., & Gallo, L. C. (2004). Hostility, anger,
aggressiveness, and coronary heart disease: An interpersonal perspective on
personality, emotion, and health. Journal of Personality, 72, 1217-1270.
32
Slopen, N., Kubzansky, L. D., McLaughlin, K. A., & Koenen, K. C. (2012).
Childhood adversity and inflammatory processes in youth: A prospective
study. Psychoneuroendocrinology, in press.
Snyder, H. R. (2012). Major depressive disorder is associated with broad impairments
on neuropsychological measures of executive function: A meta-analysis and
review. Psychological Bulletin, Advance online publication. doi:
10.1037/a0028727
Spector, P. E., & Fox, S. (2002). An emotion-centered model of voluntary work
behavior: Some parallels between counterproductive work behavior and
organizational citizenship behavior. Human Resource Management Review,
12, 269-292.
Steptoe, A., Dockray, S., & Wardle, J. (2009). Positive affect and psychobiological
processes relevant to health. Journal of Personality, 77, 1747-1776.
Steptoe, A., Wardle, J., & Marmot, M. (2005). Positive affect and health-related
neuroendocrine, cardiovascular, and inflammatory processes. PNAS:
Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences, 102, 6508-6512.
Steptoe, A., & Wardle, J. (2011) Positive affect measured using ecological
momentary assessment and survival in older men and women. PNAS:
Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences, 108(45), 18244-18248.
Strine, T. W., Mokdad, A. H., Balluz, L. S., Gonzalez, O., Crider, R., Berry, J. T., &
Kroenke, K. (2008a). Depression and anxiety in the United States: Findings
from the 2006 Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System. Psychiatric
Services, 59, 1383-1390.
Strine ,T. W., Mokdad, A. H., Dube, S. R., Balluz, L. S., Gonzalez, O., Berry, J. T.,
Manderscheid, R., & Kroenke, K. (2008b). The association of depression and
anxiety with obesity and unhealthy behaviors among community-dwelling US
adults. General Hospital Psychiatry 30, 127-137.
Stutzer, A., & Frey, B. S. (2006). Does marriage make people happy, or do happy
people get married? Journal of Socio-Economics, 35, 326-347.
Tay, L., & Diener, E. (2011). Needs and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 101, 354-365.
Tay, L., Tan, K., Diener, E., & Gonzalez, E. (2012). Social relations, health
behaviors, and health outcomes: A survey and synthesis. Applied Psychology:
Health and Well-Being, in press.
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., Weber, K., Assenheimer, J. S., Strauss, M. E., &
McCormick, R. A. (1995). Testing a tripartite model: II. Exploring the
symptom structure of anxiety and depression in student, adult, and patient
samples. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 15-25.
33
Weiss, A., Adams, M. J., & King, J. E. (2011). Happy orangutans live longer lives.
Biology Letters, 7, 872-874
Wiest, M., Schuz, B., Webster, N., & Wurm, S. (2011). Subjective well-being and
mortality revisited: Differential effects of cognitive and emotional facets of
well-being on mortality. Health Psychology, 30, 728-735.
Williamson, D. M., Abe, K., Bean, C., Ferre, C., Henderson, Z., & Lackritz, E. (2008).
Current research in preterm birth. Journal of Women’s Health, 17, 1545-1549.
Wise, R.A. (2004). Dopamine, Learning, and Motivation. Nature Reviews:
Neuroscience, 5, 1-12.
Wisner, K. L., Sit, D. K. Y., Hanusa, B.H., Moses-Kolko, E. L., Bogen, D. L., Hunker,
D. F., Perel, J. M., Jones-Ivy, S., Bodnar, L. M., & Singer, L.T. (2009). Major
depression and antidepressant treatment: Impact on pregnancy and neonatal
outcomes. Focus, 7, 374-384.
Yang, J., & Diefendorff, J. M. (2009). The relations of daily counterproductive
workplace behavior with emotions, situational antecedents, and personality
moderators: A diary study in Hong Kong. Personnel Psychology, 62, 259-295.
Zorrilla, E. P., Luborsky, L., McKay, J. R., Rosenthal, R., Houldin, A., Tax, A.,
McCorkle, R., Seligman, D. A., & Schmidt, K. (2001). The relationship of
depression and stressors to immunological assays: A meta-analytic review.
Brain Behavior and Immunity, 15, 199-226.
1 Appleton et al. (2011); Slopen et al. (2012).
2 Edwards & Cooper (1988); Kiecolt-Glaser, McGuire, Robles, & Glaser (2002); Cohen, Doyle,
Turner, Alper, & Skoner (2003).
3 Bhattacharyya, Whitehead, Rakhit, & Steptoe (2008).
4 Ong (2010); Steptoe, Wardle, & Marmot (2005); Steptoe, Dockray, & Wardle (2009).
5 Ostir, Markides, Peek, & Goodwin (2001); Davidson, Mostofsky, & Whang (2010); Cohen et al.
(2003).
6 Blanchflower, Oswald, & Stewart-Brown (2012); Stepney (1982); Pettay (2008); Schneider, Graham,
Grant, King, & Cooper (2009); Garg, Wansink, & Inman (2007); Strine et al. (2008a, 2008b); Grant,
Wardle, & Steptoe (2009); Kubzansky, Gilthorpe, & Goodman (2012).
7 Fredrickson (2001); Fredrickson & Levenson (1998); Fredrickson, Mancuso, Branigan, & Tugade
(2000).
8 Danner, Snowdon, & Friesen (2001); Pressman & Cohen (2012); Wiest, Schuz, Webster, & Wurm
(2011); Russ et al. (2012); Bush et al. (2001); Chida & Steptoe (2008); Epel et al. (2004); Steptoe &
Wardle (2011).
9 Oswald, Proto, & Sgroi (2012).
10 Peterson, Luthans, Avolio, Walumbwa, & Zhang (2011).
11 Ashby, Valentin, & Turken (2002); Jovanovic & Brdaric (2012); Leitzel (2001); Isen, Daubman, &
Nowicki (1987); Amabile, Barsade, Mueller, & Staw (2005); George & Zhou (2007); Baas, De Dreu,
& Nijstad (2008); Davis (2009).
12 Baron, Fortin, Frei, Hauver, & Shack (1990); Barsade (2002); Carnevale (2008); Forgas (1998);
Baron (1990); Baron, Rea, & Daniels (1992); Carnevale & Isen (1986); Lawler, Thye, & Yoon (2000);
Hertel, Neuhof, Theuer, & Kerr (2000); Lount (2010).
13 Judge, Piccolo, Podsakoff, Shaw, & Rich (2010); Diener, Nickerson, Lucas, & Sandvik (2002);
Graham, Eggers, & Sandip (2004); Marks & Fleming (1999).
14 De Neve & Oswald (2012).
34
15 Bockerman & Ilmakunnas (2012); Harter, Schmidt, Asplund, Killham, & Agrawal (2010); Edmans
(2011, 2012).
16 Ifcher & Zarghamee (2011a).
17 Aspinwall (1998); Fry (1975).
18 Guven (2009).
19 Krause (2012).
20 Goudie, Mukherjee, De Neve, Oswald, & Wu (forthcoming).
21 Morrison, Tay, & Diener (2012); Oishi, Diener, & Lucas (2007); Aknin et al. (2013).
22 Lyubomirsky, King, & Diener (2005); Myers (2000); Diener & Seligman (2002); Cunningham
(1988b); Baron (1987, 1990); Berry & Hansen (1996).
23 Cunningham (1988b); Mehl, Vazire, Holleran, & Clark (2010).
24 Lucas, Diener, Grob, Suh, & Shao (2000); Tay & Diener (2011); Fowler & Christakis (2008).
25 Chida & Steptoe (2008); Cohen & Pressman (2006); Diener & Chan (2011); Howell, Kern, &
Lyubomirsky (2007); Lyubomirsky et al. (2005); Pressman & Cohen (2005).
26 Appleton et al. (2011); Slopen et al. (2012).
27 Edwards & Cooper (1988); Kiecolt-Glaser et al. (2002).
28 Ostir et al. (2001).
29 Christian, Graham, Padgett, Glaser, & Kiecolt-Glaser (2006).
30 Tay & Diener (2011).
31 Cacioppo & Patrick (2008).
32 Boehm & Kubzansky (2012); Boehm, Peterson, Kivimaki, & Kubzansky (2011).
33 Fredrickson et al. (2000); Fredrickson (1998).
34 Bray & Gunnell (2006).
35 Smith, Glazer, Ruiz, & Gallo (2004).
36 Dinan (2009).
37 Gotz, Martin, & Volker (2008); Neggers, Goldenberg, Cliver, & Hauth (2006); Wisner et al. (2009).
38 Field, Diego, & Hernandez-Reif (2006); Field et al. (2009); Orr & Miller (1995); Williamson et al.
(2008).
39 Pelled & Xin (1999).
40 Ashby et al. (2002); Jovanovic & Brdaric (2012).
41 Isen et al. (1987).
42 Baas et al. (2008); Davis (2009).
43 Baron et al. (1990); Barsade (2002); Carnevale (2008); Forgas (1998).
44 Baron et al. (1990); Baron, Rea, & Daniels (1992).
45 Carnevale, & Isen (1986).
46 Lawler, Thye, & Yoon, (2000).
47 Hertel et al. (2000); Lount (2010).
48 Judge & Kinger (2007).
49 Judge, Thoreson, Bono, & Patton (2001).
50 Riketta (2008).
51 Judge et al. (2010).
52 Diener, Nickerson, Lucas, & Sandvik (2002; Graham, Eggers, & Sandip (2004); Marks & Fleming
(1999).
53 De Neve (2011); De Neve, Christakis, Fowler, & Frey (2012); Rietveld et al. (2013).
54 Similarly, Edmans (2011, 2012) found evidence consistent with the importance of employee
satisfaction for firm performance.
55 Fredrickson & Branigan (2005); Estrada, Isen, & Young (1997); Isen, Rosenzweig, & Young (1991).
56 Aspinwall (1998).
57 Guven (2012).
58 Ibid.
59 Goudie et al. (forthcoming) estimate that the probability of being involved in an accident is reduced
by a factor of about 0.9.
60 Johnson & Fredrickson (2005).
61 Carlson, Charlin, & Miller (1988).
62 Anik, Aknin, Norton, & Dunn (2009).
63 Lyubomirsky et al. (2005); Myers (2000).
64 Diener & Seligman (2002).
65 Cunningham (1988b).
35
66 Baron (1987); Baron (1990).
67 Watson et al. (1995).
68 Coyne (1976).
69 Tay & Diener (2011).
70 Berry & Hansen (1996).
71 Fowler & Christakis (2008).
72 Gotlib & Hammen (2009).
73 Beck & Koenig (1996).
74 Romera et al. (2010).
75 Pressman & Cohen (2005).
76 Fredrickson (1998, 2001); Fredrickson & Branigan (2005); Kok et al. (2013).
77 Schmitz, De Rosa, & Anderson (2009).
78 Hölzela et al. (2011).
79 De Neve & Oswald (2012).
80 Ashby, Isen, & Turken (1999).
81 Lyubomirsky & Boehm (2010); Graham & Oswald (2010).
82 The Gallup World Poll is a representative sample of 941,161 individuals from 160 nations.
... The benefits include health improvements, marital satisfaction, friendship, social interactions, financial status, and job performance. Notably, the happiness of employees significantly enhances business outcomes by boosting labor productivity, creativity, teamwork, and the overall quality of products and services (Bellet et al., 2023;De Neve et al., 2013;Krekel et al., 2019;Lester et al., 2022;Oswald et al., 2015). The well-being of employees directly influences their innovative work behavior (Mehmood et al., 2022), and positive emotions play a crucial role in diminishing their experience of burnout and absenteeism (Lapalme et al., 2023). ...
... In the context of this paper, the literature confirms that it would be beneficial for employers to hire happy and satisfied employees. Happy laborers are more productive (Bellet et al., 2023;De Neve et al., 2013;Lester et al., 2022;Oswald et al., 2015), more innovative (Mehmood et al., 2022), and less likely to suffer burnout and absenteeism from work (Lapalme et al., 2023). Happy and fulfilled employees positively affect business sales and profits (Edmans, 2011(Edmans, , 2012. ...
... The term subjective well-being (SWB) is based on a tripartite model consisting of life satisfaction, positive and negative emotional behavior [1], and responses to both cognitive and affective components [2]. Evidence suggests that SWB is a key marker of healthy longevity and productive and social behavior [3] and a key indicator of positive youth development [4]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Subjective well‐being (SWB), including cognitive and affective components, may be influenced by the management of type 1 diabetes mellitus (T1DM). Physical fitness and adherence to physical activity (PA) are associated with better glycaemic stability and diabetes management. However, the relationship with SWB is not yet understood. The aim of the study was to determine the relationship between physical fitness, PA, and SWB over two years in youths with T1DM. This longitudinal study involved 83 participants (aged 6–18 years; 44.6% girls) from the Diactive‐1 Cohort. Physical fitness was assessed by spirometry (peak oxygen consumption) and dynamometry (handgrip strength). PA domains were measured using triaxial accelerometers, whereas the number of days participating in muscle‐strengthening activities was assessed using a question. The Cuestionario Único de Bienestar Escolar was used to determine SWB. Glycaemic stability was determined when glycosylated hemoglobin < 7%, coefficient of variability ≤ 36%, and time‐in‐range target > 70% were met. The linear mixed models used considered three assessments and subject variability, showing significant associations between overall physical fitness (unstandardized beta coefficient [B] = 1.778, 95% confidence interval [CI] 0.413–3.158; p = 0.012) and total PA (B = 0.034, 95% CI 0.005–0.063; p = 0.020) with SWB. Although time did not moderate the association with physical fitness (p = 0.116), this was true for PA only in the second year (p = 0.020). In contrast, glycaemic stability did not moderate the associations (p > 0.05). In conclusion, physical fitness and PA are associated with greater SWB in young people with T1DM, which may play a role in the psychological aspects of disease management.
... En particular los hombres tuvieron correlaciones más altas que las mujeres, quizá porque el bienestar es más valorado por los hombres que por las mujeres y perderlo atenta contra su satisfacción con la vida. Al respecto Chirico (20 18), reporta que los hombres son quienes presentan mayores niveles de bienestar subjetivo y material a diferencia de las mujeres, quizá esto se deba a que el bienestar tiene beneficios deseables (De Neve, Diener, Tay, & Xuereb, 2013;Diener et al., 2015), así mismo reduce la posibilidad de experimentar eventos negativos, como la separación matrimonial y la pérdida del empleo (Luhmann, Lucas, Eid, & Diener, 2013), así como quizá la violencia. ...
... Research shows that subjective well-being can significantly influence various aspects of objective wellbeing. For example, happier individuals tend to be more productive, engaged, and successful in their careers, which can lead to greater financial stability and professional growth (De Neve et al., 2013). Furthermore, positive emotions have been associated with better physical health outcomes, such as improved immune function, lower stress levels, and higher life expectancy (Diener & Chan, 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
The concept of well-being has garnered significant attention across diverse academic disciplines due to its important role in understanding quality of life and societal progress. In this study, well-being is studied through various strands, including objective well-being based on the fulfilment of basic human needs, subjective well-being encompassing positive mental states, and the capability approach assessing individuals' opportunities and freedoms. This study aims to investigate the emergence of creative cities within an international context through the theoretical frameworks of happiness and well-being by examining country-specific factors such as happiness , life satisfaction, freedom of choice, innovation, and economic inequality in relation to the number of creative cities designated by the UNESCO Creative Cities Network. Employing Poisson regression methodologies on data from international organizations, our findings reveal positive associations between happiness and life satisfaction with the number of creative cities, while economic inequality within a country slightly negatively impacts the emergence of creative cities. The paper ends with a discussion of the results and proposes future avenues for research and several policy implications.
Article
Schools present an ideal setting for intervention. We demonstrate that a greater prevalence of group discussion used in schools positively affects students' life satisfaction and noncognitive skills, and has no negative impact on test scores, based on a sample from the 2015 PISA, which includes more than 35 thousand students from approximately 1500 schools in 14 countries. We perform regressions of student life satisfaction on school‐level group discussion and lecturing, including a battery of controls and random intercepts by school. For robustness, we use instrumental variables and methods to account for school‐selection. The impact of group discussion is meaningful—a one‐standard‐deviation increase leads to an increase in life satisfaction that is about one‐half of the negative‐association with grade repetition. In contrast, lecturing does not have any effects. We are the first to show group discussion improves student life satisfaction and noncognitive skills, and thereby likely positively affects later‐life outcomes.
Article
Full-text available
This survey explores the landscape of well-being research in an urban context with focus on data. First, we conduct a review of well-being in general and efforts to measure it. Then, we perform an analysis of relevant research papers, reports, surveys, and standards published between January 2018 and September 2023 for the identification of reported factors contributing to urban well-being. These indicators were placed into distinct categories, creating a taxonomy of current efforts towards capturing urban well-being. This taxonomy, presented in a table format, details each indicator alongside its measurement method, data source, and other relevant information. Therefore, we are able to gain a deeper understanding of how urban well-being is assessed, what are the challenges, current research focus and gaps. This taxonomy could serve as a valuable tool for practitioners, researchers and policymakers in the field, providing a comprehensive framework to understand and assess various aspects of capturing well-being in urban context.
Book
Full-text available
Welcome to "Campus to Corporate" This book is dedicated to all those embarking on the transition from academia to the professional world, seeking to equip themselves with the essential skills and knowledge needed to thrive in today's competitive job market. The journey from campus to corporate is an exciting yet challenging one, marked by a significant shift in responsibilities, expectations, and opportunities. "Campus to Corporate" serves as your guide through this transformational phase, offering insights, strategies, and practical advice to help you navigate the complexities of the professional landscape and achieve your career goals. Whether you are a recent graduate stepping into your first job or a seasoned professional looking to advance in your career, this book provides valuable insights into the skills and attributes that employers value most. From communication and teamwork to problem-solving and adaptability, each chapter delves into the key competencies needed to succeed in the corporate world.
Article
Full-text available
Development of any country, particularly from the economic aspect, largely depends on the performance of its small and medium-sized enterprises (SMEs). Accordingly, this study aims to dynamically model the sustainability of SMEs emphasizing on economic sustainability dimension. Research participants include the managers of in growth SMEs of Iran in 2023, out of them 18 managers were selected through purposive sampling. To develop the sustainability assessment model, a systematic review of the literature identified 71 sustainability assessment indicators across three dimensions of economic, social, and environmental. Subsequently, principal component analysis was employed to evaluate the indicators of each dimension and determine the key indicators. Based on the results, five social indicators (employees' health and safety, employees' satisfaction, organizational culture, equity, diversity and inclusion (EDI), and compliance with laws and regulations), five environmental indicators (energy consumption optimization, recycling capability, use of renewable resources, green product design, and environmental commitment), and seven economic indicators (productivity improvement, financial strength, resilience, profitability, investment management, quality, and economic value added) were identified as the main sustainability indicators. Finally, based on the key indicators, research causal loop diagram was depicted and used for simulating the SMEs sustainability. In policy design phase, six potential scenarios were defined based on the data availability and feasibility to evaluate how different strategic interventions such as increasing product prices, employee training hours, and the use of recycled materials, effect the economic performance of SMEs. The results show the superiority of Scenario 6 in enhancing SMEs' economic performance over a 10-year horizon.
Article
Full-text available
The hypothesis that happy persons are more cooperative than sad persons has become a popular presumption in social and applied psychology. However, empirical evidence for this notion is less clear than often assumed. We argue that mood affects the process of decision making rather than (or in addition to) affecting the level of cooperation, increasing heuristic processing when persons feel good or secure, but leading to more systematic processing when persons feel sad or insecure. As a consequence, feeling states should moderate persons' reactions to heuristic cues, as for example the expected or perceived behaviour of others. Two experiments are reported varying feeling states, descriptive social norms, and the perceived behaviour of other group members in a chicken dilemma game. As expected, happy (Experiment 1) or secure participants (Experiment 2) showed shorter decision latencies and heuristically imitated others' behaviour in the chicken dilemma, whereas sad or insecure participants exhibited more systematic and rational behaviour, tending to defect when others' cooperation was high, but to increase their investment for the common when others' cooperation was low. No main effect of mood on cooperation occurred in either experiment.
Article
Full-text available
We explore the influence of genetic variation on subjective well-being by employing a twin design and genetic association study. In a nationally-representative twin sample, we first show that about 33% of the variation in life satisfaction is explained by genetic variation. Although previous studies have shown that baseline happiness is significantly heritable, little research has considered molecular genetic associations with subjective well-being. We study the relationship between a functional polymorphism on the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR) and life satisfaction. We initially find that individuals with the longer, transcriptionally more efficient variant of this genotype report greater life satisfaction (n=2,545, p=0.012). However, our replication attempts on independent samples produce mixed results indicating that more work needs to be done to better understand the relationship between this genotype and subjective well-being. This work has implications for how economists think about the determinants of utility, and the extent to which exogenous shocks might affect individual well-being.
Article
Male and female subjects performed several tasks either in the presence or absence of an environmental source of positive affect (pleasant artificial scents produced by two commercially manufactured air-fresheners). Consistent with the findings of previous research on the impact of positive affect, results indicated that several aspects of subjects' behavior were influenced by this variable. Participants exposed to pleasant scents set higher goals on a clerical coding task and were more likely to adopt an efficient strategy for performing this task than subjects not exposed to such conditions. In addition, males (but not females) reported higher self-efficacy in the presence of pleasant artificial scents than in their absence. Participants exposed to pleasant scents also set higher monetary goals and made more concessions during face-to-face negotiations with an accomplice. Finally, subjects exposed to pleasant scents reported weaker preferences for handling future conflicts with the accomplice through avoidance and competition. Analyses of covariance suggested that these differences stemmed largely from contrasting levels of positive affect among subjects in the neutral and pleasant scent conditions. Together, these results suggest that pleasant artificial scents may provide a potentially useful means for enhancing the environmental quality of work settings, and hence the performance and attitudes of persons in them.
Article
This paper attempts to answer an interesting but empirically challenging question: Do changes in well-being (life satisfaction or happiness) lead to changes in consumption and savings behavior? The paper uses regional sunshine as an instrument for personal happiness using the Dutch Household Survey from the Netherlands. Sunshine improves happiness significantly. Instrumenting happiness with sunshine, happier people are found to save more, spend less, and have a lower marginal propensity to consume. Happier people take more time for making decisions and have more control over expenditures; they expect a longer life and (accordingly) seem more concerned about the future than the present; they also expect less inflation in the future.
Article
In this article, the author describes a new theoretical perspective on positive emotions and situates this new perspective within the emerging field of positive psychology. The broaden-and-build theory posits that experiences of positive emotions broaden people's momentary thought-action repertoires, which in turn serves to build their enduring personal resources, ranging from physical and intellectual resources to social and psychological resources. Preliminary empirical evidence supporting the broaden-and-build theory is reviewed, and open empirical questions that remain to be tested are identified. The theory and findings suggest that the capacity to experience positive emotions may be a fundamental human strength central to the study of human flourishing.
Article
This introduction to the two-part special issue reviews recent evidence that suggests that positive mood may play a beneficial, multifaceted, and flexible role in self-regulatory processes that cannot be explained by most current theories. First, under some conditions positive mood seems to facilitate careful processing of goal-relevant information, even negative information. Second, the relation of positive mood to cognition and behavior seems to be strongly moderated by goal-relevant features of the task context. Three frameworks (mood as input, processing advantages conferred by positive mood, and mood as resource) that may account for these facilitating effects of positive mood on self-regulation are discussed.