ArticlePDF Available

A Theoretical Model of Musical Memorization.

Authors:

Abstract

The purpose of this article is to describe a model of how music is memorized for performances. The process of memorization appears to comprise three stages: preview, practice, and over-learning. Previous experience and enculturation also are very important in informing the memorization process. Each stage is subdivided. The preview stage is divided into notational overview, aural overview, and performance overview. The practice stage is divided into notational practice and conscious memorization. The over-learning stage is divided into re-learning, automatization, and maintenance rehearsal. For the purposes of this model, the four most commonly discussed memorization strategies (aural, visual, kinesthetic, and analytical) are divided and grouped into sensory learning styles (consisting of aural, visual, and kinesthetic memories), and an analytical learning style (discussed separately). Four processing strategies also are discussed: holistic, serial, additive, and segmented. The amount of time and effort expended during each stage is a matter of individual preferences, performance goals, task difficulty, training, and ability. Furthermore, the stages and the subdivisions are flexible and not necessarily sequential, nor indeed compulsory, for all performers.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
A THEORETICAL MODEL OF MUSICAL MEMORIZATION
Jennifer Mishra
Psychomusicology; Spring 2005; 19, 1; ProQuest Direct Complete
pg. 75
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
... Incidental memorization refers to memory recall that occurs automatically (Eagle & Leiter, 1964;Mishra, 2005). It occurs when individuals engage in behaviors or activities pertinent to a specific domain. ...
... If incidental memory does not yield sufficient results, musicians engage in meaningful strategies to memorize music. This phenomenon is known as deliberate memorization (Mishra, 2005; see also Ginsborg, 2004). Deliberate memorization has been studied at length in novices (Flowers, 1987;Frewen, 2010;McPherson, 1995McPherson, , 1996McPherson, , 2005, collegiate students (Hallam, 1997;Lehmann, 1997;Lim & Lippman, 1991;Mishra, 2002a;Nuki, 1984;Woody & Lehmann, 2010), experts (Chaffin, et al., 2007;Chaffin & Imreh, 1997;Hallam, 1997;Miklaszewski, 1989;Noice, et al., 2008), and idiot savants . ...
... Incidental and deliberate memorizations occur as individuals participate in mental and deliberate practice of domain-specific material. Mental practice is defined as the cognitive process of encoding and rehearsing aural or visual input void of self-perpetuated muscular movement or auditory feedback (Coffman, 1990;Highben & Palmer, 2004;Lim & Lippman, 1991;Mishra, 2005;Rubin-Rabson, 1941). Examples include visual score study or listening to a recording. ...
... "Müzikte ezberleme stratejileri nelerdir?" alt problemiyle ilişkili olarak; en sık kullanılan ezberleme stratejileri işitsel, görsel, kinestetik ve analitik stratejiler olarak belirlenmiştir (Mishra, 2005;Highben ve Palmer, 2004, Kutadgobilik, 2019Ökdem ve Jelen, 2022). Kutadgobilik (2019), bu dört belleğin birlikte kullanılarak yapılan çalışmalarda kalıcı ve güvenli bir ezberin gerçekleşebileceği belirtirmiştir. ...
... Tunçkılıç ve Yokuş (2022), öğrencilerin çoğunlukla konserlerde ve sınavlarda eserlerini ezber çalma eğiliminde olduklarını, gitar eserlerini ezberlerken izledikleri yöntemler kapsamında ise çoğunlukla işitsel yöntemi tercih ettiklerini belirtmişlerdir. Mishra (2002;2005;, önerdiği modelinde bellek stratejilerine ek olarak dört süreç stratejisi tanımlamıştır. Bu stratejiler ise bütünsel, seri, eklemeli ve bölümlere ayrılmış stratejilerdir. ...
Article
Full-text available
Müzikte ezberleme, bir çalgı performansı sergilerken teknik ve müzikal gelişim sağlamak bakımından önemli bir konudur. Ezberleme sağlandıktan sonra notaya bakarak çalmayı bırakmak performans sırasında tekniğe ve müzikal ifadeye odaklanarak dinleyicilere etkili bir müzik sunmaya yardımcı olur. Bu bağlamda çalışmanın amacı, ulusal ve uluslararası literatürdeki çalışmaların derlenmesi yoluyla müzikte ezberlemenin önemine dikkat çekmektir. Ulaşılan bilgiler sonucunda, müzikte ezberleme ile ilgili uluslararası çalışmaların ağırlıklı olarak; ezberlemede tercih edilen öğrenme stratejileri, müzisyenlerin algısal öğrenme ve ezberleme stilleri, tekrara dayalı zihinsel ve fiziksel çalışmanın ezberlemeye etkisi, bilgiyi kodlama ve hatırlamada ipuçları kullanma, hızlı ve yavaş ezberleyen müzisyenler arasındaki farklar, çevrenin müzikal performansa etkileri, ezberlemeye etki eden bellek türlerinin incelenmesi gibi konular üzerinde yoğunlaştığı belirlenmiştir. Ulusal literatürün ise sayıca az olduğu ve uluslararası literatürden faydalanarak daha genel bir çerçeve içinde; bellek türleri, öğrenme stratejileri, öğrenme stilleri, ezber üzerine ilişkin tutumları, ezber yapmanın öğrenci başarılarına etkisi, ezberleme yöntem ve önerileri ile ezber yapma durumları gibi konularda yoğunlaştığı saptanmıştır.
... Background in music education. Several studies have demonstrated that musicians use different strategies in order to memorize music (Gieseking and Leimer, 1932/1972, Hughes, 1915, Matthay, 1926, Mishra, 2005. However, all these studies indicate clearly the role of segmentation and they emphasized on the knowledge about the music structure in order to memorize music. ...
... In the current research, we tried to identify whether the strategies that pianists used in the current research in order to memorize atonal music fit in their models. According to Mishra's (2005) model for the memory, the memorization procedure includes three stages: the preview stage (where the performer acquires a visual representation of the music (notation preview), an aural and a performance representation, the practice stage (where the performer can choose a strategy either the segmented, holistic, serial or the additive strategy) and over-learning stage (which is based on discovering repetitions and patterns and focuses on the recognition of the structure and finding familiar patterns). Matthay (1926), Hughes (1915), Gieseking andLeimer (1932/1972) described the aural, visual and kinaesthetic methods used by performers in order to memorize the music. ...
Article
Background in music education. Several studies have demonstrated that musicians use different strategies in order to memorize music (Gieseking and Leimer, 1932/1972, Hughes, 1915, Matthay, 1926, Mishra, 2005). However, all these studies indicate clearly the role of segmentation and they emphasized on the knowledge about the music structure in order to memorize music. Background in music psychology. Musicians may use their knowledge about the music structure in order to memorize the music, by segmenting the piece according to the structure. Observational studies, testing experts and non expert pianists have shown that the more experienced pianists segmented the piece according to the formal structure (Chaffin and Imreh, 2001, Williamon and Valentine, 2002), relying on highly ordered retrieval structures confirming the Long Term-Working Memory Theory (Ericsson and Kintsch, 1995). Aims. The aim of this study was to find out the strategies that different experienced pianist used in order to memorize an atonal music excerpt and how they segment the piece in order to memorize it.
... Conjunto de pensamientos o pautas de conducta planificados sistemáticamente, que son llevados a cabo por el instrumentista de forma intencional para lograr un resultado musical satisfactorio por medio de la división de lo complejo y de la obtención de pequeños dominios parciales previos al Bloomfield−Zeisler, 1913;Bunting, 1999;Burwell y Shipton, 2013;Carlevaro, 2000;Casas, 2013;Chafin y Imreh, 2001;Chaffin, Lisboa, Logan y Begosh, 2010;Coso,1992;Coyle, 2009;Gabrielsson, 2003;Galamian, 1998;Ginsborg, 2004; dominio completo, dentro de un plan de retroalimentación tras la oportuna concepción, puesta en práctica y evaluación. Jørgensen, 2004;Miklaszewski, 1989;Miller, 1956;Mishra, 2005;Neuhaus, 1987;Rosenbaum, Kenny y Derr, 1983;. 2-Practicar con máxima concentración durante la ejecución instrumental, es decir, manteniendo una conciencia constante sobre un tema específico excluyendo momentáneamente otras cuestiones. ...
Article
Full-text available
Cabe pensar que un estudiante de instrumento que accede al Grado Superior de Música, tras aproximadamente 10 años de formación, debería tener una práctica efectiva y, por lo tanto, un manejo adecuado de numerosas estrategias. En este sentido, el estudio se planteó con objeto de corroborar el estado de conocimientos previos de los estudiantes que ingresan al Conservatorio Superior de Música de Aragón con respecto a diversas estrategias de práctica instrumental de eficacia avalada por investigaciones previas y/o la experiencia profesional de grandes intérpretes. La investigación de carácter descriptivo presenta una metodología de carácter cualitativo en la que las técnicas de recogida de datos han sido el cuestionario y la entrevista personal con una posterior triangulación de resultados. Los resultados evidencian que, de media, tan solo el 38.89% de los estudiantes conocían y manejaban las estrategias de práctica propuestas antes de su participación. De modo que, los contenidos del programa didáctico implementado eran, en general, desconocidos, sobre todo para los instrumentistas de viento-metal. En resumen, los hallazgos obtenidos alertan acerca de la necesidad de formar a los estudiantes de grado superior en la práctica estratégica con objeto de optimizar su estudio individual.
... For fluent expert performance with or without a score, multimodal mental representations of the music need to be securely encoded in memory in order to be recalled under performance conditions (Chaffin & Imreh, 2002;Hallam, 1997). Auditory, motor, conceptual, structural, visual, and linguistic images may all concurrently contribute to performance (Chaffin, Logan, & Begosh, 2009;Mishra, 2005;Williamon & Valentine, 2002). The manner in which performers attend to different aspects of a musical image is likely to be context-dependent and idiosyncratic. ...
Article
Full-text available
Imagery has long been considered an important part of music practice, yet its use in professional musicians’ practice and teaching has not yet received a great amount of research attention. A participant observation study of a 5-day masterclass for 11 expert pianists, given by the pianist and Alexander technique teacher Nelly Ben-Or (NBO), examined deliberate multimodal musical imagery techniques in a real-world setting. The study aimed to generate an interpretive description of NBO’s pedagogy, to investigate how pianists experienced and implemented her techniques, and to explore the possible mechanisms by which teaching according to these principles might work. Data collection incorporated observation, video documentation, interviews, notes, questionnaires, and e-mail correspondence. Thematic analysis was used across the corpus. NBO’s pedagogy focuses on creating a clear image of the piece to be performed via multimodal imagery techniques and chunking strategies, which can be used during memorization as well as to improve problematic aspects of performance. Imagery rehearsal may thus provide a means of managing complex tasks in discrete stages and lead to an enhanced sense of integration between intention and action, by diverting attention from the process of playing to the goal outcome, internalized as a multimodal representation. Deliberate imagery strategies were associated with positive outcomes but were also often experienced as challenging. The findings imply that these strategies can be taught and improved over time, their efficacy modulated by skill level and motivation. Overall, participants reported that imagery rehearsal led to improvements in technical facility, musical quality, and memory security. Issues for further investigation and application in other domains are discussed.
Article
The intention of this basic qualitative study was to investigate the ways in which professional guitarists describe their musical memory, in an attempt to develop a conceptual understanding of musical memory from a guitarist's point of view, including the guitarist's experience and understanding of musical memory. Research into the experiences of professional classical guitarists is limited, and there is not necessarily agreement on which method is best for developing musical memory. While the process of musical memory has been investigated in other groups of instruments, similarities and differences between instrumental approaches are not properly understood. Data were collected through live online interviews conducted with 12 professional concert guitarists, purposely selected for their impressive musical memory. The findings revealed that the caliber of recall and remembering for the purpose of performing without sheet music is highly dependent on the quality of the learning processes involved and also on the extent of truly understanding the music. A conceptual model was identified in which five main themes emerged: pre-learning; active learning; understanding; performance: preparation, alterations, and learning; and remembering. The conceptual model describes a continuous, circular process that applies to the learning process both pre- and post-performance.
Article
The convention of performing from memory is a well-established practice among pianists, but an exception is often made for contemporary piano repertoire. Even so, a number of renowned pianists continue performing highly demanding compositions of this genre from memory, and this practice is commended by contemporary composers. No research to date has systematically investigated musicians’ views on this matter and explored detailed accounts of how to prepare and memorise such repertoire. In this study six renowned professional pianists with experience in performing contemporary piano repertoire were interviewed on their approaches to learning and memorising this music. Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis of the pianists’ accounts revealed a variety of views on performing contemporary music from memory, with some pianists advocating benefits from performing by heart and others from using the score. The general practice strategies reported resembled approaches described by skilled musicians in relation to standard repertoire, with some variations related to different types of contemporary music styles. Memorisation accounts emphasised the importance of strategies such as mental rehearsal, chunking, and reliance on different types of memory and their combination.
Article
This research aims to study the implementation of the strategy of fragmented practice or chunking in the teaching-learning process of 30 piano students of the Conservatory of Music of Aragon. The research was realized through observational methodology and ideographic design, multidimensional and followed over time, with the field format as observational instrument. For the analysis of data, the detection of T-patterns was carried out by means of the THEME program. The results show evidence that, in all occasions in which the strategy was put into practice, the student achieved a satisfactory musical interpretation. Similarly, temporary patterns were obtained regarding the sequences of the appearance of errors committed by the student in the interpretation following implementation of the strategy, which allows us a glimpse into its didactic and musical implications.
Chapter
Studying musical memory offers a unique window to investigate how culture, cognition and consciousness are interconnected. Proponents of situated cognitive theories believe that memory is neither constructive nor reproductive, but a richly contextual segue between present agency and past experience. Developing musical skill requires the functional coupling of culturally embodied memory with spontaneous action-based restructuring that empowers a fluid consciousness that includes the self, external symbol systems and the environment. In traditional settings of imitative musical learning, cultural prerogatives are psychocognitively embodied as memories using a variety of multisensory strategies—from metaphors, visual patterns, synaesthetic experiences, auditory imagery, invoking people, places, images and a rich tapestry of contextual sound information. This chapter reviews current neuroscientific findings on how musical memory is embodied in response to cultural stimuli, how brain neural pathways are formed in imitative learning and current scientific, epistemological and methodological models available to investigate the brain-cognition-culture nexus from neuroanthropological and cultural neuroscience perspectives. Such a review is crucial to understand how culture is embedded in the brain as a synergistic product of behavioural, neural and cognitive events.
Article
Full-text available
The theoretical framework presented in this article explains expert performance as the end result of individuals' prolonged efforts to improve performance while negotiating motivational and external constraints. In most domains of expertise, individuals begin in their childhood a regimen of effortful activities (deliberate practice) designed to optimize improvement. Individual differences, even among elite performers, are closely related to assessed amounts of deliberate practice. Many characteristics once believed to reflect innate talent are actually the result of intense practice extended for a minimum of 10 years. Analysis of expert performance provides unique evidence on the potential and limits of extreme environmental adaptation and learning.
Article
Full-text available
This article reports a study on a musical idiot savant (NP) who is capable of memorizing large-scale pieces of piano music in three or four hearings. Attempts to memorize two contrasting pieces are documented, one a tonal composition by Grieg, the other an atonal piece by Bartok. The results are compared with those provided by a professional pianist. Transcription of the reproductions shows that NP's ability is confined to tonal music and is structurally based. In this respect, it resembles the performance of high IQ memorizers and supports the view that general intelligence is not a prerequisite for structure-based skill.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to determine if there was a strategic difference in memorization between musicians who memorized quickly and those requiring more time. After memorizing a 36 measure exercise, eight university instrumentalists were chosen for an in depth analysis of the memorization process. Subjects were chosen based on speed of memorization with four representing the fastest memorizers (8.66 to 17 minutes) and four slower memorizers (66.83 to 100 minutes). A qualitative analysis of the practice was performed using a graphic analysis of the practice session, frequency distributions of starts, stops, pauses, and errors, transcriptions of interviews, and markings in the notation. Four strategies emerged from the data: Segmental, Holistic, Additive, and Serial (which is characterized by a return to the beginning following an error or memory lapse). The faster memorizers tended to favor the Holistic or Additive strategies and the slower memorizers favored the Segmental and Serial strategies.
Article
Full-text available
Two experiments plus a pilot investigated the role of melodic structure on short-term memory for musical notation by musicians and nonmusicians. In the pilot experiment, visually similar melodies that had been rated as either "good" or "bad" were presented briefly, followed by a 15-sec retention interval and then recall. Musicians remembered good melodies better than they remembered bad ones: nonmusicians did not distinguish between them. In the second experiment, good, bad, and random melodies were briefly presented, followed by immediate recall. The advantage of musicians over nonmusicians decreased as the melody type progressed from good to bad to random. In the third experiment, musicians and nonmusicians divided the stimulus melodies into groups. For each melody, the consistency of grouping was correlated with memory performance in the first two experiments. Evidence was found for use of musical groupings by musicians and for use of a simple visual strategy by nonmusicians. The nature of these musical groupings and how they may be learned are considered. The relation of this work to other studies of comprehension of symbolic diagrams is also discussed.
Article
Full-text available
A concert pianist the second author videotaped herself learning J.S. Bach's Italian Concerto Presto , and commented on the problems she encountered as she practised. Approximately two years later the pianist wrote out the first page of the score from memory. The pianist's verbal reports indicated that in the early sessions she identified and memorised the formal structure of the piece, and in the later sessions she practised using this organisation to retrieve the memory cues that controlled her playing. The practice and recall data supported this account. Both were organised by the formal structure of the music. Practice segments were more likely to start and stop at boundaries of the formal structure than at other locations, and recall was higher for the beginnings of sections than for later portions. Like other forms of expert memory, pianistic memory appears to be based on use of a highly practised retrieval scheme which permits rapid retrieval of information from long-term memory.
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated an expert pianist's nine-month preparation for a public music performance (recital) through the collection of practice diaries and MIDI recordings of the eight scheduled pieces. Recordings were made under the experimentally varied conditions of solitary performance and public performance. The practice diaries revealed that the expert (an advanced student performer) allocated practice time consistently across the entire preparation period and tended to use mornings to practice the pieces perceived as being more difficult. Total preparation time for each of the pieces could be predicted on the basis of the pianist's subjective ratings of complexity and independent ratings of complexity given by other experts. An analysis of the performance data showed that, near the time of the recital, variability in performance tempo was large between pieces but very small for multiple renditions of the same piece, even under the different experimental conditions. Thus, to attain a highly reproducible public performance, the expert allocated practice time in response to task demands and engaged in specific preparations that would safeguard the performance against unexpected problems.
Article
Full-text available
Theories arguing that specific skills are acquired through extended practice cannot easily account for some musicians’ ability to perform unfamiliar music without preparation at first sight (sight-reading). This study identified the source of individual differences in this ability among expert pianists by relating component abilities of sight-reading and biographical indicators of skill acquisition to actual sight-reading performance. Sixteen advanced pianists of comparable skill played music without rehearsal (sight-reading) and after brief rehearsal (accompanying). Performances were paced by a recorded melody line. Pianists then performed experimental tasks designed to capture isolated subskills of sight-reading such as improvisation, recall, and kinesthetic ability. Sight-reading and accompanying performance correlated significantly with performance on the component tasks and with interview data on subjects’ training background, including the accumulated amount of time spent with accompanying-related activities and the size of accompanying repertoire. After controlling for the effects of subskills, age, professional specialization, and an indicator of general pianistic skill, accumulated accompanying experience and size of accompanying repertoire still accounted for significant unique variance in sight-reading and accompanying performance. Individual differences in unrehearsed performance among expert pianists reflect the consequences of domain-relevant activities (accompanying) and deliberate skill-building efforts (increase in size of relevant repertoire).
Article
Full-text available
Classically-trained instrumental musicians' practising strategies, and changes in strategy use as expertise develops, have been identified in observational and case studies. Unlike instrumentalists, classically-trained singers perform words as well as music and must therefore use different memorising strategies. The present study aimed to identify these, to compare the extent to which they were used by singers of varying levels of expertise and to assess which strategies were most likely to be effective. Thirteen participants learned and memorised the same song over the course of six 15-minute practice sessions, which were audiotaped. One major finding was that experienced professional singers were not necessarily faster, more accurate memorisers than student and amateur singers, so the strategies they used were not necessarily the most effective. Generally, participants began by practising the music separately from the words and went on to practise words and music together. They began by reading the song from the musical score and went on to practise it from memory once they were confident that they could sing it accurately. Fast, accurate memorisers began memorising earlier and were more likely than slower, less accurate memorisers to count beats aloud during the learning process. This suggests that effective song memorisation requires not only basic musical expertise but also the use of a strategic approach to the memorising task.
Article
Performances of the Preludes from Cello Suites I, II and III by J. S. Bach were recorded on video-tape across five separate conditions, differing with respect to menmorisation and the presence of a music stand. Fifty "musicians" and thirtysix "non-musicians" were asked to watch and rate one video-taped performance of each Prelude on four performance aspects: overall quality, musical understanding, technical proficiency and communicative ability. Ratings indicated that (1) performing from memory was superior to playing from the score, (2) visibility of the performer influenced audiences' ratings of performances in a favourable direction, (3) the extra time spent preparing for the memorised performances was beneficial and (4) musicians seemed biased in favour of performances without a music stand. Results are discussed in relation to possible implications for musical performance and pedagogy. © 1999 by the Society for Research in Psychology of Music and Music Education.