ArticlePDF Available

Building a Practically Useful Theory of Goal Setting and Task Motivation: A 35Year Odyssey

American Psychological Association
American Psychologist
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The authors summarize 35 years of empirical research on goal-setting theory. They describe the core findings of the theory, the mechanisms by which goals operate, moderators of goal effects, the relation of goals and satisfaction, and the role of goals as mediators of incentives. The external validity and practical significance of goal-setting theory are explained, and new directions in goal-setting research are discussed. The relationships of goal setting to other theories are described as are the theory’s limitations.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Building a Practically Useful Theory of Goal Setting
and Task Motivation
A 35-Year Odyssey
Edwin A. Locke University of Maryland
Gary P. Latham University of Toronto
The authors summarize 35 years of empirical research on
goal-setting theory. They describe the core findings of the
theory, the mechanisms by which goals operate, modera-
tors of goal effects, the relation of goals and satisfaction,
and the role of goals as mediators of incentives. The
external validity and practical significance of goal-setting
theory are explained, and new directions in goal-setting
research are discussed. The relationships of goal setting to
other theories are described as are the theory’s limitations.
I
n the 1950s and 1960s, the study of motivation in
North American psychology was not considered a re-
spectable pursuit. The field was dominated by behav-
iorists, and “motivation” was argued by them to lie outside
the person in the form of reinforcers and punishers. When
internal mechanisms were acknowledged, as in drive re-
duction theory, it was said that they were primarily
physiological.
McClelland, a nonbehaviorist, argued for the exis-
tence of internal motives, such as need for achievement, but
these were asserted to be subconscious (McClelland, At-
kinson, Clark, & Lowell, 1953) and hence measurable only
by projective tests. Behaviorists, drive reductionists, and
advocates of subconscious motives all agreed that intro-
spection was not a valid method of understanding human
motivation. This ruled out the possibility of studying the
conscious regulation of action.
An exception to the anticonsciousness zeitgeist was
the work of Ryan. Anticipating the cognitive revolution in
psychology, Ryan (1970) argued that “it seems a simple
fact that human behavior is affected by conscious purposes,
plans, intentions, tasks and the like” (p. 18). For Ryan,
these, which he called first-level explanatory concepts,
were the immediate motivational causes of most human
action.
Lewin and his colleagues (e.g., Lewin, Dembo, Fest-
inger, & Sears, 1944) studied conscious goals, or levels of
aspiration, years prior to Ryan’s work. However, they
treated levels of aspiration as a dependent rather than an
independent variable. Mace (1935), a British investigator
who was perhaps less influenced than others by American
behaviorism, was the first to examine the effects of differ-
ent types of goals on task performance. His work was
largely ignored, however, except for a citation in Ryan’s
classic text with Smith on industrial psychology (Ryan &
Smith, 1954).
Goal-setting theory was formulated inductively
largely on the basis of our empirical research conducted
over nearly four decades. It is based on Ryan’s (1970)
premise that conscious goals affect action. A goal is the
object or aim of an action, for example, to attain a specific
standard of proficiency, usually within a specified time
limit. As industrial– organizational psychologists, our pri-
mary interest has been to predict, explain, and influence
performance on organizational or work-related tasks. Thus,
we focused on the relationship between conscious perfor-
mance goals and level of task performance rather than on
discrete intentions to take specific actions (e.g., to apply to
graduate school, to get a medical examination). The latter
type of intention has been studied extensively by social
psychologists, such as Fishbein and Ajzen (1975).
Core Findings
The first issue we addressed was the relationship of goal
difficulty to performance. Atkinson (1958), a student of
McClelland, had shown that task difficulty, measured as
probability of task success, was related to performance in a
curvilinear, inverse function. The highest level of effort
occurred when the task was moderately difficult, and the
lowest levels occurred when the task was either very easy
or very hard. Atkinson did not measure personal perfor-
mance goals or goal difficulty. Moreover, his task-difficulty
findings have not been replicated when task performance
Editor’s note. Denise C. Park served as action editor for this article.
Author’s note. Edwin A. Locke, R. H. Smith School of Business, Uni-
versity of Maryland; Gary P. Latham, Joseph L. Rotman School of
Business, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
This article is based in part on Edwin A. Locke’s G. Stanley Hall
lectures at the annual meetings of the American Psychological Associa-
tion in 1999 and of the Southeastern Psychological Association in 2000.
It is also based on Gary P. Latham’s presidential address to the Canadian
Psychological Association in 2001 and on his invited address to the
American Psychological Society in 2001.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Edwin
A. Locke, 32122 Canyon Ridge Drive, Westlake Village, CA 91361.
E-mail: elocke@rhsmith.umd.edu
705September 2002
American Psychologist
Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0003-066X/02/$5.00
Vol. 57, No. 9, 705–717 DOI: 10.1037//0003-066X.57.9.705
goals were measured.
1
We found a positive, linear function
in that the highest or most difcult goals produced the
highest levels of effort and performance. Goal difculty
effect sizes (d) in meta-analyses ranged from .52 to .82
(Locke & Latham, 1990). Performance leveled off or de-
creased only when the limits of ability were reached or
when commitment to a highly difcult goal lapsed (Erez &
Zidon, 1984).
We also compared the effect of specic, difcult goals
to a commonly used exhortation in organizational settings,
namely, to do ones best. We found that specic, difcult
goals consistently led to higher performance than urging
people to do their best. The effect sizes in meta-analyses
ranged from .42 to .80 (Locke & Latham, 1990). In short,
when people are asked to do their best, they do not do so.
This is because do-your-best goals have no external refer-
ent and thus are dened idiosyncratically. This allows for a
wide range of acceptable performance levels, which is not
the case when a goal level is specied. Goal specicity in
itself does not necessarily lead to high performance be-
cause specic goals vary in difculty. However, insofar as
performance is fully controllable, goal specicity does re-
duce variation in performance by reducing the ambiguity
about what is to be attained (Locke, Chah, Harrison, &
Lustgarten, 1989). Goal studies have also compared the
effects of learning versus performance goals and proximal
versus distal goals. These results are discussed below in
relation to the moderating effects of task complexity.
Expectancy and Social–Cognitive
Theories
Goal-setting theory appears to contradict Vrooms (1964)
valenceinstrumentality expectancy theory, which states
that the force to act is a multiplicative combination of
valence (anticipated satisfaction), instrumentality (the be-
lief that performance will lead to rewards), and expectancy
(the belief that effort will lead to the performance needed to
attain the rewards). Other factors being equal, expectancy
is said to be linearly and positively related to performance.
However, because difcult goals are harder to attain than
easy goals, expectancy of goal success would presumably
be negatively related to performance.
The apparent contradiction between the two theories is
resolved by distinguishing expectancy within versus ex-
pectancy between goal conditions. Locke, Motowidlo, and
Bobko (1986) found that when goal level is held constant,
which is implicitly assumed by valenceinstrumentality
expectancy theory, higher expectancies lead to higher lev-
els of performance. Across goal levels, lower expectancies,
associated with higher goal levels, are associated with
higher performance.
This within between distinction is not an issue in
social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1986, 1997). Self-
efcacy (task-specic condence) is measured by getting
efcacy ratings across a whole range of possible perfor-
mance outcomes rather than from a single outcome (Locke
et al., 1986). The concept of self-efcacy is important in
goal-setting theory in several ways. When goals are self-
set, people with high self-efcacy set higher goals than do
people with lower self-efcacy. They also are more com-
mitted to assigned goals, nd and use better task strategies
to attain the goals, and respond more positively to negative
feedback than do people with low self-efcacy (Locke &
Latham, 1990; Seijts & B. W. Latham, 2001). These issues
are addressed further below.
Goal Mechanisms
Goals affect performance through four mechanisms. First,
goals serve a directive function; they direct attention and
effort toward goal-relevant activities and away from goal-
irrelevant activities. This effect occurs both cognitively and
behaviorally. For example, Rothkopf and Billington (1979)
found that students with specic learning goals paid atten-
tion to and learned goal-relevant prose passages better than
goal-irrelevant passages. Locke and Bryan (1969) observed
that people who were given feedback about multiple as-
pects of their performance on an automobile-driving task
improved their performance on the dimensions for which
they had goals but not on other dimensions.
Second, goals have an energizing function. High goals
lead to greater effort than low goals. This has been shown
with tasks that (a) directly entail physical effort, such as the
ergometer (Bandura & Cervone, 1983); (b) entail repeated
performance of simple cognitive tasks, such as addition; (c)
include measurements of subjective effort (Bryan & Locke,
1
Task and goal difculty are not synonymous and can be measured
separately. An example of a difcult task would be solving a complex
anagram or a student pilot landing a plane. The term performance goal, as
we use it, refers to the score one attains on the task (e.g., how many
anagrams solved in three minutes or the prociency level one attains in
practice landings). A learning goal refers to the number of ideas or
strategies one acquires or develops to accomplish the task effectively.
Edwin A.
Locke
706 September 2002
American Psychologist
1967a); and (d) include physiological indicators of effort
(Sales, 1970).
Third, goals affect persistence. When participants are
allowed to control the time they spend on a task, hard goals
prolong effort (LaPorte & Nath, 1976). There is often,
however, a trade-off in work between time and intensity of
effort. Faced with a difcult goal, it is possible to work
faster and more intensely for a short period or to work more
slowly and less intensely for a long period. Tight deadlines
lead to a more rapid work pace than loose deadlines in the
laboratory (Bryan & Locke, 1967b) as well as in the eld
(Latham & Locke, 1975).
Fourth, goals affect action indirectly by leading to the
arousal, discovery, and/or use of task-relevant knowledge
and strategies (Wood & Locke, 1990). It is a virtual axiom
that all action is the result of cognition and motivation, but
these elements can interact in complex ways. Below is a
summary of what has been found in goal-setting research:
1. When confronted with task goals, people automat-
ically use the knowledge and skills they have already
acquired that are relevant to goal attainment. For example,
if the goal involves cutting logs, loggers use their knowl-
edge of logging without the need for additional conscious
planning in their choice to exert effort and persist until the
goal is attained (Latham & Kinne, 1974).
2. If the path to the goal is not a matter of using
automatized skills, people draw from a repertoire of skills
that they have used previously in related contexts, and they
apply them to the present situation. For example, Latham
and Baldes (1975) found that truck drivers who were as-
signed the goal of increasing the weight of their truck loads
made modications to their trucks so that they could better
estimate truck weight before driving to the weighing
station.
3. If the task for which a goal is assigned is new to
people, they will engage in deliberate planning to develop
strategies that will enable them to attain their goals (Smith,
Locke, & Barry, 1990).
4. People with high self-efcacy are more likely than
those with low self-efcacy to develop effective task strat-
egies (Latham, Winters, & Locke, 1994; Wood & Bandura,
1989). There may be a time lag between assignment of the
goal and the effects of the goal on performance, as people
search for appropriate strategies (Smith et al., 1990).
5. When people are confronted with a task that is
complex for them, urging them to do their best sometimes
leads to better strategies (Earley, Connolly, & Ekegren,
1989) than setting a specic difcult performance goal.
This is because a performance goal can make people so
anxious to succeed that they scramble to discover strategies
in an unsystematic way and fail to learn what is effective.
This can create evaluative pressure and performance anx-
iety. The antidote is to set specic challenging learning
goals, such as to discover a certain number of different
strategies to master the task (Seijts & G. P. Latham, 2001;
Winters & Latham, 1996).
6. When people are trained in the proper strategies,
those given specic high-performance goals are more
likely to use those strategies than people given other types
of goals; hence, their performance improves (Earley &
Perry, 1987). However, if the strategy used by the person is
inappropriate, then a difcult performance-outcome goal
leads to worse performance than an easy goal (Audia,
Locke, & Smith, 2000; Earley & Perry, 1987). For a
detailed discussion of the relation of task goals and knowl-
edge, see Locke (2000).
Moderators
Goal Commitment
The goalperformance relationship is strongest when peo-
ple are committed to their goals. Seijts and Latham (2000a)
found goal commitment questionnaires to have high reli-
ability and validity. Commitment is most important and
relevant when goals are difcult (Klein, Wesson, Hollen-
beck, & Alge, 1999). This is because goals that are difcult
for people require high effort and are associated with lower
chances of success than easy goals (Erez & Zidon, 1984).
Two key categories of factors facilitating goal com-
mitment are (a) factors that make goal attainment important
to people, including the importance of the outcomes that
they expect as a result of working to attain a goal, and (b)
their belief that they can attain the goal (self-efcacy).
Importance. There are many ways to convince
people that goal attainment is important. Making a public
commitment to the goal enhances commitment, presum-
ably because it makes ones actions a matter of integrity in
ones own eyes and in those of others (Hollenbeck, Wil-
liams, & Klein, 1989). Goal commitment can also be
enhanced by leaders communicating an inspiring vision
and behaving supportively. In eld settings (e.g., Ronan,
Latham, & Kinne, 1973) and laboratory settings (e.g.,
Gary P.
Latham
707September 2002
American Psychologist
Latham & Saari, 1979b), the supervisors legitimate au-
thority to assign goals creates demand characteristics.
An alternative to assigning goals is to allow subordi-
nates to participate in setting them. The theory is that this
would make goals more important to the person because
one would, at least in part, own the goals. A series of
studies by Latham and his colleagues revealed that, when
goal difculty is held constant, performances of those with
participatively set versus assigned goals do not differ sig-
nicantly (e.g., Dossett, Latham, & Mitchell, 1979; Latham
& Marshall, 1982; Latham & Saari, 1979a, 1979b; Latham
& Steele, 1983). Erez and her colleagues (Erez, 1986; Erez,
Earley, & Hulin, 1985; Erez & Kanfer, 1983), however,
reached the opposite conclusion.
Working collaboratively, with Locke as mediator,
Latham and Erez explored reasons for their contradictory
ndings. They found that from a motivational perspective,
an assigned goal is as effective as one that is set participa-
tively provided that the purpose or rationale for the goal is
given. However, if the goal is assigned tersely (e.g., Do
this . . . ) without explanation, it leads to performance that
is signicantly lower than for a participatively set goal
(Latham, Erez, & Locke, 1988). Meta-analyses of the ef-
fects of participation in decision making on performance,
for those studies that measured performance objectively,
yielded an effect size of only .11 (Wagner & Gooding,
1987a, 1987b).
Subsequently, Locke, Alavi, and Wagner (1997)
found that the primary benet of participation in decision
making is cognitive rather than motivational in that it
stimulates information exchange. For example, Latham et
al. (1994) found that with goal difculty level controlled,
participation in goal setting had no benecial effect on
performance. However, people who participated with oth-
ers in formulating task strategies performed signicantly
better and had higher self-efcacy than those who did not
participate in formulating strategies.
Monetary incentives are one practical outcome that
can be used to enhance goal commitment. However, there
are important contingency factors. The rst is the amount
of the incentive; more money gains more commitment.
Second, goals and incentive type interact. When the goal is
very difcult, paying people only if they reach the goal
(i.e., a task-and-bonus system) can hurt performance. Once
people see that they are not getting the reward, their per-
sonal goal and their self-efcacy drop and, consequently,
so does their performance. This drop does not occur if the
goal is moderately difcult or if people are given a difcult
goal and are paid for performance (e.g., piece rate) rather
than goal attainment (Latham & Kinne, 1974; Latham &
Yukl, 1975; T. Lee, Locke, & Phan, 1997).
Latham (2001) developed an empathy box to help
managers identify nonnancial outcomes that employees
expected as a result of committing to or rejecting a specic
difcult goal. In a study where the goal was to reduce theft,
when self-efcacy regarding honest behavior was high,
actions taken to change outcome expectancies led to a
signicant decrease in stolen material (Latham, 2001).
Self-efficacy. As noted, self-efcacy enhances
goal commitment. Leaders can raise the self-efcacy of
their subordinates (a) by ensuring adequate training to
increase mastery that provides success experiences, (b) by
role modeling or nding models with whom the person can
identify, and (c) through persuasive communication that
expresses condence that the person can attain the goal
(Bandura, 1997; White & Locke, 2000). The latter may
involve giving subordinates information about strategies
that facilitate goal attainment. Transformational leaders
raise the efcacy of employees through inspiring messages
to and cognitive stimulation of subordinates (Bass, 1985).
Feedback
For goals to be effective, people need summary feedback
that reveals progress in relation to their goals. If they do not
know how they are doing, it is difcult or impossible for
them to adjust the level or direction of their effort or to
adjust their performance strategies to match what the goal
requires. If the goal is to cut down 30 trees in a day, people
have no way to tell if they are on target unless they know
how many trees have been cut. When people nd they are
below target, they normally increase their effort (Matsui,
Okada, & Inoshita, 1983) or try a new strategy. Summary
feedback is a moderator of goal effects in that the combi-
nation of goals plus feedback is more effective than goals
alone (Bandura & Cervone, 1983; Becker, 1978; Erez,
1977; Strang, Lawrence, & Fowler, 1978).
Control theory (Carver & Scheier, 1981) also empha-
sizes the importance of goal setting and feedback for mo-
tivation. The assumptions that underlie control theory,
however, are questionable (Locke, 1991a, 1994; Locke &
Latham, 1990). In essence, the theory is based on a ma-
chine model derived from cybernetic engineering (Powers,
1978). The source of motivation is asserted to be a negative
feedback loop (such as that characterizing a thermostat)
that eliminates goalperformance discrepancies. The natu-
ral state of the organism is, by implication, one of motion-
lessness or rest.
Control theory is in effect a mechanistic version of
Hulls drive reduction theory, which was abandoned de-
cades ago. However, machines do not possess internal
motivational states and do not have goals of their own.
Their goals are those of the machines builders. Further-
more, discrepancy reduction is a consequence rather than a
cause of goal-directed behavior. As Bandura (1989) stated,
goal setting is rst and foremost a discrepancy-creating
process. Motivation requires feed-forward control in addi-
tion to feedback. After people attain the goal they have
been pursuing, they generally set a higher goal for them-
selves. This adoption of higher goals creates rather than
reduces motivation discrepancies to be mastered. Self
motivation thus involves a dual cyclic process of disequili-
bratory discrepancy production followed by equilibratory
reduction (Bandura, 1989, p. 38).
Task Complexity
A third moderator of goal effects is task complexity. As the
complexity of the task increases and higher level skills and
708 September 2002
American Psychologist
strategies have yet to become automatized, goal effects are
dependent on the ability to discover appropriate task strat-
egies. Because people vary greatly in their ability to do this,
the effect size for goal setting is smaller on complex than
on simple tasks. Meta-analyses (Wood, Mento, & Locke,
1987) have revealed goal difculty effect sizes (d) of .48
for the most complex tasks versus .67 for the least complex
tasks. For specic difcult goals versus a goal to do ones
best, the effect size was .41 for the most complex tasks
versus .77 for the least complex tasks.
Because people use a greater variety of strategies on
tasks that are complex than on tasks that are easy, measures
of task strategy often correlate more highly with perfor-
mance than do measures of goal difculty (Chesney &
Locke, 1991). In addition, there are often goalstrategy
interactions, with goal effects strongest when effective
strategies are used (Durham, Knight, & Locke, 1997).
R. Kanfer and Ackerman (1989) found that in an air
trafc controller simulation (a highly complex task), hav-
ing a performance-outcome goal actually interfered with
acquiring the knowledge necessary to perform the task.
People performed better when they were asked to do their
best. However, Winters and Latham (1996) showed that the
fault was with the type of goal that had been set rather than
with the theory. They found that when a specic difcult
learning goal rather than a performance goal was set,
consistent with goal-setting theory, high goals led to sig-
nicantly higher performance on a complex task than did
the general goal of urging people do their best.
Another factor that may facilitate performance on
new, complex tasks is the use of proximal goals. Latham
and Seijts (1999), using a business game, found that do-
your-best goals were more effective than distal goals, but
when proximal outcome goals were set in addition to the
distal outcome goal, self-efcacy and prots were signi-
cantly higher than in the do-your-best condition or in the
condition where only a distal outcome goal had been set. In
dynamic situations, it is important to actively search for
feedback and react quickly to it to attain the goal (Frese &
Zapf, 1994). As Dorner (1991) noted, performance errors
on a dynamic task are often due to decient decomposition
of a distal goal into proximal goals. Proximal goals can
increase what Frese and Zapf (1994) called error manage-
ment. Proximal feedback regarding errors can yield infor-
mation for people about whether their picture of reality is
aligned with what is required to attain their goal.
Personal Goals as Mediators of
External Incentives
What Locke (1991b) called the motivation hub, meaning
where the action is, consists of personal goals, including
goal commitment, and self-efcacy. These variables are
often, though not invariably, the most immediate, con-
scious motivational determinants of action. As such, they
can mediate the effects of external incentives.
For example, assigned goal effects are mediated by
personal or self-set goals that people choose in response to
the assignment, as well as by self-efcacy. The relation-
ships among assigned goal difculty, self-set goal dif-
culty, self-efcacy, and performance are shown in Figure 1.
Observe that assigning a challenging goal alone raises
self-efcacy because this is an implicit expression of con-
dence by a leader that the employee can attain the goal.
The correlation between self-set goals and self-efcacy is
higher when no goals are assigned.
The mediating effect of self-set goals and self-efcacy
on monetary incentive effects was noted earlier (T. Lee et
al., 1997). However, not all incentive studies have found a
mediating effect. Wood, Atkins, and Bright (1999) showed
that incentive effects were mediated by instrumentality or
outcome expectancies rather than by goals and efcacy.
Also, when summary feedback is provided without any
goals, the feedback effects are mediated by the goals that
are set in response to the feedback (Locke & Bryan, 1968).
Bandura and Cervone (1986) found that both goals and
self-efcacy mediated feedback effects. Self-efcacy is
especially critical when negative summary feedback is
given because the persons level of self-efcacy following
such feedback determines whether subsequent goals are
raised or lowered.
As noted earlier, the benets of participation in deci-
sion making are primarily cognitive rather than motiva-
tional. However, Latham and Yukl (1976) and Latham,
Mitchell, and Dossett (1978) found that employees who
were allowed to participate in setting goals set higher goals
and had higher performance than those who were assigned
goals by the supervisor. The higher the goals, the higher the
performance. Finally, Kirkpatrick and Locke (1996) found
that goals and self-efcacy mediated the effect of visionary
leadership on employee performance.
Satisfaction
Goals are, at the same time, an object or outcome to aim for
and a standard for judging satisfaction. To say that one is
trying to attain a goal of X means that one will not be
satised unless one attains X. Thus, goals serve as the
inection point or reference standard for satisfaction versus
dissatisfaction (Mento, Locke, & Klein, 1992). For any
given trial, exceeding the goal provides increasing satisfac-
tion as the positive discrepancy grows, and not reaching the
goal creates increasing dissatisfaction as the negative dis-
Figure 1
Relationships Among Assigned Goals, Self-Set Goals,
Self-Efficacy, and Performance
Note. Adapted from A Theory of Goal Setting and Task Performance (p. 72),
by E. A. Locke and G. P. Latham, 1990, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Copyright 1990 by Prentice Hall. Reprinted with permission.
709September 2002
American Psychologist
crepancy grows. Across trials, the more goal successes one
has, the higher ones total satisfaction.
There is a paradox here, however. How can people who
produce the most, those with difcult goals, be the least
satised? The answer is implicit in the question. People with
high goals produce more because they are dissatised with
less. The bar for their satisfaction is set at a high level. This is
why they are motivated to do more than those with easy goals.
But why would people be motivated to set high goals?
People can expect many psychological and practical out-
comes from setting and attaining those goals. For example,
undergraduate business students reported four benecial
outcomes that they expected as a result of having a grade
point average of A versus B versus C (Mento et al., 1992).
These outcomes were pride in performance; academic out-
comes, such as admission into graduate school or receiving
a scholarship; future benets, such as an excellent job offer
or a high starting salary; and life benets, such as career
success. At the same time, expected satisfaction with per-
formance showed the opposite pattern. The highest degree
of anticipated satisfaction, averaged across all grade out-
comes, was for students with a goal of C, and the lowest
was for students with a goal of earning an A. The relation-
ships found by Mento et al. (1992) are shown in Figure 2.
Setting specic challenging goals is also a means of en-
hancing task interest (Locke & Bryan, 1967) and of helping
people to discover the pleasurable aspects of an activity
(Harackiewicz, Manderlink, & Sansone, 1984).
Figure 2
Achievement Valence and Instrumentality Functions for Grade Goals or Outcomes
Note. From “Relationship of Goal Level to Valence and Instrumentality,” by A. Mento, E. Locke, and H. Klein, 1992, Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, p. 401.
Copyright 1992 by the American Psychological Association.
710 September 2002
American Psychologist
Practical Applications
Productivity and Cost Improvement
Numerous studies have shown that setting a specic dif-
cult goal leads to signicant increases in employee produc-
tivity (Locke & Latham, 1984). For example, loggers cut
more trees (Latham & Kinne, 1974; Latham & Yukl,
1975), and unionized truck drivers increased the logs
loaded on their trucks from 60% to 90% of the legal
allowable weight (see Figure 3) as a result of assigned
goals. The drivers saved the company $250,000 in 9
months (Latham & Baldes, 1975). A subsequent study
saved $2.7 million dollars in 18 weeks by assigning union-
ized drivers the goal of increasing their number of daily
trips to the mill (Latham & Saari, 1982). Word-processing
operators with specic high goals increased their perfor-
mance regardless of whether the goal was assigned or set
participatively (Latham & Yukl, 1976). In a survey of
companies from Duns Business Rankings, Terpstra and
Rozell (1994) found a signicant correlation between goal
setting and organizational protability.
Performance Appraisal
Engineers and scientists who set goals for their scores on a
behavioral index of their performance had higher subse-
quent performance than those who were urged to do their
best (Latham et al., 1978). Unionized telecommunications
employees had high performance and high satisfaction with
the performance appraisal process when specic high goals
were set. Moreover, self-efcacy correlated positively with
subsequent performance (Brown & Latham, 2000a). As
was the case with the engineers and scientists, the higher
the goal, the higher and more positive the performance
appraisal.
Selection
Latham, Saari, Pursell, and Campion (1980) developed the
situational interview to assess an applicant prior to employ-
ment. In brief, applicants are presented with situations,
based on a job analysis. Each question contains a dilemma
that assesses an applicants goals or intentions for what he
or she would do when confronted by these situations.
Meta-analyses have shown that the situational interview
Figure 3
Effect of Goal Setting on Percentage of Legal Net Weight Achieved by Logging Truck Drivers
Note. From The Practical Significance of Lockes Theory of Goal Setting, by G. P. Latham and J. Baldes, 1975, Journal of Applied Psychology, 60, p. 123.
Copyright 1975 by the American Psychological Association.
711September 2002
American Psychologist
has high criterion-related validity (e.g., Huffcutt & Arthur,
1994; Latham & Sue-Chan, 1999). McDaniel, Whetzel,
Schmidt, and Maurer (1994) concluded that the mean cri-
terion-related validity of the situational interview is higher
than that of all other selection interviews.
Self-Regulation at Work
A key variable in self-regulation is goal setting. Job atten-
dance is a prerequisite of job performance. Consequently,
Frayne and Latham (1987) adapted F. H. Kanfers (1970,
1996) methodology for the development of a training pro-
gram to teach unionized state government employees ways
to overcome obstacles they perceived to coming to work.
The training in self-regulation taught those employees to
set specic high goals for attendance, to monitor ways in
which their environment facilitated or hindered attainment
of their goal, and to identify and administer rewards for
making goal progress, as well as punishments for failing to
make progress toward goal attainment.
Not only did this training in self-management enable
these employees to cope effectively with personal and
social obstacles to their job attendance but, consistent with
social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1986, 1997), it increased
their self-efcacy because they could exercise inuence
over their behavior. Increases in self-efcacy correlated
signicantly with subsequent increases in job attendance.
Moreover, the job attendance of the people who were
taught these self-management skills was signicantly
higher than that of the control group three months after the
training had taken place.
In a follow-up study, Latham and Frayne (1989) found
that the increase in self-efcacy and the increase in job
attendance were maintained over a nine-month period.
Then, the control group was provided with the same train-
ing in self-regulation skills as the original experimental
group. Three months later, their job attendance and self-
efcacy regarding job attendance increased to the same
level as that of the original experimental group. Similarly,
Brown and Latham (2000b) studied the team-playing be-
havior of masters-level business students in their respec-
tive study groups. Those students who set specic high
goals regarding their evaluation by peers and who received
training in verbal self-guidance regarding goal attainment
had higher team-playing skills than those who did not set
goals.
Mental practice is symbolic guided rehearsal of a task
in the absence of any physical involvement (Richardson,
1967). Using Richardsons (1967) methodology, mental
practice in which goal setting was either implicit or explicit
was investigated by Morin and Latham (2000) as a transfer-
of-training intervention to improve the communication
skills of supervisors interacting with the union. Six months
later, self-efcacy was signicantly higher for the supervi-
sors who had received training in mental practice and goal
setting than for those in the control group. Self-efcacy
correlated signicantly with goal commitment and commu-
nication skills on the job.
Finally, goal-setting research led to the development
of the high-performance cycle (Latham, Locke, & Fassina,
2002; Locke & Latham, 1990). The high-performance cy-
cle explains how high goals lead to high performance,
which in turn leads to rewards, such as recognition and
promotion. Rewards result in high satisfaction as well as
high self-efcacy regarding perceived ability to meet future
challenges through the setting of even higher goals. This
cycle explains the lack of a direct connection between job
satisfaction and subsequent productivity, an issue that has
long puzzled psychologists (e.g., Hersey, 1932; Kornhauser
& Sharp, 1932). In the high-performance cycle, high sat-
isfaction is the result, not the cause, of high performance
when rewards are commensurate with performance. The
subsequent effect of satisfaction on action is therefore
indirect rather than direct. Job satisfaction leads to perfor-
mance only if it fosters organizational commitment, only if
this commitment is to specic and challenging goals, and
only if the moderator variables discussed in this article are
taken into account. Goal setting also has been applied
successfully to nonwork domains, such as sports (Lerner &
Locke, 1995) and health management (Gauggel, 1999). It is
applicable to any self-regulated activity.
New Directions and Limitations
Goal Conflict
In organizational settings, the organizations goal and the
goal of the individual manager are sometimes in conict.
For example, working to attain the organizations goals
could be detrimental to the monetary bonus of a manager if
managers are rewarded more for the performance of the
people they lead than for the performance of the overall
organization. Goal conict undermines performance if it
motivates incompatible action tendencies (Locke, Smith,
Erez, & Schaffer, 1994). Seijts and Latham (2000b) found
that when specic, difcult goals of the person are aligned
with the groups goal of maximizing performance, the
groups performance is enhanced. Without such alignment,
personal goals have a detrimental effect on a groups
performance.
Learning and Performance Goals
We noted earlier that on tasks that are complex for people,
learning goals can be superior to performance goals. How-
ever, there have been almost no studies examining the use
of both together. Intriguing ndings have been obtained by
Harackiewicz, Barron, Carter, Lehto, and Elliott (1997)
with college students. Performance goals improved grades
but did not affect interest, whereas learning goals enhanced
interest in the class but did not affect grades. The perfor-
mance effect is contrary to Dwecks (e.g., Dweck & Leg-
gett, 1988) assertion that performance goals have a delete-
rious effect on a wide range of educationally relevant
outcome measures.
Goals and Risk
Organizations must take risks to remain competitive in the
marketplace. Prospect theory (Kahneman & Tversky,
1979) emphasizes reference points, as does goal theory, but
it does not incorporate the concept of aspiration level.
712 September 2002
American Psychologist
Knight, Durham, and Locke (2001) found that difcult
performance goals increased the riskiness of the strategies
participants chose to use in a computer game and improved
performance. Obviously, higher risk strategies sometimes
lead to worse performance outcomes than lower risk ones,
and the conditions under which better or worse outcomes
occur need to be studied further.
Personality
Two related issues are involved here. First, do goals, along
with self-efcacy, mediate personality effects? It must
be noted here that there is some doubt about the meaning
of personality test scores (Bandura, 1986, 1997). If a
trait measure refers only to past behavior, then trait
performance correlations involve predicting current behav-
ior from past behavior. Traits, in this case, would predict
but not explain behavior. If, however, traits are viewed,
explicitly or implicitly, as reecting, in some form, under-
lying motives, then they would constitute an explanation.
Regardless of ones opinion on this issue, a number of
studies have found that goals, along with self-efcacy,
mediate the effect of personality measures on work perfor-
mance (see Locke, 2001, for a summary). For example,
Matsui, Okada, and Kakuyama (1982) found that the rela-
tionship between achievement motivation (measured with a
questionnaire) and performance disappeared when the dif-
culty level of self-set goals was controlled. Barrick,
Mount, and Strauss (1993) found that the effects of con-
scientiousness on sales performance were partially medi-
ated by goals and goal commitment. VandeWalle, Cron,
and Slocum (2001) found that goals and self-efcacy me-
diated the effect of a persons goal orientation on student
performance.
The second issue involved is whether goals are better
predictors of action than traits. If goals mediate personality
effects, then the former should predict better than the latter.
Adler and Weiss (1988), in their review of the goal-setting
literature, argued that a performance goal is a strong vari-
able that masks personality differences. Yukl and Latham
(1978) found that goals predicted performance and satis-
faction better than a measure of need for achievement.
Dweck and her colleagues (Dweck, 1986; Dweck & Leg-
gett, 1988; Elliott & Dweck, 1988) found that two person-
ality traits characterized children in the classroom. Those
with a desire to acquire knowledge and skillsthat is,
those with a learning goal orientation (LGO)performed
better on school-related subjects than whose focus was
primarily on attaining a performance outcome such as a
grade or scorethat is, those with a performance goal
orientation (PGO). In industrial/organizational psychology,
PGO has been measured as a desire not for challenging
goals but rather for certain or easy success that results in
praise (Button, Mathieu, & Zajac, 1996)a focus that goal
theory predicts would be associated with low performance.
Thus, it would be an LGO rather than a PGO that would be
more likely to lead people to undertake challenges, thus
leading to high performance, as was found by VandeWalle
et al. (2001). As noted, the latter study found LGO effects
to be fully mediated by goals.
Seijts and B. W. Latham (2001) conducted a study to
determine whether a learning goal too is a strong variable.
They found that individuals who have a high PGO but are
given a specic, difcult learning goal perform as well as
those with a learning goal who have an LGO. In short,
assigned goals neutralize goal orientation effects.
Further evidence that the setting of a high goal creates
a strong situation was obtained by Harackiewicz and Elliott
(1998). Using a pinball machine, they found that, regard-
less of whether a person scored high or low on need for
achievement (as measured by a self-report questionnaire), a
specic high-performance goal that was accompanied by a
rationale or purpose that emphasized a PGO resulted in a
higher level of intrinsic motivation (e.g., continued perfor-
mance during free time) than a performance goal that
emphasized an LGO. In the absence of a statement of
purpose, a specic high-performance goal that emphasized
an LGO resulted in the highest level of intrinsic interest in
the task.
Goals and Subconscious Motivation
Because we had studied conscious goals and McClelland et
al. (1953) had asserted that achievement motivation was
subconscious, McClelland collaborated with Locke and
others to see if there was a relationship between these two
concepts. The results, involving entrepreneurs in the print-
ing business, showed that need for achievement, measured
by the Thematic Apperception Test (TAT; a projective
test), had no concurrent or longitudinal relationship with a
rms performance and no relationship to entrepreneur-set
goals. However, goal effects were highly signicant in both
cases (Tracy, Locke, & Renard, 1999).
Howard and Bray (1988) collected TAT data in a
25-year study of managers at AT&T. Collaborating with
McClelland, they constructed a pattern score for leaders
motives from the TAT. This score combined need for
power, power inhibition, and need for afliation (weighted
negatively). Howard also possessed unscored TAT proto-
cols that she subsequently added to the data set. In addition,
she made scoring adjustments, after consulting with Mc-
Clelland about recent renements in the procedure, for
calculating leader motive pattern. In the reanalysis, the
leader motive pattern was not signicantly related to man-
agers number of promotions. However, managers ambi-
tion was a signicant motivational predictor of number of
promotions over the 25-year period. Howard (personal
communication) noted that the core item in the ambition
factor was a single interview question: How many levels
up do you want to go? This was the managers conscious
goal for number of promotions. Managers motive patterns
and their promotion goals were unrelated.
Two tentative conclusions may be drawn from
Howard and Brays (1988) results. First, even over a long
period, Ryans (1970) rst-level explanation of motivation,
namely, conscious goal setting, may be more reliably and
directly tied to action than are second-level explanations
(e.g., motives). Second, the conscious and subconscious
aspects of achievement motivation are unrelated.
713September 2002
American Psychologist
Despite the above results, there can be no doubt that
the subconscious is a storehouse of knowledge and values
beyond that which is in focal awareness at any given time
(Murphy, 2001). People can take action without being fully
aware of what is motivating them or what stored knowl-
edge is affecting their choices. The lack of focus on the
subconscious is a limitation of goal-setting theory. In the
19th century, the Wurzburg school (Ach, Watt, Kulpe, and
others)a school that inuenced Lewinshowed that as-
signed goals (they used the term tasks) could affect action
at a later time without people being aware of it. Research is
now needed on the effect of the subconscious on goals and
on the ways in which goals arouse and affect subconscious
knowledge. As an initial step in this direction, Wegge and
Dibbelt (2000) found that hard goals automatically en-
hanced the speed of information processing.
Goal-setting theory states that, irrespective of the sub-
conscious, conscious motivation affects performance and
job satisfaction. This is especially true for people who
choose to be purposeful and proactive (Binswanger, 1991).
As Bandura (1997) noted, people have the power to ac-
tively control their lives through purposeful thought; this
includes the power to program and reprogram their sub-
conscious, to choose their own goals, to pull out from the
subconscious what is relevant to their purpose and to ignore
what is not, and to guide their actions based on what they
want to accomplish.
Conclusion
The essential elements of goal-setting theory, along with
the high-performance cycle model, are summarized in
Figure 4. Goal-setting theory is fully consistent with
social cognitive theory in that both acknowledge the im-
portance of conscious goals and self-efcacy. The two
theories differ in emphasis and scope. The focus of goal-
setting theory is on the core properties of an effective goal.
These properties are as follows: specicity and difculty
level; goal effects at the individual, group, and organization
levels; the proper use of learning versus performance goals;
mediators of goal effects; the moderators of goal effects;
the role of goals as mediators of other incentives; and the
effect of goal source (e.g., assigned vs. self-set vs. partici-
patively set).
Goal-setting theory is not limited to but focuses pri-
marily on motivation in work settings. Social cognitive
theory and the research that underlies it are primarily
focused on self-efcacy, its measurement, its causes, and
its consequences at the individual, group, and societal
levels in numerous domains of functioning. Social cogni-
tive theory also discusses the effects of and processes
underlying modeling, cognitive development, moral judg-
ment, language development, and physiological arousal.
Despite these differences, the two theories agree about
what is considered important in performance motivation. A
detailed discussion of the relationship of goal theory to
other work motivation theories is presented in Locke
(1997).
The effects of goal setting are very reliable. Failures to
replicate them are usually due to errors, such as not match-
ing the goal to the performance measure, not providing
feedback, not getting goal commitment, not measuring the
persons personal (self-set) goals, not conveying task
knowledge, setting a performance goal when a specic
high-learning goal is required, not setting proximal goals
when the environment is characterized by uncertainty, or
not including a sufcient range of goal difculty levels (see
Locke & Latham, 1990, chapter 2).
A key issue involved in theory building is that of
generalization. With goal-setting theory, specic difcult
goals have been shown to increase performance on well
over 100 different tasks involving more than 40,000 par-
ticipants in at least eight countries working in laboratory,
simulation, and eld settings. The dependent variables have
included quantity, quality, time spent, costs, job behavior
measures, and more. The time spans have ranged from 1
minute to 25 years. The effects are applicable not only to
the individual but to groups (OLeary-Kelly, Martocchio,
& Frink, 1994), organizational units (Rogers & Hunter,
1991), and entire organizations (Baum, Locke, & Smith,
2001). The effects have been found using experimental,
quasi-experimental, and correlational designs. Effects have
been obtained whether the goals are assigned, self-set, or
set participatively. In short, goal-setting theory is among
the most valid and practical theories of employee motiva-
tion in organizational psychology (C. Lee & Earley, 1992;
Miner, 1984; Pinder, 1998). Isnt it time that psychologists
took consciousness, including conscious motivation, seri-
ously (Locke, 1995)?
REFERENCES
Adler, S., & Weiss, H. M. (1988). Recent developments in the study of
personality and organizational behavior. In C. L. Cooper & I. T.
Robertson (Eds.), International review of industrial and organizational
psychology (pp. 307330). New York: Wiley.
Atkinson, J. (1958). Towards experimental analysis of human motivation
in terms of motives, expectancies and incentives. In J. Atkinson (Ed.),
Figure 4
Essential Elements of Goal-Setting Theory and the
High-Performance Cycle
714 September 2002
American Psychologist
Motives in fantasy, action and society (pp. 288 305). Princeton, NJ:
Van Nostrand.
Audia, P., Locke, E., & Smith, K. (2000). The paradox of success: An
archival and a laboratory study of strategic persistence following a
radical environmental change. Academy of Management Journal, 43,
837 853.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social
cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Bandura, A. (1989). Self-regulation of motivation and action through
internal standards and goal systems. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Goal con-
cepts in personality and social psychology (pp. 1985). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efcacy: The exercise of control. New York:
Freeman.
Bandura, A., & Cervone, D. (1983). Self-evaluative and self-efcacy
mechanisms governing the motivational effects of goal systems. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 10171028.
Bandura, A., & Cervone, D. (1986). Differential engagement of self-
reactive inuences in cognitive motivation. Organizational Behavior
and Human Decision Processes, 38, 92113.
Barrick, M., Mount, M., & Strauss, J. (1993). Conscientiousness and
performance of sales representatives: Test of the mediating effects of
goal setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 715722.
Bass, B. (1985). Leadership and performance beyond expectations. New
York: Free Press.
Baum, R., Locke, E., & Smith, K. (2001). A multi-dimensional model of
venture growth. Academy of Management Journal, 44, 292303.
Becker, L. (1978). Joint effect of feedback and goal setting on perfor-
mance: A eld study of residential energy conservation. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 63, 428 433.
Binswanger, H. (1991). Volition as cognitive self-regulation. Organiza-
tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 154 178.
Brown, T. C., & Latham, G. P. (2000a). The effects of goal setting and
self-instruction training on the performance of unionized employees.
Industrial Relations, 55, 80 94.
Brown, T. C., & Latham, G. P. (2000b, August). The effects of training in
verbal self-guidance and goal setting on teamplaying behavior. Paper
presented at the meetings of the Academy of Management, Toronto,
Ontario, Canada.
Bryan, J., & Locke, E. (1967a). Goal setting as a means of increasing
motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 274 277.
Bryan, J., & Locke, E. (1967b). Parkinsons law as a goal-setting phe-
nomenon. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 2, 258
275.
Button, S., Mathieu, J., & Zajac, D. (1996). Goal orientation in organi-
zational research: A conceptual and empirical foundation. Organiza-
tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 67, 26 48.
Carver, C., & Scheier, M. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: A control
theory approach to human behavior. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Chesney, A., & Locke, E. (1991). An examination of the relationship
among goal difculty, business strategies, and performance on a com-
plex management simulation task. Academy of Management Journal,
34, 400 424.
Dorner, D. (1991). The investigation of action regulation in uncertain and
complex situations. In J. Rasmussen & B. Brehmer (Eds.), Distributed
decision making: Cognitive models for cooperative work (pp. 349
354). Chichester, England: Wiley.
Dossett, D. L., Latham, G. P., & Mitchell, T. R. (1979). Effects of
assigned vs. participatively set goals, knowledge of results, and indi-
vidual differences on employee behavior when goal difculty is held
constant. Journal of Applied Psychology, 64, 291298.
Durham, C., Knight, D., & Locke, E. (1997). Effects of leader role,
team-set goal difculty, efcacy, and tactics on team effectiveness.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 72, 203231.
Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American
Psychologist, 41, 1040 1048.
Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A socialcognitive approach to
motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256 273.
Earley, P. C., Connolly, T., & Ekegren, G. (1989). Goals, strategy devel-
opment and task performance: Some limits on the efcacy of goal
setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 24 33.
Earley, P. C., & Perry, B. (1987). Work plan availability and performance:
An assessment of task strategy priming on subsequent task completion.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 39, 279
302.
Elliott, E. S., & Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: An approach to motivation
and achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
512.
Erez, M. (1977). Feedback: A necessary condition for the goal
performance relationship. Journal of Applied Psychology, 62, 624
627.
Erez, M. (1986). The congruence of goal-setting strategies with socio-
cultural values and its effect on performance. Journal of Management,
12, 585592.
Erez, M., Earley, P. C., & Hulin, C. L. (1985). The impact of participation
on goal acceptance and performance: A two step model. Academy of
Management Journal, 28, 50 66.
Erez, M., & Kanfer, F. H. (1983). The role of goal congruence in goal
setting and task performance. Academy of Management Review, 8,
454 463.
Erez, M., & Zidon, I. (1984). Effects of goal acceptance on the relation-
ship of goal setting and task performance. Journal of Applied Psychol-
ogy, 69, 69 78.
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention and behavior:
An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-
Wesley.
Frayne, C. A., & Latham, G. P. (1987). The application of social learning
theory to employee self-management of attendance. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 72, 387392.
Frese, M., & Zapf, D. (1994). Action as the core of work psychology: A
German approach. In H. C. Triandis & M. D. Dunnette (Eds.), Hand-
book of industrial and organizational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 271
340). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Gauggel, S. (1999). Goal setting and its inuence on the performance of
brain-damaged patients. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Philipps
University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany.
Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Carter, S. M., Lehto, A. T., & Elliott,
A. J. (1997). Predictors and consequences of achievement goals in the
college classroom: Maintaining interest and making the grade. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1284 1295.
Harackiewicz, J. M., & Elliott, N. J. (1998). The joint effects of target and
purpose goals on intrinsic motivation. Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy Bulletin, 24, 675 689.
Harackiewicz, J. M., Manderlink, G., & Sansone, C. (1984). Rewarding
pinball wizardry: Effects of evaluation and cue value on intrinsic
interest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 287300.
Hersey, R. B. (1932). Workers emotions in shop and home: A study of
individual workers from the psychological and physiological stand-
point. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Hollenbeck, J., Williams, C., & Klein, H. (1989). An empirical examina-
tion of the antecedents of commitment to difcult goals. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 74, 18 23.
Howard, A., & Bray, D. (1988). Managerial lives in transition. New
York: Guilford Press.
Huffcutt, A. I., & Arthur, W. A. (1994). Hunter and Hunter (1984)
revisited: Interview validity for entry-level jobs. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 79, 184 190.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of
decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263291.
Kanfer, F. H. (1970). Self-regulation: Research, issues and speculation. In
C. Neuringer & J. L. Michael (Eds.), Behavior modication in clinical
psychology (pp. 178 220). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Kanfer, F. H. (1996). Motivation and emotion in behavior therapy. In
K. S. Dobson & K. D. Craig (Eds.), Advances in cognitivebehavioral
therapy (pp. 130). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Kanfer, R., & Ackerman, P. L. (1989). Motivation and cognitive abilities:
An integrative aptitude treatment interaction approach to skill acquisi-
tion. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 657 690.
Kirkpatrick, S., & Locke, E. (1996). Direct and indirect effects of three
core charismatic leadership components on performance and attitudes.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 3651.
Klein, H., Wesson, M., Hollenbeck, J., & Alge, B. (1999). Goal commit-
ment and the goal-setting process: Conceptual clarication and empir-
ical synthesis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 885 896.
715September 2002
American Psychologist
Knight, D., Durham, C., & Locke, E. (2001). The relationship of team
goals, incentives and efcacy to strategic risk, tactical implementation
and performance. Academy of Management Journal, 44, 326 338.
Kornhauser, A. W., & Sharp, A. A. (1932). Employee attitudes: Sugges-
tions from a study in a factory. Personnel Journal, 10, 393404.
LaPorte, R., & Nath, R. (1976). Role of performance goals in prose
learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 68, 260 264.
Latham, G. P. (2001). The importance of understanding and changing
employee outcome expectancies for gaining commitment to an organi-
zational goal. Personnel Psychology, 54, 707716.
Latham, G. P., & Baldes, J. (1975). The practical signicance of
Lockes theory of goal setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 60,
122124.
Latham, G. P., Erez, M., & Locke, E. (1988). Resolving scientic disputes
by the joint design of crucial experiments by the antagonists: Applica-
tion to the ErezLatham dispute regarding participation in goal setting.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 753772.
Latham, G. P., & Frayne, C. A. (1989). Self-management training for
increasing job attendance: A follow-up and a replication. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 74, 411 416.
Latham, G. P., & Kinne, S. B. (1974). Improving job performance through
training in goal setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 59, 187191.
Latham, G. P., & Locke, E. A. (1975). Increasing productivity with
decreasing time limits: A eld replication of Parkinsons law. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 60, 524526.
Latham, G. P., Locke, E. A., & Fassina, N. E. (2002). The high perfor-
mance cycle: Standing the test of time. In S. Sonnentag (Ed.), Psycho-
logical management of individual performance (pp. 201228). Chich-
ester, England: Wiley.
Latham, G. P., & Marshall, H. A. (1982). The effects of self-set, partici-
patively set, and assigned goals on the performance of government
employees. Personnel Psychology, 35, 399404.
Latham, G. P., Mitchell, T. R., & Dossett, D. L. (1978). The importance
of participative goal setting and anticipated rewards on goal difculty
and job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 63, 163171.
Latham, G. P., & Saari, L. M. (1979a). The effects of holding goal
difculty constant on assigned and participatively set goals. Academy of
Management Journal, 22, 163168.
Latham, G. P., & Saari, L. M. (1979b). The importance of supportive
relationships in goal setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 64, 151
156.
Latham, G. P., & Saari, L. M. (1982). The importance of union acceptance
for productivity improvement through goal setting. Personnel Psychol-
ogy, 69, 569 573.
Latham, G. P., Saari, L. M., Pursell, E. D., & Campion, M. (1980). The
situational interview. Journal of Applied Psychology, 65, 422 427.
Latham, G. P., & Seijts, G. H. (1999). The effects of proximal and distal
goals on performance on a moderately complex task. Journal of Orga-
nizational Behavior, 20, 421 429.
Latham, G. P., & Steele, T. P. (1983). The motivational effects of
participation versus goal setting on performance. Academy of Manage-
ment Journal, 26, 406 417.
Latham, G. P., & Sue-Chan, C. (1999). A meta-analysis of the situational
interview: An enumerative review of reasons for its validity. Canadian
Psychology, 40, 56 67.
Latham, G. P., Winters, D., & Locke, E. (1994). Cognitive and motiva-
tional effects of participation: A mediator study. Journal of Organiza-
tional Behavior, 15, 49 63.
Latham, G. P., & Yukl, G. A. (1975). Assigned versus participative goal
setting with educated and uneducated wood workers. Journal of Ap-
plied Psychology, 60, 299 302.
Latham, G. P., & Yukl, G. (1976). Effects of assigned and participative
goal setting on performance and job satisfaction. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 61, 166 171.
Lee, C., & Earley, P. C. (1992). Comparative peer evaluations of orga-
nizational behavior theories. Organization Development Journal, 10,
37 42.
Lee, T., Locke, E., & Phan, S. (1997). Explaining the assigned goal
incentive interaction: The role of self-efcacy and personal goals.
Journal of Management, 23, 541559.
Lerner, B., & Locke, E. A. (1995). The effects of goal setting, self-
efcacy, competition and personal traits on the performance of an
endurance task. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 17, 138
152.
Lewin, K., Dembo, T., Festinger, L., & Sears, P. (1944). Level of
aspiration. In J. Hunt (Ed.), Personality and the behavior disorders
(Vol. 1). New York: Ronald Press.
Locke, E. A. (1991a). Goal theory vs. control theory: Contrasting ap-
proaches to understanding work motivation. Motivation and Emotion,
15, 9 28.
Locke, E. A. (1991b). The motivation sequence, the motivation hub and
the motivation core. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Processes, 50, 288 299.
Locke, E. A. (1994). The emperor is naked. Applied Psychology: An
International Review, 43, 367370.
Locke, E. A. (1995). Beyond determinism and materialism, or isnt it time
we took consciousness seriously? Journal of Behavior Therapy and
Experimental Psychiatry, 26, 265273.
Locke, E. A. (1997). The motivation to work: What we know. In M.
Maehr & P. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement
(Vol. 10, pp. 375 412). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Locke, E. A. (2000). Motivation, cognition, and action: An analysis of
studies of task goals and knowledge. Applied Psychology: An Interna-
tional Review, 49, 408 429.
Locke, E. A. (2001). Self-set goals and self-efcacy as mediators of
incentives and personality. In M. Erez, H. U. Kleinbeck, & H. Thierry
(Eds.), Work motivation in the context of a globalizing economy (pp.
1326). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Locke, E. A., Alavi, M., & Wagner, J. (1997). Participation in decision-
making: An information exchange perspective. In G. Ferris (Ed.),
Research in personnel and human resources management (Vol. 15, pp.
293331). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Locke, E., & Bryan, J. (1967). Performance goals as determinants of level
of performance and boredom. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51,
120 130.
Locke, E. A., & Bryan, J. (1968). Goal setting as a determinant of the
effects of knowledge of score in performance. American Journal of
Psychology, 81, 398 406.
Locke, E. A., & Bryan, J. (1969). The directing function of goals in task
performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 4,
35 42.
Locke, E. A., Chah, D., Harrison, S., & Lustgarten, N. (1989). Separating
the effects of goal specicity from goal level. Organizational Behavior
and Human Performance, 43, 270287.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1984). Goal setting: A motivational
technique that works! Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task
performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Locke, E. A., Motowidlo, S., & Bobko, P. (1986). Using self-efcacy
theory to resolve the conict between goal-setting theory and expec-
tancy theory in organizational behavior and industrial/organizational
psychology. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 4, 328 338.
Locke, E. A., Smith, K., Erez, M., Chah, D., & Schaffer, A. (1994). The
effects of intra-individual goal conict on performance. Journal of
Management, 20, 6791.
Mace, C. A. (1935). Incentives: Some experimental studies (Report No.
72). London: Industrial Health Research Board.
Matsui, T., Okada, A., & Inoshita, O. (1983). Mechanism of feedback
affecting task performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Per-
formance, 31, 114 122.
Matsui, T., Okada, A., & Kakuyama, T. (1982). Inuence of achievement
need on goal setting, performance, and feedback effectiveness. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 67, 645648.
McClelland, D., Atkinson, J., Clark, J., & Lowell, E. (1953). The achieve-
ment motive. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
McDaniel, M. A., Whetzel, D. L., Schmidt, F. L., & Maurer, S. D. (1994).
The validity of employment interviews: A comprehensive review and
meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 599 616.
Mento, A., Locke, E., & Klein, H. (1992). Relationship of goal level to
valence and instrumentality. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 395
405.
Miner, J. B. (1984). The validity and usefulness of theories in emerging
organizational science. Academy of Management Review, 9, 296306.
Morin, L., & Latham, G. P. (2000). Effect of mental practice and goal
716 September 2002
American Psychologist
setting as a transfer of training intervention on supervisors self-efcacy
and communication skills: An exploratory study. Applied Psychology:
An International Review, 49, 566578.
Murphy, S. T. (2001). Feeling without thinking: Affective primacy and
the nonconscious processing of emotion. In J. A. Bargh & D. K. Apsley
(Eds.), Unraveling the complexities of social life: A festschrift in honor
of Robert B. Zajonc (pp. 3953). Washington, DC: American Psycho-
logical Association.
OLeary-Kelly, A., Martocchio, J., & Frink, D. (1994). A review of the
inuence of group goals on group performance. Academy of Manage-
ment Journal, 37, 12851301.
Pinder, C. (1998). Work motivation in organizational behavior. Engle-
wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Powers, W. (1978). Quantitative analysis of purposive systems: Some
spadework at the foundations of scientic psychology. Psychological
Review, 85, 417 435.
Richardson, A. (1967). Mental practice: A review and discussion: Part I.
Research Quarterly, 38, 95107.
Rogers, R., & Hunter, J. (1991). Impact of management by objectives on
organizational productivity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 322
336.
Ronan, W. W., Latham, G. P., & Kinne, S. B. (1973). The effects of goal
setting and supervision on worker behavior in an industrial situation.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 58, 302307.
Rothkopf, E., & Billington, M. (l979). Goal-guided learning from text:
Inferring a descriptive processing model from inspection times and eye
movements. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 310 327.
Ryan, T. A. (1970). Intentional behavior. New York: Ronald Press.
Ryan, T. A., & Smith, P. C. (1954). Principles of industrial psychology.
New York: Ronald Press.
Sales, M. (1970). Some effects of role overload and role underload.
Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 5, 592 608.
Seijts, G. H., & Latham, B. W. (2001, June). Can goal orientation be
induced? Further exploration of the state versus trait debate. In C.
Sue-Chan (Chair), Justice, efcacy, goal orientation, culture, and cre-
ativity: New ndings in motivation. Symposium conducted at the annual
meeting of the Canadian Psychological Association, St. Foy, Quebec,
Canada.
Seijts, G. H., & Latham, G. P. (2000a). The construct of goal commit-
ment: Measurement and relationships with task performance. In R.
Gofn & E. Helmes (Eds.), Problems and solutions in human assess-
ment (pp. 315332). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Kluwer Academic.
Seijts, G. H., & Latham, G. P. (2000b). The effects of goal setting and
group size on performance in a social dilemma. Canadian Journal of
Behavioural Science, 32, 104 116.
Seijts, G. H., & Latham, G. P. (2001). The effect of learning, outcome, and
proximal goals on a moderately complex task. Journal of Organiza-
tional Behavior, 22, 291302.
Smith, K., Locke, E., & Barry, D. (1990). Goal setting, planning and
organizational performance: An experimental simulation. Organiza-
tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 46, 118 134.
Strang, H. R., Lawrence, E. C., & Fowler, P. C. (1978). Effects of
assigned goal level and knowledge of results on arithmetic compensa-
tion: A laboratory study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 63, 446 450.
Terpstra, D. E., & Rozell, E. J. (1994). The relationship of goal setting to
organizational protability. Group and Organization Management, 19,
285294.
Tracy, K., Locke, E., & Renard, M. (1999, August). Conscious goal
setting versus subconscious motives: Longitudinal and concurrent ef-
fects on the performance of entrepreneurial rms. Paper presented at
the annual meeting of the Academy of Management, Chicago, IL.
VandeWalle, D., Cron, W., & Slocum, J. (2001). The role of goal
orientation following performance feedback. Journal of Applied Psy-
chology, 86, 629 640.
Vroom, V. (1964). The motivation to work. New York: Wiley.
Wagner, J., & Gooding, R. (1987a). Effects of societal trends on partic-
ipation research. Administrative Science Quarterly, 32, 241262.
Wagner, J., & Gooding, R. (1987b). Shared inuence and organizational
behavior: A meta-analysis of situational variables expected to moderate
participation outcome relationships. Academy of Management Journal,
30, 524 541.
Wegge, J., & Dibbelt, S. (2000). Zur wirkung von Zielsetzungen auf die
Informationsverarbeitung bei Buchstabenvergleichsaufgaben [Effects
of goal setting on information processing in letter-matching tasks].
Zeitschrift fuer Experimentelle Psychologie, 47, 89114.
White, S., & Locke, E. (2000). Problems with the Pygmalion effect and
some proposed solutions. Leadership Quarterly, 11, 389 415.
Winters, D., & Latham, G. (1996). The effect of learning versus outcome
goals on a simple versus a complex task. Group and Organization
Management, 21, 236 250.
Wood, R., Atkins, P., & Bright, J. (1999). Bonuses, goals and instrumen-
tality effects. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 703720.
Wood, R., & Bandura, A. (1989). Social cognitive theory of organiza-
tional management. Academy of Management Review, 14, 361384.
Wood, R., & Locke, E. (1990). Goal setting and strategy effects on
complex tasks. In B. Staw & L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in orga-
nizational behavior (Vol. 12, pp. 73109). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Wood, R., Mento, A., & Locke, E. (1987). Task complexity as a moder-
ator of goal effects. Journal of Applied Psychology, 17, 416425.
Yukl, G. A., & Latham, G. P. (1978). Interrelationships among employee
participation, individual differences, goal difculty, goal acceptance,
goal instrumentality, and performance. Personnel Psychology, 31, 305
324.
717September 2002
American Psychologist
... Goal setting enables people to focus their efforts on activities that will help them reach their objectives and ignore those that are not as vital [23]. This goal describes what should be done and how much effort is needed to achieve it [24], [25]. Goals can be self-set, collaboratively set by participants, or imposed by others, and they remain effective regardless of their origin [26]. ...
... R. S. Weinberg [27] declares that athletes who have specific goals will perform better than those trying without goals or goals are more general. According to E. A. Locke and G. P. Latham [24], 35 years of researching goal setting suggests that 90% of the study results show a positive effect on work performance and a goal or goal is defined as achieving a certain standard of ability in a task, usually bound within a certain time limit [27]. In other words, athlete achievement is the end outcome, and goal setting serves as a mediator to guide the impact of parents, social support, and selfconfidence. ...
... Athletes who have specific goals are more disciplined in practicing, because they know clearly the purpose of the exercises they do [16]. The factors that influence goal setting are commitment and feedback [24]. E. A. Locke and G. P. Latham [24] explain the application of goal setting through four mechanisms, namely: (1) The target or goal functions directive as a direction determinant, this is a form of effort and attention will directly lead to the specified goal which affects both cognitive and behavioral, (2) Serves as an energizer (energizing) which makes athletes try harder to achieve it, (3) The specified target makes individuals do tenacity/persistence repeatedly so that the target can be achieved, (4) It functions indirectly to direct strategy, focus efforts, and use their knowledge and skills to achieve the specified target. ...
... This approach enables students to experience success, thereby gradually stimulating and enhancing their selfconfidence (Li and Chi, 2007). Second, teachers should instruct students to establish clear and challenging goals (Locke and Latham, 2002), incorporate tools to assess self-efficacy (e.g., self-efficacy scales) into training (Biddle, 1999), and regularly monitor athletes' confidence levels. These strategies have been shown to significantly improve athletes' self-confidence and performance (Weinberg and Gould, 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Existing studies on athletes’ sports performance (SP) have identified significant associations between competitive state anxiety (CSA) and goal orientation (GO). However, few studies have explored the intrinsic relationships among CSA, GO, and SP. Moreover, there is a lack of attention paid to college track and field (T&F) athletes as a specific group, particularly regarding the role of GO in this process. We investigated the relationship between CSA and GO as well as their effects on athletes’ SP using a sample of Chinese college T&F athletes ( N = 87, 44.8% female). Specifically, the mediating role of GO in the relationship between CSA and SP was examined. Task orientation significantly mediated the relationship between self-confidence and SP, with higher levels of task orientation leading to shorter completion times for the 1500-meter race. However, ego orientation was not associated with sports performance. These findings suggest that fostering task orientation and enhancing self-confidence are crucial to improving SP. This study deepens our understanding of CSA and GO in athletes by contributing to the current theoretical framework. Furthermore, our results offer practical recommendations for coaches and educators to develop evidence-based training strategies aimed at optimizing athletes’ psychological and sports performance.
... This aligns with self-regulation theory (Carver and Scheier, 1982) and the PSD model (Oinas-Kukkonen and Harjumaa, 2018), both of which emphasize that self-monitoring (tracking steps, calories burned) enhances intrinsic motivation and persistence. Exercise tracking and progress management are key design elements for improving behavioral monitoring and feedback, strengthening users' sense of purpose and commitment to their goals (Carver and Scheier, 1982;Locke and Latham, 2002;Gagnon et al., 2018). These features have been identified as some of the most favored by female exercisers (Ehlers and Huberty, 2014;Li et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Physical exercise is a widely recognized and practical approach to postnatal rehabilitation. In recent years, its delivery through mobile applications has become increasingly prevalent due to their accessibility and affordability. These mobile applications are crucial in supporting postpartum women in restoring physical and mental well-being and promoting sustainable health behaviors. The intention to use such mobile applications is a key determinant of user behavior and offers valuable guidance for designing and improving digital health services. Despite this, current research has largely overlooked the intention to use postpartum exercise rehabilitation mobile applications from the user experience perspective. This gap has contributed to a limited understanding of the specific needs of postpartum women, which are frequently underestimated or disregarded in the design process. Objective This study aims to identify the key factors influencing postpartum women’s use of postpartum exercise rehabilitation mobile applications and to develop corresponding evaluation scales and behavioral models related to their intention to use. The ultimate goal is to promote the health and well-being of postpartum women while offering a theoretical foundation for future research and design practices in relevant domains. Methods A mixed-methods approach was employed, integrating qualitative and quantitative research techniques. Initially, user interviews, open-ended questionnaires, and a review of existing literature were conducted to identify the factors that postpartum women consider most important when evaluating postpartum exercise rehabilitation mobile applications. These factors were then synthesized to develop an evaluation scale measuring users’ intention to use. Subsequently, exploratory factor analysis and linear regression analysis were applied to identify the key influencing factors, examine their relationships with intention to use, and construct a behavioral model reflecting the determinants of user intention. Results 300 valid questionnaires were collected and used for factor analysis and linear regression analysis. The study identified five key factors. The standardized path coefficients between these factors and the intention to use were 0.254, 0.205, 0.198, 0.015, and 0.142, respectively. All factors, except factor 4 (p = 0.77), demonstrated statistically significant relationships with intention to use (p < 0.05). These findings indicate that exercise safety assurance, physical activity tracking, emotional social support, and health benefits significantly influence users’ intention to use postpartum exercise rehabilitation mobile applications. In contrast, dialogue support does not have a direct effect. Conclusion Postpartum women using exercise rehabilitation mobile applications prioritized the practical features of these tools, particularly the safety and efficacy of the exercise regimens and the emotional and social support provided. These findings highlight the importance of aligning functional benefits with users’ health objectives.
... According to traditional T.L. literature, these leaders guide and encourage employee mindfulness by articulating a vision that heightens employees' consciousness and consideration of corporate values, goals, and performances (Jensen & Bro, 2018;. Vision articulation, rationales, and an effective leadership style promote goal recognition by allowing followers to achieve organizational goals (Locke & Latham, 2002;Steinmann et al., 2018). Hence, transformational leadership motivates followers to prioritize corporate interests first and to put in extra effort to serve the organization's vision . ...
Thesis
Full-text available
The phenomenon of Black male leadership is an understudied topic, specifically in emotional intelligence. This qualitative descriptive study explores how Black male leaders use emotional intelligence to positively impact job satisfaction and career advancement. The perceived benefits of the study are increased self-awareness, social awareness, self-management, and relationship management of Black male leaders and their subordinates. Emotional intelligence is associated with effective leadership, and this study focuses on the added value of EQ to Black male leaders as a tool to influence career advancement and job satisfaction. The emotional intelligence, job satisfaction theory, and career development theoretical frameworks guided this study. Additionally, purposive and snowball sampling was used to gather perceptions from nine Black male leaders who led at least one direct report. The findings suggest that Black male leaders focus on increasing job satisfaction and career advancement by cultivating strong relationships, building support networks, seeking mentors, and engaging in professional development opportunities. Keywords: Emotional intelligence, career advancement, job satisfaction, Black male leaders, diversity, executive leadership.
Article
Purpose The study aims to investigate how badge incentives influence systems shape longitudinal behavioral patterns in online knowledge communities. While prior research based on the goal-gradient hypothesis has documented temporary increases in user contributions preceding badge attainment, the mechanisms underlying sustained behavioral evolution remain underexplored. Design/methodology/approach We employed group-based trajectory modeling to identify distinct behavioral pattern ( N = 2,055) through longitudinal analysis of user activity records. Users’ posting frequency before and after badge acquisition was systematically tracked to observe the steering effect across global and subgroup levels. Multinomial logistic regression analyses examined co-evolutionary relationships between behavioral trajectories and badge incentives. Findings The results reveal three key insights: 1) Badge attainment exerts time-dependent steering effects that differentially influence engagement trajectories across user subgroups. 2) The evolution of user behavior has a close positive correlation with badge incentive, in addition to being influenced by user knowledge characteristics. 3) Strategic adjustment of the number of badges rather than the overall level shows greater potential for activating low-engagement users. Originality/value This research advances incentive mechanism literature by 1) developing a dynamic framework for modeling behavioral trajectories in reward systems, 2) demonstrating heterogeneous badge effects across user segments, especially for low-engagement users and 3) proposing evidence-based guidelines for optimizing badge architectures. The findings provide actionable insights for platform designers seeking to balance engagement boosting and quality maintenance through targeted incentive engineering.
Article
Purpose Empirical findings and arguments regarding the relationship between goal level and crowdfunding performance have been inconsistent. This research seeks to clarify these inconsistencies by distinguishing crowdfunding performance into two types of performance metrics, target-based (e.g. goal success, fund-goal ratio and goal completion ratio) and fundraising-based (e.g. total raised funds, number of contributors and amount per contributor). It aims to examine how goal level differentially influences these two types of performance metrics. Design/methodology/approach A meta-analysis of published studies and analyses of crowdfunding data from two heterogeneous platforms (i.e. Kickstarter and Kakao Together) were conducted. Findings Meta-analysis and crowdfunding data analyses consistently present evidence that the type of performance metrics moderates the relationship between goal level and performance. The goal level is negatively related to target-based performance metrics, indicating that lower-level goals perform better in goal success, fund-goal ratio and goal completion ratio. On the contrary, the goal level is positively related to fundraising-based performance metrics, with higher-level goals performing better in the total raised funds, number of contributors and amount per contributor. Practical implications Findings suggest that goal setting in crowdfunding should be guided by the types of performance metrics used to define success, as these metrics are inherently shaped by the platform’s operational model (all-or-nothing vs keep-it-all). Originality/value This research addresses and resolves conflicting findings in the existing literature on the relationship between goal level and crowdfunding performance by clarifying the role of performance metrics in evaluating crowdfunding success.
Article
Full-text available
Recent studies have used algorithm tailoring on digital platforms to provide household energy-saving advice. Such ‘recommender systems’ have successfully used the psychometric Rasch model as an advice algorithm, matching energy-saving measures in terms of their difficulty to consumers’ ability levels. While these previous studies indicated positive user experiences, tailored advice did not lead to higher savings overall; not even when also using persuasive nudges, such as displaying social norm percentages in the system. One possible reason for these results was that the system was used exploratively, allowing users to pick energy measures as they liked without tapping into goal setting or value-based motivational frames (e.g., signposts). In this study, 202 participants used and evaluated our ‘Saving Aid’ Rasch recommender system, choosing energy-saving measures they would like to perform at home. Through a 3 × 2-between subject design, we examined whether guided goal setting and signposts (kWh/Euro/CO2) affected user experience and energy savings. Following the signpost literature, we examined the moderation of these effects by user values, such as environmental concern (New Environmental Paradigm (NEP) score). A structural equation model analysis revealed that goal setting did not affect outcome variables, while signpost framing had varying effects, although these were not in line with prior expectations. Still, the overall system remains promising, with users achieving a 316 kWh yearly savings with the chosen recommendations.
Article
Full-text available
221 high school and college students studied a 1,481-word passage to achieve prememorized goals in 3 experiments. Inspection times and eye movements were recorded in goal-relevant and nonrelevant text neighborhoods. Averaged group data indicated 2 inspection modes: (a) relatively rapid inspection of incidental text, and (b) slow goal processing. Goal-relevant sentences resulted in over twice as many fixations, each 15 msec longer than incidental sentences. Relative goal-processing time and goal achievements were positively correlated. Contrary to previous conjecture, the obtained descriptive model suggests that goal-guided reading is time-efficient when density of goal-relevant information in text is low. Qualitative differences were observed in style of Ss' responses to task demands. General processing models of goal-guided learning may have to be elaborated to accommodate inspection style differences among readers. (34 ref)
Article
Full-text available
This study examined the relationship of goal orientation and performance over a series of 2 challenging performance events. After providing performance feedback on the 1st event, the authors found that the relationship between a learning goal orientation and performance remained positive for the 2nd event, the relationship between a proving goal orientation and performance diminished from a positive to a nonsignificant level, and the relationship between an avoiding goal orientation and performance remained negative. Data analysis also indicated that the relationships between the 3 goal orientation dimensions and the performance event were differentially mediated by goal setting, self-efficacy, and effort.
Chapter
Decision theory and the theory of rational choice have recently been the subjects of considerable research by philosophers and economists. However, no adequate anthology exists which can be used to introduce students to the field. This volume is designed to meet that need. The essays included are organized into five parts covering the foundations of decision theory, the conceptualization of probability and utility, pholosophical difficulties with the rules of rationality and with the assessment of probability, and causal decision theory. The editors provide an extensive introduction to the field and introductions to each part.
Article
Investigations into the process of mentally practicing a perceptual motor skill began about 30 years ago. However, there has been a marked increase in the amount of interest in this area of research in the last 10 years. This is the first of two articles, which together aim to present a critical review of work so far completed, possible explanations of the phenomenon, and an indication of the directions that future research might take. In this article the evidence for improvement under conditions of mental practice is reviewed.
Book
This is the eighteenth in the most prestigious series of annual volumes in the field of industrial and organizational psychology. The series provides authoritative and integrative reviews of the key literature of industrial psychology and organizational behavior. The chapters are written by established experts and topics are carefully chosen to reflect the major concerns in the research literature and in current practice. Specific issues covered in this volume reflect the growth and complexity of the organizational psychology field, for example: Implicit Knowledge and Experience in Work and Organizations, Flexible Working Arrangements, Web-based Recruiting and Testing, Economic Psychology, Workaholism, and a review of Ethnic Group Differences and Measuring Cognitive Ability. Each chapter offers a comprehensive and critical survey of a chosen topic, and each is supported by valuable bibliography. For advanced students, academics, and researchers, as well as professional psychologists and managers, this remains the most authoritative and current guide to developments and established knowledge in the field of industrial and organizational psychology.
Chapter
IntroductionThe High Performance CycleDemands Influence PerformanceMediatorsModeratorsPerformance Leads to Organization CommitmentSatisfaction Leads to Organization CommitmentDiscussionAcknowledgementReferences