ArticlePDF Available

A Comparison of Popular Online Fonts: Which Size and Type is Best?

Authors:
January 2002, Vol. 4 Issue 1
|
Volume 4 Issue 1 | Past Issues | A-Z List |
Usability News is a free web newsletter that is produced by the Software Usability Research Laboratory
(SURL) at Wichita State University. The SURL team specializes in software/website user interface
design, usability testing, and research in human-computer interaction.
Barbara S. Chaparro, Editor
A Comparison of Popular Online Fonts: Which Size and Type is Best?
By Michael Bernard, Bonnie Lida, Shannon Riley,
Telia Hackler, & Karen Janzen
In the last edition of Usability News we discussed our findings in regard to the performance and
preference of twelve different fonts at the 12-point size. We are now able to compare these fonts at the
10-, 12-, and 14-point sizes. To do this, we examined some of the most commonly used fonts for
differences in reading effectiveness, reading time, perceptions of font legibility, font attractiveness, and
general preference. The fonts that were examined are listed below in Table 1.
Table 1. The eight fonts studied (
View a sample of each font type)
Serif Fonts Sans Serif Fonts
Century Schoolbook (Schoolbook)
Courier New (Courier)
Georgia
Times New Roman (Times)
Arial
Comic Sans MS (Comic)
Tahoma
Verdana
Currently, text that is viewed on computer screens consist of an amalgamation of both serif and sans
serif fonts that were designed specifically for computer use, as well as those that were originally
intended for print (serif fonts cross-strokes that project from the main stroke of a letter, whereas sans
serif fonts do not). Fonts designed for print, such as Times, were created for both legibility and
economy of print space. Georgia, on the other hand, was designed specifically for computer-display.
Georgia is somewhat similar in appearance to Times. However to make Georgia more legible for
computer-screen viewing, its uppercase characters were lightened and the letters’ x-height (the height
of the torso for lowercase letters, such as an ‘x’) was increased. Research by Boyarski, Neuwirth,
Forlizzi, and Regli (1998) examining Times, Georgia, and Verdana fonts on computer screens has found
that Georgia was significantly perceived to be easier to read, sharper, and more legible than Times.
Another commonly used serif font is Schoolbook. Schoolbook was designed for maximum legibility, and
is still used in elementary school texts. Courier, on the other hand, was originally designed as a
typewriter face and is currently the most commonly used mono-spaced font.
The most commonly used sans serif font is reported to be Arial (Ramsden, 2000). However, Tahoma,
and Verdana are also very popular. Arial has a rather large x-height and the letters are spaced so they
do not touch. Both Tahoma and Verdana were specifically intended for viewing on computer-screens by
also having wider letter spacing and a large x-height. In addition, great effort was taken to make the
lowercase letters, j, l, and I more distinctive on computer screens. Tahoma and Verdana are fairly
similar to each other, except that Tahoma has a greater letter width than Verdana. Another sans serif
font that has become quite popular is Comic. Comic was designed to mimic print found on comic strips,
and it is generally preferred among children (Bernard, Mills, Frank, & McKown, 2001).
METHOD
Participants
Sixty participants (16 males and 44 females) volunteered for this study. They ranged in age from 18 to
55, with a mean age of 24 (S.D. = 7.8 years). All participants had 20/40 or better unaided or corrected
vision as assessed by a Snellen near acuity test. Thirty-three percent wore prescription glasses.
Ninety-three percent of the participants reported to have regularly read documents on computer
screens at least a few times per week, and 78 percent of them had at least four years of college.
Task Design
The task design and procedure was the same as discussed in the last issue of Usability News (Bernard,
Mills, Peterson, & Storrer, 2001) except that in this study, type size was examined as well. A Pentium II
based PC computer, with a 60 Hz, 96dpi 17” monitor with a resolution setting of 1024 x 768 pixels was
used in this experiment.
Results and Discussion
A three-factor mixed ANOVA design was used to analyze objective and subjective differences between
the 10-, 12-, and 14-point sizes and fonts. The between-subjects measures were the three type sizes
and the within-subject measures were the font types. Ranked font preference was measured by using a
Friedman Χ
2
.
Reading Efficiency
Examining reading efficiency (reading time/accuracy) resulted in no significant font size or type effects.
That is, fonts that were read faster were generally read less accurately, and thus had comparable
reading efficiency scores. This was also found in previous studies (i.e., Bernard & Mills, 2000; Boyarski,
et al.,1998), which suggest that differences between the examined fonts at these sizes are not great
enough to substantially affect reading efficiency. On average, however, larger text sizes are considered
more readable than smaller sizes (Mills & Weldon, 1987; Rudnicky & Kolers, 1984). Yet these readability
differences are often not significantly apparent until the size difference becomes quite large (Tinker,
1963). To a large extent, this may be true for online reading.
Reading Time
Examining the mean reading time for each font type irrespective of their accuracy, revealed significant
font type [F(7, 399) = 2.79, p < .01] and size [F(2, 57) = 4.10, p < .05] differences. Post hoc analysis
indicated that both Times and Arial were read significantly faster than Courier, Schoolbook, and
Georgia. Fonts at the 10-point size were read significantly more slowly than fonts at the 12-point size
(see Figure 1). The average difference between the fastest and slowest read font was 99.4 seconds. For
a two-page online document, this difference is not that great. And since the majority of online text is
shorter than this, the difference in reading time between fonts should typically be quite small.
Figure 1. Reading time in seconds (longer bars indicated longer reading times)
Perceived Legibility
Assessing the perception of font legibility revealed a significant font type x size interaction [F(14, 399)
= 2.21, p <.01]. Post hoc analysis revealed that 10-point Tahoma was perceived as more legible than
12-point schoolbook. In addition, 12-point Verdana and Courier were significantly perceived as being
more legible than 10-point Comic, Schoolbook and Verdana. Also, 12-point Courier was perceived as
more legible than 12-point Schoolbook and Tahoma, as well as 14-point Comic. Interestingly, 10-point
Georgia had a significantly higher perception of legibility than 12-point Tahoma and Schoolbook.
Fourteen-point Arial was perceived as being more legible than14-point Comic, and 10-point Arial was
perceived as more legible than 12-point Tahoma. Moreover at the 14-point size, only Arial was
significantly perceived as being more legible than fonts at other sizes (10-point Schoolbook and Comic).
It is thus possible that, in general, increasing text size does not add to the perceived legibility of fonts
(at least at these sizes). Overall, Arial and Courier were considered the most legible fonts, whereas
Comic was perceived as the most illegible font (see Figure 2).
Figure 2. Perceived font legibility (1 = “Not at all” and 6 = “Completely”)
Perceived Attractiveness
Examining participants’ impressions regarding a particular font as being attractive revealed significant
font type differences [F(7, 399) = 5.64, p <.001]. Post hoc analysis indicated that Georgia was
significantly perceived as being more attractive (regardless of font size) than Arial, Courier, and Comic,
while Times was significantly perceived as more attractive than Courier (see Figure 3). It is possible
that Georgia and Times were considered attractive because of their widespread use in both print and on
computer screens (Times also serves as the primary default for Microsoft Office
TM
software suites) and,
thus, participants were more familiar with this type of font. In fact, the majority of the participants had
Times as their default font (32%). Arial came in second with 10% having it as their default font. It is, of
course, also possible that the more ornate style of the serf fonts, Times and Georgia, made them
appear more attractive than the sans serif fonts (for a more detailed discussion of the subjective
reactions associated with the examined fonts at the 12-point size, see Bernard, Mills, Perterson, &
Storrer, 2001).
Figure 3. Perceived font attractiveness (1 = “Not at all” and 6 = “Completely”)
Font Preference
10-point
Analysis of the participants’ font preference at the 10-point size revealed significant differences in
ranking [c
2
(7, N = 20) = 59.38, p < .001]. Post hoc analysis indicated that Arial, Courier, Comic,
Georgia, and Verdana were significantly preferred to Times. Verdana was significantly preferred to
Schoolbook. Verdana was the most preferred and Times was the least preferred font at this size, which
is not that surprising since Verdana was designed specifically for the computer screen whereas Times
was not.
12-point
Comparing fonts at the 12-point size also revealed significant preference differences [Χ
2
(7, N = 20) =
20.67, p < .01]. Arial, which was the most preferred font, was significantly preferred to Times, which
was the least preferred font at this size.
14-point
At the 14-point size, preference differences approached significance [Χ
2
(7, N = 20) = 13.33, p =
.064]. Comic was the most preferred and Times was the least preferred font at this size. It is likely the
preference differences were not as striking at this size because at the 14-point size, the letters for all
fonts were generally large enough to be comfortably read.
Overall, the analysis of the participants’ font preference when considering all three sizes revealed a
significant difference [c
2
(7, N = 60) = 72.77, p < .001] in that Times was significantly less preferred
to all fonts except Schoolbook. Schoolbook was significantly less preferred to Verdana. Overall, Verdana
was the most preferred font, while Times was the least preferred font (see Figure 4).
Figure 4. Font preference (longer bars indicates more preferred font choice).
CONCLUSION
Several observations can be made regarding the examined font types. First, no significant differences in
reading efficiency were detected between the font types at any size. There were, however, significant
differences in reading time. Generally, Times and Arial were read faster than Courier, Schoolbook, and
Georgia. Fonts at the 12-point size were read faster than fonts at the 10-point size. In addition, a font
type x size interaction was found for the perception of font legibility. In general, however, Arial, Courier,
and Georgia were perceived as the most legible.
For font attractiveness, Georgia was perceived as being more attractive than Arial, Courier, and Comic,
while Times was perceived as more attractive than Courier. This contrasts with participants’ general
preference for a particular font type. Overall, Verdana was the most preferred font, while Times was the
least preferred. Thus it seems that the Georgia and Times serif fonts are considered more attractive,
but they are generally less preferred. Of the fonts studied, Verdana appears to be the best overall font
choice. Besides being the most preferred, it was read fairly quickly and was perceived as being legible.
As with all studies that examine reading performance, caution should be made in generalizing these
outcomes to other font types. Many factors should be taken into account, such as individual text
characteristics, the text size, the line and character spacing, the computer settings, as well as the user
characteristics.
REFERENCES
Bernard, M., & Mills, M. (2000). So, what size and type of font should I use on my website? Usability
News 2.2 [Online].
http://psychology.wichita.edu/surl/usabilitynews/22/font.asp
Bernard, M., Mills, M., Frank, T., & McKown J. (2001). Which font do children prefer to read online?
Usability News 3.1 [Online].
http://psychology.wichita.edu/surl/usabilitynews/31/fontJR.asp
Bernard, M., Mills, M., Peterson, M., & Storrer, K. (2001). A comparison of popular online fonts: Which
are best and when? Usability News, 3. 2. [Online].
http://psychology.wichita.edu/surl/usabilitynews//32/font.asp
Boyarski, D., Neuwirth, C., Forlizzi, J., and Regli, S. H. (1998). A study of fonts designed for screen
display. Proceedings of CHI’ 98, 87-94.
Mills, C. B. & Weldon, L. J. (1987). Reading test from computer screens. ACM Computing Surveys, 19
(4), 329-358.
Ramsden, A. (2000).Annabella’s HTML Help. [Online].
http://www.geocities.com/annabella.geo/fontface.html
Rudnicky, A. I., & Kolers, P. A. (1984). Size and case of type as stimuli in reading, Journal of
Experimental Psychology in Human Perception and Performance, 10, 231-249.
Tinker, M. (1963). Legibility of Print. Ames, IA: Iowa State University Press.
SUBSCRIBE to Usability News!
... • Times New Roman is usually near the end of the ranking. This is in good agreement with the results published in the literature ( [1][2][3]). The popularity of this font is partly an aesthetic and historic issue, but for long texts the serifs help to follow the lines. ...
... This varying nature of the Comic Sans font is observed in Refs. [3,4], and [5] and is probably behind the confusing human acceptance of this typeface. ...
Article
Full-text available
Selecting an appropriate typeface is crucial in typography, web design, and other applications where text readability is essential. A key concept of this topic is legibility, the quality that shows how easy it is to recognize the letters of a particular font set. Previous works have measured legibility by human experiments, which has several limitations; for example, the methodology and circumstances were not entirely uniform, and the results may be distorted by the fatigue of the test subjects. This paper presents a new method using self-developed software to substitute human measurements in legibility testing. The software simulates the human retina's distortion effects (direction-dependent acuity) on test images of the assessed font's characters. Then, its output is analyzed using optical character recognition software. By integrating these techniques, we model the optical, biological, and cognitive steps of human character recognition as well. Although the simulation is imperfect, the software can perform significantly more measurements than human experiments with higher uniformity and give reproducible legibility information about significantly more fonts in various circumstances. In addition to the two scaling methods used in the literature (x-height, font-height), the tests are also performed with two other self-developed scaling methods, which provide a fairer comparison in the case of non-standard character types. This paper contains the legibility measurement results for 22 fonts under various simulated scenarios. The derived font ranking aligns closely with findings from prior human-based studies, demonstrating the robustness and reliability of the proposed method. Moreover, this approach provides valuable insights into font legibility across a broader spectrum of use cases, highlighting its potential for practical applications in typography and design.
... serif Bernard et al., 2002;Morris et al., 2002 Bigelow, 2019 Kaspar et al., 2015 Times New Roman Arial Juni & Gross, 2008Song & Schwarz, 2008 Barrett & Russell, 1999 Bar & Neta, 2006;Bertamini et al., 2016;Palumbo et al., 2015Bar & Neta, 2007Watson et al., 2012;Arono et al., 1988G*Power Faul et al., 2007 t dz= 0.5 α=.05, Examples of the words used in the valence judgment task (a) and the nonsense string used in the impression rating task (b) Note. Rounded and spiky fonts are HachiMaruPop and Onryo , respectively. ...
Article
This study investigates whether typeface influences affective judgments of words consisting of two kanji. Japanese words with either positive or negative meanings were presented in either rounded or spiky fonts. Participants ( N=92) were asked to rapidly and accurately judge the valence of the words. Participants also rated their impressions of the fonts with the semantic differential (SD) method. The results indicate participants judged more positive valences for the rounded font than the spiky font and that the median reaction time was shorter when word valence and font valence were congruent than when incongruent. The congruency effect was larger for rounded fonts than for spiky fonts. These findings suggest that typefaces can influence the valence judgments of word meanings, even when typeface is task-irrelevant.
... Numerous empirical studies have examined the characteristics of written text and its perception when read on paper [9][10][11] as well as on electronic devices [12][13][14]. However, the findings from these studies concerning the correlation between reading speed and typeface characteristics are diverse and occasionally conflicting [15]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Teachers often serve as creators of written instructions that students use to complete assignments. This research investigates whether the speed at which the instructions are read is impacted by different visual designs (such as typeface shape, colour, and layout) and the media through which they are presented (digital or traditional). The objective is to determine if these factors influence reading time; therefore, an experiment was carried out. The experiment employed a 5 × 5 Latin square design, with student participants reading the same task instructions in both digital (near and far) and traditional formats. Each set of instructions was presented in a different typographic solution. Reading times were self-recorded and analysed using ANOVA to assess the effects of typographic solutions and reading media. The study did not reveal any statistically significant differences in reading speed based on the colour of the text and background as well as typographic solutions. However, it did find a statistically significant variation between reading media, with variations between digital and traditional formats. These results are crucial for enhancing the design of instructional materials, suggesting the necessity of carefully considering reading media in educational contexts.
... It is crucial to highlight that the benchmark typefaces were not selected based on their legibility performance; instead, they represent various stylistic approaches to typefaces for average readers. Specifically, TNR is a serif and a widely used typeface among average readers [89], Courier is a slab serif and monospaced, while Helvetica is sans serif. The benchmark typefaces used are not recommended by the British Dyslexia Association (BDA) for their readability for dyslexic readers because their letterform characteristics are designed for mainstream readers. ...
Article
Full-text available
Dyslexia-friendly typefaces for the Latin script have been proliferating during the past decade. The typefaces are designed to tackle the challenges faced in a dyslexic reading experience by manipulating their letter forms and typographic attributes; several studies reported a positive effect on the reading experience. To this date, no working dyslexia-friendly Arabic typefaces are available for the public. The present study is part of a larger practice-based research, where a novel dyslexia-friendly Arabic typeface is designed using a user-centred design approach. The current visual analysis marks the developmental phase, identifying the letterform features of dyslexia-friendly Latin typefaces that can be mapped to the Arabic script. This article explores the typographic features of dyslexia-friendly Latin typefaces by conducting a qualitative visual analysis; a proposed modified version of Leeuwen’s Typographic Distinctive Features Framework is employed. The results are discussed considering the Arabic script’s visual implications in a dyslexic reading experience. The findings of this study are used to create a list of design considerations for a dyslexia-friendly Arabic typeface.
... The usage of Arial typeface, especially in boldface and in all-uppercase helped in bringing more emphasis as to what the memes wanted to convey. Arial is observed to be the most used typeface on the memes used in the study due to its perceived legibility in line with what Bernard, Lida, Riley, Hackler & Janzen (2002) stated in their study. This legibility brought by Arial typeface also affected the readability of the text. ...
Article
Full-text available
During the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic, memes were mass-produced, and it was observed that they featured humor, entertainment, and awareness. This study aimed to analyze the images used and text placements of selected viral COVID-19 memes. Semiology concepts from Saussure (1984) and Peirce (1991) were employed amidst the content analysis of the aforementioned. The images used and text placements were identified as the semiotic patterns of each meme. The study found that the image selection is based on popular culture. Photographs of people and screen capture images from movies and TV/cartoon shows are mostly used. The texts used in the memes are mostly placed in the center, overlaying the image or background used. An all-white Arial typeface with 28-48 pt. is the most used typography. With this text-image relationship, the memes are read more easily, leading to better comprehension and virality. Hence, the study concluded that memes may be utilized to a) associate or represent people, emotions, experiences, and sentiments and b) communicate using another form of media. The study recommends the integration of memes in teaching a lesson, specifically in English at the secondary level, to promote humor and creativity in the language classroom.
Book
Full-text available
Typography is an intricate form of visual refinements for the written language, and as such it comes with a rather considerable impact on reading not limited to its mere aesthetics. This study examines how typography affects cognitive comprehension and emotional engagement in reading. An analysis of three critical typographical variables-typeface, font size, and line spacing-showed that each had different effects on the experience of reading. Serif fonts like Times New Roman improved readability and message clarity more than Script fonts such as The Nautigal increased emotional engagement despite reduced legibility. Fonts such as Helvetica, sans-serif, have been balanced in usage in terms of readability and engagement. The best font size is 12px because it increases any more than which would add less; instead, it causes a less readable output; and maximum line spacing is set at 1.4 lines for maximum readability comfort and efficiency. These findings support valuable knowledge for designers and educators to optimize text presentation to impact comprehension and emotional effects.
Article
Full-text available
This article delves into the intricate relationship between typography and graphic design in a comprehensive manner. Within this context, the pivotal role of typography in enhancing readability, directing cognitive processes, and establishing emotional connections has been emphasized. Starting with the identification of the fundamental relationship between typography and graphic design, the study examines how the choice and design of typefaces play a critical role across a broad spectrum, from readability to visual hierarchy, and from brand identity to aesthetic value. Drawing insights from various disciplines, the profound influence of typography on cognitive and emotional responses has been discussed. Key elements such as legibility, readability, space utilization, color contrast, and typeface complexity and their impact on the effectiveness of written communication are meticulously explored. Moreover, the study underscores that typography is not merely a functional tool but also intertwines with societal values and individual experiences. Based on academic references, this review demonstrates how typography enriches both the aesthetic and functional facets of communication design.
Article
Full-text available
By far the two most common types of fonts currently used on the Web are the serif font, Times New Roman (TNR) and the sans serif font, Arial. The question is, which one is more legible and at which size? In the past, it has been determined that serif fonts, which have ornamental strokes at the tip and base of each letter, are easier to read on paper than sans serif fonts, which do not have serifs. This is because it is believed that serifs help distinguished each individual letter (Albers, 1963). However, this benefit may be reduced or even eliminated on computer screens because of their display particularities, like poorer screen resolution and aliasing or "jaggies," as it is commonly known. The use of anti-aliased text, which is seen in documents that use formats such as Adobe's PDF TM , as well as 'smoothed' text from graphical files like JPEG, attempt to smooth out jagged edges that are seen with common dot matrix fonts. Anti-aliased formats are an attempt to make the text more legible, however this smoothing can tend to blur the text to some degree, thus possibly making it less legible for specific types and sizes of fonts than dot matrix fonts. The question we asked in this study was as follows: What font type (TNR or Arial), what size (10-or 12-point), and what text format (dot matrix or anti-aliased) is most readable and most preferred by web readers? (View a sample of the font conditions). This study examined TNR and Arial fonts for readability (accuracy in reading text material), reading time, perceptions of font legibility and sharpness, as well as general font type/size/format preference. To accomplish this, we used an IBM compatible computer and chose a monitor resolution of 1024 x 768 to reflect the current trend towards higher resolutions (GVU, 1998).
Article
Full-text available
Children today are reading large amounts of text on computer screens, either in the classroom or for leisure. In fact, currently there is a drive to supplement or even replace some traditional pen and paper lectures and tests with computer-based ones. However, to date there has been no research specific to a younger population investigating preferences for different types and sizes of fonts for reading online. This study sought to address this need by examining four types of fonts at 12-and 14-point sizes in order to help determine the font combination that is perceived as most readable on computer screens and most preferred by children. Participants Twenty-seven participants (10 males, 17 females) volunteered for this study. They ranged in age from 9 to 11 years old and attended 4th, 5th, or 6th grade. All participants had 20/20 or 20/20 corrected vision. Most participants (96%) had at least some experience reading text on computer screens. The children received $10.00 for participating in this experiment.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
This study examined the readabiity.and subjective preferences of a set of fonts designed for screen display. Two new binary bitmap fonts performed well, suggesting that designers should consider incorporating similar attributes into default fonts for on- line type. This study examines reading for comprehension and users' pref- erences of fonts designed sp&icaUy for computer screens. Since the introduction of the personal computer and the more recent increase in the use of on-line sources of information (on- line help, database searching, and especially hypertext resources on the World Wide Web), the option of reading from paper rather than from computer screens has been drastically reduced and in some cases eliminated. A substantial literature on the legibility, readability, and users' preferences for fonts exists (see (2) for a relatively recent com- prehensive review). Recent work has studied users' performance on high resolution bit-mapped displays for a range of reading u&s such as proof-reading (3,4,5J, reading for comprehension and skimming IS). These studies indicate that there is no differ- ence., at least under some circumstances, between paper and high quality screen displays. Nevertheless, reading from screens remains anecdotally problematic. In part, this situation may be due to differences between the conditions of the experiments and conditions of typical on-line presentation (e.g., (4,5) studied anti-aliased text whereas actual practice for many long passages of on-line information is binary bitmap text; (S) studied text set in boldface whereas actual practice is regular face - strokes may appear thinner on a screen than on paper-with boldface reserved for cuemg of highlights). The current study was designed to look at users' performance with text as it is typically presented on-line - using fonts that were not designed speciically for screen display - with text that is likely to be presented on computer screens in the near future - using fonts designed spec-tically for screen display. The goal of the study was not to determine whether the newly designed screen fonts are better than those appearing on paper,
Article
The role of size and case of print have provoked a number of experiments in the recent past. One strongly argued position is that the reader abstracts a canonical representation from a string of letters that renders its variations irrelevant and then carries out recognition procedures on that abstraction. An alternate view argues that the reader proceeds by analyzing the print, taking account of its manifold physical attributes such as length of words, their orientation, shape, and the like. In the present experiments size and case were varied in several ways, and the task was also varied to include both silent reading and reading aloud. Clear evidence for shape-sensing operations was brought forward, but they were shown to be optional rather than obligatory processes, used when it served the reader's purpose to do so. However, it was also shown that such skills, normally useful, could be tricked into operating even when their presence hindered the reader's performance. The conclusion is drawn that reading goes forward in many ways at once rather than through an orderly sequence of operations, consistent with the reader's skills and the requirements of the task. Overarching theories of performance seem premature in the absence of detailed analysis of task components.
Annabella's HTML Help
  • A Ramsden
Ramsden, A. (2000).Annabella's HTML Help. [Online].
A comparison of popular online fonts: Which are best and when?
  • M Bernard
  • M Mills
  • M Peterson
  • K Storrer
Bernard, M., Mills, M., Peterson, M., & Storrer, K. (2001). A comparison of popular online fonts: Which are best and when? Usability News, 3. 2. [Online].