ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Communication researchers and practitioners have suggested that framing climate change in terms of public health and/or national security may make climate change more personally relevant and emotionally engaging to segments of the public who are currently disengaged or even dismissive of the issue. To evaluate these assumptions, using a nationally representative online survey of U.S. residents (N = 1,127) conducted in December, 2010, we randomly assigned six previously identified audience segments on climate change to one of three experimental conditions. Subjects were asked to read uniquely framed news articles about climate change emphasizing either the risks to the environment, public health, or national security and the benefits of mitigation and adaptation-related actions. Results show that across audience segments, the public health focus was the most likely to elicit emotional reactions consistent with support for climate change mitigation and adaptation. Findings also indicated that the national security frame may possibly boomerang among audience segments already doubtful or dismissive of the issue, eliciting unintended feelings of anger.
Content may be subject to copyright.
LETTER
A public health frame arouses hopeful emotions
about climate change
A Letter
Teresa A. Myers & Matthew C. Nisbet & Edward W. Maibach & Anthony A. Leiserowitz
Received: 18 January 2012 /Accepted: 22 May 2012 /Published online: 28 June 2012
#
The Author(s) 2012. This article is published with open access at Springerlink.com
Abstract Communication researchers and practitioners have suggested that framing climate
change in terms of public health and/or national security may make climate change more
personally relevant and emotionally engaging to segments of the public who are currently
disengaged or even dismissive of the issue. To evaluate these assumptions, using a nationally
representative online survey of U.S. residents (N0 1,127) conducted in December, 2010, we
randomly assigned six previously identified audience segments on climate change to one of
three experimental conditions. Subjects were asked to read uniquely framed news articles
about climate change emphasizing either the risks to the environment, public health, or
national security and the benefits of mitigation and adaptation-related actions. Results show
that across audience segments, the public health focus was the most likely to elicit emotional
reactions consistent with support for climate change mitigation and adaptation. Findings also
indicated that the national security frame may possibly boomerang among audience seg-
ments already doubtful or dismissive of the issue, eliciting unintended feelings of anger.
1 Introduction
Effectively engaging the American public on climate changeincluding its causes, impacts,
and solutionsremains both a major research question and a communication challenge.
Climatic Change (2012) 113:11051112
DOI 10.1007/s10584-012-0513-6
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:10.1007/s10584-012-0513-6)
contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
T. A. Myers
:
E. W. Maibach
Center for Climate Change Communication, George Mason University, Fairfax, VA 22030, USA
T. A. Myers
e-mail: tmyers6@gmu.edu
M. C. Nisbet (*)
School of Communication, American University, Washington, DC 20016, USA
e-mail: nisbet@american.edu
A. A. Leiserowitz
School of Forestry, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06511, USA
Effective public engagement requires understanding the cognitive, affective, and behavioral
nature of audiences (Lorenzoni et al. 2007). In this paper we focus on the affective dimension
of climate change engagementa dimension that has received little research attention
examining the potential for various frames to elicit emotional reactions consistent with
climate change mitigation and adaptation goals.
Social scientists have long understood that the way an issue is framed has important
consequences (Goffman 1974; Gamson and Modigliani 1989). Framingwhether inten-
tional or notinvolves selectively emphasizing certain dimensions of an issue over others,
setting the context for per ception and discussion aro und specific causes, risks, policy
actions, and costs/benefits that might result from these actions. The frame used suggests
both the diagnosis of the problem as well as prescriptions for what should be done about it
(Nisbet 2009; Scheufele 1999). Moreover, the way an issue is framedin the media or in
other communicationsinteracts with aud ience members pre -existing schema, mental
models and values. As a consequence, a specific message frame is particularly influential
to the extent that it is relevantor applicableto the audiences preexisting predispositions
and perceptions (Nisbet and Scheufele 2009; Scheufele 1999).
Recent analyses of the U.S. public have mapped how climate change-related predispo-
sitions and perceptions vary across six distinct interpretive communities of individuals
called Global Warmings Six Americas (Maibach et al. 2010). These audience segments
range along a continuum of knowledge, attitudes, and behavior, from the Alarmed (who
accept climate change as a problem, are concerned, and who are looking for opportunities to
take personal and political action) to the Dismissive (who reject the reality of climate change
and strongly oppose action). Individuals in the four interpretive communities in the middle
of the continuum are less certain in their views on climate change, more ambivalent about
the risks and relative importance of the issue, and more likely to be disengaged personally
and politically. (See Fig. 1 for the most recent distribution of the American public across
these six segments; Leiserowitz et al. 2011).
Climate change has historically been framed as an environmental problem. More recently,
it is increasingly being framed as a poli tical problem. Social scientists, however, have
suggested a variety of potential alternative frames, each of which can validly and accurately
emphasize other relevant dimensions of the issuesuch as public health, national security,
extreme weather events, and/or the economy (Nisbet 2009; Nisbet and Scheufele 2009;
Maibach et al. 2010). Such alternative frames may be more effective at engaging audiences
than the environmental or political frames that currently dominate public discourse.
Fig. 1 Global warmings six Americas. Source: Yale/George Mason University
1106 Climatic Change (2012) 113:11051112
Research on a public health frame, for example, suggests that when climate change is
introduced as a human health issue, a broad cross-section of audienceseven segments
otherwise skeptical of climate sciencefind the information to be compelling and useful
(Maibach et al. 2010). Framing research can identify not only which frames are likely to be
effective, and with whom, but also which are likely to boomerang. For example, several
studies find that messages emphasizing catastrophic, dire consequences or threats that are
geographically remote can result in less concern and more hopelessness among audiences
(Hart and Nisbet 2011;ONeill and Nicholson-Cole 2009).
Peoples emotional reactions to messages about climate change are an important consid-
eration, yet to date they have often been overlooked both in research and in communication
efforts. Thagard and Findlay (2011) argue that encouraging belief changeabout climate
change requires dealing with emotional constraints as well as cognitive ones (p. 342) and
Mar kowitz and Shariff (2012) identify positive emotional ap peals as one of the most
promising strategies for climate change communication. Indeed, emotional responses to
novel information occur effortlessly and almost instantaneously, influencing subsequent
cognitive processing of information (Kahneman 2011).
Emotions serve as affective prompts for engagement with an issue and lead to forming
predispositions for action when a relevant situation arises (Baumeister et al. 2007). Further-
more, emotions themselves can serve as frames by which future information is interpreted (Nabi
2003). Negative emotions, such as anger or fear, motivate us to the need to be on high-alert
and to seek more information about an issue (Baron et al. 1994), can lead to greater risk
perceptions and greater policy support (Leiserowitz 2006), and have the potential to transform
apathy or indifference into perceived importance and behavioral action (Thomas et al. 2009).
However , eliciting anger against the claims or proposed recommendations emphasized in a
communication strategy also has the danger of inciting action counter to the communicators
intent. Moreover, the effect of arousing anger is likely not uniform across audience segments, as
the target and content of an individualsangermayvary.Forinstance,topicsthatelicitanger
among people alarmed about climate change might include how ecosystems, disadvantaged
people, and specific social groups are being harmed. By contrast, the very claim of climate
changesrealitymightelicitangeramongpeopledismissiveoftheissue.Thus,agivenmessage
could generate an angry backlash by some recipients, leading to an unintended social response,
while provoking an equally angryyet intendedsocial response by other recipients.
Positive emotions can also play an important role in message response (Monahan 1995).
Feelings of hope related to climate change, for example, likely increase the probability that
individuals will choose to engage with the issue and adopt beliefs and behaviors consistent
with efforts to stem the problem (Markowitz and Shariff 2012 ; Snyder 2002; Swim et al.
2010). Hope is often aroused as perceptions of efficacy, the belief that an individual has the
capacity to implement a proposed response to a threat and that the recommended action can
effectively mitigate the threat, increase (Bandura
1977). Feelings of hope and efficacy are
strongly
correlated with a willingness to engage in pro-environmental behaviors and to
support climate change policies (Lorenzoni et al. 2007). Conversely, feelings of hopelessness
and inefficacy related to climate change are linked with a tendency to ignore the problem or to
rationalize inaction (Norgaard 201 1).
With these considerations in mind, in this study we investigate how unique audience
segmentsGlobal Warmings Six Americasemotionally react to news articles crafted to
reflect three distinct climate change message frames: (1) a traditional environmental frame,
which emphasizes the consequences of climate change to ecosystemsand the benefits to
ecosystems of adaptation and mitigation-related actions, (2) a national security frame, which
highlights the risks to U.S. national security and the benefits to national security of
Climatic Change (2012) 113:11051112 1107
adaptation and mitigation-related actions and (3) a health frame, which stresses the health
risks associated with climate change and the potential benefits to health of adaptation and
mitigation-related actions.
2 Method
In order to assess how members of different audience segments responded to three distinct
messag e frames, we condu cted a randomized, control led message experiment using a
nationally representative online sample of 1,127 individuals. Participants were randomly
assigned to read one of three specifically framed articles each of approximately 610 words,
structured identically, but emphasizing different dimensi ons of the climate change
problem. Am ong subjects, 376 p articipant s (33 %) were asked to read an arti cle
emphasizing the environmental dimension, 351 participants (31 %) were asked to
read an article emphasizing national security, and 400 participants (36 %) were shown
amessageemphasizingthehealthdimension[SeeSupplementaryMaterialsformore
detail about the sample and the text of the articles].
2.1 Six Americas audience segmentation
Following the methodology presented in Maibach et al. (2011), participants were catego-
rized into one of six audience segments based on their existing climate change-related
attitudes, belief s, know ledge, preferences, and behaviors. These segments included the
Alarmed (180 participants, 16 %), Concerned (235, 21 %), Cautious (245, 22 %), Disen-
gaged (115, 10 %), Doubtful (191, 17 %), or Dismissive (161, 14 %).
2.2 Emotional reaction
In each condition, respondents were asked to indicate which parts of the framed message
made them feel hopeful and which parts of the message made them feel angry by clicking on
those sentences. The number of sentences that respondents clicked for each emotion was
summed to create a separate Hopeful and Angry measures.
2.3 Analysis
To test respondents emotional reactions to the environment, public health, and national security-
framed messages, interaction terms were created(usingdummycodes),multiplyingsegmentby
frame. (The reference category was rotated across each analysis to allow for comparison of all
groups.) Models predicting the emotional responses of hope and anger were fit using Poisson
regression (as is appropriate for count data). All models were weighted with apost-stratification
weight to match national demographic and audience segmentation characteristics.
3 Results
3.1 Predicting hope
First, using audience segment and message frame as predictors, a model was fit to predict the
number of hope-inducing sentences (complete model results shown in online supplemental
1108 Climatic Change (2012) 113:11051112
material , Table 1). Both were significant predictors, although message frame was only
marginally significant.
Controlling for audience segment, the health frame was the most likely to generate
feelings of hope, followed by the environment frame, and then the national security frame.
However, the frame that gave respondents the most hope differed by audience segment (see
Fig. 2). Of particular note, the health frame generated more hope than either the environment
or national security frames among the Cautious, Disengaged, and Dismissive segments (see
online supplemental material, Table 2). The Alarmed found high levels of hope in all the
messages with slightly more hope in the national security frame, while the Concerned found
the most hope in the environment message. Overall, consistent with their pre-ex isting
orientations, the audience segments on the left end of the spectrum responded more
hopefully than those on the right.
3.2 Predicting anger
Next, again using audience segment and message frame as predictors, a model was fit to predict
the number of anger-inducing sentences (complete model results shown in online supplemental
material, Table 1). Both audience segment and message frame were significant predictors.
Controlling for audience segment, the national security message was the most likely to
generate anger, followed by the environment message, while the health message generated
the least amount of anger. The frame that caused respondents to feel the most anger differed
by audience segment (see Fig. 3 and online supplementary materials, Table 3).] Overall, the
Dismissive experienced the most anger. However, they also had the most between-frame
variation in their anger responses: the national security frame elicited the most anger, and the
public health frame the least. The Doubtful also reacted most angrily to the national security
frame, and least angrily to the health frame. The Disengaged and the Cautious indicated
relatively limited amounts of anger, regardless of the message frame.
1
Among the
Concerned, the environment frame generated more anger than the national security frame,
and the health frame generated the least anger. The Alarmed were most likely to experience
anger in response to the national security frame.
4 Discussion
Our findings are consistent with past scholarship describing the influence of different
message frames in public responses to climate change (Nisbet 2009). Our results are also
consistent with past research that has found that a diversity of audience segments respond
positively to an emphasis on the public health consequences of climate change and the health
benefits of action (Maibach et al. 2010). Furthermore, although emphasizing the national
security implications of climate change has been assumed to be an effective strategy for
engaging conservatives and other groups (Boykoff 2011), our findings show that in this
1
The emotion of anger may lead to quite different actions, depending on the target and content of the anger. It
is likely that the anger experienced by audience segments convinced that climate change is occurring will lead
to action consistent with adaptation and mitigation goals. However, anger experienced by audience segments
unconvinced that climate change is occurring will likely lead to action counter to adaptation and mitigation
goals. (Analysis of the anger-producing sentences chosen by each segment supports this perspective. See the
sentence analysis in the online supplemental materials. Furthermore, analysis of the influence of anger on
support for reducing US emissions shows that the effects differed by segmentsee Fig. 2 in the online
supplemental materials).
Climatic Change (2012) 113:11051112 1109
instance (for reasons we speculate below) the national security frame actually generated
substantial anger among the Doubtful and Dismissive segments of the public, both of whom
lean conservative in their political outlook. Importantly, an emphasis on the environmental
risks of climate changehistorically the dominant frame in climate change communication
effortsneither inspired hope nor balanced out feelings of anger among the Disengaged,
Doubtful, and Dismissive segments of the public.
Across all audience segments, participants who read the public health message reacted
with at least some feelings of hope. Overall, 57 % of all subjects assigned to the health frame
responded hopefully to the following proposed action, suggesting that a localization of the issue
may also be an important element of effective communication (see supplemental materials
Table 4 Top Hopeful Statements by Frame and Across Audience Segment for details):
Redesigning our cities and towns to make it easier and safer to travel by foot, bicycle
and public transportation will reduce the number of cars on the road, reduce carbon
dioxide emissions, reduce traffic injuries and fatalities, and help people become more
physically active, lose weight, strengthen their bones, and possibly even to remain
mentally sharp as they age.
Fig. 3 Feelings of anger by frame condition across audience segment. Note: This figure depicts the estimated
marginal means of the sement by frame interaction as generated by the model
Fig. 2 Feelings of hope by frame condition across audience segment. Note: This figure depicts the estimated
marginal means of the sement by frame interaction as generated by the model
1110 Climatic Change (2012) 113:11051112
The results also suggest that the national security frame may generate an angry backlash,
especially among segments already Doubtful or Dismissive of climate change. Across the
three different frame conditions, the top two anger-producing sentences among the Doubtful
and Dismissive were both about national security (see supplemental materials Table 5 Top
Angry Statements by Frame and Across Audience Segment for details):
The most recent Quadrennial Defense Review - a national security report prepared
every four years by the Pentagon for the U.S. Congress - concludes that global
warming is a key issue likely to harm U.S. national security in many ways.
They also argue that efforts to limit global warming are a win-win because they will
reduce the risks of global warming and improve Americas national security.
Very few members of the other segments reacted angrily to these two statements. It is
possible that members of the Doubtful and Dismissive segments perceived the article to be
an attempt to make a link between an issue they may care deeply about (national security)
and an issue that they tend to dismiss (climate change), or they felt the article was attempting
to co-opt values they care strongly about, thereby producing a negative reaction. Alterna-
tively, it may be that participants were experiencing anger toward the experimenters for
having presented claims about global warming and national security that they as respondents
did not perceive as authenticorcredible.Furtherresearchneedstobeconductedto
understand the nature and extent of this potential boomerang effect.
Finally, outside of this experimental condition, in the context of real world communica-
tion efforts, these effects may differ due to a variety of factors. One particularly important
factor is the messenger, specifically the congruence between the messenger and the frame.
Other potentially important factors include whether the message is delivered by the media or
face-to-face, the availability of competing frames or arguments, the nature of any policy
actions proposed to address the problem, and the salience of other linked trendsparticu-
larly those involving the economy and/or political polarization more generally.
Our study, overall, underscores the importance of careful theoretical and empirical
investigation of how climate change communication efforts differentially shape Americans
views, understanding, and preferences on climate change. This research demonstrates the
potential of the public health frame to inspire hope in the context of the climate change
discussion, while simultaneously demonstrating the possibility that certain frames may be
poorly received within some interpretive communities. More research is needed to under-
stand how frames both set the context for perception on the issue and interact with emotions
in guiding public decisions and behavior
Acknowledgments This study was supported by a Robert Wood Johnson Foundation Health Policy Inves-
tigator Award and by the Grantham Foundation for the Protection of the Environment.
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License which
permits any use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and the source
are credited.
References
Bandura A (1977) Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psych Rev 34(2):191215
Baron RS, Logan H, Lilly J, Inman ML, Brennan M (1994) Negative emotion and message processing. J Exp
Soc Psych 30:181201. doi:10.1006/jesp.1994.1009
Baumeister RF, Vohs KD, DeWall CN, Zhang L (2007) How emotion shapes behavior: feedback, anticipation,
and reflection, rather than direct causation. PSPR 11:167203. doi:10.1177/1088868307301033
Climatic Change (2012) 113:11051112 1111
Boykoff J (2011 May 20) US military goes to war with climate skeptics. Guardian. http://www.guardian.
co.uk/commentisfree/ci famerica/2011/may/20/climate-change-cl imate-change-sc epticism. Accessed 29
August 2011
Gamson WA, Modigliani A (1989) Media discourse and public opinion on nuclear power: a constructionist
approach. Am J Soc 95:137
Goffman E (1974) Frame analysis: an essay on the organization of experience. Harper & Row, New York
Hart PS, Nisbet EC (2011) Boomerang effects in science communication: How motivated reasoning and
identity cues amplify opinion polarization about climate mitigation policies. Commun Res doi:10.1177/
0093650211416646
Kahneman D (2011) Thinking fast and slow. Farrar, Straus, and Giroux, New York
Leiserowitz A (2006) Climate change risk perception and policy preferences: the role of affect, imagery, and
values. Clim Chang 77:4572. doi:10.1007/s10584-006-9059-9
Leiserowitz A , Maibach E, Roser-Renouf C, Smith N (2011) Global WarmingsSixAmericas,May
2011. Yale University and George Mason University. New Haven, CT: Yale Project on Climate
Change Communication. http://environment.yale.edu/climate/files/SixAmericasMay2011.pdf
Accessed 25 October 2011
Lorenzoni I, Nicholson-Cole S, Whitmarsh L (2007) Barriers perceived to engaging with climate change
among the UK public and their policy implications. Glob Environ Chang 17:445459. doi:10.1016/
j.gloenvcha.2007.01.004
Maibach EW, Nisbet M, Baldwin P, Akerlof K, Diao G (2010) Reframing climate change as a public health
issue: an exploratory study of public reactions. BMC Pub Health 10:299. doi:10.1186/1471-2458-10-299
Maibach E, Leiserowitz A, Roser-Renouf C, Mertz CK (2011) Identifying like-minded audiences for climate
change public engagement campaigns: An audience segmentation analysis and tool development. PLoS
ONE 6(3):e17571. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0017571
Markowitz EM, Shariff AF (2012) Climate change and moral judgement. Nat Clim Chang. doi:10.1038/
NCLIMATE1378
Monahan JL (1995) Thinking positively: using positive affect when designing health messages. In: Maibach
E, Parrott RL (eds) Designing health messages: approaches from communication theory and public health
practice. Sage, London, pp 8198
Nabi RL (2003) Exploring the framing effects of emotion: do discrete emotions differentially influence
information accessibility, information seeking, and policy preference? Commun Res 30:224247.
doi:10.1177/0093650202250881
Nisbet MC (2009) Communicating climate change: why frames matter for public engagement. Environ
51:1223. doi:10.3200/ENVT.51.2.12-23
Nisbet MC, Scheufele DA (2009) Whats next for science communication? Promising directions and lingering
distractions. Am J Bot 96:17671778. doi: 10.3732/ajb.0900041
Norgaard KM (2011) Living in denial: Climate change, emotions, and everyday life. The MIT Press,
Cambridge
ONeill S, Nicholson-Cole S (2009) Fear wontdoit: promoting positive engagement with climate change
through visual and iconic representations. Sc Comm 30:355279. doi:10.1177/1075547008329201
Scheufele DA (1 999) Framing as a theory of media effects. J C omm 49( 1):103122. doi:10.1111/
j.1460-2466.1999.tb02784.x
Snyder CR (2002) Hope theory: rainbows in the mind. Psych Inq 13:249275
Swim J, Clayton S, Doherty T, Gifford R, Howard G, Reser J, Stern P, Weber E (2010) Psychology and global
climate change: Addressing a multi-faceted phenomenon and set of challenges. A report by the American
Psychological Associations Task For ce on the Inte rface be tween Psychology and Global Climate
Change. http://www.apa.org/science/about/publications/climate-change.aspx Accessed 25 August 2011
Thagard P, Findlay S (2011) Changing minds about climate c hange: Belief revision, coherence, a nd
emotion. In Olsson EJ, Enqvist S (eds.),
Belief revision meets philosophy of science.
doi
:10.1007/
978-90-481-9609-8
Thomas EF, McGarty C, Mavor KI (2009) Transforming apathy into movement: the role of prosocial
emotions in motivating change. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 13:31333. doi:10.1177/1088868309343290
1112 Climatic Change (2012) 113:11051112

Supplementary resource (1)

... Climate opinion and concern among the public has also proven difficult to move. Some studies show promising results (Myers et al. 2012;Goldberg et al. 2021), yet research on persuasive political communication shows that most efforts do not succeed (Kalla and Broockman 2018). Given these tradeoffs and uncertainties, environmental groups must evaluate the net effects of electoral interventions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Environmental organizations play an active role in electoral politics, yet these interventions have received far less study than the movement’s efforts at public persuasion or policy advocacy. We examine the effect of environmental voter mobilization on turnout and attitudes among supporters of a Canadian environmental organization. Through a field experiment during the 2017 British Columbia election, we evaluate two prevalent types of get-out-the-vote (GOTV) conversations – a regular GOTV conversation focused on vote plan-making, and an issue GOTV conversation that first engaged respondents in a personal discussion about environmentalism. For both GOTV interventions, we estimate a positive yet borderline significant effect on turnout. Neither GOTV intervention strengthened environmental attitudes, and the regular GOTV intervention may have even decreased en-vironmental issue salience. Our research illuminates the challenges that climate advocates face in mobilizing their constituents, while demonstrating their potential for influence on the electorate.
... For example, framing climate change as an international problem can lead people to feel powerless, resulting in lower civic participation and engagement 9 . By contrast, highlighting the health impacts of climate change has been shown to increase public engagement 18 and support for climate change mitigation and adaptation initiatives 19 . In a similar vein, media coverage of climate change has been found to influence human behavior, such as mobility choices 20 or investment decisions 21,22 . ...
Article
Full-text available
The implementation and acceptance of climate policies depend on public perceptions of climate change. The media play a crucial role in informing the public discourse. While previous research has predominantly focused on written news, television remains the primary source of information globally. Here, we present an algorithm based on natural language processing techniques for identifying climate change coverage from subtitles of the leading German television news program, Tagesschau. Combining a dictionary approach with neural topic modeling, we classify the topics of over 28,000 news items (2015–2023). Our results show that climate change accounts for 4% of the total coverage, surpassed, for example, by sports coverage (9%). Acute crises, such as COVID-19, are covered more frequently and positioned more prominently. 80% of climate change coverage reports on climate policy, while only 10% covers climate impacts, like weather extremes. The latter tend to be covered in later news slots, indicating lower news value.
... We selected two environmental appeals for this study. While we acknowledge that health-related appeals tend to be more familiar and personally relevant to most consumers (Myers et al., 2012), campaigns by environmental NGOs typically employ environmental appeals to promote more sustainable meat consumption. Specifically, we focused on the two environmental appeals most frequently used by such organizations: environmental degradation and animal welfare (Laestadius et al., 2016). ...
... 16). Hope is a positive emotion associated with global warming 95,96 . For instance, several studies have found that feeling hopeful about global warming is a key predictor of people's support for and engagement in climate-related activism and social movements [97][98][99][100][101][102][103] . ...
Article
Full-text available
Some climate activists use nonviolent civil disobedience (NVCD) to protest the slow pace of climate policy action. Civil disobedience theorists posit that building a critical mass of support for and participation in NVCD increases the likelihood of policy success. Here we investigate predictors of public support for and personal willingness to engage in NVCD using data from a nationally representative survey of U.S. adults ( n = 1303). Linear regression analysis revealed the following significant predictors of public willingness to support and engage in NVCD: collective efficacy; anger; perceived risk; identification with the Democratic Party; identification with climate activists; and exposure to liberal news media. Similarly, all these variables were significant in the relative weight analysis. These findings provide theoretical and practical insights into the role of NVCD in the climate movement.
... In countries such as China and India, mitigating local air pollution has been one of the strongest motivations to shift away from fossil-fuel use 6,7 . In other countries, the potential co-benefits of improved air quality and reduced health impacts have helped to increase public support for climate and clean-energy policies [8][9][10] . ...
Article
The transition to a low-carbon energy system could simultaneously mitigate carbon emissions, reduce air pollution and protect human health. Although these potential benefits have been widely acknowledged, major questions remain regarding key factors that determine the impacts of energy strategies on health and health disparities, as well as ways to incorporate health benefits into real-world energy decisions. In this Perspective, we summarize the current understanding and analytical framework related to the pollution and health impacts of the energy transition, discuss the analytical challenges and knowledge gaps in assessing and projecting the magnitude and distribution of the pollution and health impacts, and identify plausible entry points to enhance the real-world relevance of health benefits in decision-making about energy systems. The energy transition will affect pollution and health impacts at the global, national and subnational scales, resulting in complex distributional effects across regions and population groups. Although current analytical frameworks are useful to quantify the general patterns of health benefits, they are often insufficient for characterizing distributional effects, quantifying potential trade-offs, and incorporating considerations of deep and interacting uncertainties. Given the complexity of the actors involved and the policymaking landscape, it will be necessary to make knowledge actionable by, for example, establishing a co-production process between researchers and practitioners. Sections
Article
There are still problems in the communication of climate change issues. Based on the extended parallel process model (EPPM), this article explores the role of news texts containing emotional appeals in motivating the public's climate action intention. Three web‐based, between‐subjects experimental studies were conducted. The three studies collected 429, 762, and 1669 valid questionnaires respectively. Results based on moderation and mediation analyses indicated that perceived threat and perceived efficacy positively influenced climate action intention, and that fear and hope emotions mediated the relationship between perceived threat/perceived efficacy and climate action intention. In addition, guilt and encouragement moderated the effect of perceived threat/perceived efficacy on climate action intention. This article provides insights for media information dissemination in achieving the carbon peaking and carbon neutrality goals, as well as provides effective suggestions for the improvement of information content when relevant organizations mobilize the public to participate in low‐carbon environmental protection actions.
Thesis
Full-text available
Background: Research into environmental pollution and health impairment caused by man-made influences is particularly advanced in the areas of air, water and noise pollution. The topic of light pollution however is still given very little consideration both by the general public and in (communication)research, which this master's thesis is intended to change as one of the first studies in this subject area. It examines the extent to which an information-based intervention on the topic of light pollution with different frames (environmental frame, health frame and combination frame) can improve attitudes towards measures to reduce light pollution among adolescents and young adults of Generation Z in Germany. Methods: A single-factorial online experiment with four conditions was used to investigate whether interventions with differently framed texts on the topic of light pollution have a positive effect on attitudes towards reduction measures. Factors such as risk perception and objective and subjective previous knowledge were also recorded. Results: The study reveals that neither the three intervention conditions nor risk perception had a positive influence on attitudes towards reduction measures. However, the factors of prior knowledge and gender proved to be important indicators. Respondents with prior knowledge and women rate the measures on average more positively than respondents without prior knowledge or men. Implication: It was shown that a one-off information-based framing intervention cannot positively influence attitudes towards light pollution reduction measures. It can be concluded that the significant factors of prior knowledge and gender are rather indicators of this attitude and that the focus of communication strategies and intervention development should be on long-term knowledge building and group specifics.
Article
Science communication is a key skill for undergraduates, but little research explores how biology students communicate about societally important, yet controversial topics like climate change. In this study, we explored whether and how biology students took on the role of science communicators about climate change. We surveyed 191 biology students at 38 universities about their climate change communication frequency and preparedness. We interviewed 25 of the survey participants about their experiences communicating about climate change and their needs when learning about climate change communication. We found that students were communicating about climate change and felt confident discussing the causes and effects of climate change, but they were less confident discussing the solutions to climate change. Students tended to “preach to the choir” by mostly communicating with those who already accepted climate change and avoiding interacting with others who disagreed with them about climate change. Students described a lack of science communication training but had a desire to be taught effective communication skills. Our interviews indicate that if these students felt more prepared to communicate, it may make them more willing to discuss climate change and particularly with people who have different views from them.
Article
Full-text available
Presents an integrative theoretical framework to explain and to predict psychological changes achieved by different modes of treatment. This theory states that psychological procedures, whatever their form, alter the level and strength of self-efficacy. It is hypothesized that expectations of personal efficacy determine whether coping behavior will be initiated, how much effort will be expended, and how long it will be sustained in the face of obstacles and aversive experiences. Persistence in activities that are subjectively threatening but in fact relatively safe produces, through experiences of mastery, further enhancement of self-efficacy and corresponding reductions in defensive behavior. In the proposed model, expectations of personal efficacy are derived from 4 principal sources of information: performance accomplishments, vicarious experience, verbal persuasion, and physiological states. Factors influencing the cognitive processing of efficacy information arise from enactive, vicarious, exhortative, and emotive sources. The differential power of diverse therapeutic procedures is analyzed in terms of the postulated cognitive mechanism of operation. Findings are reported from microanalyses of enactive, vicarious, and emotive modes of treatment that support the hypothesized relationship between perceived self-efficacy and behavioral changes. (21/2 p ref)
Technical Report
Full-text available
This report extends and updates an ongoing program of research analyzing Americans’ interpretations of and responses to climate change. The research segments the American public into six audiences that range along a spectrum of concern and issue engagement from the Alarmed, who are convinced of the reality and danger of climate change, and who are highly supportive of personal and political actions to mitigate the threat, to the Dismissive, who are equally convinced that climate change is not occurring and that no response should be made. The Six Americas are not very different demographically, but are dramatically different in their beliefs and actions, as well as their basic values and political orientations. The groups were first identified in a nationally representative survey conducted in the fall of 2008, and were re-assessed in January and June of 2010. The current report is the fourth in the series; in it we provide new insights into the informational needs of the six groups, their understanding of the health impacts of global warming, beliefs about current environmental impacts of global warming in the U.S., and support for local adaptation and mitigation policies.
Article
Full-text available
The deficit-model of science communication assumes increased communication about science issues will move public opinion toward the scientific consensus. However, in the case of climate change, public polarization about the issue has increased in recent years, not diminished. In this study, we draw from theories of motivated reasoning, social identity, and persuasion to examine how science-based messages may increase public polarization on controversial science issues such as climate change. Exposing 240 adults to simulated news stories about possible climate change health impacts on different groups, we found the influence of identification with potential victims was contingent on participants’ political partisanship. This partisanship increased the degree of political polarization on support for climate mitigation policies and resulted in a boomerang effect among Republican participants. Implications for understanding the role of motivated reasoning within the context of science communication are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Converging evidence from the behavioural and brain sciences suggests that the human moral judgement system is not well equipped to identify climate change — a complex, large-scale and unintentionally caused phenomenon — as an important moral imperative. As climate change fails to generate strong moral intuitions, it does not motivate an urgent need for action in the way that other moral imperatives do. We review six reasons why climate change poses significant challenges to our moral judgement system and describe six strategies that communicators might use to confront these challenges. Enhancing moral intuitions about climate change may motivate greater support for ameliorative actions and policies.
Article
An analysis of why people with knowledge about climate change often fail to translate that knowledge into action. Global warming is the most significant environmental issue of our time, yet public response in Western nations has been meager. Why have so few taken any action? In Living in Denial, sociologist Kari Norgaard searches for answers to this question, drawing on interviews and ethnographic data from her study of "Bygdaby," the fictional name of an actual rural community in western Norway, during the unusually warm winter of 2000-2001. In 2000-2001 the first snowfall came to Bygdaby two months later than usual; ice fishing was impossible; and the ski industry had to invest substantially in artificial snow-making. Stories in local and national newspapers linked the warm winter explicitly to global warming. Yet residents did not write letters to the editor, pressure politicians, or cut down on use of fossil fuels. Norgaard attributes this lack of response to the phenomenon of socially organized denial, by which information about climate science is known in the abstract but disconnected from political, social, and private life, and sees this as emblematic of how citizens of industrialized countries are responding to global warming. Norgaard finds that for the highly educated and politically savvy residents of Bygdaby, global warming was both common knowledge and unimaginable. Norgaard traces this denial through multiple levels, from emotions to cultural norms to political economy. Her report from Bygdaby, supplemented by comparisons throughout the book to the United States, tells a larger story behind our paralysis in the face of today's alarming predictions from climate scientists.
Book
Editors' Introduction.- Raul Carnota and Ricardo Rodriguez: "AGM Theory and Artificial Intelligence".- Sven Ove Hansson: "Changing the Scientific Corpus".- Hans Rott: "Idealization, Intertheory Explanations and Conditionals".- Gerhard Schurz: "Abductive Belief Revision in Science".- Sebastian Enqvist: "A Structuralist Framework for the Logic of Theory Change ".- Peter Gardenfors and Frank Zenker: "Using Conceptual Spaces to Model the Dynamics.- of Empirical Theories".- Bengt Hansson: "A Note on Theory Change and Belief Revision".- Horacio Arlo-Costa and Arthur Paul Pedersen: "Social Norms, Rational Choice and Belief Change".- David Westlund: "Rational Belief Changes for Collective Agents".- Emmanuel Genot: "The Best of All Possible Worlds: Where Interrogative Games Meet.- Research Agendas".- Isaac Levi: "Knowledge as True Belief".- Caroline Semmling and Heinrich Wansing: "Reasoning about Belief Revision".- Paul Thagard: "Changing Minds about Climate Change: Belief Revision, Coherence, and.- Emotion".- Jonas Nilsson and Sten Lindstrom: "Rationality in Flux: Formal Representations of.- Methodological Change".
Article
In this essay, we review research from the social sciences on how the public makes sense of and participates in societal decisions about science and technology. We specifically highlight the role of the media and public communication in this process, challenging the still dominant assumption that science literacy is both the problem and the solution to societal conflicts. After reviewing the cases of evolution, climate change, food biotechnology, and nanotechnology, we offer a set of detailed recommendations for improved public engagement efforts on the part of scientists and their organizations. We emphasize the need for science communication initiatives that are guided by careful formative research; that span a diversity of media platforms and audiences; and that facilitate conversations with the public that recognize, respect, and incorporate differences in knowledge, values, perspectives, and goals.
Article
The persuasive effects of emotions have been the focus of burgeoning interest in recent years. Rather than considering how emotions function within traditional paradigms of attitude change, this research explores the possibility that emotions serve as frames for issues, privileging certain information in terms of accessibility and thus guiding subsequent decision making. This study's results offer evidence that fear and anger can differentially affect information accessibility, desired information seeking, and policy preference, though these effects may be contingent on schema development. These findings support not only the relationship between emotions and frames but also the importance of the discrete emotion perspective in persuasive contexts.