ArticlePDF Available

Atheists are rejecting today's culturally evolved religions, not a “first” natural religion

Authors:
Francis, L. (1984). Dimensions of Christian belief. Educational Studies,10, 103111.
Park, C.L. (in press). Attending to the construct of beliefs in research on religion/spirituality and health:
Commentary on Beyond Belief.Journal of Health Psychology.
Saroglou, V. (2010). Religiousness as a Cultural adaptation of basic traits: A ve-factor model perspective.
Personality and Social Psychology Review,14, 108 125.
Sherkat, D.E. (2008). Beyond belief: Atheism, agnosticism, and theistic certainty in the United States.
Sociological Spectrum,28, 438459.
Atheists are rejecting todays culturally evolved religions, not a ‘‘first’’
natural religion
John R. Shook*
Science and the Public EdM program, University at Buffalo, New York, USA, and Center for
Neurotechnology Studies, Potomac Institute for Policy Studies, Arlington, Virginia, USA
Caldwell-Harris proposes that specific personality traits and thinking styles, such as
cognitive flexibility and individualism, will understandably correlate with dissent from
a dominant cultural ideology like a religion. This sociological basis for explaining the
traits of atheists is more convincing than simplistic biological theories about religions
origins. It cannot be stressed enough that ignoring sociocultural context can only
distort scientific research into religion. Brains do not confront reality directly, and they
do not generate identities or narratives by themselves. Any sophisticated belief system
that exists today is more the result of cultural evolution than natural selection, and
there was no ‘‘first form of religion’’ from which all other religions descended.
The quest for the natural religion or the natural belief in God is as hopeless as
searching for the natural government or the natural art form. Neither religion or
irreligion is more ‘‘natural’’ for humanity as a whole, and anyone can be a non-
believer. Todays cultures have a vast supply of ideas about unnatural things, from
tooth fairies to demons, so an overactive agency detector cannot be observed in its
pure natural state. If a hypothetical child were raised in isolation from all notions of
anything unnatural and from teleological explanations for natural affairs, what are
the odds that any spontaneous suggestions about unseen agents would bear any
resemblance to Yahweh or even a sprite?
We must be skeptical towards theories proposing that religious beliefs were
originally conceived in pretty much the same way across humanity, or that there could
be much in common between some ‘‘original’’ religions and any religion on record.
Religious partisans are nevertheless rhetorically draping themselves in the ‘‘naturality’’
of religion; yet, curiously, they are not promoting early more natural? religious
practices, which (mostly Christian) academics label animism, ancestor worship,
totemism, shamanism, polytheism, and so on. Listening to some proponents of
religion, youd think that the ‘‘original’’ and most natural religion was about Trinitarian
supernaturalism, monotheistic deism, or at least spiritualistic pantheism (although
none of these conceptions are older than the chariot). Naturally, religions must evolve
or perish, and religions that were useful long ago could not be so useful today. Yet the
*Email: pragmatism.org
38 J.R. Shook
Downloaded by [John Shook] at 09:01 04 May 2012
rampant cultural relativism accurately predicted by a pragmatic theory of religion
clashes with a search for religious beliefs ‘‘natural’’ to all humanity.
The origins of religious ideas from long ago must be disconnected from inquiries
into the modern appeal of religion. Many religions traditionally connected them
because their current plausibility rested on their allegedly divine origins. Many
secularists have connected them because discovering a naturalistic origin for religion
could make atheism more plausible now. All the same, relying on some connection
between origins and plausibility will let both sides down. What was plausible long
ago lacks the same appeal today, as William James remarked concerning religions
moral progress (James, 1902); and what people of today find meaningful may not
leave them content with mere atheism (Comte-Sponville, 2008).
As for observed correlations between personality types and degrees of religiosity,
perhaps we live in unusual times. In societies dominated by one religion, to the degree
that nearly all publicly conform, most personality types would find a religious role.
Emotional piety can sit in the pews with stoic simplicity and intellectual curiosity,
and a sophisticated religion can co-opt even the most intelligent and skeptical,
guiding them into the roles of teachers, scholars, leaders, and reformers. Religions
have benefitted from the mutations of freethinking and heresy as much as any
cultural structure. In the west, perhaps only science has displayed as much change as
Christianity over the past 500 years. For many reasons, Christianity is now in a
highly fractured condition. Its capacity for appealing to most personality types is
technically still on display, so long as ‘‘Christianity’’ is left vaguely defined.
Our current situation is not one of unitary religious dominance; religious
pluralism and denominational variety, along with religious mobility, social freedom,
and permissible free thought provide a noticeably different climate from just 50 years
ago. Denominations themselves can be more flexible by subtly or not so subtly
shifting their public stances to better ‘‘compete’’ in the wide-open marketplace of
ideas and identities. This mutual self-definition implies further factors about peoples
religiosity today: (1) people are more self-selective; (2) they are more self-reflective;
(3) they develop more of a religious self-narrative; and (4) these self-narratives can be
partially borrowed from their religious group. Because people are not only self-
selecting their public identities, but also selecting and self-identifying with the
narratives that justify those identities, they can respond to questioning using those
justifications.
Atheism, for its part, is not a denomination, but it can play the role of an
ideological camp in a religiously contested social arena, where the same sociological
factors apply. Many non-believers will be atheist ‘‘self-identifiers,’’ or even admit
doubting God to themselves (as ‘‘self-admitters’’), only if the public narrative about
atheism sounds acceptable. During Enlightenment times, atheism justified itself by
claiming to be more rational and anti-authoritarian. And as Caldwell-Harris argues,
so long as the public image of ‘‘the atheist’’ remains a caricature of a rationalistic
nonconformist, statistical portraits of the self-identifying atheist will lean towards
that profile. Furthermore, non-believers uninterested in identifying as an atheist can
still rely on atheisms group narrative of intellectual independence to frame their
justifications. Non-believers need not fit that cultural profile, but that profile is
supplied to fit for them. Hypothetically, if the public image of the non-believer had
long been a civic-minded altruistic humanist (a fond wish of humanist organiza-
tions), a different set of personality types would be now stepping forward. Stepping
back to make Caldwell-Harriss larger point again, if non-belief were not a matter of
Religion, Brain & Behavior 39
Downloaded by [John Shook] at 09:01 04 May 2012
contested ideology and public dissent, the observed effects would largely subside.
What personality types are correlated with disbelief in Santa Claus? Correlations
found among 4-year-olds would not be mirrored among adults.
Until research into individual identity choices becomes even more contextually
nuanced, raw correlations between personality types and announced degrees of
religious belief will mostly reflect ongoing public relations struggles and ideological
propaganda.
References
Comte-Sponville, A. (2008). The little book of atheist spirituality. New York: Penguin.
James, W. (1902). The varieties of religious experience: A study in human nature. New York: Longmans,
Green & Co.
RESPONSE
What theoretical frameworks do scholars need? What type of society do
we all want?
Catherine Caldwell-Harris*
Department of Psychology, Boston University, USA
My target article had the goal of normalizing atheism or non-belief, characterizing it
as emerging out of expected and even adaptive aspects of human nature, in contrast
to the historical view of atheism as deviant or, at least, unnatural. Given respondents
expertise in psychology, personality, and religion, they found this uncontroversial.
Respondents discussed ideas for future research and how to characterize non-belief.
They argued for different theoretical frameworks and some criticized the view that
religious belief is a natural, or automatic, consequence of human cognition. On the
more controversial side, Johnson broached the topic of whether religious institutions
or beliefs are better than secular ones for maximizing individual and societal well-
being. Ill review and expand on these topics in order of least to most controversial.
More nuanced views of non-believers open up new directions for research
My goal was to find generalizations relevant to the broad category of non-believers,
but Hood wonders if my conclusions about atheistspossibly low sociality are
primarily relevant to the subset of non-believers who avoid religious institutions. In
the Ecklund and Lee (2011) study cited by Hood, a small percentage (17%) of atheist
scientists reported attending a religious service at least twice in the prior year. Those
scientists reported doing so to please a spouse or partner, or out of desire for
community, or so their children could learn about religion to make their own choices.
Those who care about sharing an activity with their spouse or partner may be more
social than those non-believers who always avoided religious services. This is a
*Email: charris@bu.edu
40 C. Caldwell-Harris
Downloaded by [John Shook] at 09:01 04 May 2012
... non-belief, Humanism, horizontal transcendence, atheism, types of Buddhism and many more) when researchers assert that one group bears a greater claim to these things than another group (typically a minority group). Taking into context such arguments, as foci of the social scientific study of religion and non-religion seems much more of a debate about names, terminologies and the right for one group to possess them vs. another (Shook 2012). Thus the timing seems right (i.e. research into religiosity and religion is increasing as well as the acceleration of research into atheism) to introduce and advance a humanistic approach that remains methodologically agnostic and allows for the comparison of "like things" (i.e. both believers and non-believers have things of ultimate value and exceptionally profound experiences present in their lives). ...
... Barrett 2010; Barrett and Lanman 2008;Bering 2010Bering , 2012). Yet others, seeking to slow down such premature theoretical cognitivist chauvinism (McCutcheon 2010;Caldwell-Harris 2012;Shook 2012;Johnson 2012) find such assertions to be hasty to say the least. ...
Article
Full-text available
Much of the reigning research on non-religion and non-belief focuses on demographics and personality characteristics. While this is a necessary foundation on which future research may be built upon, such data does not necessarily produce theory. In many ways the dominant cultural milieu of religions along with the benign intent of some researchers force a person who holds no belief in a God to assume an oppositional identity in relation to religion. This oppositional identity tautologically sets researchers up to continually define its object by the absence of something. This something cannot always function as a normative point of reference in which to tell researchers what to look for. This article provides one such normative trajectory, termed “horizontal transcendence.”
... However, the issue is somewhat clouded in that there is a sense in which every one is an atheist insofar as they deny a particular view of theism. Furthermore, under the umbrella of secularity are those who are simply nonbelievers, not even engaged enough with religious beliefs to deny them (Shook, 2012). One seldom hears of astronomers spending much thought on denying astrological claims, nor do modern chemists seek to refute alchemical claims. ...
Book
Atheology is the intellectual effort to understand atheism, defend the reasonableness of unbelief, and support nonbelievers in their encounters with religion. This book presents a historical overview of the development of atheology from ancient thought to the present day. It offers in-depth examinations of four distinctive schools of atheological thought: rationalist atheology, scientific atheology, moral atheology, and civic atheology. John R. Shook shows how a familiarity with atheology's complex histories, forms, and strategies illuminates the contentious features of today's atheist and secularist movements, which are just as capable of contesting each other as opposing religion. The result is a book that provides a disciplined and philosophically rigorous examination of atheism's intellectual strategies for reasoning with theology. Systematic Atheology is an important contribution to the philosophy of religion, religious studies, secular studies, and the sociology and psychology of nonreligion.
Article
The Varieties of Religious Experience : a Study in Human Nature / William James Note: The University of Adelaide Library eBooks @ Adelaide.