ArticlePDF Available

Assessing Children's Environmental Worldviews: Modifying and Validating the New Ecological Paradigm Scale for Use With Children

Authors:

Abstract

The authors revised and validated the New Ecological Paradigm (NEP) Scale for use with upper elementary students. Researchers use the NEP Scale extensively with adults, but it was not designed for children. Interviews with 5th grade students helped the authors revise the NEP Scale for use with children. The authors spent 2 years validating the modified instrument with larger numbers of students. After analyzing their results, the authors suggest that a 3-dimensional modified NEP Scale for Children, with 10 instead of 15 items and revised wording, is appropriate for use with children aged 10-12 years.
SUMMER 2007, VOL. 38, NO. 4 3
REPORTS & RESEARCH
Assessing Children’s Environmental
Worldviews: Modifying and
Validating the New Ecological
Paradigm Scale for Use With
Children
Constantinos C. Manoli, Bruce Johnson, and Riley E. Dunlap
ABSTRACT: The authors revised and validated the New Ecological Paradigm (NEP) Scale for use
with upper elementary students. Researchers use the NEP Scale extensively with adults, but it was
not designed for children. Interviews with 5th grade students helped the authors revise the NEP
Scale for use with children. The authors spent 2 years validating the modified instrument with
larger numbers of students. After analyzing their results, the authors suggest that a 3-dimensional
modified NEP Scale for Children, with 10 instead of 15 items and revised wording, is appropriate
for use with children aged 10–12 years.
KEYWORDS: earth education, environmental worldviews, New Ecological Paradigm Scale
rom the late 1960s onward, the growing recognition of the seriousness of environmental
problems led to hundreds of studies of environmental attitudes and beliefs. Yet, because
many of the studies contributing to this literature had not undergone a rigorous develop
-
ment process from a psychometric point of view, only a modest amount of knowledge accumulated
(Dunlap & Jones, 2002; Gray, 1985). Rather than conducting replications with well-established
measures, researchers in the majority of studies used instruments developed for a specific project,
Constintinos C. Manoli is a postdoctoral research coordinator for the Earth Education Research and
Evaluation Team in the Department of Teaching and Teacher Education at The University of Arizona
in Tucson. Bruce Johnson is an associate professor and department head of Teaching and Teacher
Education at The University of Arizona and director of the Earth Education Research and Evaluation
Team. He is also international program coordinator for The Institute for Earth Education. Riley E.
Dunlap is Regents Professor of Sociology at Oklahoma State University, in Stillwater. Copyright ©
2007 Heldref Publications
F
4 THE JOURNAL OF ENVIRONMENTAL EDUCATION
providing little evidence of their validity and reliability (Gray; Leeming, Dwyer, & Bracken, 1995).
In a major review, Leeming, Dwyer, Porter, and Cobern (1993) examined 34 studies that assessed
changes in environmental attitudes, knowledge, and behavior. They reported finding scales with
limited use beyond the original study and little evidence regarding validity and reliability.
These problems are especially apparent in studying childrens environmental orientations because
few studies have used well-developed instruments. Of all the instruments reviewed by Gray (1985)
and by Dunlap and Jones (2002), not one was designed for children. Leeming et al. (1993) reviewed
several relevant studies but found only one, Armstrong and Impara (1991), that they felt was of high
quality. Leeming et al. (1995) encouraged more research on environmental attitudes and behavior of
children because “early attitudes and knowledge shape the later thinking of adolescents and adults
(p. 23), and empirical research in the past had underrepresented this population.
In our study, we present an adaptation of the New Ecological Paradigm (NEP) Scale for use
with upper elementary students. As an adaptation of the NEP, the most widely used instrument for
studying environmental orientation among adults, our measuring instrument should prove useful for
studying childrens environmental orientations.
Theoretical Perspective: From the Dominant Social Paradigm (DSP) to the NEP
Numerous early analysts of environmental problems argued that Americans commitment to
abundance, progress, prosperity, laissez faire, individualism, and property rights have contributed
to environmental problems and posed barriers to the effective solution of such problems (Caldwell,
1970; Campbell & Wade, 1972; Dunlap & Van Liere, 1978; Whisenhunt, 1974). Three decades
ago, Pirages and Ehrlich (1974) pointed out that a new worldview was beginning to challenge the
Dominant Social Paradigm (DSP) consisting of the traditional values, attitudes, and beliefs noted
above. This new social paradigm or worldview, called the New Environmental Paradigm (NEP) by
Dunlap and Van Liere (1978) and more recently the New Ecological Paradigm by Dunlap, Van
Liere, Mertig, & Jones (2000), challenges the DSP by rejecting the anthropocentric notion that
nature exists solely to serve human needs (Barbour, 1973; Commoner, 1971; Daly, 1973; Meadows,
Meadows, Randers, & Behrens, 1972). In addition to rejecting the idea that nature has no value
beyond human use, the NEP emphasizes that modern industrial societies are exceeding ecological
limits and disrupting ecosystems (Dunlap & Van Liere).
The new worldview has evolved from early concerns about specific environmental problems
and natural resources to recognizing that humans may be fundamentally altering the functioning
of the global ecosystem, with the result that unpredictable and irreversible changes may occur
(Dunlap et al. 2000). What many once viewed as discrete, local problems have evolved into global
problems with complex, synergistic causes and unpredictable, possibly irreversible effects. As a
result, researchers have made an observable turn toward studying public perceptions and aware-
ness of global ecological problems and the human–environment relationships that produce them
(Dunlap, 1998; Stern, 1992).
Recognition of the importance of examining the relationship between modern societies and the
environments that produced these problems led to the construction of the 12-item NEP Scale as
a means of measuring public acceptance of the emerging paradigm (Dunlap & Van Liere, 1978).
Since then, the NEP Scale has become the most widely used measure of environmental concern
(Stern, Dietz, & Guagnano, 1995), used in scores of studies, including a growing number outside
the United States (Bechtel, Verdugo, & Pinheiro, 1999; Schultz & Zelezny, 1999). Although Dunlap
and Van Liere found that the NEP Scale measured a single dimension, others have found that it
measures two, three, or even four dimensions (Bechtel et al.; Edgell & Nowell, 1989; Furman, 1998;
SUMMER 2007, VOL. 38, NO. 4 5
Gooch, 1995; Noe & Hammitt, 1992; Noe & Snow, 1989, 1990; Roberts & Bacon, 1997; Scott &
Willits, 1994; Shetzer, Stackman, & Moore, 1991).
Researchers recently revised and updated the NEP Scale, renaming it the New Ecological
Paradigm Scale (Dunlap et al., 2000). The revised version contains 15 items, three each designed to
tap five key facets of the NEP: (a) limits to growth, (b) anti-anthropocentrism, (c) fragility of natures
balance, (d) rejection of human exemptionalism, and (e) belief in eco-crisis. These facets did not
form distinct dimensions in the original study because Dunlap et al. found one primary dimension
reflecting endorsement of an ecological worldview. The authors suggest, however, that the number
of dimensions may vary for different populations and that “in many cases it will no doubt be more
appropriate to treat the NEP as multidimensional” (Dunlap et al., p. 436).
Purpose of Our Study
Both the original NEP Scale (Dunlap & Van Liere, 1978) and the revised scale (Dunlap et al.,
2000) were designed for use with adults, but many environmental learning programs are designed
for children. Assessing environmental worldviews during childhood is of great interest to both those
offering such programs and researchers investigating the development of these worldviews.
Recently, researchers have developed new instruments designed to measure childrens environ-
mental worldviews, most notably the Childrens Attitudes Toward the Environment Scale (CATES;
Musser & Malkus, 1994) and the Childrens Environmental Attitude and Knowledge Scale
(CHEAKS; Leeming et al., 1995). CATES has not been widely used and employs an awkward
bipolar answer format, whereas CHEAKS assesses attitudes toward environmental issues through
both verbal commitment and self-reported actual behaviors. These instruments measure different
dimensions than NEP Scale.
We designed our study to determine if the revised NEP Scale (Dunlap et al., 2000) was suitable
for use with children ages 10–12 years and, if so, what modifications were necessary for use with this
age group. Our study also investigates whether the belief systems of children, as measured by the
NEP, is unidimensional or multidimensional. If the NEP Scale were applicable to children, research-
ers could (a) investigate childrens environmental worldviews and compare childrens worldviews
with those of adults, using a comparable measure and (b) study how worldviews change as children
become adults.
Method
Revision: Year 1
During the revision phase, from February to April 2003, we examined childrens comprehension
of the NEP Scale through interviews with 30 fifth-grade students (17 girls, 13 boys) from a school
in Pennsylvania, who were participants in Earthkeepers, an earth education program (Van Matre &
Johnson, 1988). Following our first interview with 13 students, which focused on the vocabulary in
the NEP Scale, we revised most of the items to make them more child friendly without changing
their meaning. Next, we used the NEP Scale with the revised items in interviews with 17 other fifth
graders, checking again for understanding of vocabulary but adding a focus on comprehension of
the meaning of the items. On the basis of those interviews, we further revised a few scale items and
tentatively constructed the NEP Scale for Children.
Finally, 54 fifth-grade students (32 girls, 22 boys) from two schools in Arizona and Pennsylvania
completed the revised scale as a pilot test. The students from Arizona were primarily Caucasian and
Hispanic whereas the students from Pennsylvania were mainly Causcasian. The tentative NEP Scale
6 THE JOURNAL OF ENVIRONMENTAL EDUCATION
for Children contained 15 items and the same 5-point Likert-type scoring system used in the adult
NEP Scale, with responses ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). We reversed the
scoring for negatively worded items: 1 (strongly agree) to 5 (strongly disagree). The total scale score
ranged from a possible low of 15 (endorsement of the DSP) to a high of 75 (endorsement of the
NEP). The midpoint score of 45 can be arbitrarily interpreted as neutrality with regard to the two
competing worldviews.
Validation: Year 2
The participants in the first year of the validation phase of the study (September 2003 to
April 2004) were 672 fourth-, fifth-, and sixth-grade students from 23 schools in Louisiana and
Pennsylvania (52% girls, 48% boys). Students from Louisiana were mostly African American and
Caucasian, and students from Pennsylvania were mostly Caucasian. The students had participated
in either Earthkeepers (Van Matre & Johnson, 1988) or Sunship Earth (Van Matre, 1979), two
earth education programs that were part of their schools curriculum. The schools served students
of low- to middle-income levels. Both programs focus on encouraging participants to develop an
understanding of ecological concepts, build positive feelings for the natural world, and change their
environmental behaviors. Because the authors of the programs designed them to affect students
worldviews and feelings about the earth, they are appropriate for the purpose of this study. The stu-
dents completed the NEP Scale for Children as a pretest 1 week before they began the earth educa-
tion programs and as a posttest approximately 1 month after completing the programs.
We made a small change in the scale from Year 1, adding a Do not understand response to the
Likert-type scale to enable us to further detect problematic items. At the end of Year 2, we randomly
split the data in two, using one half for exploratory factor analysis (EFA; principal-components
analysis with varimax rotation) and the other half for confirmatory factor analysis (CFA; Pedhazur
& Schmelkin, 1991; Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996).
Validation: Year 3
In Year 3 (September 2004 to April 2005), on the basis of the second-year students’ responses, we
slightly revised the NEP Scale for Children one last time. We eliminated items that received a high
rate of the response Do not understand in Year 2. The 515 fourth-, fifth-, and sixth-grade students
(53% girls, 47% boys) involved in the third-year data collection came from the same schools in
Louisiana, Pennsylvania, and Arizona and had participated in either Earthkeepers or Sunship Earth.
Using the entire sample, we ran a second CFA to test how well Year 3 data fit the Year 2 model. We
also performed a paired sample t-test analysis with the 186 students who participated in the Sunship
Earth program
1
to validate the scale for its sensitivity to detecting change in childrens environmental
worldviews as a result of program participation.
Results
Revision: Year 1
During the first set of interviews, we identified 36 words as problematic for most of the students.
We replaced those words with easier and more familiar synonyms or short phrases in 13 of the 15
items. During the second set of interviews, only five additional words appeared to be problematic,
and 3 of the 17 students had comprehension problems with two items. After a researcher read those
items aloud, the students seemed to understand them, suggesting that the problem was in reading
rather than understanding. We further revised those two items before including them in the tentative
NEP Scale for Children.
SUMMER 2007, VOL. 38, NO. 4 7
Validation: Year 2
EFA. Using a random half (n = 336) of the Year 2 sample, an unconstrained EFA initially revealed
five factors with Eigenvalues greater than 1.0, explaining 53.68% of the variance. However, several
items loaded almost equally on two different factors. We ran further analyses with different items
deleted each time. The best solution appeared to be one with 11 of the original 15 items arranged
in three factors, explaining 48.6% of the variance (see Table 1). We named the three factors Rights
of Nature, Eco-Crisis, and Human Exemptionalism (factors proposed by Dunlap and colleagues;
Dunlap & Van Liere, 1978; Dunlap et al., 2000), because a majority of the items on each dimen-
sion clearly reflected these facets of an ecological worldview. However, each of the three factors also
includes one item that the NEP authors intended as an indicator of a different facet.
CFA. We conducted a structural equation model (SEM) using AMOS software with the second half
(n = 336) of the sample to test the three-factor, 11-item model resulting from the EFA. In SEM, a
researcher posits a theoretical model, in this case on the basis of results of the EFA, and runs an analy-
sis to see how well the data fit the proposed model. In our study, the data were a good fit to the model
(see Figure 1). Goodness of Fit Index (GFI = .96), Adjusted Goodness of Fit Index (AGFI = .93), and
Comparative Fit Index (CFI = .90) were at or above the frequently recommended minimum level
of .90. Root Mean Square of Approximation (RMSEA = .05) is better than the often-recommended
TABLE 1. Exploratory Factor Analysis of the 11-Item New Ecological Paradigm
(NEP) Scale for Children in Year 2 (
n = 336)
Factor
Factor Scale items 1 2 3
1. Rights of Nature 7. People are supposed to rule over the rest of nature. .695
1. Plants and animals have as much right as people
to live. .691
4. People must still obey the laws of nature. .640
2. Eco-Crisis 10. If things dont change, we will have a big disaster
in the environment soon. .707
2. There are too many (or almost too many) people
on earth. .663
8. People are treating nature badly. .733
5. When people mess with nature it has bad results. .459
3. Human 3. People are clever enough to keep from ruining
Exemptionalism the earth. .667
6. Nature is strong enough to handle the bad effects
of our modern lifestyle. .614
9. People will someday know enough about how
nature works to be able to control it. .685
11. The so-called “environmental crisis” facing people
has been blown out of proportion (exaggerated).
a
.497
Note. Loadings of .35 or less are not included in the table. Eigenvalues for the factors are 2.6 (Factor 1), 1.5
(Factor 2), and 1.2 (Factor 3). The percentage of variance for the factors are 17.0 (Factor 1), 16.4 (Factor 2),
and 15.2 (Factor 3). We dropped four items from the scale from the analysis in Year 2.
a
We dropped this item from the Year 3 analysis because many students had trouble understanding it. We have
not included the item in the final version of the NEP Scale for Children.
level of .10 and at the level of .05 suggested by others (e.g., Hu & Bentler, 1999). The results thus
confirm the factor structure revealed in the EFA analysis (see Table 1).
We also examined values indicating the strength of the relationships shown in the model. First,
the values associated with items constituting each of the three factors indicate how well the items
within a factor relate to each other. As shown in Figure 1, items within each of the factors have
parameter estimates in similar ranges. The exception is in the Eco-Crisis factor, in which Item
1 has a parameter estimate that is substantially lower than the others in that factor. Second, the
parameter estimates for the relationships among the three factors show how well the three factors
fit together. In this case, Human Exemptionalism has a substantially lower value than have either
of the other two factors.
Because the original NEP authors suggested that for an adult sample a one-factor model is best
(Dunlap et al., 2000), we also tested a unidimensional model structure. The results, GFI (.91), AGFI
(.87), CFI (.64), and RMSEA (.09), were not as good as the results of the three-factor model. Thus,
it appears that the three-factor model is more appropriate for these students. The fit for the unidi-
mensional model is not greatly different, however, and it was clear that the model could be improved.
Thus, further testing in Year 3 seemed warranted.
Validation: Year 3
CFA. On the basis of student responses, we used only 10 of the 11 items from the Year 2 model
in Year 3. We dropped Item 11, “The so-called ‘environmental crisis’ facing people has been blown
out of proportion (exaggerated),” because it received a high number of Do not understand responses,
8 THE JOURNAL OF ENVIRONMENTAL EDUCATION
FIGURE 1. Confirmatory factor analysis on the 11-Item New Ecological Paradigm
(NEP) Scale for Children During Year 2 (
n = 336). Rights of Nature, Eco-Crisis,
and Human Exemptionalism are factors of the NEP Scale for Children. The val
-
ues above the lines between the factors and the scale indicate the parameter
estimates of the factors. The boxed numbers represent items on the NEP Scale.
The values above the lines between the factors and the scale items indicate the
parameter estimates for the items.
1
4
7
3 6 11 9
2
5
8
10
Rights of
Nature
NEP Scale for
Children
Human
Exemptionalism
Eco-Crisis
.63
.56
.47
.64
.24
.43
.37
.47
.46
.83
.27
.43
.66
.64
indicating that it was difficult for some children to grasp. Table 2 contains the response frequencies
in Year 3 for each of the remaining 10 items (renumbered from 1 to 10).
A CFA with the whole sample (N = 515) in Year 3 revealed a good fit of the data to the Year 2 CFA
model: GFI = .96, AGFI = .93, CFI = .75, RMSEA = .066 (see Figure 2). The results provided fur-
ther evidence that the three-factor structure offers a good solution for the NEP Scale for Children.
Examination of parameter estimates revealed that the three factors fit together better than they
did in Year 2. All three values are between .86 and 1.0, indicating strong relationships. For the
most part, the items within each factor also have strong parameter estimates, although one item in
Rights of Nature and one in Eco-Crisis does not fit as strongly as do the others. The factor Human
Exemptionalism has items with low parameter estimates, indicating that its items do not fit together
as strongly as do those in the other two factors.
In Year 3, the unidimensional model also fits the data well. Model fit statistics are in the same
range as those for the three-factor structure: GFI = .94, AGFI = .90, CFI = .83, RMSEA = .085.
Thus, it appears that, for this student sample, we can treat the NEP Scale for Children as three-
dimensional, giving each student three separate scores, or as a single measure, giving each student
an overall NEP score.
Program assessment. A paired-sample t test of 186 students who participated in the Sunship Earth
program provided evidence of the scales effectiveness in detecting changes in students environmental
SUMMER 2007, VOL. 38, NO. 4 9
TABLE 2. Frequency Distributions of the Responses to the 10-Item New Ecological
Paradigm (NEP) Scale for Children in Year 3 (
N = 515)
Responses (% of students)
Strongly Not Strongly
Scale item agree Agree sure Disagree disagree
1. Plants and animals have as much
right as people to live. 64.7 22.5 7.6 2.7 2.5
2. There are too many (or almost too
many) people on earth. 8.7 15.0 39.0 17.1 20.2
3. People are clever enough to keep
from ruining the earth. 18.3 20.2 36.9 14.2 10.5
4. People must still obey the laws of
nature. 55.0 24.7 9.9 4.9 5.6
5. When people mess with nature it
has bad results. 38.4 28.5 23.5 5.8 3.7
6. Nature is strong enough to handle
the bad effects of our modern lifestyle. 7.6 9.9 40.4 25.2 16.9
7. People are supposed to rule over
the rest of nature. 6.8 8.2 14.2 27.0 43.9
8. People are treating nature badly. 30.5 34.0 23.7 5.2 6.6
9. People will someday know enough
about how nature works to be able
to control it. 14.8 21.6 39.0 13.0 11.7
10. If things dont change, we will have
a big disaster in the environment soon. 30.5 29.7 27.8 5.4 6.6
Note. Items 3, 6, 7, and 9 (anti-environmental) were reverse scored to obtain an overall NEP score.
worldviews in a pre- and postprogram assessment (see Table 3). We computed factor and total NEP
scores using average mean scores (on the 5-point scale) across relevant items: Rights of Nature (1, 4, and
7), Eco-Crisis (2, 5, 8, and 10), and Human Exemptionalism (3, 6, and 9). Items 3, 6, 7, and 9 were
reverse scored (i.e., strongly agree = 1 instead of 5). Before performing the statistical analysis, we exam-
ined the data for normal distribution because distortion violates the assumption on which the t test is
based. None of the variables had a distribution far enough from normal for a violation to occur.
10 THE JOURNAL OF ENVIRONMENTAL EDUCATION
FIGURE 2. Confirmatory factor analyses on the 10-Item New Ecological Paradigm
(NEP) Scale for Children During Year 3 (
N = 515). Rights of Nature, Eco-Crisis,
and Human Exemptionalism are factors of the NEP Scale for Children. The val
-
ues above the lines between the factors and the scale indicate the parameter
estimates of the factors. The boxed numbers represent items on the NEP Scale.
The values above the lines between the factors and the scale items indicate the
parameter estimates for the items.
1
4
7
3
6
9
2
5
8
10
Rights of
Nature
NEP Scale for
Children
Human
Exemptionalism
Eco-Crisis
.97
.24
.77
.63
1.00
.09
–.35
.15
.86
.25
.64
.64
.65
TABLE 3. Comparison of Mean Pre- and Posttest Scores on New Ecological Paradigm
for Children (
n = 186)
Pretest Posttest
Factor M SD M SD t df p Effect size
Rights of Nature 4.22 .70 4.40 .65 –3.71 185 .000
*
.27
Eco-Crisis 3.58 .63 3.72 .67 –2.97 185 .003
*
.27
Human Exemptionalism 2.93 .74 3.12 .74 3.41 185 .001
*
.25
Total score for scale 3.58 .47 3.74 .74 –5.51 185 .000
*
.39
*
Statistically significant difference (p < .05) between pre-and posttest scores.
Despite that the majority of students attending the program started with pro-ecological scores on two
of the three factors, Rights of Nature and Eco-Crisis, their worldviews changed significantly toward a
more pro-ecological perspective after the program (see Table 3). We loosely interpreted the increase in
their scores on the factor of Human Exemptionalism, from below the midpoint to above it, as reflecting
a shift from a slightly anthropocentric to a slightly ecocentric perspective. Their total scores for the NEP
Scale for Children also increased significantly. Although modest in magnitude, all of these changes were
statistically significant, as shown in the effect size data in Table 3.
Other studies have evaluated the effect of the Sunship Earth program, but all used study-specific
questionnaires (Bires, Johnson, & McFadden, 1982; Keen, 1991; Mulligan, 1989; Payne, 1981; van
Wissen, 1992). Those researchers’ results have been mixed, with most finding little or no change in
attitude. None of the instruments used in those studies, however, were developed using a theoreti-
cally sound construct of attitude or worldview. The NEP Scale for Children, designed specifically to
measure endorsement of an ecological worldview, revealed that the program produced a significant
effect on childrens worldviews. Our data clearly indicate that participation in the Sunship Earth
program produces a shift toward a more pro-ecological worldview among children.
Conclusion
Our results suggest that the NEP Scale for Children, with 10 instead of 15 items and revised word-
ing, is appropriate for use with children ages 10–12 years. We further found that the instrument mea-
sures three interrelated dimensions of the New Ecological Paradigm: Rights of Nature, Eco-Crisis, and
Human Exemptionalism. Dunlap et al. (2000) proposed all three of these dimensions in the design
of the revised NEP Scale. We also found it possible to treat the scale as a unidimensional measure
providing one overall score on the anthropocentric (DSP, low score) to ecocentric (NEP, high score)
continuum, after reverse scoring the negatively worded items of the scale (items 3, 6, 7, and 9).
It is not surprising that the change produced by participation in the Sunship Earth program was
not large. Before participating in the program, the students held pro-environmental (or pro-ecologi-
cal) views on two of the three factors as indicated by the pretest scores, leaving little room for change
in the pro-ecological direction. Anticipation of participation in the program or classroom activities
in preparation for it could have presensitized students toward nature (Bogner, 1998; Howie, 1974).
On the other hand, environmental worldviews are deeply rooted beliefs, and change in them appears
inevitably to be slow (Bogner). Consequently, for a 5-day intervention to produce statistically sig-
nificant effects is impressive. The finding is highly valuable because researchers have not conducted
comparable studies of potential changes in adult worldviews in response to educational programs.
We are currently using the NEP Scale for Children in studies of students participating in earth
education programs in Australia and other parts of the United States. Using the instrument with
larger numbers of students in a variety of locations will help determine the generalizability of the
NEP Scale for Children. Additional administrations also allow for further testing of the three-
dimensional model that emerged in this study. In addition, the NEP Scale for Children can be
a useful instrument for evaluating the ability of environmental learning programs to produce
changes in environmental worldviews. We are using the NEP Scale for Children along with
other instruments, observations, and interviews with children and their parents to investigate the
relationships among environmental worldviews, ecological understandings, and environmental
actions and how education affects those variables.
Despite the positive results of the study, we must use caution when interpreting the findings. The results
may not apply to children in other locations. We cannot generalize our results until we and other research-
ers have conducted further studies with children from other backgrounds and in other locations.
SUMMER 2007, VOL. 38, NO. 4 11
12 THE JOURNAL OF ENVIRONMENTAL EDUCATION
For more than a quarter of a century, researchers have successfully used the New Environmental
Paradigm Scale and the more recent version, the New Ecological Paradigm Scale, to investigate adult
environmental worldviews. The NEP Scale for Children allows for similar investigations of how
childrens environmental worldviews develop, how they change as a result of new experiences or edu-
cational programs, and how the environmental worldviews of children from different socioeconomic
or cultural backgrounds compare. These are important questions, and the NEP Scale for Children
can play an important role in answering them.
NOTE
1. For pre- and posttest comparison, we selected students who had participated in the Sunship Earth program. Because
Sunship Earth is a longer residential program than Earthkeepers, participation in Sunship Earth is more likely to affect
worldviews (Bogner, 1998).
REFERENCES
Armstrong, J. B., & Impara, J. C. (1991). The impact of environmental education program on knowledge and attitude.
The Journal of Environmental Education, 22(4), 36–40.
Barbour, I. G. (Ed.). (1973). Western man and environmental ethics. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Bechtel, R. B., Verdugo, V. C., & Pinheiro, J. de Q. (1999). Environmental belief systems: United States, Brazil, and
Mexico. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30, 122–128.
Bires, F., Johnson, B., & McFadden, J. (1982). Evaluation of the spring 1982 Sunship Earth program at McKeever
Environmental Learning Center, Sandy Lake, Pennsylvania. Unpublished master’s project, George Williams College,
Downers Grove, IL.
Bogner, F. X. (1998). The influence of short-term outdoor ecology education on long-term variables of environmental
perspective. The Journal of Environmental Education, 29(4), 17–29.
Caldwell, L. K. (1970). Authority and responsibility for environmental administration. Annals of the American Academy of
Political and Social Science, 389, 107–115.
Campbell, R. R., & Wade, J. L. (1972). Value systems. In R. R. Campbell & J. L. Wade (Eds.), Society and environment:
The coming collision (pp. 337–345). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Commoner, B. (1971). The closing circle. New York: Knopf.
Daly, H. E. (Ed.). (1973). Toward a steady-state economy. San Francisco: Freeman.
Dunlap, R. E. (1998). Lay perceptions of global risk: Public views of global warming in cross national context. International
Sociology, 13, 473–498.
Dunlap, R. E., & Jones, R. E. (2002). Environmental concern: Conceptual and measurement issues. In R. E. Dunlap &
W. Michelson (Eds.), Handbook of environmental sociology (pp. 482–524). Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.
Dunlap, R. E., & Van Liere, K. D. (1978). The New Environmental Paradigm. The Journal of Environmental Education,
9(4), 10–19.
Dunlap, R. E., Van Liere, K. D., Mertig, A. G., & Jones, R. E. (2000). Measuring endorsement of the New Ecological
Paradigm: A revised NEP scale. Journal of Social Issues, 56(3), 425–442.
Edgell, M. C. R., & Nowell, D. E. (1989). The New Environmental Paradigm scale: Wildlife and environmental beliefs
in British Colombia. Society and Natural Resources, 2, 285–286.
Furman, A. (1998). A note on environmental concern in a developing country: Results from an Istanbul survey.
Environment and Behavior, 30, 520–534.
Gooch, G. D. (1995). Environmental beliefs in Sweden and the Baltic states. Environment and Behavior, 27, 513–539.
Gray, D. B. (1985). Ecological beliefs and behaviors: Assessment and change. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.
Howie, T. R. (1974). Indoor and outdoor environmental education. The Journal of Environmental Education, 6(2), 32–36.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus
new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55.
Keen, M. (1991). The effect of the Sunship Earth program on knowledge and attitude development. The Journal of
Environmental Education, 22(3), 28–32.
Leeming, F. C., Dwyer, W. O., & Bracken, B. A. (1995). Childrens environmental attitude and knowledge scale:
Construction and validation. The Journal of Environmental Education, 26(3), 22–33.
Leeming, F. C., Dwyer, W. O., Porter, B. E., & Cobern, M. K. (1993). Outcome research in environmental education: A
critical review. The Journal of Environmental Education, 24(4), 8–21.
Meadows, D. H., Meadows, D. L., Randers, J., & Behrens, W. W. III. (1972). The limits to growth. New York: Universe
Books.
Mulligan, M. E. (1989). Environmental attitudinal change: Does the Sunship Earth program make it happen? Unpublished
master’s thesis, Brock University, St. Catharines, Ontario, Canada.
Musser, L. M., & Malkus, A. J. (1994). The childrens attitudes toward the environment scale. The Journal of Environmental
Education, 25(3), 22–26.
Noe, F. P., & Hammitt, W. E. (1992). Environmental attitudes and the personal relevance of management actions in a
park setting. Journal of Environmental Management, 35, 205–216.
Noe, F. P., & Snow, R. (1989). Hispanic culture influence on environmental concern. The Journal of Environmental
Education, 21(2), 27–34.
Noe, F. P., & Snow, R. (1990). The new environmental paradigm and further scale analysis. The Journal of Environmental
Education, 21(4), 20–26.
Payne, P. G. (1981). A comparative study of the effects of two outdoor/environmental education instructional approaches on the
attitudes and knowledge of selected sixth grade children. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Oregon, Eugene.
Pedhazur, E. J., & Schmelkin, L. P. (1991). Measurement, design, and analysis: An integrated approach. Hillside, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Pirages, D. C., & Ehrlich, P. R. (1974). Ark II: Social response to environmental imperatives. San Francisco: Freeman.
Roberts, J. A., & Bacon, D. R. (1997). Exploring the subtle relationships between environmental concern and ecologically
conscious consumer behavior. Journal of Business Research, 40, 79–89.
Schultz, P. W., & Zelezny, L. (1999). Values as predictors of environmental attitudes: Evidence for consistency across 14
countries. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 19, 255–265.
Scott. D., & Willits, F. K. (1994). Environmental attitudes and behavior: A Pennsylvania survey. Environment and
Behavior, 26, 239–260.
Shetzer, L., Stackman, R. W., & Moore, L. F. (1991). Business-environment attitudes and the new environmental para-
digm. The Journal of Environmental Education, 22(4), 14–21.
Stern, P. C. (1992). Psychological dimensions of global environmental change. Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 269–302.
Stern, P. C., Dietz, T., & Guagnano, G. A. (1995). The New Ecological Paradigm in social-psychological context.
Environment and Behavior, 27(6), 723–743.
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariates statistics (3rd ed.). New York: HarperCollins College.
Van Matre, S. (1979). Sunship Earth: An earth education program for getting to know your place in space. Martinsville, IN:
American Camping Association.
Van Matre, S., & Johnson, B. (1988). Earthkeepers: Four keys for helping young people live in harmony with the earth.
Greenville, WV: Institute for Earth Education.
Van Wissen, F. A. (1992). Promoting responsible environmental behavior through earth education camps: Sunship Earth and
Earthkeepers. Unpublished master’s thesis, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada.
Whisenhunt, D. W. (1974). The environment and the American experience. Port Washington, NY: Kennikat Press.
SUMMER 2007, VOL. 38, NO. 4 13



    















 
 

 
 

 

 


 

 
 
 
 
 

... Existe una brecha en la literatura existente sobre las actitudes y creencias ambientales de los seres humanos, especialmente en los jóvenes (Manoli, Johnson & Dunlap, 2007). Esta investigación tuvo como objetivo caracterizar las actitudes ambientales en jóvenes adolescentes en Panamá, enmarcado en el cambio que ha surgido del paradigma dominante social sobre el ambiente (DSP) hacia un nuevo paradigma ecológico (NEP), enfocado en tres dimensiones: los derechos de la naturaleza; un sentido de urgencia de que la naturaleza se encuentra en eco-crisis; y el excepcionalismo humano. ...
... There is a dearth of available research about environmental attitudes and beliefs of human beings, especially among youth (Manoli, Johnson & Dunlap, 2007). The objective of this study was to characterize environmental attitudes in young adolescents in Panama, framed by the shift from the dominant social paradigm (DSP) towards a new ecological paradigm (NEP), and focused on three dimensions: the rights of nature; a sense of urgency that nature is in eco-crisis; and human exceptionalism. ...
... Este estudio contribuye a la creciente literatura que reconoce la importancia de generar entendimiento sobre los efectos que tienen las decisiones de los seres humanos en el medio ambiente (Hadjichamis et al., 2015). También contribuye a los llamados reflejados en la literatura de incrementar el entendimiento de las actitudes y creencias ambientales de los seres humanos (Manoli, Johnson & Dunlap, 2007). Es relevante para el campo de estudio ambiental ya que muchos estudios de actitudes aplicados no han seguido un proceso riguroso desde un punto de vista psicométrico, y por ende se ha acumulado poco conocimiento (Manoli, Johnson & Dunlap, 2007). ...
Article
Full-text available
Existe una brecha en la literatura existente sobre las actitudes y creencias ambientales de los seres humanos, especialmente en los jóvenes (Manoli, Johnson & Dunlap, 2007). Esta investigación tuvo como objetivo caracterizar las actitudes ambientales en jóvenes adolescentes en Panamá, enmarcado en el cambio que ha surgido del paradigma dominante social sobre el ambiente (DSP) hacia un nuevo paradigma ecológico (NEP), enfocado en tres dimensiones: los derechos de la naturaleza; un sentido de urgencia de que la naturaleza se encuentra en eco-crisis; y el excepcionalismo humano. Se empleó la escala NEP para niños, previamente desarrollada y validada. La muestra del estudio estuvo constituida por estudiantes de pre-media, en séptimo, octavo y noveno grado (n=99) de seis escuelas en Panamá que formaron parte de un programa de educación ambiental. Los estudiantes obtuvieron altos puntajes en actitudes sobre derechos de la naturaleza, pero bajos puntajes en actitudes de exepcionalismo humano. No se encontraron diferencias significativas en los promedios del NEP entre participantes que estudian en la provincia de Chiriquí y participantes que estudian en la provincia de Panamá o Panamá Oeste. Tampoco se encontraron diferencias significativas entre niños y niñas en los puntajes del NEP. Se observaron correlaciones positivas débiles de la edad y el grado con los puntajes del NEP, pero sin significancia estadística. Este estudio sirve de punto de partida para entender las actitudes de jóvenes sobre las tres dimensiones que mide el NEP. El análisis cualitativo reveló que los estudiantes utilizan frases consistentes con los diferentes factores alineados con el nuevo paradigma ecológico, en donde se percibe una crisis ecológica (sentido de urgencia), se atribuye responsabilidad directa al ser humano (excepcionalismo humano), y se reconocen los derechos de la naturaleza. A futuro, se pudiese ampliar la población encuestada con una muestra probabilística mayor. También, se pudiera aplicar el NEP como una medición pre y post para evaluar el impacto de programas de educación ambiental.
... Las encuestas diseñadas para un público adulto requieren de una edición y pilotaje previo antes de su aplicación en población infantil a fin de garantizar una redacción adecuada a la edad y opciones de respuesta tipo escala Likert comprensibles. Manoli et al. (2007), por ejemplo, crearon una versión infantil de la escala Nuevo Paradigma Ambiental (NEP por sus siglas en inglés) (Dunlap et al., 2000). Eliminaron el ítem 11 -"la denominada 'crisis ambiental' que enfrenta la gente se ha desproporcionado"-porque recibió un gran número de respuestas de no entiendo por parte de su público piloto de jóvenes. ...
... Para desarrollar una versión en español de la escala NEP para niños (Manoli et al., 2007), Corraliza et al. (2013) aplicaron la técnica de traducción inversa, primero traduciendo la escala al español y luego nuevamente al inglés, para comparar similitudes con el instrumento original. Al adaptar la escala del Modelo de Valores Ecológicos (MEV por sus siglas en inglés) para su uso con niños mexicanos, Schneller et al. (2015) también siguió procedimientos de traducción inversa. ...
... Sin embargo, las versiones iniciales de nuestra encuesta destinadas a adquirir dichos datos requerían de un lenguaje y conceptos que eran demasiado difíciles de entender para los participantes. De maera similar, otros investigadores han notado que las estructuras de preguntas complejas que buscan abordar las actitudes de alto nivel y la comprensión de conceptos globales no funcioa con poblaciones preadolescentes, lo que refleja una desconexión entre las perspectivas académicas y la realidad en terreno (Evans et al., 2007;Kellert, 2002;Larson et al., 2011;Manoli et al., 2007). ...
... The findings of different studies show that exposure to nature as part of an EE intervention does increase children's pro-environmentalism. For instance, Manoli, Johnson and Dunlap (2007) examined the effect on children's pro-environmental beliefs of participating in an outdoors EE programme aimed at developing an understanding of ecological concepts, building positive feelings for the natural world, and increasing pro-environmental behaviours. Children's pro-environmental beliefs were higher after attending the EE programme. ...
... Por isso, ferramentas que auxiliem a estudar e compreender crenças e percepções do ser humano acerca da sua relação com o meio ambiente são imprescindíveis (CORRALIZA; COLLADO; BETHELMY, 2013; EVANS; BRAUCHLE; HAQ; STECKER; WONG; SHAPIRO, 2007). Manoli, Johnson e Dunlap (2007) acreditam que quanto mais cedo se buscar desenvolver atitudes ambientais em crianças, maior poderá ser a chance de isto moldar o pensamento e comportamento da futura geração de adultos. (2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
O presente trabalho teve por objetivo traduzir, adaptar semanticamente e investigar evidências de validade de conteúdo do New Ecological Paradigm Scale for Children (NEP C), instrumento de medida de atitudes ambientais em crianças, para o contexto brasileiro. Neste sentido, foram conduzidos dois estudos. O primeiro contou com um comitê multidisciplinar que avaliou os itens do novo instrumento considerando três critérios: clareza de linguagem, pertinência do constructo e correspondência semântica. Posteriormente, o Coeficiente de Validade de Conteúdo foi empregado. Após tais procedimentos, realizou-se um estudo piloto com 5 crianças pertencentes à população-alvo para se verificar a compreensão da escala pelas mesmas. Enquanto resultados, observou-se que a versão brasileira do NEP C possui equivalência semântica com a escala original e apresenta evidências de validade de conteúdo.
... La recherche sur les attitudes et les comportements environnementaux des enfants est limitée (Cheng et Monroe, 2012;Larson, Green et Castleberry, 2011;Manoli, Johnson et Dunlap, 2007 ;Evans, Brauchle, Haq, Stecker, Wong, et Shapiro, 2007) et il n' y'a pas beaucoup d'informations sur les facteurs et les modèles comportementaux qui conduisent les enfants à se comporter de manière respectueuse de l'environnement (Collado et al, 2015;Cheng et Monroe, 2012 ;Evans et al, 2007). Cependant, au cours de la dernière décennie, les chercheurs ont commencé à accorder plus d'attention à cette population et ont proposé quelques recommandations quant aux facteurs qui semblent affecter les attitudes et les comportements environnementaux des enfants. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
The objective of this research is to provide a better understanding of the child's ecological behavior at school. This is done by looking more specifically at the influence of the concern for self-image and the perceived effectiveness of environmental actions on the child's ecological behavior at school. Through this research work, we have also sought to study the moderating effect of age, gender, membership in environmental clubs and parental influence on the relationship between a child's ecological behavior at school and his antecedents. To achieve this objective, a survey was conducted with a sample of 171 children aged between 8 and 12 years old to test hypothetical relationships proposed in the theoretical framework. The results of this study showed that the child's ecological behavior at school is positively influenced by the perceived effectiveness of his or her behavior and concern for his or her image.
... Por el contrario, el otro colegio está situado en el centro de la ciudad, con un diseño típicamente urbano sin incluir ningún elemento natural en su interior. Todos los participantes respondieron a un cuestionario sobre características sociodemográficas, a la escala Children Environmental Perceptions Scale (CEPS; Larson, et al., 2011), la escala New Environmental Paradigm (NEP; Manoli et al., 2007) y realizaron un dibujo sobre lo que simboliza para ellos y ellas la naturaleza, que fue analizado cualitativamente mediante el programa MaxQDA. ...
... " This is regarded as one of the best direct predictors of behavior change (Klöckner, 2013;Ashley et al., 2019). Furthermore, pro-environmental attitudes in general were measured with the revised version of the ten-item New Ecological Paradigm scale (NEP; Dunlap et al., 2000), that is adapted for children and adolescents (Manoli et al., 2007), with a five-point Likert agreement scale. We used the multidimensional purpose of the NEP scale to evaluate three factors, i.e., the extent to which one endorses the rights of nature (Rights of Nature; e.g. ...
Article
Full-text available
Plastic pollution is both a societal and environmental problem and citizen science has shown to be a useful tool to engage both the public and professionals in addressing it. However, knowledge on the educational and behavioral impacts of citizen science projects focusing on marine litter remains limited. Our preregistered study investigates the impact of the citizen science project Citizen Observation of Local Litter in coastal ECosysTems (COLLECT) on the participants' ocean literacy, pro-environmental intentions and attitudes, well-being, and nature connectedness, using a pretest-posttest design. A total of 410 secondary school students from seven countries, in Africa (Benin, Cabo Verde, Côte d'Ivoire, Ghana, Morocco, Nigeria) and Asia (Malaysia) were trained to sample plastics on sandy beaches and to analyze their collection in the classroom. Non-parametric statistical tests (n = 239 matched participants) demonstrate that the COLLECT project positively impacted ocean literacy (i.e., awareness and knowledge of marine litter, self-reported litter-reducing behaviors, attitudes towards beach litter removal). The COLLECT project also led to higher pro-environmental behavioral intentions for students in Benin and Ghana (implying a positive spillover effect) and higher well-being and nature connectedness for students in Benin. Results are interpreted in consideration of a high baseline in awareness and attitudes towards marine litter, a low internal consistency of pro-environmental attitudes, Frontiers in Psychology 02 frontiersin.org the cultural context of the participating countries, and the unique settings of the project's implementation. Our study highlights the benefits and challenges of understanding how citizen science impacts the perceptions and behaviors towards marine litter in youth from the respective regions.
Book
In eleven chapters the book offers data, relevant literature and educational tools to address current ecological crises in formal education, paying specific attention to reading aloud activities (reviewing the “Green Books aloud per una transizione ecologica e culturale" project), and to assessment methods and tools.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: Humans’ attitude toward nature, based on the anthropocentric philosophy and belief in human exceptionalism are the root causes of environmental crisis. New Ecological Paradigm (NEP) presumes to step away from anthropocentrism and recognizing the limits to growth, which is based on unsustainable exploitation of the environment. Worldwide, the level of acceptance for NEP is measured by the NEP Scale (New Ecological Paradigm Scale) consisting of 12–15 theses, for which answers are expressed in a Likert scale. Research Aim: Using the NEP Scale for examination of academic youth’ senvironmental awareness. Method: Online survey including a 15-element NEP Scale translated into Polish was used; for questionnaire and data collecting Qualtrics Survey App was applied. The collected data from among N = 260 students were subjected to ordering, re-coding, and statistical analysis, including descriptive statistics, frequencies, and t-test for the NEP index in the groups of genders, using SPSS Statistics 28.0.1.0. Respondents’ support for NEP was estimated by frequency analysis of “pro-ecological” responses, comparison of means, and the NEP index. Results: The frequency of responses expressing support for NEP was 74.5; the mean for individual questions valued from 1 (no support) to 5 (maximum support) was 4.13. The NEP index (scale from 0 to 1) was 0.78. T-test confirmed gender influence on NEP Index, which was higher among women than men (mean 0.79 vs. 0.72), with p = 0.007. Conclusions: The NEP Index values indicate the high environmental awareness of the studied academic youth. Higher support for NEP among women is in line with the worldwide trend observed in numerous studies, in which women’s awareness and behaviour are more pro-ecological than men’s.
Article
Full-text available
AN APPLICATION OF THE NEP SCALE IN STUDYING ENVIRONMENTAL AWARENESS OF ACADEMIC YOUTH Introduction: Humans’ attitude toward nature, based on the anthropocentric philosophy and belief in human exceptionalism are the root causes of environmental crisis. New Ecological Paradigm (NEP) presumes to step away from anthropocentrism and recognizing the limits to growth, which is based on unsustainable exploitation of the environment. Worldwide, the level of acceptance for NEP is measured by the NEP Scale (New Ecological Paradigm Scale) consisting of 12–15 theses, for which answers are expressed in a Likert scale. Research Aim: Using the NEP Scale for examination of academic youth’ senvironmental awareness. Method: Online survey including a 15-element NEP Scale translated into Polish was used; for questionnaire and data collecting Qualtrics Survey App was applied. The collected data from among N = 260 students were subjected to ordering, re-coding, and statistical analysis, including descriptive statistics, frequencies, and t-test for the NEP index in the groups of genders, using SPSS Statistics 28.0.1.0. Respondents’ support for NEP was estimated by frequency analysis of “pro-ecological” responses, comparison of means, and the NEP index. Results: The frequency of responses expressing support for NEP was 74.5; the mean for individual questions valued from 1 (no support) to 5 (maximum support) was 4.13. The NEP index (scale from 0 to 1) was 0.78. T-test confirmed gender influence on NEP Index, which was higher among women than men (mean 0.79 vs. 0.72), with p = 0.007. Conclusions: The NEP Index values indicate the high environmental awareness of the studied academic youth. Higher support for NEP among women is in line with the worldwide trend observed in numerous studies, in which women’s awareness and behaviour are more pro-ecological than men’s. Keywords: environmental awareness, New Environmental Paradigm, NEP scale, environmental pedagogy, academic youth
Article
Full-text available
Dunlap and Van Liere's New Environmental Paradigm (NEP) Scale, published in 1978, has become a widely used measure of proenvironmental orientation. This article develops a revised NEP Scale designed to improve upon the original one in several respects: ( 1 ) It taps a wider range of facets of an ecological worldview, ( 2 ) It offers a balanced set of pro- and anti-NEP items, and ( 3 ) It avoids outmoded terminology. The new scale, termed the New Ecological Paradigm Scale, consists of 15 items. Results of a 1990 Washington State survey suggest that the items can be treated as an internally consistent summated rating scale and also indicate a modest growth in pro-NEP responses among Washington residents over the 14 years since the original study.
Article
This volume fills the void created by the lack of a book-length, critical, and systematic treatment of ecological attitudes and behaviors. It emphasizes psychometrics and experimentation within a broad behavioral-cognitive framework focused on the natural world. Gray summarizes and integrates existing research and reviews major alternative approaches to measuring ecological attitudes, while presenting his own ecological attitude domain model. Russell Weigel and Richard Borden provide state-of-the-art reviews of the research on the relationship between ecological attitudes and actions and on the linkage between personality and ecological concern. Gray himself integrates the theoretical perspectives of social psychologists Milton Rokeach and Martin Fishbein in his construction of a paradigm for ecological change. Using this as background, he reviews existing behavioral studies, differentiating between those that rely on information and those that use reinforcement to produce a desired change in behavior. Finally, he suggests that the key to large-scale change is the creation of a true environmental ethic in our society.
Article
The influence of the earth education program, Sunship Earth, on ecological knowledge and environmental attitudes was examined. It was found that students who attended the Sunship Earth program increased their ecological knowledge significantly. Participation in the Sunship Earth program did not result in more positive environmental attitudes. Part of the success of the Sunship Earth program is attributed to the setting in which it occurs and the techniques used to develop ecological concepts. The need for more opportunities for teachers to develop skills in environmental education and to participate in environmental education curriculum development that would link extension education more firmly to the school curriculum is discussed.