ArticlePDF Available

Relationships of Parents’ Theories of Intelligence With Children’s Persistence/Learned Helplessness: A Cross-Cultural Comparison

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The present study employed the learned helplessness paradigm to explore the possibility that culturally based parent beliefs influence the way in which young children approach academic tasks. Children, aged between 7 and 8 years, from New Zealand, the United States of America, China, and Japan participated in three different school-related tasks. Each of the tasks contained three levels, varying from easy to hard. Success was prevented for the medium and hard levels, allowing for exploration of child reactions to failure. The results revealed that parental support of the incremental theory of intelligence was indirectly and positively related to high child persistence in the Asian culture. Furthermore, parents’ reports of their own reactions to frustrating events and efforts to encourage their children operated as mediators in both cultures, although in somewhat different ways between the Asian and Western cultures.
Content may be subject to copyright.
http://jcc.sagepub.com/
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology
http://jcc.sagepub.com/content/43/6/999
The online version of this article can be found at:
DOI: 10.1177/0022022111421633
2012 43: 999 originally published online 10 October 2011Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology
Paul E. Jose and Marisa A. Bellamy
Helplessness : A Cross-Cultural Comparison
Relationships of Parents' Theories of Intelligence With Children's Persistence/Learned
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
On behalf of:
International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology
can be found at:Journal of Cross-Cultural PsychologyAdditional services and information for
http://jcc.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:
http://jcc.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:
http://jcc.sagepub.com/content/43/6/999.refs.htmlCitations:
What is This?
- Oct 10, 2011 OnlineFirst Version of Record
- Jul 2, 2012Version of Record >>
at Victoria Univ of Wellington on February 6, 2013jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology
43(6) 999 –1018
© The Author(s) 2012
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0022022111421633
jccp.sagepub.com
421633
JCCXXX10.1177/0022022111421633Jose and
BellamyJournal of Cross-Cultural Psychology
1
Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand
Corresponding Author:
Paul E. Jose, School of Psychology, P. O. Box 600, Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand.
Email: paul.jose@vuw.ac.nz
Relationships of Parents’
Theories of Intelligence
With Children’s Persistence/
Learned Helplessness:
A Cross-Cultural
Comparison
Paul E. Jose
1
and Marisa A. Bellamy
1
Abstract
The present study employed the learned helplessness paradigm to explore the possibility that
culturally based parent beliefs influence the way in which young children approach academic
tasks. Children, aged between 7 and 8 years, from New Zealand, the United States of America,
China, and Japan participated in three different school-related tasks. Each of the tasks contained
three levels, varying from easy to hard. Success was prevented for the medium and hard levels,
allowing for exploration of child reactions to failure. The results revealed that parental sup-
port of the incremental theory of intelligence was indirectly and positively related to high child
persistence in the Asian culture. Furthermore, parents’ reports of their own reactions to frus-
trating events and efforts to encourage their children operated as mediators in both cultures,
although in somewhat different ways between the Asian and Western cultures.
Keywords
learned helplessness, cultural comparisons, parent and child, persistence
Academic achievement is highly valued in both Western and Asian cultures (Hess, Chang, &
McDevitt, 1987). Since a number of studies have shown over the last several decades that Asian
students (and Asian immigrant students in Western countries) academically outperform Western
students (Chen & Stevenson, 1995), a great deal of attention has been devoted to identifying the
reasons for this difference. It has been suggested that one reason is the way that Asian students,
particularly Chinese and Japanese students, approach skill development and academic achieve-
ment and the value they place on effort and persistence (Hau & Salili, 1991; Rao, Noely, &
Sachs, 2000; Shimahara, 1986). The present study sought to determine whether cross-cultural
differences between Asian and Western children and parents could be documented as a way to
at Victoria Univ of Wellington on February 6, 2013jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
1000 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 43(6)
substantiate the view that culturally based beliefs guide and shape how young children approach
academic tasks.
The framework that we used to approach this question was the learned helplessness paradigm
developed by Dweck over the last several decades (e.g., Dweck, 1999). The beliefs and values of
Asian parents concerning their children’s acquisition of academic skills and knowledge seem to
be very similar to those underlying what Dweck refers to as the “incremental theory of intelli-
gence” (Dweck & Leggett, 1988). The incremental theory states that intelligence (or more
broadly, knowledge and academic skills) can be increased and developed through learning, prac-
tice, and effort. The incremental view has been linked to increased academic motivation, persis-
tence in the face of failure (termed “mastery orientation”), and ultimately greater academic
achievement (Elliott & Dweck, 1988; Mueller & Dweck, 1998). Within Western culture another
view concerning the root cause of academic achievement has been described by Dweck: namely,
a focus on intelligence as a fixed, stable entity termed the “entity theory of intelligence” (Dweck
& Leggett, 1988). The entity theory has been found to be predictive of learned helplessness,
which encompasses behaviors including increased negative affect, decreased persistence, and
denigration of ability following failure (Dweck, 1999). Failure is interpreted by someone who
holds an entity view as a sign of a lack of intelligence. As a consequence, he or she becomes
focused on achieving successful outcomes (e.g., passing a test) rather than learning (Diener &
Dweck, 1978; Elliott & Dweck, 1988; Mueller & Dweck, 1998).
Parents
Eccles, Wigfield, and Schiefele’s (1998) influential review of child academic achievement
accords an important place for parental effects. Their model states that parental beliefs, expecta-
tions, and behavior influence the academic motivation of children, and a host of other research-
ers echo this position (Heyman, Dweck, & Cain, 1992; Pomerantz & Dong, 2006; Smiley &
Lee, 2005). Hundreds of studies have documented the fact that parental characteristics are
associated with child academic outcomes (Wigfield, Eccles, Schiefele, Rosser, & Davis-Kean,
2006), but what needs further work is exploration of the mediators between parental and child
variables. Surprisingly little research has been conducted on the matter of how parental theories
of intelligence predict child outcomes, and that is the focus of the present research. It is possible
that part of the parental influence on children occurs through the transmission (explicit teaching
and/or modeling) of parental theories of intelligence to children’s persistence behavior.
Elliot and Thrash (2004) have reported that fear of failure, which can elicit avoidance behav-
ior, is often passed from parent to child. They found that the relationship between parent and
child fear of failure was mediated through parental love withdrawal, which suggests that parental
beliefs can manifest themselves through parent behavior. It may be, then, that children observed
and imitated the behavior of their parents. Hokoda and Fincham (1995) observed that child moti-
vation can be influenced and modified by the way that parents react to child difficulty. Mothers
of mastery-oriented children encouraged their children to persist, offered support, and taught
these children new strategies. In contrast, mothers of performance-oriented children were much
less responsive, offered less help, and encouraged their child to quit when a task seemed too hard
(Hokoda & Fincham, 1995).
Ames and Archer (1987) reported that mastery-oriented mothers emphasize the importance
of challenging tasks that encourage learning and the role of effort in achievement. However,
performance-focused mothers were more concerned with child grades and tasks that promoted
success. In this vein, Kamins and Dweck (1999) have found that helpless and mastery responses
can result from certain types of criticism children receive from their parents. For example, help-
less children were reported to expect person-oriented, rather than task-oriented, criticism from
at Victoria Univ of Wellington on February 6, 2013jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Jose and Bellamy 1001
parents following poor performance. As another example, some parents express helpless patterns
of behavior themselves, which appear to be mirrored in their children (Hokoda & Fincham,
1995). Cole, Warren, Dallaire, Lagrange, Travis, and Ciesla (2007) have noted that learned help-
lessness in kindergarten children is predicted by parental stress levels and harsh parenting styles.
These findings suggest that parental behaviors and attitudes might mediate between parental
theories of intelligence and child behavioral outcomes. Thus, one chief aim of the current
research was to investigate the influence of parent theories of intelligence and behavior on chil-
dren’s reactions to success and failure, and a second was to examine whether parental encourage-
ment of children and parent reports of their reactions to frustrating situations were mediators of
this relationship.
Cross-Cultural Comparisons of Learned Helplessness
Although the existence of learned helplessness has been widely established within European
American cultures, little research has explored learned helplessness in samples outside of North
America (Peterson, 1995). The research that has been conducted suggests that while learned
helplessness is similarly manifested in other cultures, including Germany (Ziegler, Finsterwald,
& Grassinger, 2005), Australia (Craske, 1985), South Africa (Nenty & Ogwu, 2009), Hong
Kong (Au, Watkins, & Hattie, 2010), and the Netherlands (De Bruyn, Dekovic, & Meijnen,
2003), this may not be the case for all cultures. For example, Storey (2008) has found evidence
that bicultural Native Americans experienced less frustration in the learned helplessness para-
digm than marginal or traditional Native Americans.
Of particular interest to the current investigation is the prevalence and manifestation of
learned helplessness in Asian cultures as compared to Western cultures. Specifically, the present
research was directed at examining learned helplessness in Chinese and Japanese cultures as
contrasted with the American and New Zealand cultures. Previous research has revealed that the
Chinese and Japanese cultures hold similar beliefs about intelligence and effort (Holloway,
1988; Rao et al., 2000). Thus, in the present study, data gained from both of these two countries
were combined to create a larger cultural group. Also it was thought that New Zealand and
American cultures would also hold similar beliefs about intelligence to each other. Western cul-
ture tends to conceptualize skill and knowledge acquisition in a different way to the Asian cul-
ture (Chao, 1996; Chen & Stevenson, 1995), so it is of interest to compare the prevalence and
manifestation of learned helplessness between these two broad cultural groups.
It has been widely recognized that Asian students and immigrants achieve at higher academic
levels than their American-born peers (Chen & Stevenson, 1995; Stevenson, Lee, Chen, Stigler,
Fan, & Ge, 1990; Tseng, 2004). Asian American students also evidence higher academic motiva-
tion, and according to Tseng (2004), this is due in part to an increased feeling of family obliga-
tions. High academic achievement of Asian students has also been attributed to an increased
emphasis on effort within these cultures (Lay & Tsai, 2005; Rao et al., 2000). Confucian doctrine
places great importance on the role that effort plays in achievement, and the internalization of
these ideas has been linked to increased academic achievement in Asian children and adolescents
(Chen & Stevenson, 1995). Diligence and effort are highlighted as the path to academic success
(Rao et al., 2000). Furthermore, effort is viewed as a moral principle, while laziness is seen as
immoral (Cheng, Lay, & Tseng, 2005). Chinese students are taught to work hard, even when the
probability of success is low, and to take responsibility for their academic achievement (Hau &
Salili, 1991).
Research exploring the Japanese population has revealed similar cultural effects to those of
Chinese populations. Japanese society also places a high value on effort as well as perseverance
(Holloway, 1988). Shimahara (1986) avers that there is a Japanese cultural tendency to focus on
at Victoria Univ of Wellington on February 6, 2013jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
1002 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 43(6)
the process rather than the outcome, resulting in an increased focus of the education system on
enhancing learning, motivation, and work habits. It is believed that all children are capable of
developing the ability to learn well. This belief leads Japanese parents and students to attribute
achievement to effort (Shimahara, 1986). Furthermore, because all children are capable of devel-
oping high intellectual ability through effort, schools avoid grouping students based on ability
and ignore IQ scores (Shimahara, 1986).
Not only do Asian parents endorse effort as a desirable behavior, but Asian students also
express a belief that effort is important in attaining high achievement or success (Hau & Salili,
1991; Rao et al., 2000). Lee (1996), for example, has stated that:
there is an extraordinary emphasis on effort, willpower or concentration of the mind in the
Confucian tradition. Because there is a strong belief in attainability by all, there is also a
strong belief that one’s failure is not due to one’s internal make-up or ability, but one’s effort
and willpower. (p. 39)
Both Asian parents and students have high expectations for child achievement, and Asian
students spend significantly more time studying outside of school than do their American peers
(Chen & Stevenson, 1995). Chen and Stevenson (1995) conclude that it is the beliefs and atti-
tudes of Asian students and their parents that result in high academic motivation and achieve-
ment. The beliefs of Japanese and Chinese parents documented in various studies seem to be
very similar to those described in the incremental theory of intelligence.
At the same time, academic achievement is also highly valued within Western cultures
(Dweck, 2006; Eccles et al., 1998). The Protestant ethic postulates that success is achieved with
strong effort and hard work, and many Westerners have adopted this directive as their guideline
for how to approach achievement situations. However, Dweck (1999) has noted that within
American samples, some adults and children attribute academic success at least partly to innate
ability (i.e., genetics), so it is possible that the role that effort plays in achieving these successes
may not be as strong for this culture as for Asian cultures. Although some preliminary cross-
cultural work (Zhou, Kwok, & Bond, 2009) has been performed on the link between reward for
application (effort) and academic achievement, the present authors are unaware of any quantita-
tive study comparing relative frequencies of effort versus ability explanations for academic
achievement between Asian and Western cultures. Consequently, we are not able to base our
present predictions on empirical findings; instead, we are chiefly relying on theoretical stances
and qualitative observations. In this vein, Stankov (2010), in a review on this topic, has said that
individuals from Confucian cultures hold “the belief that effort rather than ability is the primary
source of success” (p. 555) compared to those in Western and European cultures. Against this
diverse backdrop of theorists and piecemeal studies, it can be conjectured that the entity theory
of intelligence might be more relevant and learned helplessness more prevalent within Western
culture, whereas the incremental theory might be more important in explaining the achievement
motivation of Asian children.
The Goals of the Present Study
The current study was designed to explore the similarities and differences in the prevalence and
associations among the beliefs and behavior relevant to persistence and learned helplessness by
7- and 8-year-old children and their parents in Asian and Western cultures. Parents were asked
to complete questionnaires assessing their theory of intelligence, their own reactions to frustrat-
ing situations, and how they interact with their child when he or she is working on a difficult
task. The child participated in a learned helplessness paradigm in which he or she experienced
at Victoria Univ of Wellington on February 6, 2013jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Jose and Bellamy 1003
easy, medium, and difficult tasks that resulted in successes and failures. The child was asked
about his or her feelings concerning these outcomes, and their answers were coded as a single
measure of the bipolar construct of persistence versus learned helplessness.
The first aim of the current investigation was to explore the differences in the incidence of
persistence/learned helplessness across cultures. The first hypothesis was that Asian parents
would report more support for the incremental view of intelligence, more persistent behavior of
their own, and higher encouragement of children. In contrast, Western parents were expected to
report more support for the entity view, higher levels of expressed frustration, and less encour-
agement of children (Chao, 1996). The second hypothesis was that Western children (American
and New Zealanders) would display more helpless/less persistent behavior than children in the
two Asian cultures (Japanese and Chinese) (Dweck, 1999; Hess et al., 1987). No age or gender
differences were expected to be revealed for the current sample.
The second aim of this investigation was to better understand how parental views, values, and
behaviors predict children’s persistence/learned helplessness. The third prediction was that we
expected that parent endorsement of the incremental theory of intelligence would be a positive
predictor of child persistence/learned helplessness, and parent endorsement of the entity theory
would be a negative predictor of child persistence/learned helplessness. Furthermore, the rela-
tionships between parent theories of intelligence and the child outcome were expected to be
mediated through parent reactions to failure and parent encouragement (Elliot & Thrash, 2004;
Nolen-Hoeksema, Wolfson, Mumme, & Guskin, 1995); in particular, the fourth hypothesis was
that parental incremental views would be positively related to parental persistence and parental
encouragement and negatively related to parental reports of frustration, and these in turn would
predict children’s persistence. In contrast, parental endorsement of entity views should be nega-
tively related to parental reports of persistence and encouragement and positively related to
parental reports of frustration, and these in turn should predict children’s persistence. And last, a
review of previous literature does not allow for the development of hypotheses regarding cultural
differences in the strength of specific relationships between parent and child variables; however,
for Hypothesis 5, we thought that the incremental theory of intelligence would be more impor-
tant in explaining persistence for the Asian culture, and in contrast, the entity theory was expected
to be more influential in the Western culture.
Method
Participants
Data were collected from 197 children and parents living within New Zealand, the United States
of America, the People’s Republic of China, and Japan (see Table 1 for demographic informa-
tion). All children were aged between 7 and 8 years old, and on the critical variables of age and
gender, these four samples were very similar. This age group was selected because academic
motivation is a salient issue for children of this age (Bempechat, 1991), and this age range is one
that is typically studied in regards to academic motivation (e.g., Cain & Dweck, 1995; Hokoda
& Fincham, 1995).
The New Zealand sample was collected from four urban schools within the city of Wellington.
This sample was made up of 55 children (21 seven-year-olds and 34 eight-year olds) and one
parent of each child. All but one of these children had attended a preschool. Only one child
included in the New Zealand sample was not born in New Zealand. The ethnic breakdown of this
sample was 71% European New Zealanders, 11% Asian New Zealanders, 4% Maori Pacific
Nations, 6% Pakeha Pacific Nations, 3% Pakeha Maori, 3% Pacific Nations, and 3% Other. This
sample contains a higher proportion of Asian New Zealanders and slightly fewer Maori partici-
pants than is seen in the total population.
at Victoria Univ of Wellington on February 6, 2013jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
1004 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 43(6)
Table 1. Demographic Variables for Participants
Country
No. of
Participants
7-Year-Olds 8-Year-Olds
Male Female Total Male Female Total
New Zealand 55 15 11 26 14 15 29
America 39 12 11 23 7 9 16
China 46 12 12 24 10 12 22
Japan 57 19 17 36 11 10 21
Mean Age No. of Parents
Country 7-Year-Olds 8-Year-Olds Mothers Fathers Siblings Preschool (Years)
New Zealand 7.4 8.4 49 6 1.3 2.9
America 7.4 8.3 37 2 1.7 2.1
China 7.5 8.2 34 11 0 3.6
Japan 7.5 8.6 50 5 1.4 3.8
The American participants included 39 children and 39 parents. Twenty-three 7-year-old and
sixteen 8-year-old children were gathered from three schools located within the northern suburbs
of Chicago, Illinois. The ethnic makeup of this sample was 70% European American, 25% Asian
American, and 5% Hispanic American. The ethnic makeup of the American sample was not typi-
cal of the national population as it did not include any African American participants and it had
a higher percentage of Asian American participants.
The Chinese participants were gained from schools within the urban setting of Beijing. The
Chinese sample consisted of 24 seven-year-old and 22 eight-year-old students. The ethnicity of
all children within this sample was identified by parents as Chinese. Finally, the Japanese sample
was composed of 36 seven-year-old children and 21 eight-year-old children, obtained from the
city of Matsuyama. The ethnicity of all children within this sample was reported by parents as
Japanese.
The four samples differ somewhat on the reported demographic variables. The ethnic makeup
of the Western samples is more diverse than the Asian samples, and this fact reflects the multi-
cultural nature of New Zealand and America. In addition, none of the Chinese children reported
having siblings, which was the result of the “one child” policy in the People’s Republic of China.
Although it is desirable to gain samples that are comparable across various demographic vari-
ables, this was logistically impossible to achieve in the present case for all variables because we
collected data from four very different countries.
Materials
The data used in this research were gained from parental questionnaires and observations of
children participating in the learned helplessness protocol. Questionnaires were sent home to
parents, via their children, from schools. Parents were asked to complete the questionnaires, sign
a consent form for their child, and return them to the experimenters. The questionnaires were
originally written in English and then translated into the Chinese and Japanese languages by
bilingual speakers. They were then back-translated by other individuals, and the original and the
back-translated versions were compared. Minor differences in phrasing and vocabulary were
reconciled by the two translators.
at Victoria Univ of Wellington on February 6, 2013jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Jose and Bellamy 1005
Questionnaires. Parents were asked to complete three questionnaires designed for the present
study that assessed (a) parental reports of reactions to frustration, (b) parental reports of their
own reactions to the child’s frustration, and (c) parental beliefs about intelligence. Parents also
provided demographic information about their child and family. Parents also reported their edu-
cational attainment, job status, and languages spoken in the home.
(a) Parent reports of their own reactions to frustrating events. This questionnaire included 20
items scored on a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 = never felt this way to 5 = always felt this way).
Parents were asked to recall three frustrating events they had encountered and to consider how
they had responded in those situations when answering the questionnaire. Items included “I gave
up trying to finish the task because it was impossible” and “I remained calm and unemotional.”
For a complete list of all items and measures, please contact the first author.
(b) Parental responses to child frustration. This questionnaire measured how parents respond to
their child when he or she has encountered frustration. The questionnaire contained 16 items
measured on a 5-point Likert-type scale. Items included “Encourage him/her to keep on working
until he/she has succeeded” and “Do what I can to make the task easier.”
(c) Attitudes About Intelligence Scale. This questionnaire was designed by Dweck (1999) to
measure parental theories of intelligence. The measure allows the parent to endorse items tap-
ping the entity and incremental theories of intelligent. An example of an item measuring entity
beliefs is “To be honest, you can’t really change how intelligent you are.” An item measuring
incremental beliefs was “How much you practice determines how successful you are.” The ques-
tionnaire consisted of 20 items measured on a 5-point Likert-type scale. Ten items measured
entity beliefs, and 10 items measured incremental beliefs.
Learned Helplessness Protocol
After being brought to a quiet room, children were asked to participate in three different tasks.
These tasks were loosely based on the puzzle task used by Cain and Dweck (1995) to assess
learned helplessness in young children. Cain and Dweck’s (1995) task involved giving children
three unsolvable puzzles followed by one solvable puzzle. Following all four puzzles, children
were asked which one they would like to try again and why. In the current study, children were
asked to work on three different puzzle-like tasks (Memory, Wooden Pattern Blocks, and
Construct-O-Straws). We hoped that using three tasks instead of one would provide more reli-
able and valid data than would be obtained from just a single task. These three tasks were
school-related in that they assessed school-relevant abilities including memory, spatial reason-
ing, and task sequencing.
Within each of the three tasks the child encountered three increasingly levels of difficulty
(low, medium, and hard). The first level—low difficulty—was easy and all children were guar-
anteed success. The second level was of medium difficulty, and all children were ensured of
failure due to a time limit that prevented them from finishing the task (90 seconds). The third
level was of hard difficulty, and all children also failed this task because of its complexity and
time limit (2 minutes). The experimenter held a stopwatch and glanced at it frequently during the
child’s attempts to solve the puzzle. On the latter two tasks, if a child appeared to be close to
finishing the task within the given time limit, then their available time was covertly cut short, and
in these few cases children were apparently not aware that they were not given full time. The
order of difficulty was always easy, medium, and hard for all three tasks. Children were not told
of the difficulty of the tasks, but they were made aware of the time limits in each case.
Memory. This game involves a set of picture cards in which there are pairs of cards for each
picture. Children are presented with a set of cards containing a certain number of matched pairs
placed face down on the table in front of them. Children were asked to form pairs of matching
at Victoria Univ of Wellington on February 6, 2013jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
1006 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 43(6)
cards by turning over two cards at a time. If the two cards match, then they are placed to the side.
If they do not match, then the two cards are returned to their face down position and the child
selects two more cards to reveal. On the easy level, children were given four pairs of cards to
match, the medium level involved matching 10 pairs, and the final level contained 12 pairs.
Wooden Pattern Blocks. Children were presented with a box of small two-dimensional wooden
shapes as well as the outline of one of three pictures. They were asked to fill in the outline with
the provided wooden shapes. The outline presented on Level 1 was that of an arrow, Level 2
involved filling the outline of a horse, and a flower in a pot was the Level 3 picture.
Construct-O-Straws. For this task, children were given a previously constructed model that had
been built out of hollow plastic straws and plastic connector pieces. In addition, they were given
a pile of straws and connector pieces required to replicate the model. The participants were asked
to replicate the experimenter’s model exactly, using pieces provided to the child. The model used
for the first level was a flower made up of three petals. Level 2 required the construction of a dog,
and on the final level, children were asked to replicate a model of a man skiing.
Data Reduction
Factor analysis was conducted on the three parent questionnaires: Parent Reactions to Frustration,
Parent Responses to Child Frustration, and the Attitudes About Intelligence Scale. Factor analy-
sis was also conducted on the self-report items that constituted the measure of persistence/child
helplessness. These factor analyses were performed to reduce the number of measures and to create
homogeneous variables.
Parent reactions to frustrating events. A principal components factor analysis with varimax rota-
tion was conducted on the 20 items contained in the Parental Reports of Own Reactions to Frus-
tration scale. The factor analysis on this questionnaire suggested, on the basis of the scree plot,
examining correlations among and internal reliabilities of the derived factors, and parallel analy-
sis that two factors existed within these items. The first of these factors measured persistent
parental behavior. The seven items included statements such as “I kept trying because I was
convinced I could succeed” and “I saw the task as challenging and interesting.” The internal consis-
tency for this subfactor on the entire sample was acceptable (Cronbach’s α = .72). At the country
level, the internal consistency was consistently good across the four countries (α = .71 for
New Zealand, .76 for China, .81 for Japan, and .80 for the United States) and for the Asian and
Western cultures as well (αs = .80 and .75, respectively).
The second subfactor within this questionnaire measured parental expressions of frustration.
It contained eight items, which included “I expressed emotions of frustration” and “I yelled at
somebody else because of my frustration.” For the entire sample, the internal consistency for this
factor was acceptable (Cronbach’s α = .76) and remained acceptable for the four separate coun-
tries (α = .79 for New Zealand, .71 for China, .76 for Japan, and .72 for the United States) as well
as the two broad cultures (α = .71 for Asian and .77 for Western parents).
Parental reports of responses to child frustration. A principal components factor analysis
with varimax rotation was conducted on the 16 items of the Parent Responses to Child Frustration
questionnaire. These items assessed what parents taught their child about how to deal with dif-
ficult or frustrating tasks. Only one factor was found within this questionnaire. This factor mea-
sured parental encouragement of children and displayed high internal consistency for the entire
sample (Cronbach’s α = .84). The 10 items that made up this factor included “Encourage him/her
to keep on working until he/she has succeeded” and “Offer encouragement and emotional sup-
port.” The internal consistency was acceptable for each of the four countries (α = .71 for New
Zealand, .85 for China, .81 for Japan, and .77 for the United States) and for the Asian and
Western cultures as well (αs = .90 and .76, respectively).
at Victoria Univ of Wellington on February 6, 2013jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Jose and Bellamy 1007
Parent Attitudes About Intelligence Scale. A principal components factor analysis with varimax
rotation was conducted on the 20 items of this scale. We sought to verify whether splitting the
items of this questionnaire into two clusters based on the theory of intelligence was merited. The
factor analysis supported the predicted division of the items. The first factor measured parental
support of beliefs underlying the entity theory of intelligence and contained eight items. The
internal consistency for the entire sample on this factor was high (Cronbach’s α = .87). The inter-
nal consistency for individual countries was good (α = .90 for New Zealand, .78 for China, .89
for Japan, and .92 for the United States) as well as for the Asian and Western cultures broadly
(αs = .81 and .91, respectively).
The second factor contained within this questionnaire measured parental support of beliefs
underlying the incremental theory of intelligence. This factor contained 11 items that yielded an
internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .68) that can be termed nearly acceptable. The internal
consistencies for individual countries varied (α = .65 for New Zealand, .62 for China, .67 for
Japan, and .75 for the United States), and it was deemed to be low for the broad Asian culture (α =
.67 for the Asian culture and .70 for the Western culture). Despite the fact that some countries
yielded sub-.70 reliabilities, this construct was retained in the current study because of its theo-
retical importance.
Child persistence/learned helplessness. The child persistence/learned helplessness variable was
developed using self-reported choices and affective reports from the learned helplessness proto-
col. This variable assesses child persistence/learned helplessness on a bipolar scale, with high
scores indicating persistence and low scores being indicative of learned helplessness, and it was
modeled on procedures used in Cain and Dweck (1995) and Smiley and Dweck (1994). The vari-
able was created by averaging child responses from nine questions. Following each task, chil-
dren were asked to report how they felt about their performance using a 3-point scale: 1 = not
good, 2 = Ok, and 3 = very good. Child-reported feelings for the medium and hard levels (because
of the experience of failure) from each of the three tasks were included in this variable: Children
who exemplify the mastery orientation are more likely to report positive affect in the face of
failure (i.e., higher scores). In addition, once a child had attempted all three levels within a task,
they were asked which level they would like to repeat if there was more time. Task levels were
coded so that 1 indicated the easiest level (success), 2 the medium level (failure), and 3 the hard-
est level (failure). Cain and Dweck (1995) and other related studies by Dweck have coded choice
of the successful easy level as “learned helpless” and choice of a failed task as “mastery,” so in
this case low numbers indicated learned helplessness and high numbers indicated persistence.
This composite variable achieved high internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .79) for the entire
sample. However, the internal consistency varied by country and culture levels (α = .70 for
New Zealand, .71 for China, .71 for Japan, and .69 for the United States; and .84 for the Asian
culture and .70 for the Western culture). Although the internal consistency of this variable was
found in one case to be slightly below the accepted cut-off for internal reliability, this variable
was included in the present study because of its importance as an outcome variable. Scores from
the level children chose to repeat (3 items) and child-reported feelings on the medium and hard
levels of each task (6 items) were linearly combined to create the final variable.
Procedure
Schools were recruited from Beijing (China), Matsuyama (Japan), Chicago (USA), and
Wellington (New Zealand). All of the 7- and 8-year old children within schools who agreed to
participate were provided with information packets to take home to their parents. Information
packets included an information sheet, consent forms, and parent questionnaires. The children
included in this research returned a signed consent form from their parents or caregivers and
at Victoria Univ of Wellington on February 6, 2013jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
1008 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 43(6)
signed an assent form prior to participation as well. Ethical approval was granted by a University
Ethics Committee in New Zealand, and further approval was sought and obtained in each of the
other three countries from school boards and principals.
Once a parent had returned the questionnaire and the parental consent form, child interviews
were conducted. A research assistant native to the culture visited each school to interview chil-
dren individually. Children were told that they would be asked to participate in three different
games. Games were counterbalanced to control for order effects. Children were informed that
the experimenter was interested in understanding how children learn and that they should try
their best on each task.
After a child had attempted all three levels of a task, he or she was asked, “Which one do you
think was the easiest?” and “Which one do you think was the hardest?” as a manipulation check.
They were also asked to decide which of the levels they would like to try again if there was to be
extra time available at the end of the session. Following the child’s response to this question,
they were asked about why they had chosen to repeat that level rather than one of the other two.
These questions were designed to assess child persistence following frustration and failure. Once
each child had completed all three games, which took about 45 to 50 minutes on average, he or
she was provided with an oral debriefing and given a small school-related token of appreciation
(i.e., a pen or eraser). Parents did not receive compensation for their time.
Results
Group Differences in Parent Beliefs and Behavior: Hypothesis 1
Group differences in parent variables were examined by child age, child gender, and country.
Hypothesis 1 stated that Asian parents would report more support of the incremental theory of
intelligence, higher child encouragement, and more persistence. In contrast, Western parents
were expected to report more support to the entity theory, less child encouragement, and more
frustration than Asian parents.
A MANOVA was conducted, and the first notable result was a nonsignificant country main
effect for entity beliefs. On the other hand, a significant main effect for country was found for
endorsement of incremental beliefs, F(3, 160) = 14.15, p < .001. Contrary to predictions, a
Student-Newman-Keuls test revealed that American parents (M = 1.02, SD = .64) endorsed the
incremental theory of intelligence most strongly, followed by the other three groups: New
Zealand (M = .79, SD = .50), Chinese (M = .72, SD = .56), and Japanese (M = .63, SD = .46). No
significant differences were found between the last three groups. These findings are incongruent
with expectations, with the American culture reporting the highest support.
A significant main effect for country was found for parent encouragement of children,
F(3, 160) = 26.21, p > .001. A Student-Newman-Keuls test revealed that Chinese parents (M =
4.41, SD = .46) reported encouraging their children significantly more than did parents from
New Zealand (M = 3.64, SD = .52), America (M = 3.74, SD = .52), or Japan (M = 3.54, SD = .48).
No significant differences were found among the remaining three countries. These results pro-
vide partial support for the second hypothesis in that Chinese parents reported the highest level
of encouragement; however, the prediction that Japanese parents would also show high levels of
child encouragement was not supported.
A significant main effect for country was also found for parent frustration, F(3, 160) = 7.12,
p < .001. A Student-Newman-Keuls test revealed that New Zealand parents (M = 1.28, SD = .81)
reported significantly higher levels of expressed frustration than parents from any of the other
three countries. American parents (M = 1.00, SD = .60) showed the second highest level of
at Victoria Univ of Wellington on February 6, 2013jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Jose and Bellamy 1009
expressed frustration, but this was not significantly higher than the frustration expressed by
Japanese parents (M = .84, SD = .84). Chinese parents (M = .63, SD = .62) reported the lowest
level of expressed frustration, but this was not significantly different from that reported by
Japanese parents. These results provide partial support for the hypothesis that Western parents
would report higher amounts of expressed frustration than Asian parents.
No significant main effects or interactions were found for child gender or age across all parent
variables.
Group Differences in Child Manifestation of
Persistence/Helpless Behavior: Hypothesis 2
It was also hypothesized that the American and New Zealand children would display less
persistence/more helpless behavior than Chinese and Japanese children. Age and gender were
also included in this analysis; however, no significant differences were expected to be revealed
for these two variables and none were.
A three-way ANOVA was conducted on the dependent variable of child persistence/learned
helplessness (LH) where child age, child gender, and country were the three independent vari-
ables. A significant main effect was found for country, F(3, 165) = 39.93, p < .001. A Student-
Newman-Keuls test revealed a significant difference in persistent/LH behavior between China
(M = 2.72, SD = 0.32) and the two Western cultures (New Zealand and America). Chinese chil-
dren expressed significantly more persistent behavior than did New Zealand (M = 2.22, SD =
0.30) and American children (M = 2.17, SD = 0.32). Contrary to prediction, Japanese children
(M = 2.25, SD = 0.36) were not found to show significantly more persistent behavior than
Western children. This finding provides partial support for the hypothesis that Asian children
would show more persistent behavior than Western children. The prediction was confirmed for
Chinese children but not Japanese children.
Association Between Parent Endorsement of Incremental and Entity
Theories of Intelligence and Child Persistence/LH Behavior: Hypothesis 3
We expected to find that parental incremental theory would be positively predictive of child
persistence/LH behavior, and parental entity theory would be negatively predictive of child
persistence/LH behavior. A linear regression analysis for the overall sample yielded a significant
result for parental incremental theory (β = .21, p < .05), but parental entity theory did not predict
the child outcome (β = .06, p = .59). This result is taken as partial support for this hypothesis
because one of the two predictors was related to the outcome in the expected direction.
Mediation of the Relationships Between Parent Theories
of Intelligence and Child Persistence/LH: Hypothesis 4
We expected that parental incremental views would be positively predictive of parental persistence/
LH and parental encouragement and negatively related to parental reports of frustration, and
these in turn would predict children’s persistence/LH. On the other side, parental endorsement
of entity views were expected to be negatively predictive of parental reports of persistence and
encouragement and positively related to parental reports of frustration, and these in turn were
expected to predict children’s persistence/LH.
To conduct path model analysis, the SEM program AMOS 18 (Arbuckle, 2009) was employed.
The initial predicted model was constructed as described above, but it proved to be a poor fitting
at Victoria Univ of Wellington on February 6, 2013jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
1010 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 43(6)
Parent
endorsement of
incremental theory
Parent report of
own
persistence
Parent
endorsement of
enty theory
Parent report of
own
frustraon
Parent report of
encouragement
of child
Child
Persistence/LH
.15*
.43***
-.14*
-.20**
.20**
-.18*
Figure 1. Mediation of the Relationship Between Parent Theories of Intelligence and Child Persistence
by Parent-Reported Emotional Reaction Styles and Encouragement of Their Child for the Overall Sample
Note. N = 197. Standardized regression coefficients (betas) are presented to indicate strength of relationships.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
model, so Hypothesis 4 in its most detailed and stringent sense was not supported. The next step
was to use model pruning to obtain a data-based model (see Kline, 2005, for a description of this
technique). A model obtained in this fashion is ad hoc and cannot be considered to be definitive
in terms of testing theory, but it can be useful for suggesting future directions in research efforts.
In this technique, nonsignificant paths were pruned from the model until all remaining paths
proved to be statistically significant. In this way, a good fitting model was obtained, χ
2
(8) = 3.73,
p = .88, χ
2
to df ratio = .47, NFI = .95, CFI = .99, RMSEA = .01, Critical N = 814. Standardized
regression correlation coefficients are reported in Figure 1 to facilitate interpretation.
For the overall sample, parent persistence did not seem to mediate the relationship between
parent incremental theory and child persistence/LH behavior (i.e., no link was found to exist
between parent incremental theory and parent persistence), but parent encouragement of child
did seem to function as a mediator between these two variables. Parent incremental theory pre-
dicted parent encouragement (β = .20, p < .01), and parent encouragement, in turn, predicted
child persistence/LH behavior (β = .15, p < .05). A bootstrapped evaluation of the indirect effect
was performed in AMOS, and although the size of the standardized indirect effect was not large
(.03), it did yield a significant 95% bias-corrected confidence interval result (lower = .01 and
upper = .06).
And although no significant base relationship was noted between parent entity theory and
child persistence/LH behavior (see section above), it seems that parent entity theory might have
exerted a weak influence on the outcome variable through reduced parent persistence and reduced
parent encouragement. As before, a bootstrapped evaluation of the indirect effect was performed
in AMOS, and although the size of the standardized indirect effect was smaller than in the previ-
ous analysis (-.01), it did yield a significant 95% bias-corrected confidence interval result (lower =
-.01 and upper = -.03). On balance, this resulting path model provides partial support for the
predicted mediational roles of parent persistence and parent encouragement, but it must be
admitted that the sizes of the indirect effects were small.
at Victoria Univ of Wellington on February 6, 2013jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Jose and Bellamy 1011
Parent
endorsement of
incremental theory
Parent report of
own
persistence
Parent
endorsement of
enty theory
Parent report of
own
frustraon
Parent report of
encouragement
of child
Child
persistence/LH
.32***
.03
NS
.32***
.12
NS
.22***
-.09
NS
.45***
.37***
-.25**
.12
NS
-.36***
-.07
NS
-.04
-.20**
Figure 2. Significant Differences Between the Asian and Western Groups in the Relationships Between
Parent Beliefs, Parent-Reported Behavior, and Encouragement of Child on Child Persistence/LH
Note. Asian cultural group: N = 103; Western cultural group: N = 94. Standardized regression coefficients (betas) are
presented to indicate strength of relationships. The beta on top signifies the estimated parameter for the Asian group
and the beta on the bottom signifies the estimated parameter for the Western group.
ns
p > .05. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
Comparisons of the Relationships Between Parent Theories of Intelligence
and Child Mastery Within the Asian and Western Cultures: Hypothesis 5
The overall dataset was disaggregated into two cultural groups: Asian (Japan and China) and
Western (New Zealand and the United States). We began with the original predicted model and
performed a two group run. The overall model fit was not adequate, so, in line with the method
described above, we pruned parameters that were nonsignificant for both cultural groups until
we obtained the resulting model, χ
2
(15) = 19.99, p = .17, χ
2
/df ratio = 1.33, IFI = .95, CFI = .94,
RMSEA = .04, Critical N = 245. Follow-up equality contrasts between the two groups yielded
7 significant differences in the model (see Figure 2), all at p < .05. We predicted that incremen-
tal views would play a stronger role in Asian families than entity views, and the resulting path
model supports this hypothesis. Incremental beliefs were found to have an indirect effect on
child persistence/LH behavior through the mediators of mother persistence and mother encour-
agement. In notable contrast, only one of these four links (i.e., parent persistence to parent
encouragement) for Western parents was statistically significant. In addition, we found that
Asian parents’ expression of frustration with difficult situations was significantly and negatively
predictive of parent encouragement, which suggests that Asian parents encouraged their children
more if they individually experienced frustration in these situations. And last, parent entity
theory beliefs for Asian parents did not impart any significant impact on child persistence/LH
behavior. In summary, the obtained model supports the view that incremental theory beliefs
were more strongly associated with the child outcome for Asian compared to Western parents.
It was also predicted that Western parents would evidence a stronger web of relationships in
the lower half of the model, but we obtained only scant evidence for this prediction. As pre-
dicted, entity views exerted an indirect effect on child persistence/LH through lessened parent
persistence (β = -.20, p < .01) and parent encouragement, but this effect was slight. The resulting
model does not support the view that entity theory beliefs were more strongly associated with the
child outcome for Western compared to Asian parents.
at Victoria Univ of Wellington on February 6, 2013jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
1012 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 43(6)
Discussion
The current investigation was designed to assess cultural differences in the incidence and char-
acteristics of persistence/learned helplessness in children and to evaluate whether cultural dif-
ferences exist in a pathway of mediation between parent theories of intelligence and child
persistence/LH. First, results showed that Chinese children, as expected, were more persistent
in the learned helplessness paradigm than Western children, but Japanese children did not evi-
dence the same elevated level of persistence. Second, support was obtained for the prediction
that parents who endorsed higher levels of the incremental theory beliefs would have children
who would be more persistent, but no (negative) relationship was obtained between parent entity
theory beliefs and child persistence. Third, the predicted mediation pathway from parent theory
beliefs to child persistence/LH through parent persistence and parent encouragement of child
was partially supported: parent encouragement was seen to function as a mediator for the entire
sample, but parent persistence did not. And fourth, examination of differences in the path model
supported the prediction that parent incremental beliefs would play a stronger role in predicting
child persistence/LH for Asian families than for Western families. Each of these findings will
be discussed below.
In the present study, the Chinese children evidenced significantly higher levels of persistence
than the American and New Zealand children. While higher persistence was also expected from
the Japanese children, we did not find this result. Due to a cultural emphasis on hard work, aca-
demic success, and effort (Rao et al., 2000; Shimahara, 1986), it was predicted in the present
study that children within both Asian cultures would be considerably more persistent than
Western children. One reason for why the Japanese children more closely resembled the Western
children than the Chinese children may be that Japan has been considerably more exposed to
Western influences than has China. At the end of World War II, Japan was occupied by American
troops and has since been heavily exposed to Western media and ideas (Morton & Olenik, 2004).
This situation contrasts with that in China, which in the period between the Communist victory
in 1949 and the economic liberalization of the last 20 years has been slow to embrace Western
ideas, an avoidance which has only begun to lift in recent years (Wang, Huters, & Karl, 2006). It
is also possible that our Japanese sample was not typical of the Japanese population in that we
may have inadvertently selected more Westernized families.
When considering the group differences in parent variables across countries, it was expected
that Asian parents would report higher support of the incremental theory of intelligence and
encouragement. In contrast, Western parents were expected to report higher rates of entity theory
and expressed frustration. This hypothesis was expected because Asian parents have been seen
to place more emphasis on effort and believe that with hard work all children can achieve aca-
demically (Hess et al., 1987; Rao et al., 2000; Shimahara, 1986). As predicted, it was parents
from the two Western countries who reported the highest expression of frustration. However, it
was only the Chinese parents who reported providing their children with more encouragement
than New Zealand and American parents. Unexpectedly, Japanese parents reported levels similar
to the Western participants. Furthermore, against expectations, it was parents from the two
Western countries who reported the highest support for the incremental theory, and no country
differences were noted for parental entity beliefs. It seems that the simple Asian-effort versus
Western-ability prediction was not supported, and this result suggests that further theory and
empirical study of cross-cultural differences and similarities of these two dimensions are war-
ranted. Examination of the covariance relationships was expected to yield more interpretable
results, and in fact the path models did yield more support for the predictions.
The third prediction was partially supported: As expected, parents who endorsed the incre-
mental view had children who persisted more in the learned helplessness procedure. The main
at Victoria Univ of Wellington on February 6, 2013jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Jose and Bellamy 1013
objective in assessing both parent and child variables was to explore the idea that child persistent/
LH behavior is linked to parent theories of intelligence and behavior. Helpless behavior seems to
develop early, as it has been observed not only in primary school children but also preschool
children (Cain & Dweck, 1995; Cole et al., 2006; Smiley & Dweck, 1994). Parental beliefs about
intelligence, behavior, and academic goals have been shown to influence child grades and aca-
demic achievement (Elliot & Thrash, 2004; Gutman, 2006; Smiley & Lee, 2005). Socialization
of children by parents may be responsible for this association.
On the other hand, parental endorsement of entity theory views was not predictive of persis-
tent behavior/LH in children. In retrospect we would suggest that this non-relationship may be
due to the nature of the outcome measure. The child persistence/LH measure was constituted of
two components: self-reports of affect after failing a task and choice of difficulty level of an
anticipated re-doing of the puzzle (following procedures used in Cain & Dweck, 1995; Smiley &
Dweck, 1994). These two sources of data correlated with each other well—that is, a better
Cronbach’s alpha was obtained with all of these items rather than choice of difficulty level alone,
but it may be that this composite variable represented the presence of persistence more than the
lack of learned helplessness. Although persistence and helplessness are often construed as bipo-
lar opposites, and we followed this view in the present article, it is possible that they are not.
Future work would do well to determine whether persistence and helplessness are opposite ends
of the same continuum because this has implications for studies such as the present one.
The fourth hypothesis sought to examine whether parental reports of reactions to difficult
situations (i.e., persistence or frustration) and parental reports of encouragement of their children
would mediate between endorsements of the two parent theories of intelligence and the child
outcome. Research on this topic is scarce but Eccles et al. (1989, see p. 1054) have proposed a
general model of parental influences on children’s motivation and achievement that is very rel-
evant here: Parental endorsement of theory beliefs would be located in their box entitled “par-
ents’ general beliefs and behavior,” and parental reports of reactions to difficult situations and
parental reports of encouragement would be located in their box entitled “parent-specific behav-
ior.” Research specifically focused on some of the measures taken here have been performed by
Elliot and Thrash (2004) and Nolen-Hoeksema et al. (1995), and this work suggests that investi-
gating the mechanisms by which parental beliefs affect child behavior would be fruitful. Of
course, since we do not have longitudinal data in the present case, we cannot say that parental
theory beliefs caused parents to behave in certain ways (with themselves or with their children),
and these behaviors in turn caused children to behave in particular ways. However, the findings
obtained in Figure 1 suggest, at the very least, that parental views are related to parental self-
reports of behaviors, and these in turn are related to child behavior. We consider these findings
to be an early step toward unpacking the “black box” of socialization—namely how parental
views and behaviors concerning the acquisition of knowledge and skills lead to certain child
outcomes in a school achievement-type situation—and it is heartening that two significant (albeit
small) mediations were obtained in the predicted model.
And then finally, we predicted that the incremental theory of intelligence would be more
important in explaining academic motivation for the Asian culture, and it was hypothesized that
the entity theory would be more important for the Western culture. The path model results pro-
vided support for the first part of this hypothesis but not the second part. As predicted, the media-
tional pathways originating with incremental theory beliefs and passing through parent
persistence and parent encouragement and ending with child persistence were all statistically
significant for the Asian sample, but only one of these four links was significant for the Western
sample. Asian cultures place a large amount of emphasis on the role of effort in academic success
(Chen & Stevenson, 1995; Rao et al., 2000), and it is seen to be the job of the parent to instruct,
model, and encourage persistent behavior in various settings, and particularly in the academic
at Victoria Univ of Wellington on February 6, 2013jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
1014 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 43(6)
domain. Hence, the finding that persistent behavior of Asian students is more effectively
explained through parental support of the incremental theory than the entity theory is congruent
with our prediction. This finding supports the argument made by Lay and Tsai (2005) that the
achievement motivation of Asian students would be better explained through beliefs about effort
rather than ability.
All of the relationships revealed within the Asian model were in the predicted direction. A
body of research has established a positive connection between endorsement of the incremental
theory of intelligence and persistent behavior among children (Diener & Dweck, 1980; Dweck
& Leggett, 1988; Mueller & Dweck, 1998). Furthermore, support of the entity theory has consis-
tently displayed negative consequences for motivation (Anderson & Jennings, 1980; Dweck &
Leggett, 1988; Henderson & Dweck, 1990; Stipek & Gralinski, 1996). These findings have been
found to be the case for child samples, and some research has supported these predictions for
adults (Dweck, 1999). However, almost all of this research has been conducted with European
American samples (e.g., Cain & Dweck, 1995; Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 1995; Smiley & Lee,
2005; Stipek & Gralinski, 1996). Nevertheless, the relationships displayed within the Asian path
model indicate that the incremental theory of intelligence held by Asian parents is predictive of
child mastery behavior, just as one would expect based on Western research. While incremental
theory beliefs seem to be a significant predictive factor within the Asian culture, this does not
appear to be the case for entity theory beliefs. Parent support of the entity theory for Asian par-
ents was found to be irrelevant in predicting child behavior.
In contrast, little support was obtained for the proposed emphasis on parent entity beliefs by
Western parents. We found that incremental and entity beliefs were more strongly negatively
correlated for Asian parents (r = -.36, p < .001) than for Western parents (r = -.07, ns). It seems,
then, that Asian parents think of incremental and entity views as more opposite than do Western
parents, who apparently view them as more orthogonal. Many New Zealand and American par-
ents reported high levels of both incremental and entity views, so they seemed to view them as
not as incompatible as the Asian parents did.
In sum, the obtained findings suggest that a simple view that Asian parents and children
endorse higher levels of incremental views of knowledge and skill acquisition whereas Western
parents and children endorse higher levels of entity views does not seem to be supported. A large
amount of theory and qualitative investigation would have one believe that Asian parents and
children hold and implement incremental-type beliefs (e.g., effort and persistence) to a greater
extent than Western parents and children, but little support was found for this view. The present
results are important because it should serve to motivate researchers to try to empirically verify
this long-accepted truism about Asian people.
The second important outcome of the present research is that significant and interpretable
differences in the association between parental variables and child persistence/LH were found.
The combined Japanese/Chinese sample evidenced a coherent trail of correlated variables begin-
ning from parental endorsement of incremental views through parental reports of their own
behavior to children’s persistence/LH, and the Western sample did not. So although the mean
group differences analyses did not verify that Asian parents endorsed incremental views to a
greater extent than Western parents, the final path model persuasively argues that they evidence
a more integrated and cohesive set of linkages between parental views and child behavior. These
two sets of findings taken together suggest that incremental views of intelligence (and related
parent-reported behaviors) seem to be more salient and coordinated for Asian cultures compared
to Western cultures, reflecting important cultural values and beliefs in these cultures, but Asian
parents and children do not necessarily endorse incremental views to a higher extent than Western
parents and children on a fixed Likert-type scale. Further work will be necessary to tease apart
this paradox.
at Victoria Univ of Wellington on February 6, 2013jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Jose and Bellamy 1015
Limitations of the Present Study
It is recognized that the sample sizes used in the current investigation are small and the samples
may not be entirely representative of the respective countries. Statistical analysis was limited
due to the small samples gained from each culture and country, and power analyses indicated
that only medium-sized effects would have been identified with country-level analyses. Larger
samples would have strengthened the obtained results and would have made it possible to ana-
lyze the relationships between parent and child variables at the country level. Some of the vari-
ables, including the outcome variable of child persistence/LH, exhibited low internal reliability,
so further development of self-report and behavioral measures would ensure more veridical
measurement of the relevant constructs. Model testing using SEM failed to confirm the pre-
dicted models, so the data-based models described here should be seen as suggestive signposts
for future work: Replication is needed to determine if these obtained relationships are replicable.
The current study only measured the beliefs and behaviors of one parent for each child. It
would be of interest to explore the effects that both parents have on child theories of intelligence.
Such research should consider whether dyads of parents are likely to hold similar beliefs about
intelligence. Future work could also involve siblings and determine whether siblings support the
same theory. If not, then the individual factors that protect children from or predispose them to
developing helpless patterns of behavior could be explored. Consideration of teacher beliefs
about intelligence may also be important in explaining the development of child theories of intel-
ligence and motivation. Future work would benefit from assessing children’s theories of intelli-
gence to determine how parental variables impact on both these as well as persistent/LH behavior.
Recent work by Dweck (2006) on “mindsets” provokes a need, as well, to examine how fixed
and growth mindsets map onto learned helplessness and persistence not just for children but also
for children in different cultures around the globe.
In the current investigation, data gained from the Chinese and Japanese cultures were com-
bined to create a broader Asian sample. This was done because previous literature has indicated
that these two cultures hold similar beliefs about academic achievement, effort, and intelligence
(Chen & Stevenson, 1995; Holloway, 1988; Rao et al., 2000; Shimahara, 1986). Although little
research has included New Zealand participants, it was assumed that the New Zealand and
American cultures would also hold similar beliefs to each other about effort and intelligence.
However, is it important to recognize that these are four separate and distinct cultures, and we
obtained evidence here to suggest that the Chinese and Japanese samples were not similar in all
regards. Furthermore, research involving greater numbers of Western and Asian countries would
be useful to determine commonalities within and differences between these two broad groups.
The path models were conducted on concurrent data so the predictive flow of supposed causal
patterns cannot be definitively supported or rejected. It may be the case, for example, that child
behavior influences parental theories of intelligence instead of the direction postulated here.
Longitudinal research is essential to clarify the direction of causal relations.
Conclusion
A cross-cultural comparison of persistence/learned helplessness was conducted to explore dif-
ferences and similarities in child behavior for the Asian and Western cultures. Although, as
expected, Chinese children displayed higher levels of persistent behavior than New Zealand and
American children, an unexpected finding was that the Japanese sample behaved similarly to the
Western sample. This result may reflect a stronger presence of and openness to Western influ-
ences within Japan compared to China. The most striking result in this study was the obtained
differences between the Western and Asian path models with regard to parent-reported incremental
at Victoria Univ of Wellington on February 6, 2013jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
1016 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 43(6)
theory beliefs and associated behaviors. As predicted, parent support of the incremental views
led to greater child persistence (and lower LH) through the mediators of parent persistence and
parent encouragement for Asian parents but not for Western parents. These results suggest that
Asian parents believe in, model, and behave according to a different motivational schema than
do Western parents.
Acknowledgments
Appreciation is expressed to Mariko Fukui for collecting data and to Dr. Carol Huntsinger and Dr. Hou
Yubo for assisting in the collection of data.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or
publication of this article.
Funding
The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
References
Ames, C., & Archer, J. (1987). Mothers’ beliefs about the role of ability and effort in school learning. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 79(4), 409-414.
Anderson, C. A., & Jennings, D. L. (1980). When experiences of failure promote expectations of success:
The impact of attributing failure to ineffective strategies. Journal of Personality, 48(3), 393-407.
Arbuckle, J. L. (2009). AMOS Ver. 18.0. Crawsfordville, FL: AMOS Development Corporation.
Au, R. C. P., Watkins, D. A., & Hattie, J. A. C. (2010). Academic risk factors and deficits of learned
helplessness: A longitudinal study of Hong Kong secondary school students. Educational Psychology,
30(2), 125-138.
Bempechat, J. (1991). Children’s conceptions of ability in major domains: An interview and experimental
study. Child Study Journal, 21(1), 11-36.
Cain, K. M., & Dweck, C. S. (1995). The relation between motivational patterns and achievement cogni-
tions through the elementary school years. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 41(1), 25-52.
Chao, R. K. (1996). Chinese and European American mothers’ beliefs about the role of parenting in chil-
dren’s school success. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 27(4), 403-423.
Chen, C., & Stevenson, H. W. (1995). Motivation and mathematics achievement: A comparative study of
Asian-American, Caucasian-American, and East Asian high school students. Child Development, 66,
1215-1234.
Cheng, Y., Lay, K., & Tseng, H. (2005). Is industry an essence of morality among Chinese adolescents?
Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development Biennial Meeting, Atlanta, GA.
Cole, D. A., Warren, D. E., Dallaire, D. H., Lagrange, B., Travis, R., & Ciesla, J. A. (2007). Early predic-
tors of helpless thoughts and behaviors in children: Developmental precursors to depressive cognitions.
Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 12(2), 295-312.
Craske, M. L. (1985). Improving persistence through observational learning and attribution retraining. British
Journal of Educational Psychology, 55, 138-147.
De Bruyn, E. H., Dekovic, M., & Meijnen, G. W. (2003). Parenting, goal orientations, classroom behavior,
and school success in early adolescence. Applied Developmental Psychology, 24, 393-412.
Diener, C. I., & Dweck, C. S. (1978). An analysis of learned helplessness: Continuous changes in per-
formance, strategy, and achievement cognitions following failure. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 36(5), 451-462.
Diener, C. I., & Dweck, C. S. (1980). An analysis of learned helplessness: II. The processing of success.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(5), 940-952.
at Victoria Univ of Wellington on February 6, 2013jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Jose and Bellamy 1017
Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia:
Psychology Press.
Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of success. New York: Random House.
Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psycho-
logical Review, 95(2), 256-273.
Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motivation to succeed. In W. Damon (Series Ed.)
and N. Eisenberg (Volume Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (5th ed., pp. 1017-1095). New York:
John Wiley and Sons.
Elliott, E. S., & Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: An approach to motivation and achievement. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 54(1), 5-12.
Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2004). The intergenerational transmission of fear of failure. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(8), 957-971.
Gutman, L. M. (2006). How student and parent goal orientations and classroom goal structures influence
the math achievement of African Americans during the high school transition. Contemporary Educa-
tional Psychology, 31, 44-63.
Hau, K., & Salili, F. (1991). Structure and semantic differential placement of specific causes: Academic
causal attributions by Chinese students in Hong Kong. International Journal of Psychology, 26(2),
175-193.
Henderson, V. L., & Dweck, C. S. (1990). Motivation and achievement. In S. Feldman & G. Elliott (Eds.),
At the threshold: The developing adolescent (pp. 308-329). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Hess, R. D., Chang, C., & McDevitt, T. M. (1987). Cultural variations in family beliefs about children’s
performance in mathematics: Comparisons among People’s Republic of China, Chinese-American, and
Caucasian-American families. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79(2), 179-188.
Heyman, G. D., Dweck, C. S., & Cain, K. M. (1992). Young children’s vulnerability to self-blame and
helplessness: Relationship to beliefs about goodness. Child Development, 63, 401-415.
Hokoda, A., & Fincham, F. D. (1995). Origins of children’s helpless and mastery achievement patterns in
the family. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87(3), 375-385.
Holloway, S. D. (1988). Concepts of ability and effort in Japan and the United States. Review of Educa-
tional Research, 58(3), 327-345.
Kamins, M. L., & Dweck, C. S. (1999). Person versus process praise and criticism: Implications for contin-
gent self-worth and coping. Developmental Psychology, 35(3), 835-847.
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practices of structural equation modelling (2nd ed.). New York:
Guilford Press.
Lay, K.- L., & Tsai, Y.- M. (2005). Moral, pragmatic, and entity concepts of academic effort among Chinese
adolescents: A developmental perspective. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Develop-
ment Biennial Meeting, Atlanta, GA.
Lee, W. O. (1996). The cultural context for Chinese learners: Conceptions of learning in the Confucian
tradition. In D. A. Watkins & B. Biggs (Eds.), The Chinese learner: Cultural, psychological and con-
textual influences (pp. 25-41). Hong Kong and Melbourne; Comparative Education Research Centre
and Australian Council for Educational Research.
Morton, W. S., & Olenik, J. K. (2004). Japan: Its history and culture (4th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Mueller, C. M., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Praise for intelligence can undermine children’s motivation and
performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(1), 33-52.
Nenty, H. J., & Ogwu, N. E. (2009). Influence of gender and learned helplessness on some mathematics-
related cognitive behavior of Lesotho senior secondary school students. Gender & Behavior, 7(1),
2123-2135.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wolfson, A., Mumme, D., & Guskin, K. (1995). Helplessness in children of depressed
and nondepressed mothers. Developmental Psychology, 31(3), 377-387.
at Victoria Univ of Wellington on February 6, 2013jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
1018 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 43(6)
Peterson, C. (1995). Entity and incremental world views: Some lessons from learned helplessness theory
and research. Psychological Inquiry, 6(4), 307-311.
Pomerantz, E. M., & Dong, W. (2006). Effects of mothers’ perceptions of children’s competence: The mod-
erating role of mothers’ theories of competence. Developmental Psychology, 42(5), 950-961.
Rao, N., Noely, B. E., & Sachs, J. (2000). Motivational beliefs, study strategies, and mathematics attain-
ment in high and low-achieving Chinese secondary school students. Contemporary Educational Psy-
chology, 25, 287-316.
Shimahara, N. K. (1986). The cultural basis of student achievement in Japan. Comparative Education,
22(1), 19-25.
Smiley, P. A., & Dweck, C. S. (1994). Individual differences in achievement goals among young children.
Child Development, 65, 1723-1743.
Smiley, P. A., & Lee, A. R. (2005). Parent beliefs about intelligence and learning processes. Paper pre-
sented at the Society for Research in Child Development Biennial Meeting, Atlanta, GA.
Stankov, L. (2010). Unforgiving Confucian culture: A breeding ground for high academic achievement, test
anxiety and self-doubt? Learning and Individual Differences, 20(6), 555-563.
Stevenson, H. W., Lee, S., Chen, C., Stigler, J., Fan, L., & Ge, F. (1990). Mathematics achievement of
children in China and the United States. Child Development, 61, 1053-1066.
Stipek, D., & Gralinski, J. H. (1996). Children’s beliefs about intelligence and school performance. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 88(3), 397-407.
Storey, A. H., Jr. (2008). The impact of biculturalism on human learned helplessness with Northern Plains
Native Americans. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of North Dakota.
Tseng, V. (2004). Family interdependence and academic adjustment in college: Youth from immigrant and
U.S.-born families. Child Development, 75(3), 966-983.
Wang, H., Huters, T., & Karl, R. E. (2006). China’s new order: Society, politics, and economy in transition.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Schiefele, U., Rosser, R. W., & Davis-Kean, P. (2006). Development of achieve-
ment motivation. In W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (6th ed., pp. 278-313). New York:
John Wiley.
Zhou, F., Kwok, L., & Bond, M. H. (2009). Social axioms and achievement across cultures: The influence
of reward for application and fate control. Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 366-371.
Ziegler, A., Finsterwald, M., & Grassinger, R. (2005). Predictors of learned helplessness among average
and mildly gifted girls and boys attending initial high school physics instruction in Germany. The Gifted
Child Quarterly, 49(1), 7-18.
at Victoria Univ of Wellington on February 6, 2013jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
... Prior studies have documented significant direct links between parental mindsets and students' academic outcomes such as persistence (e.g., Cheng et al., 2017;Jose & Bellamy, 2012;Song et al., 2022) and achievement (e.g., Matthes & Stoeger, 2018;Song et al., 2022). For example, in a correlational study involving first, second, and third graders and their parents, Song et al. (2022) found that parents' growth mindset was positively associated with their children's persistence in learning, even after controlling for their socioeconomic status. ...
... For example, in a correlational study involving first, second, and third graders and their parents, Song et al. (2022) found that parents' growth mindset was positively associated with their children's persistence in learning, even after controlling for their socioeconomic status. Likewise, Jose and Bellamy (2012) observed that parental growth mindset positively predicted their children's persistence following failure. Matthes and Stoeger (2018) also reported that parental growth mindset positively predicted the academic achievement of their fourth-grade children, both directly and indirectly through the increase in children's growth mindset and the decrease in unconstructive parenting practices such as controlling behavior and homework-related conflict with their children. ...
... These results suggest that there could be some overlapping mechanisms underlying these relationships. We strongly suspect overt parenting behaviors to be one such mechanism (Jose & Bellamy, 2012;Matthes & Stoeger, 2018) as illustrated in Fig. 1, but future research should ascertain these mediating paths by assessing all relevant factors in the same study. ...
Article
Full-text available
Parents’ beliefs about the nature of ability are communicated to their children through parent–child interactions. Parental mindsets are one of the parental beliefs that have received increasing attention over recent years. However, their role in children’s motivation and achievement outcomes remains relatively underexplored. Moreover, most existing studies have measured parental mindsets as either reported by parents themselves or perceived by children only, making it difficult to develop a comprehensive understanding of their role. Yet, parents’ mindsets and children’s perceptions of their parents’ mindsets may not be identical, and one may be more important than the other in shaping children’s motivation and achievement. Using the data from 507 third- and fourth-graders and their parents in Korea, we examined how parents’ growth and fixed mindsets, both self-reported and child-perceived, predicted children’s achievement goals, persistence, and achievement in mathematics. Whereas the growth and fixed mindsets of parents correlated negatively with each other, there was no significant relationship between self-reported and child-perceived parental mindsets. Parents’ growth mindset positively predicted children’s mastery goals. In contrast, parents’ fixed mindset negatively predicted children’s mastery goals and positively predicted children’s ability-focused and normative performance goals. Children’s mastery goals in turn positively predicted their persistence and achievement. The indirect paths from parental mindsets to children’s persistence and achievement through children’s mastery goals were also significant. In general, the child-perceived mindsets of parents demonstrated a stronger predictive power than the self-reported mindsets of parents. The underexplored role of parental mindsets is discussed concerning children’s achievement goal adoption and learning.
... Most extant work on mindsets in family contexts explores the impact of parents' mindsets about intelligence. For example, parents with stronger growth mindsets of intelligence report fewer homework-related conflicts and fewer controlling parenting behaviors (Matthes and Stoeger 2018) and are more likely to implore their children to persist on difficult tasks (Jose and Bellamy 2012). Additionally, parents' mindsets of intelligence predict their child's achievement goals and are related to their child's mental health (Muenks et al. 2015). ...
... That is, do parents believe they can change their child's beliefs? While much research has focused on parents' mindsets about their child's intelligence (e.g., Jose and Bellamy 2012;Matthes and Stoeger 2018;Muenks et al. 2015), less attention has been given to parents' beliefs about their own ability to influence their child's mindsets. Overall, the current work is limited in scope but can hopefully inform future inquiry into multiple types of parenting beliefs. ...
Article
Parenting can bring both joy and stress. Identifying factors that foster greater engagement and enjoyment while also helping parents cope with inevitable stressors is critical for healthy child development and parental wellbeing. In the current work, we build on growth mindset theory to explore individual differences in beliefs about the changeable nature of parenting ability. Specifically, across three correlational studies ( N = 1170), we investigated if growth mindsets about parenting related to coping in the wake of parenting setbacks and to parental engagement. Growth mindsets predicted more positive expectations and less avoidant coping after parenting challenges, and these processes, and growth mindsets, correlated with parental engagement. Across the three studies, average links between growth mindsets and the two primary outcomes of interest—avoidant coping and engagement—were r = −0.31 and r = 0.20, respectively.
... Researchers also found that parents and teachers with a growth mindset may not simply pass it on to children or students (Haimovitz & Dweck, 2017). In other words, children's growth mindset might not naturally come from their parents or teachers, but rather from supporting behaviors, like encouragement (Jose & Bellamy, 2012), process-focused teaching (Sun, 2015), and positive response to failure (Haimovitz & Dweck, 2016). Here, parental support for children's PA may serve as a bridge for transmitting parents' mindset of fitness to their children. ...
... Parents may convey their mindset of fitness to their children through PA support, similar evidence can be found in earlier studies on growth mindset in the teaching and parenting setting, including modeling (Navarro, 2020), encouragement (Jose & Bellamy, 2012), and providing opportunities (Kroeper et al., 2022). Apart from these already-known elements, parental activity support in our study, as introduced in the measurement part, also included parental limits on the time their child spent in sedentary behaviors and the monitoring of sedentary behaviors, which has been found to be positively related to children's PA behavior (Arredondo et al., 2006). ...
... Although cross-cultural work on mindsets is somewhat limited, one recent study found that compared to Chinese students, US American students tended to espouse stronger growth mindsets [54]. Other work has similarly found stronger growth mindsets among (a) teachers raised in the UK (versus teachers raised in East Asia) [73] and (b) US American parents (compared to parents in New Zealand, China, and Japan) [74]. Because the present data cannot shed light on the precise source of these differences, future studies should disentangle the contribution of age versus culture to young children's mindsets. ...
Article
Full-text available
Beliefs about the malleability of intellectual ability—mindsets—shape achievement. Recent evidence suggests that even young children hold such mindsets; yet, no reliable and valid instruments exist for measuring individual differences in young children’s mindsets. Given the potential relevance of mindsets to children’s achievement-related behavior and learning, we developed and tested the psychometric properties of the Growth Mindset Scale for Children (GM-C). Among other psychometric properties, we assessed this instrument’s (a) factor structure, (b) measurement invariance, (c) internal consistency, (d) temporal stability (test-retest reliability), (e) concurrent validity, and (f) cross-cultural robustness in samples of US children (Study 1; N = 220; ages 4 through 6; 50% girls; 39% White) and South African children (Study 2; predominantly grades 4 and 5; N = 331; 54% girls; 100% non-White). The GM-C scale exhibited four factors, representing beliefs about the instability of low ability, the malleability of low ability, the instability of high ability, and the malleability of high ability. The GM-C scale also demonstrated invariance across age, acceptable internal consistency (αs between .70 to .90), and moderate temporal stability over approximately one month (rs between .38 to .72). Concurrent validity was supported by significant relations between children’s scores on the subscales about low ability and their goal orientations (Studies 1 and 2), challenge-seeking behavior, and achievement in math and English (Study 2). These findings suggest that the GM-C scale is a promising tool for measuring mindsets in young children. We offer practical recommendations for using this new scale and discuss theoretical implications.
... In high power distance cultures with Confucian traditions, students view teachers as exemplars of virtues like helpfulness, care, and conflict avoidance, expecting them to lead the classroom (Spilt and Koomen, 2022;Hofstede, 1986;Markus and Kitayama, 1991). Jose and Bellamy (2012) and Xie et al. (2023) proposed that cultural differences in individual perceptions may explain cross-country variations in LH. ...
Article
Purpose University students often encounter academic challenges and setbacks, which can impact their beliefs about their capabilities, leading to a phenomenon known as learned helplessness. This study explores the antecedents of learned helplessness through the interaction between teacher support, self-efficacy, and cultural dimensions such as power distance, uncertainty avoidance, and masculinity. Design/methodology/approach Drawing from constructivism, the authors empirically examined a conceptual model utilizing structural equation modeling (SEM). A total of 268 university students completed a self-survey on learned helplessness via a Google Form. Findings The study shows that LH is influenced by personal attributes, perceived support, and cultural context. Higher self-efficacy is linked to lower LH scores, while teacher support indirectly affects LH through self-efficacy. Power distance and uncertainty avoidance also directly impact LH. Additionally, cultural influences indirectly affect LH through perceived support and self-efficacy. However, the hypothesized connections between masculinity and LH were not supported. Originality/value The study advances LH literature by exploring it among university students using a multidimensional, constructivist approach, providing valuable insights for educational authorities. We illuminate how individual, social, and cultural factors influence LH, underscoring the serial mediation between self-efficacy and perceived teacher support. Existing literature frequently overlooks the intricate interplay among cultural dimensions and other factors, limiting the formulation of comprehensive solutions. Our research breaks new ground in quantitative LH examination within education, delving into Hofstede’s cultural dimensions and suggesting strategies to mitigate uncertainty and power distance for student well-being and success.
... Fourth, other factors that are conducive to persistence should be extensively explored to identify effective ways to cultivate rural adolescents' persistence. This can include but is not limited to psychological factors (e.g., mindset, supportive parenting) [31,75] and can also be extended to the societal level, such as income inequality [76]. Finally, both expectations and values are important factors associated with persistence [1], and the current study echoes expectancy-value theory but focuses on only one aspect (i.e., expectations) of the theory in the rural context. ...
Article
Full-text available
Adolescents’ expectations on future subjective social status (SSS) may play a critical role in the relations between perceived parental expectations and persistence; however, there is a lack of research exploring this effect in the context of families experiencing greater economic risk. This study aimed to explicitly address this issue. A total of 698 Chinese rural adolescents participated in this study (Mage = 13.32 years; 54.60% boys). The results showed that for rural adolescents with lower expected future SSS, perceived parental expectation was negatively related to persistence; for rural adolescents with higher expected future SSS, perceived parental expectation was not significantly correlated with persistence. These findings imply the adverse effects of high perceived parental expectation on rural adolescents’ persistence and that expected future SSS can alleviate this adverse relationship.
... Encouragement is a frequently used method for people to show support for one other (Hwang et al., 2011;Jose & Bellamy, 2012). According to Maxwell (2008), encouragement is a significant subject in several self-help books, focusing on enhancing leadership qualities (Dinkmeyer & Eckstein, 1996), as well as books that aim to improve marital pleasures (Rainey & Rainey, 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
The discipline of psychotherapy originated a little over a century ago. Throughout its history, the narrative of psychotherapy has been characterized by a gradual and ongoing effort to comprehend human anguish and discover effective treatments for it. The new treatments from this conference are amalgamations of ideas and approaches characteristic of at least two psychotherapies. These novel treatments are often known as integrative psychotherapies, and there are several models available. The formal practice of integrating different psychotherapy approaches began in 1983 with the establishment of the Society for the Exploration of Psychotherapy Integration (SEPI). Primary considerations were comprehending the interconnection between theory, method, and experience. Recent concerns have focused on the classification of different methods for integrating psychotherapy, the involvement of the client, and the connection between psychotherapy integration and evidence-based practice. The major focus of evidence-based therapy is of utmost importance. However, it is crucial to adopt a wide definition of evidence and stress the role of the client, the therapeutic relationship, and the integration of treatment techniques. This article focuses on the important modern integrative techniques and aims to explain their similarities and consistencies in 4 modalities of psychotherapy, namely Adlerian, Feministic, Person-Centered, and Behavior Therapy.
... We synthesized the extracted data using guidelines provided by Popay and colleagues (2006). Data from the Behaviors which involve the interpersonal communication of a positive message (Wong et al., 2019) aimed at providing support (Jose & Bellamy, 2012) or motivation (Wong, 2015) to the recipient. Examples of prosocial behaviors include positive words of encouragement for a peer (e.g., "You got this!"), and complimenting behaviors and nonverbal positive communication (e.g., a thumbs up or a high five). ...
Article
Group contingencies are well established as methods for reducing disruptive classroom behaviors and increasing academic behaviors. However, their role in increasing prosocial behaviors has not yet been explored to the same extent. We conducted a systematic review of the single-case literature to synthesize the types of prosocial and antisocial behaviors targeted in school settings, the short-term and long-term behavioral contingencies used to target these behaviors, and the quality of the literature. We searched five databases for studies exploring the use of group contingencies to increase prosocial behaviors or decrease antisocial behaviors in children. Our narrative synthesis of the twenty-two included studies noted contingencies employed in these interventions consisted of either positive reinforcement or a combination of positive reinforcement and positive punishment. Of the 22 studies included for review, none met the What Works Clearinghouse Design Standards without reservations. Interobserver agreement and demonstration of effects over time were the most commonly unmet design standards. Future research should seek to encourage greater focus on both prosocial behavior and positive behavior change mechanisms. PROSPERO ID: CRD42022337025.
Article
Full-text available
Helpless behaviors in 5- to 7-year-old children of depressed and nondepressed mothers were assessed through direct observation, interviews, and teacher ratings. The affective tone the mothers set in a joint puzzle task and their tendency to encourage mastery or become intrusive when their children became frustrated at the task were also assessed. Although depressed mothers set a more negative affective tone than nondepressed mothers during solvable puzzles, there were few significant differences between the 2 groups of mothers and children. Still, mothers who were more negative and hostile and less able to encourage mastery in their children had children who exhibited more helpless behaviors in the puzzle task, who were less likely to endorse active problem-solving approaches to frustrating situations, and whose teachers rated the children as less competent and more prone to helpless behaviors.
Chapter
Full-text available
This book has been widely acclaimed, and rapidly found a place on reading lists and in citations following its first publication in 1996. Its strengths are evident from the comments of reviewers, e.g. "The Chinese Learner lives up to its title as the reader with insights into Chinese students from the various perspectives of the investigator, the tutors of Chinese students, as well as the students themselves." Katherine Yip, Asia Pacific Journal of Education
Article
Full-text available
This study tested a framework in which goals are proposed to be central determinants of achievement patterns. Learning goals, in which individuals seek to increase their competence, were predicted to promote challenge-seeking and a mastery-oriented response to failure regardless of perceived ability. Performance goals, in which individuals seek to gain favorable judgments of their competence or avoid negative judgments, were predicted to produce challenge-avoidance and learned helplessness when perceived ability was low and to promote certain forms of risk-avoidance even when perceived ability was high. Manipulations of relative goal value (learning vs. performance) and perceived ability (high vs. low) resulted in the predicted differences on measures of task choice, performance during difficulty, and spontaneous verbalizations during difficulty. Particularly striking was the way in which the performance goal-low perceived ability condition produced the same pattern of strategy deterioration, failure attribution, and negative affect found in naturally occurring learned helplessness. Implications for theories of motivation and achievement are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Learned helplessness in mathematics has been identified as a problem that significantly influences students‟ performance. This study examines the hypotheses that gender and learned helplessness determines significantly the level performance of Lesotho secondary school students in mathematics. Three hundred and ten students from 12 senior secondary schools completed questionnaires assessing their level of learned helplessness as well as ability in mathematics so far. Using a chi-square and two-way ANOVA, results indicated that gender does not significantly determine the level of learned helplessness in mathematics although they both influence students‟ cognitive behaviour independently; there was no significant interaction between gender and level of learned helplessness. These results were discussed and appropriate recommendations made.
Article
Learned helplessness in mathematics has been identified as a problem that significantly influences students‟ performance. This study examines the hypotheses that gender and learned helplessness determines significantly the level performance of Lesotho secondary school students in mathematics. Three hundred and ten students from 12 senior secondary schools completed questionnaires assessing their level of learned helplessness as well as ability in mathematics so far. Using a chi-square and two-way ANOVA, results indicated that gender does not significantly determine the level of learned helplessness in mathematics although they both influence students‟ cognitive behaviour independently; there was no significant interaction between gender and level of learned helplessness. These results were discussed and appropriate recommendations made.
Article
Helpless children show marked performance decrements under failure, whereas mastery-oriented children often show enhanced performance. Current theories emphasize differences in the nature of the attributions following failure as determinants of response to failure. Two studies with 130 5th-grade children explored helpless vs mastery-oriented differences in the nature, timing, and relative frequency of a variety of achievement-related cognitions by continuously monitoring verbalizations following failure. Results reveal that helpless children made the expected attributions for failure to lack of ability; mastery-oriented children made surprisingly few attributions but instead engaged in self-monitoring and self-instructions. That is, helpless children focused on the cause of failure, whereas the mastery-oriented children focused on remedies for failure. These differences were accompanied by striking differences in strategy change under failure. The results suggest that in addition to the nature of the attribution one makes, the timing or even occurrence of attributions may be a critical individual difference. (18 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2006 APA, all rights reserved).