ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

The authors examine the impact of rapid socioeconomic and political transition on generational differences in values and behavioral preferences via a survey of 416 Czech managers, professionals, and business students. As predicted, the pre-transition generation favored individually beneficial upward influence strategies and values oriented toward conservation and self-enhancement; the post-transition generation favored organizationally beneficial upward influence strategies and values oriented toward openness to change and self-transcendence. Generational preferences for certain upward influence strategies are mediated by underlying differences in value orientations, which reflect historically idiosyncratic institutional conditions. These findings extend and integrate work on values and influence behaviors in the relatively unexplored context of transitional economies and have implications for those who wish to appreciate better how diverse value frameworks can be understood and managed in the context of global business.
Content may be subject to copyright.
http://jcc.sagepub.com/
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology
http://jcc.sagepub.com/content/42/2/288
The online version of this article can be found at:
DOI: 10.1177/0022022110396924
2011 42: 288Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology
Wade M. Danis, Leigh Anne Liu and Jiri Vacek
Republic
Values and Upward Influence Strategies in Transition: Evidence From the Czech
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
On behalf of:
International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology
can be found at:Journal of Cross-Cultural PsychologyAdditional services and information for
http://jcc.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:
http://jcc.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:
http://jcc.sagepub.com/content/42/2/288.refs.htmlCitations:
at GEORGIA STATE UNIVERSITY on March 2, 2011jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology
42(2) 288 –306
© The Author(s) 2011
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0022022110396924
jccp.sagepub.com
Values and Upward
Influence Strategies
in Transition: Evidence
From the Czech Republic
Wade M. Danis1, Leigh Anne Liu1, and Jiri Vacek2
Abstract
The authors examine the impact of rapid socioeconomic and political transition on generational
differences in values and behavioral preferences via a survey of 416 Czech managers, professionals,
and business students. As predicted, the pre-transition generation favored individually beneficial
upward influence strategies and values oriented toward conservation and self-enhancement; the
post-transition generation favored organizationally beneficial upward influence strategies and
values oriented toward openness to change and self-transcendence. Generational preferences for
certain upward influence strategies are mediated by underlying differences in value orientations,
which reflect historically idiosyncratic institutional conditions. These findings extend and integrate
work on values and influence behaviors in the relatively unexplored context of transitional
economies and have implications for those who wish to appreciate better how diverse value
frameworks can be understood and managed in the context of global business.
Keywords
values, Czech Republic, transition economies
The globalization of business in recent years has made it imperative for firms to explore foreign
market opportunities in order to gain and sustain competitive advantage. Market liberalization
policies around the globe over the past two decades have provided unprecedented opportunities
in numerous markets that were historically closed (Garten, 1997). At the same time, firms from
such markets are a growing presence in the global economy, and it is expected that their role will
become even more important (Aulakh, Kotabe, & Teegen, 2000; Khanna & Palepu, 2006). A
major catalyst for these phenomena has been the demise of the communist political and centrally
planned economic systems in former Soviet bloc countries. These countries, commonly referred
to as transition or transitional economies, constitute a subset of emerging markets (low-income
countries that use economic liberalization as their primary engine of growth) that have aban-
doned Soviet-style central planning in favor of market-oriented economic systems (Hoskisson,
Eden, Lau, & Wright, 2000). When compared with industry-specific changes in market-based
economies, the scale and scope of the institutional changes in transitional economies are unprec-
edented in recent history (Peng, 2003). Institutional transitions thus envelop a range of inter-
connected domains (e.g., political, social, economic), which collectively comprise a country’s ins titutional
environment (North, 1990).
1Georgia State University, Atlanta, Georgia, USA
2University of West Bohemia, Czech Republic
at GEORGIA STATE UNIVERSITY on March 2, 2011jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Danis et al. 289
This transition, which affects about a third of the world’s population, continues to have a
profound impact on people’s lives and has irreversibly altered the relationship between people
and their political, social, and economic institutions (World Bank, 1996, 2008). In consideration
of this, we seek to identify how the experience of living through profound socioeconomic transi-
tion affects the values and work-related behaviors of individuals in a transitional economy, the
Czech Republic. Following Schwartz and Bilsky (1987, 1990), we define values as concepts or
beliefs that pertain to desirable end states or behaviors. Values transcend specific situations, guide
the selection or evaluation of behavior and events, and can be ordered by relative importance. In
the context of transitional economies, the development of appropriate value systems is thought
to be a critical element in the socioeconomic change process as previously accepted values and
behaviors are often called into question (Holt, Ralston, & Terpstra, 1994). Values underlie
national institutions and business systems and shape decision-making and professional conduct
in a wide variety of business situations (Hofstede, 2001; Redding, 2005; Whitley, 2000). The
question of how institutional change affects the values and work-related behaviors of those who
have lived through such change is of critical interest to scholars and practitioners who wish to
better understand the evolving business environment in transitional economies and how diverse
and changing value frameworks can be reconciled and managed in the context of global business.
While cross-cultural value differences have been studied extensively and have been shown to
influence workplace behaviors, attitudes, and other organizational outcomes (e.g., Hofstede,
1980; House, Hanges, Javidan, Dorfman, & Gupta, 2004; Ralston et al., 2006; Ronen & Shenkar,
1985; Schwartz, 1992; Schwartz & Bilsky, 1990; Trompenaars, 1994), there has been much less
work on the value orientations of national subcultures (Egri & Ralston, 2004). We begin to
address this gap via a comparative analysis of generation cohorts in the Czech Republic, whose
values and behaviors were shaped by sharply different social, political, and economic events.
Generation cohorts are a type of national subculture that reflect the value priorities emphasized
during a particular historical period and encapsulate the nature of culture change that has taken
place in a country (Egri & Ralston, 2004). Members of a given generation share common paradigms
and orientations owing to shared socialization and event-based experiences that distinguish them
as a group (Wade-Benzoni, 2002). The identification of generational value orientations and
behaviors are important for understanding trajectories of cultural change and institutional trans-
formation within a country and can provide a more finely grained picture of this process than
would be possible by studying population-level values and behaviors. Such information is useful
for scholars, policy makers, and practitioners who wish to better understand transformational change
in societies and organizations. For example, knowledge about significant generational value dif-
ferences can provide insight into the degree of receptivity among older versus younger individuals
to economic and social policy changes at the national level, as well as to new workplace initia-
tives introduced at the organizational level.
Our focus on the Czech Republic is also notable. Because of the nature of the Czech transi-
tion, which was unique in many respects, we argue that the country experienced a sharper break
from its communist past than did most other transitional countries, where transformations were
either more gradual, or where economic and political systems were less firmly implanted in the
Soviet Bloc’s hegemony. Consequently, the generational differences we seek to identify may be
more pronounced. Moreover, most of the work on value orientations in national subcultures has
centered on Asian countries and/or the United States (e.g., Egri & Ralston, 2004; Ralston, Egri,
Stewart, Terpstra, & Kaicheng, 1999), overlooking the transitional economies of Central and
Eastern Europe. Because many of the European transitional countries are moving toward inte-
gration with the European Union, which involves harmonization of national institutions and
business practices, we believe this presents an interesting and overlooked theoretical context for
studying values in transition.
at GEORGIA STATE UNIVERSITY on March 2, 2011jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
290 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 42(2)
In order to interact effectively with members of different cultures and subcultures, it is impor-
tant to understand how underlying values shape behavior. When societal values are in flux, as
during periods of institutional transformation, this is not a simple task. Given this complexity, we
limit our focus to one critically important aspect of individual-level behavior, upward influence
strategy, which has special relevance in the context of transitional economies. Upward influence
strategies comprise tactics for obtaining desired outcomes and resources from those higher in
the organizational hierarchy (Kipnis & Schmidt, 1988). This aspect of behavior is especially
pertinent when doing business internationally since cultural values underlie ethical orientations
that determine what influence tactics are seen as appropriate or inappropriate (Ralston, Egri et al.,
2005). This issue is extremely important in the context of transitional economies because upward
influence strategies that were appropriate in the context of communism and central planning may
be unsuitable and even dysfunctional to the requirements of a market-driven economy due to
shifting value orientations and a radically changed business environment. The rapid changeover
from communism and central planning to representative democracy and free markets represents
a momentous sociohistorical event that, we argue, will produce a generational cleavage charac-
terized by different value orientations and behaviors.
This research seeks to answer three questions: (a) How do individual values differ among the
pre- and post-transition generations, (b) how do upward influence strategies differ among the pre-
and post-transition generations, and (c) how are individual values and upward influence strate-
gies related? We aim to identify differences in upward influence strategies between generation
cohorts and link them conceptually and empirically to differences in individual values, which, in
turn, may be rooted in unique sociohistorical time periods. While a few studies have examined
generational cohorts and personal values in transitional economies (Egri & Ralston, 2004; Ralston,
Thang, & Napier, 1999), and others have looked at the impact of national culture and life-stage
on upward influence strategies (Ralston, Hallinger, Egri, & Naothinsuhk, 2005), our study is the
first to explore the links between individual-level values and upward influence strategies. Our
study is also the first we know of to explicitly highlight the issue of institutional upheaval and
theorize about how it jointly impacts values and upward influence strategies.
Theoretical Framework and Hypotheses
Value Transformations in Transitional Economies
Values delineate the way individuals perceive and evaluate work situations and affect relation-
ships among individuals, groups, and their organizations. Values determine the bounds of ethical
behavior, define success or failure, and affect how managers are influenced by external pressures
(Ralston et al., 1993). Value systems can change over time, particularly when situations arise that
challenge established norms or cause a reexamination of value structures (Inglehart, 1997;
Inglehart & Welzel, 2005; Rokeach, 1973). This is precisely what transpires in transitional econ-
omies as previously accepted values and behaviors are challenged via a process of political,
economic, and social upheaval. The rate at which personal values change is partly dependent
upon age and life stage (Alwin & Jon, 1991; Bardi & Goodwin, in press; Egri & Ralston, 2004;
Schwartz & Bardi, 1997). Theories of aging stability, generational succession, and generational
persistence argue that peoples’ attitudes are shaped by socialization experiences early in adulthood
and remain resistant to change after this time (Alwin & Jon, 1991; Inglehart, 1971; Inglehart &
Carballo, 1997; Inglehart & Welzel, 2005; Mannheim, 1952). Research on personality traits sug-
gests that personality stability increases throughout adulthood (Roberts & DelVecchio, 2000).
Accordingly, the value priorities of young people tend to be less crystallized and less firmly
anchored in past experience, which may help them adapt more quickly to change, whereas older
at GEORGIA STATE UNIVERSITY on March 2, 2011jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Danis et al. 291
people who discover their value priorities are no longer adaptive are more likely to resist or
oppose societal change (Bardi & Goodwin, in press; Schwartz & Bardi, 1997). Consequently,
generational cleavages may emerge within societies experiencing institutional upheaval, result-
ing in divergent values and behaviors. Societal value systems thus change over time as succes-
sive generations adapt to different institutional conditions. However, there remains much debate
about the extent to which values change over time, the speed with which they change, and the
drivers of change (Inglehart, 1997).
The convergence, divergence, and cross-vergence perspectives on change offer three alterna-
tive predictions about how and whether societal value systems change over time (Ralston et al.,
2006). The convergence perspective argues that technological, political, and economic develop-
ments interact to produce a business ideology that provides a strong stimulus for relatively rapid
values convergence, whereas the divergence perspective contends that values are learned early
in life via sociocultural influences, are deeply embedded, endure irrespective of societal changes,
and are passed from one generation to the next (Inkeles, 1997; Ralston et al., 2006). The cross-
vergence perspective attempts to reconcile the convergence and divergence views by considering
both the sociocultural and business ideology explanations viable and inherently interactive (Ralston,
Holt, Terpstra, & Kai-Cheng, 1997). In this view, some values may change over a period of years
while others may take generations to change. By comparing the value orientations of the pre- and
post-transition generations in the Czech Republic, we seek to provide further insight into the
convergence, divergence, cross-vergence debate.
Our study used Schwartz’s values model (Schwartz, 1992), which has been used extensively
in studies of individual values, and has been cross-culturally validated in a number of countries,
including the Czech Republic. The Schwartz Value survey identifies 10 universal values. The
pattern of relations among them yields two orthogonal dimensions that delineate four higher
order values. The first dimension, self-enhancement versus self-transcendence, opposes power,
achievement, and hedonism values to universalism and benevolence values. The former empha-
size pursuit of self-interests, whereas the latter involve concern for the welfare and interests of
others. The second dimension, openness to change versus conservation, opposes self-direction
and stimulation values to security, conformity, and tradition values. The former emphasize inde-
pendent action, thought, and feeling, and readiness for new experiences, whereas the latter empha-
size self-restriction, order, and resistance to change (Knafo & Schwartz, 2001).
Values are formed through an experiential learning process within a social structure and one’s
group culture (Osland, Kolb, Rubin, & Turner, 2006). Of particular interest to this study is the
question of whether and how the political, economic, and social upheaval that characterized the
Czech transition produced differences in experiential learning among the pre- and post-transition
generations that, in turn, may manifest themselves in distinctive value systems. Theoretically,
research has demonstrated that adaptation to life circumstances is important for both individual
value formation and change (Rokeach, 1973). Adaptation does not require acceptance of an ideo-
logical message but rather suggests adjusting effectively to the opportunities and constraints that
structure one’s life (Schwartz & Bardi, 1997). Research on post-communist countries has
revealed that changing institutional and economic conditions can influence ideals and values as
people acclimate themselves to new circumstances (Bardi & Schwartz, 1996; Olson, Frieze,
Wall, & Zdaniuk, 2006; Schwartz, Bardi, & Bianchi, 2000). The idea that value formation is an
adaptive process forms the basis for the hypotheses derived below regarding the impact of transi-
tion on the value priorities of Czech managers and professionals. Next, we highlight pertinent
aspects of the Czech transition process that may have impacted the value orientations of the pre-
and post-transition generations.
at GEORGIA STATE UNIVERSITY on March 2, 2011jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
292 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 42(2)
Pre- and Post-Transition Value Orientations
Following the Prague Spring in 1968, a period of political liberalization that was forcibly
ended by the Soviet invasion in August of the same year, Czechoslovakia1 embarked in 1969
on a normalization program that reestablished a hierarchical, centralized, and autocratic politi-
cal and economic regime, which stayed firmly in place until 1989 (Soulsby & Clark, 1996).
During this time, the Communist Party approved admission to management positions. Hence,
developing a professional career and doing managerial work meant making the right contacts
with the political elite, and thereby gaining a range of privileges denied to others. Conse-
quently, managers were viewed with low esteem by their subordinates and relied on autocratic
methods to initiate and implement new initiatives (Soulsby & Clark, 1996). The tightly con-
trolled ideological system and the high level of mistrust that characterized enterprise relations
during this time period constrained behavior since individuals were reluctant to act in ways
that might jeopardize their own self-interests or upset the delicate stability of the system
(Schwartz & Bardi, 1997). This combination of self-interest and reluctance to cause trouble
should be reflected in the values of the pre-transition generation, which we expect to be oriented
to self-enhancement and conservation.
After the fall of communism in 1989, economic and social changes lead young people to
increasingly pursue higher education as the emerging market economy created a higher demand
for specialized knowledge and highly qualified workers (Sobotka, Zeman, & Kantorová, 2003).
Consequently, business students and young professionals learned the vocabulary and rhetoric of
Western management, and many internalized Western managerial values (Soulsby & Clark,
1996). Furthermore, the pervasive spread of modern technologies (mobile phones, computers,
and the Internet) meant that young people were increasingly part of wider and geographically
broader social networks, which nurtured a rapid spread of new ideas and lifestyles patterned on
Western values (Sobotka et al., 2003). In the political arena, new Czech leaders, such as Presi-
dent Václav Havel, espoused values associated with universalism and benevolence, including
social justice, equality, peace, environmentalism, honesty, and forgiveness. Given the above, and
the prominent role of younger Czechs, particularly students, in spearheading the Velvet Revolu-
tion in 1989, which led to the overthrow of the unpopular communist regime, we hypothesize that
the values of the post-transition generation will be oriented to change and self-transcendence. In
sum, the above discussion suggests that the pre- and post-transition generations will differ in their
individual value orientations such that:
Hypothesis 1a: The pre-transition generation will assign more importance to values associ-
ated with conservation and self-enhancement than the post-transition generation.
Hypothesis 1b: The post-transition generation will assign more importance to values associ-
ated with openness to change and self-transcendence than the pre-transition generation.
Upward Influence Strategies
Upward influence strategies comprise tactics for obtaining desired outcomes and resources
from those higher in the organizational hierarchy (Kipnis & Schmidt, 1988). These strategies
are not only central to personal success but can also contribute to organizational effectiveness
since the ability of supervisors and subordinates to work together effectively is critical to over-
all firm performance (Egri, Ralston, Murray, & Nicholson, 2000; Ralston et al., 2001). Appropri-
ate use of such strategies has been shown to engender effective superior-subordinate relationships
at GEORGIA STATE UNIVERSITY on March 2, 2011jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Danis et al. 293
that can enhance organizational performance (Terpstra-Yong & Ralston, 2002) and impact
individual-level outcomes such as performance evaluations, salary, and reported stress (Kipnis
& Schmidt, 1988).
Upward influence strategies may be especially salient workplace behaviors in transitional
economies owing to the historical legacies of a communist political ideology and centrally
planned economic system. In such environments, obtaining influence with politically powerful
individuals is likely to be a key goal as this assures access to resources (Holt et al., 1994). Mana-
gerial and professional careers were closely entwined with the nomenklatura system, whereby
the Communist Party allocated positions according to party membership as well as technical
competence (Martin, 1999). Under this system, which shaped workplace behaviors for the dura-
tion of the communist era (from 1948 to 1989), becoming a manager, developing a career, and
doing managerial work meant making the right contacts with the right people and influencing
those higher in the political and organizational hierarchies (Soulsby & Clark, 1996). Because
workplace norms and behaviors are slow to change (Cepl, 1997), we expect upward influence
strategies to remain a prominent feature of organizational life in the post-communist era.
Ralston, Egri, et al. (2005) classified upward influence tactics into three categories: (a)
organizationally beneficial strategies, (b) self-indulgent strategies, and (c) destructive strategies.
Organizationally beneficial strategies are the standard prescribed and sanctioned behaviors for
employees in organizations. They include working hard, volunteering, getting advanced educa-
tion, developing good working relationships, getting the job done, and working overtime. Self-
indulgent strategies are self-serving for the individual within the organization, epitomizing the
“me first” approach where self-interest is prioritized above the interests of others or the organiza-
tion. Whether they help or harm the organization is subject to interpretation and may be deter-
mined by the situation. Destructive strategies are extreme self-serving behaviors that often
directly hurt others and the organization. In many industrialized societies, these behaviors would
also be considered illegal. Compared to organizationally beneficial strategies, we consider both
self-indulgent strategies and destructive strategies as individually beneficial strategies. Of par-
ticular interest to this study is the extent to which pre- and post-transition generation Czechs may
express preferences for individually beneficial versus organizationally beneficial upward influ-
ence strategies, which we discuss next.
Pre- and Post-Transition Upward Influence Strategies
Central planning was arbitrary, corrupt, and inefficient, and incentives for managers and employ-
ees to work productively for the benefit of their organizations were weak. For the pre-transition
generation, this situation engendered a “beat the system mentality,” by which individuals devised
ingenious ways to circumvent officially sanctioned rules, policies, and procedures in order to
achieve better outcomes for themselves (Danis, 2003). The inefficiency and corruption endemic
to the institution of central planning made it necessary for virtually everyone to engage from time
to time in activities that were officially disallowed but often required for survival (Danis &
Shipilov, 2004). Given this context, we argue that the pre-transition generation would be more
likely to express a preference for individually beneficial upward influence strategies than organi-
zationally beneficial ones.
Hypothesis 2a: The pre-transition generation will be more accepting of individually ben-
eficial upward influence strategies than the post-transition generation.
The post-transition generation began their professional lives during a period of profound
institutional upheaval, at a time when new players (e.g., foreign firms, new domestic leaders)
at GEORGIA STATE UNIVERSITY on March 2, 2011jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
294 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 42(2)
with new ideas were reshaping the social, political, and economic landscape. During periods of
institutional flux, as in the Czech transition, there is a search for identity among individuals as
they develop new behavioral scripts that are better aligned with the emerging institutional template
(Johnson, Smith, & Codling, 2000). Because the previous institutional template was widely
viewed as illegitimate and corrupt (Soulsby & Clark, 1996), we argue that the post-transition
generation will aim to co-align their behavioral scripts with new rules and routines as they
emerge during the transition from communism to representative democracy and from central
planning to a free-market system. As the transition progresses, business students and young
managers and professionals in particular are exposed to alternative workplace behaviors via the
increasing presence of foreign companies, the proliferation of Western-style training and MBA
programs, and a new breed of Czech business leaders, many of whom were émigrés trained in
capitalist countries. We argue that behavioral scripts relied upon previously, as exemplified by
the individually beneficial behaviors that were endemic to the communist era, will be viewed by
the post-transition generation as illegitimate and incongruous with the newly emerging institutional
template, especially to the extent that emergence of market-oriented institutions render them
obsolete. We therefore predict that the post-transition generation will express a preference for
organizationally beneficial behaviors:
Hypothesis 2b: The post-transition generation will be more accepting of organizationally
beneficial upward influence strategies than the pre-transition generation.
In sum, owing to the different social, political, and economic environments under which they
were socialized and began their professional lives, the pre-transition generation will be predisposed
toward individually beneficial upward influence behaviors whereas the post-transition generation
will be predisposed toward organizationally beneficially behaviors.
Linking Values to Upward Influence Strategies
We hypothesize that individuals with different value orientations will express different prefer-
ences for alternative upward influence strategies. We propose that individually beneficial strate-
gies reflect underlying conservation and self-enhancement value orientations. Individuals who
value self-enhancement (comprising lower order values of power, achievement, and hedonism)
tend to emphasize social power and recognition, authority, preservation of public image, influ-
ence, and self-respect, all of which are oriented toward the self (Schwartz, 1994a). By the same
token, individually beneficial upward influence strategies are also self-indulgent in nature and
tend to emphasize position power and coercive tactics (Egri et al., 2000; Ralston et al., 2001),
which would seem highly compatible with the power and achievement values that underlie a
self-enhancement orientation. That is, coercive and self-interested tactics, which constitute indi-
vidually beneficial upward influence strategies, may be viewed as behavioral manifestations of
a self-enhancement value orientation. Therefore, we expect self-enhancement values to underlie
preferences for individually beneficial behaviors.
We further expect that individuals who value conservation (comprising lower order values of
security, tradition, and conformity) may also express preferences for individually beneficial
upward influence strategies. Those concerned with security, tradition, and conformity are likely
interested in maintaining the status quo and, by definition, conservation values emphasize
“propriety and restraint of actions and inclinations that might disrupt the prevailing order”
(Schwartz & Bardi, 1997, p. 391). When one’s environment is organized in an authoritarian man-
ner, as during the pre-transition era, individuals who value conservation may feel safe only if they
come to view the existing hierarchical system, characterized by self-interested behaviors and
at GEORGIA STATE UNIVERSITY on March 2, 2011jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Danis et al. 295
extensive use of position power and authority, as legitimate (Schwartz et al., 2000). Such indi-
viduals may thus view individually beneficial upward influence strategies as a natural and appro-
priate outcome of such a system.
We theorize that organizationally beneficial strategies reflect openness to change and self-
transcendence value orientations. Individuals who value self-transcendence (comprising lower
order values of universalism and benevolence) emphasize helpfulness, honesty, loyalty, and
responsibility (Schwartz, 1994a), suggesting a proclivity for altruistic rather than self-serving
behavior. Consequently, these individuals should favor organizationally beneficial rather than
individually beneficial upward influence strategies. We also expect that openness to change
(comprising lower order values of stimulation and self-direction) will be associated with organi-
zationally beneficial strategies. Individuals who are open to change tend to value curiosity, cre-
ativity, excitement, and variety (Schwartz, 1994a). This tendency suggests an orientation toward
constructive interaction with others in the organization (rather than the inward focus that charac-
terizes individually beneficial behaviors) and is also well-matched to the needs of most organiza-
tions (particularly those in transitional economies), which must continually adapt to changes in
their environment in order to survive and prosper. The above discussion suggests that value ori-
entations directly influence preferences for upward influence strategies, as follows:
Hypothesis 3a: Conservation and self-enhancement values will have a direct positive asso-
ciation with individually beneficial upward influence strategies.
Hypothesis 3b: Openness to change and self-transcendence values will have a direct posi-
tive association with organizationally beneficial upward influence strategies.
Our theoretical logic suggests that intergenerational differences in upward influence strategy
preferences are a function of specific underlying value orientations, which reflect the historically
distinctive social, political, and economic environments under which the pre- and post-transition
generations were socialized and began their professional lives. Formally, values mediate the
relationship between generation cohort and upward influence strategy preferences, as follows:
Hypothesis 4a: Conservation and self-enhancement values will mediate generation-based
preferences for individually beneficial upward influence strategies.
Hypothesis 4b: Openness to change and self-transcendence values will mediate generation-
based preferences for organizationally beneficial upward influence strategies.
The theoretical framework we propose (Figure 1) posits that pre- and post-transition generations
will differ in both their value orientations and their preference for upward influence strategies.
The framework also suggests that generationally based preferences for certain upward influence
strategies are a reflection of underlying value orientations, which mediate the relationship between
generation cohort and upward influence strategy preferences.
Method
Participants and Measures
We surveyed 416 Czech managers, professionals, and business students in 2003. Among these,
26.2% were business students and the rest were managers and professionals. Table 1 shows the
descriptive statistics and demographic variables of the sample. Among the companies we surveyed,
six were owned by Western European firms, three were Czech-owned, and one was U.S.-owned.
We used a questionnaire that was translated from English to Czech via standard back-translation
at GEORGIA STATE UNIVERSITY on March 2, 2011jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
296 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 42(2)
procedures. In order to reduce social desirability bias, participants were instructed that there
were no right or wrong answers and that it was their perceptions that mattered.
Values. Value orientations were measured using the 57-item Schwartz Value Survey (SVS;
Schwartz, 1992). Participants were asked to indicate on a 9-point scale ranging from -1 to 7 how
important each value was to them personally, with -1 indicating the item was “opposed to my
values,” a 0 indicating “not important,” and 7 indicating “supreme importance.” In our hypoth-
eses testing, we used the 45 SVS items that Schwartz found had cross-cultural equivalence in
meaning to measure individual values. To control for potential response bias we used within-
subject standardization prior to testing our hypotheses (Leung & Bond, 1989; Schwartz, 1994b;
Smith & Schwartz, 1997).
Upward influence strategies. We measured upward influence strategies using the Strategies
of Upward Influence (SUI) instrument developed by Ralston, Egri, and colleagues (Egri et al.,
2000; Ralston et al., 2001). The original SUI scale had three dimensions: (1) organizational ben-
eficial behaviors, (2) self-indulgent behaviors, and (3) destructive behaviors. Factor analysis
from our data showed that Factors 2 and 3 were highly correlated, so we collapsed these two
dimensions into a new factor, individual beneficial behaviors. Confirmatory factor analysis
showed that the two-factor model had significantly better fit indices (CFI = .93, IFI = .93,
SRMR = .05, χ2 = 463.29, p < .01) than the three-factor model (CFI = .75, IFI = .75, SRMR = .19,
χ2 = 829.33, Δχ2 = 366.04, p < .01). The new factor, which we label individually beneficial behav-
iors, includes both self-indulgent behaviors and destructive behaviors and encompasses the
underlying concept of a self-serving tendency.
Generation cohort. We divided the participants into pre- and post-transition generation cohorts
based on their work experience. We categorized those with work experience prior to 1990 (13 or
more years) as the pre-transition generation (n = 219) and those who started their work life after
1990 (with less than 13 years of work experience) as the post-transition generation (n = 190). The
post-transition cohort was coded as 1 and the pre-transition cohort as 0.
Control variables. Schwartz (2007) suggests that variables such as age, gender, and education
determine the life circumstances to which individuals are exposed and that other “background”
variables indicate “the socialization and learning experiences, the social roles they play, expecta-
tion and sanctions they encounter, and the abilities they develop. Thus, differences in background
characteristics represent differences in the life circumstances that affect value priorities” (p. 5).
We thus measured and controlled for age, gender (coded 1 = male, 2 = female), education (coded
1 = 4 or fewer years of completed, 2 = 5 to 8 years completed, 3 = 9 to 12 years completed, 4 = 13
Pre-transition
Post-transition
Conservation & Self-enhancement
Openness to change & self-
transcendence
Individually beneficial
Organizationally beneficial
H1a H3a
H2a
H2b
H1b H3b
H4a
H4b
Cohort Values Upward Influence Strategies
Figure 1. The Relationship Between Generation Cohort, Value Orientations, and Upward Influence
Strategy Preferences
at GEORGIA STATE UNIVERSITY on March 2, 2011jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
297
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics, Correlations, and Scale Reliabilities for Pre- and Post-Transition Generations
Variables
Pre- Post-
Mean, Median,
or Freq. (SD)
Mean, Median,
or Freq. (SD) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13
1. Age 42.56 (7.38) 25.60 (8.49) -.09 .13 .21 .01 .08 .02 .10 .08 -.12 -.09 .19 -.10
2. Gender 45.3% male 39% male -.15 -.15 -.18 .10 -.17 .03 .04 .12 .12 .08 .17 -.09
3. Education 13 to 16 years 13 to 16 years .19 -.17 .33 .04 .05 .02 .07 -.03 .12 .07 .06 .12
4. Position First level
manager
First level
manager
.34 -.26 .46 -.08 .04 .04 .12 .09 .12 .10 -.03 .07
5. Company size 62% 100-1000 57% 100-1000 .00 .14 .05 -.09 .10 .09 -.00 .05 -.04 .02 .03 .03
6. Industry 37% Service 42% Service .09 -.21 .05 .03 .11 .05 -.02 -.05 .03 .09 -.03 .05
7. Ownership 30% Czech 27% Czech .02 .03 .01 .06 .08 .07 -.05 .07 .05 -.00 .09 -.07
8. Conservation 1.41 (.48) 1.24 (.45) .15 .04 .07 .15 -.02 .03 -.03 (.80/ .76) .14 -.12 .17 -.15 .13
9. Self-enhancement 1.65 (.62) 1.48 (.61) .11 .11 -.05 .08 .04 .08 .08 .15 (.79 /.76) .10 -.15 -.14 .10
10. Openness to change 1.56 (.43) 1.67 (.49) -.10 .09 .11 .10 -.07 -.06 .05 -.12 .11 (.82/ .85) .18 .18 -.10
11. Self-transcendence .76 (.21) .83 (.23) -.12 .07 .07 .09 .03 .08 -.02 .16 -.12 .17 (.73/ .77) .19 -.07
12. Organizational
beneficial strategy
5.60 (.83) 6.37 (.83) .18 .15 .07 .05 .02 .03 .07 -.12 -.11 .16 .19 (.75/.78) -.08
13. Individual beneficial
strategy
2.51 (.59) 1.88 (.21) -.12 -.10 .12 .09 .02 .02 -.08 .10 .10 -.09 -.10 -.08 (.86/.85)
Note: N = 219 for pre-transition and N = 190 for post-transition generations. If correlation coefficients .10, p < .05; correlation coefficients .15, p < .01; correlation coefficients .19,
p < .001. Correlations for the pre-transition generation are below the diagonal and correlations for the post-transition generation are above the diagonal. Cronbach’s alphas are in
parentheses on the diagonal (pre-/post-).
at GEORGIA STATE UNIVERSITY on March 2, 2011jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
298 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 42(2)
to 16 years completed/bachelor’s degree, 5 = master’s degree, and 6 = doctorate degree), posi-
tion (coded 1 = non-supervisory staff, 2 = first-level manager, 3 = middle-level manager, and
4 = upper level manager), size of company (coded 1 = less than 100 employees, 2 = 100 to 1,000
employees, and 3 = more than 1,000 employees), industry (coded 1 = agriculture, mining, for-
estry fishing [7.3%], 2 = construction [10.7%], 3 = manufacturing [29.3%], 4 = service industry,
including utility, wholesale, retail, financial service, hotel, healthcare [37%], and 5 = other
[14.7%]), and ownership (1 = Czech owned [48%] or 2 = foreign owned [52%]). Table 1 shows
the means/median/frequency, standard deviations, correlations, and scale reliabilities (Cronbach’s
alphas) for the variables.
Because the variables in this study relied on self-report data gathered at the same time, the
influence of common method error variance is a potential concern. Multicollinearity was also a
concern. We therefore employed a number of preventive measures such as providing anonymity
and confidentiality of responses to study participants, using different scale anchors for the per-
sonal values items and upward influence strategy items, and using measures that had been dem-
onstrated as valid and reliable by previous research (Podasakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff,
2003). In addition, we performed Harman’s single-factor test and a series of confirmatory factor
analyses prior to testing our hypotheses (Korsgaard & Roberson, 1995; Podasakoff et al., 2003).
The hypothesized six-factor measurement model comprised four individual-level values factors
(openness to change, conservation, self-enhancement, and self-transcendence) and two upward
influence factors (organizationally beneficial strategies and individually beneficial strategies).
Compared to alternative models, our theoretically driven model appears to have the best fit
(CFI = .90, IFI = .91, SRMR = .057).
Results
Hypothesis 1 posited that the pre- and post-transition generations would differ in their value
orientations with the pre-transition generation assigning more importance to conservation and
self-enhancement than the post-transition generation, and the post-transition generation assign-
ing more importance to openness to change and self-transcendence than the pre-transition gen-
eration. The descriptive statistics provided in Table 1 show that the mean differences between the
pre- and post-transition generations on the four higher level individual values are in the expected
direction. To test the significance of these differences, we regressed each of the four values on
the dummy variable for generation cohort, controlling for age, gender, education, position, com-
pany size, industry, and company ownership. Results are shown in Table 2 and indicate that gen-
eration cohort is a significant predictor of value orientation, fully supporting Hypothesis 1.
To test Hypotheses 2, 3, and 4, we followed the method established by Baron and Kenny
(1986). In each case (see Table 3), we retained the same control variables as in Table 2. First, we
ran two models testing whether the dummy variable (generation) was a significant predictor of
individually beneficial strategy (Model 1) and organizationally beneficial strategy (Model 5), as
specified in Hypothesis 2. These analyses showed that the post-transition generation indicated a
significantly lower acceptance of individually beneficial strategies but a significantly higher
acceptance of organizationally beneficial strategies than did the pre-transition generation (we
note, however, that both generations favor organizationally beneficial strategies). We then added
to each of these models our four value variables: conservation (Model 2), self-enhancement
(Model 3), openness to change (Model 6), and self-transcendence (Model 7). In all four cases,
values had a significant impact on the dependent variables (as specified in Hypothesis 3) and
reduced the coefficient for generation to nonsignificance. This finding supports Hypothesis 4,
which posits that the effect of generation cohort on the dependent variables (individually or organi-
zationally beneficial strategies) is fully mediated by values. We further tested the combination
at GEORGIA STATE UNIVERSITY on March 2, 2011jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Danis et al. 299
effects of conservation and self-enhancement (Model 4) and the combination effect of openness
to change and self-transcendence (Model 8); the results corroborated the mediating effects of
values on generational difference in upward influence strategies.
Given the cross-sectional nature of our data and the high correlations among the measured
attitudinal variables, one can reasonably question the order of mediation for these variables.
Where the second and third links of the proposed chain aàbàc are highly intercorrelated, it
Table 2. Multiple Regression Results for Hypothesis 1
Dependent VariablesàConservation Self-Enhancement Openness to Change Self-Transcendence
Age .09* .10* -.10* -.11*
Gender -.05 -.06 -.09* -.06
Education .03 .05 .08* .04
Position .04 .03 -.03 .05
Company size .04 .09* .05 -.03
Industry -.02 .03 .02 -.02
Ownership -.03 -.04 .08* .04
Generation .22** .21** -.26** -.25**
Overall F10.91*** 9.53*** 13.27*** 12.08***
Adjusted R2.05 .05 .07 .07
df 8, 401 8, 401 8, 401 8, 401
Note: Generation is coded 0 = post-transition generation, 1 = pre-transition generation.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
Table 3. Multiple Regression Results for Hypothesis 2, Hypothesis 3, and Hypothesis 4
Model
Individually Beneficial Strategies Organizationally Beneficial Strategies
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
Age .09* .06* .06* .04 -.07* -.05* -.06* -.03
Gender .06* .05* .05* .04 -.05* -.03 -.03 -.02
Education -.03 .01 -.02 -.01 .02 -.01 -.02 .00
Position .03 .03 .02 .02 -.03 -.03 -.03 -.02
Company size -.05* -.04 -.04 -.04 .03 .03 .03 .03
Industry -.02 -.03 .01 -.01 .01 -.02 -.02 -.01
Ownership -.03 -.03 -.03 -.01 .05* .03 .03 .03
Generation .15** .04 .05 .03 -.12** -.05 -.05 -.02
Conservation .23** .18**
Self-Enhancement .19** .22**
Openness to
change
.28** .24**
Self-
Transcendence
.25** .23**
Overall F9.53*** 82.05*** 79.63*** 133.27*** 10.91*** 94.48*** 92.60*** 150.05***
Adjusted R2.05 .23 .21 .28 .04 .25 .24 .32
DR2.18*** .16*** .23*** .21*** .20*** .28***
df 8, 401 9,400 9,400 10,399 8, 401 9,400 9,400 10,399
Note: Standardized regression coefficients are reported. The coefficients displayed in Models 2, 3, 4, 6, 7, and 8 are
from the Step 2 models. Generation is coded 0 = post-transition generation, 1 = pre-transition generation.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
at GEORGIA STATE UNIVERSITY on March 2, 2011jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
300 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 42(2)
remains feasible, given the Baron and Kenny (1986) analysis, that the true chain might be aàcàb.
For this reason, we tested credible rival causal chains. We reasoned that a preference for the
individually beneficial upward influence strategy might be proposed as a more direct consequence
of membership in the pre-transition generation cohort and this attitude might in turn affect values
of conservation and self-enhancement. Also, a preference for the organizationally beneficial
upward influence strategy might somehow be affected by generation and that this intent might
affect the values of openness to experience and self-transcendence. The data did not support these
alternative hypotheses and were consistent with the notion that values mediated the impact of
generation on both upward influence strategies. In sum, the alternative causal chain did not hold
and upward influence strategies did not appear to mediate the generational effects on values.
Discussion
Our study extends and integrates work on national subcultures, personal values, and influence
behavior in the relatively unexplored context of transitional economies and offers important
insights into how institutional changes may impact managerial values and behavior. Our results
provide evidence for the existence of pre- and post-transition generational cohorts in the Czech
Republic, which differ in their value orientations and attitudes about suitable upward influence
strategies. As predicted, the post-transition generation attaches more importance to openness to
change and self-transcendence values and perceives organizationally beneficial upward influ-
ence strategies as more appropriate than the pre-transition generation, while the pre-transition
generation endorses the values of conservation and self-enhancement and accepts individually
beneficial behaviors more than the post-transition generation. Our results indicate that there are
significant differences in values and attitudes between the pre- and post-transition generations in
the Czech Republic, which, we argue, stem in part from the radically different institutional con-
texts under which each generation was trained and socialized. We next describe how this study
contributes to the literature by discussing its theoretical and managerial implications and suggest-
ing some directions for future research.
Theoretical Implications and Future Research Directions
This article contributes to the research evidence that upward influence strategies differ not only
across cultures (Ralston et al., 2001; Terpstra, Ralston, Jesuino, & Cheung, 2002) but also across
generations within a given national culture. Given that upward influence is an essential social
process in many aspects of group and organizational functions, such as leadership and commu-
nication (Yukl & Tracey, 1992), our findings reveal how value orientations may underlie upward
influence preferences and how institutional context might dynamically shape individual values
and upward influence behaviors. If international researchers are to develop theoretically robust
models of value-based organizational behavior, it is important to identify factors that are consis-
tent both across cultures and within them, as well as those that are different (Ralston, Hallinger
et al., 2005).
Ohmae (1995) maintains that the “global convergence [of media, goods and services] over-
lays new tastes on an established, but largely unaffected, base of social norms and values. The
contents of kitchens and closets may change, but the core mechanisms by which cultures main-
tain identity and socialize their young remain untouched” (p. 30). The dramatic social, political,
and economic changes in Central and Eastern Europe offer an opportunity to assess this proposi-
tion via a comparison of fundamental value orientations and behavioral preferences among pre-
and post-transition generation cohorts. Our findings suggest that societal value systems may not
be as monolithic as Ohmae indicates. Although changes in culture typically occur incrementally
at GEORGIA STATE UNIVERSITY on March 2, 2011jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Danis et al. 301
over centuries, it is possible that societal value systems may change within a generation due to
sudden, extreme changes in life circumstances, such as those that have taken place in post-
communist countries like the Czech Republic (Inglehart, 1997; Schwartz & Bardi, 1997; Schwartz
et al., 2000).
Inglehart’s (1997; Inglehart & Welzel, 2005) theory of culture change postulates that prevail-
ing socioeconomic, political, and existential conditions during one’s formative years shape indi-
vidual value orientations and that there is a tendency to retain a given value hierarchy throughout
one’s adult life. Changes in societal value systems take place over time as successive generation
cohorts adapt to fundamentally different conditions. While our findings seem generally consis-
tent with this premise, we note that much prior work on value formation and change has focused
rather narrowly on level of socioeconomic development (Abramson, Inglehart, Duch, & Taylor,
1994; Allen et al., 2007; Hofstede, 1980; Inglehart & Abramson, 1994; Inglehart & Welzel, 2005).
Theories linking socioeconomic development and values typically posit that higher levels of
development bring more security and opportunities for independence, which promote increased
endorsement of values related to autonomy, egalitarianism, and self-expression and decreased endorse-
ment of values related to conservation and hierarchy (Allen et al., 2007; Schwartz & Bardi,
1997). Our study suggests a more multifaceted explanation for value formation and change: adap-
tation to the prevailing institutional environment. Because institutional environments comprise
political, social, and economic domains, our perspective provides a potentially richer account of the
complex interrelations between values, value-based behaviors, and political, social, and economic
variables. A broader focus on institutional environments may also be more useful when studying
values differences among countries with similar levels of socioeconomic development.
Our finding that generation predicts values after controlling for age is theoretically significant
because it supports our interpretation that value differences in transitional economies may be due
to generationally based paradigms and orientations derived from shared socialization and event-
based experiences (Wade-Benzoni, 2002) rather than life stage. For example, there is some evi-
dence for an increase in pro-social orientation with age (Eisenberg & Fabes, 1998; Roberts, Walton,
& Viechtbauer, 2006) and a decline among older samples in the importance of self-enhancement
(Caprara, Caprara, & Steca, 2003; Knafo & Schwartz, 2001). However, our data indicate that (after
controlling for age) the younger generation gives higher importance to self-transcendence and
lower importance to self-enhancement. Again, this finding suggests generational rather than life-
stage effects and points to a potentially fruitful area for further investigation.
In sum, our study provides an empirically based theoretical account of how generational
preferences for certain upward influence strategies are mediated by underlying differences in
value orientations, which reflect historically idiosyncratic institutional conditions. Our results
have important practical implications and suggest a number of areas for future research, which
we discuss next.
Future research could extend or modify this study along several dimensions. For instance, our
results are intriguing in light of more recent work on the Schwartz value framework (Fontaine,
Poortinga, Delbeke, & Schwartz, 2008), which groups values into those that express anxiety-free
self-expansion (growth values: self-direction, universalism, benevolence, stimulation, hedonism)
versus anxiety-based self-protection (protection values: security, power, achievement, confor-
mity, tradition). While the growth/protection conceptualization is congruent with the original
formulation of the value theory (Fontaine et al., 2008; Schwartz, 1992), it provides further per-
spective on the underlying motivational schemas that may underlie value orientations among
different generations and may thus be an interesting area for future research.
Although a single country study allowed us to control for inter-country differences that might
otherwise impact values and behaviors, multi-country studies examining values and upward influ-
ence strategies in a variety of institutional contexts will be necessary for further theory development.
at GEORGIA STATE UNIVERSITY on March 2, 2011jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
302 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 42(2)
We argue that generation cohorts reflect unique value priorities emphasized during a particular
historical period and depict the nature of institutional and cultural change in a society over time,
but the cross-sectional nature of our study provides an imperfect measure of how individual
values change over time and why. Longitudinal studies hence represent a major opportunity for
future research on these questions. And although upward influence tactics are important facets of
behavior, which can impact firm performance and several individual-level outcomes (Kipnis &
Schmidt, 1988), the finite scope of our study prevented us from investigating other potentially
important behaviors that may be connected to generational value orientations. Future research
could explore the interactions of values and behaviors in other important domains of organiza-
tional behavior. Future research could also raise the level of theory development by examining
the processes by which disparate values, attitudes, and behaviors are reconciled at different levels
of analysis. For example, one might consider this process at a societal level, the interorganizational
level, or the intra-organizational level, in a variety of institutional contexts.
Managerial Implications
Awareness of generational differences due to sharp social changes will inform managerial and
professional practices since multiple generation cohorts are likely to coexist in many organiza-
tions. The identification of generational value orientations and behaviors is especially relevant
for organizations that are in the midst of transformational change, such as those in many transi-
tional and emerging economies, as well as for foreign firms seeking to do business with such
firms. Knowledge about significant generational value differences can help organizations predict
the degree of receptivity (or resistance) to planned corporate changes or new initiatives introduced
by foreign partners (Egri & Ralston, 2004).
Moreover, transitional economies offer unprecedented business opportunities for multinational
companies (MNCs), and collaboration with local firms is the dominant strategy used by these
companies to conduct business in such countries (Hitt, Dacin, Levitas, Arregle, & Borza, 2000).
Yet research on such partnerships reveals that differences in values and behaviors, which are
attributed to the different political, economic, social, and cultural contexts within which manag-
ers are trained and socialized, can be significant and problematic (Danis & Parkhe, 2002). Our
study further suggests that managers should be attuned to some of the more subtle differences
and potential reconciliations within cultures, particularly when their organizations comprise
members who have been socialized and trained in different sociohistorical circumstances.
Consistent with prior research (Inglehart & Welzel, 2005), we argue that generational differ-
ences may be especially pronounced in countries that have undergone radical institutional and
social upheaval, such as transitional economies. As attracting and retaining talent becomes a
more prominent challenge to MNCs, the generational differences in values and upward influence
strategies that we found can provide specific strategies for managing and motivating employees
in transitional economies. For pre-transition generations, MNC managers need to facilitate their
adaptation to market-oriented business practices, with which they may have relatively limited
experience, while being empathetic as to the reasons for value orientations and behaviors that
may not always mesh well with organizational objectives. For post-transition employees, MNCs
can strive to provide stimulating workplace environments and career opportunities that leverage
tendencies toward self-transcendence and openness to change.
Conclusion
Transitional economies, like the Czech Republic, provide fertile settings for studying how insti-
tutional transformation impacts the values, attitudes, and behaviors of different national subcultures.
at GEORGIA STATE UNIVERSITY on March 2, 2011jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Danis et al. 303
In this study, we sought to better understand how attitudes toward one important type of behavior,
upward influence strategies, can be explained by value orientations that derive from a particular
historical period and/or institutional context. Our results advance the literature on values and
attitudes embedded in the political, economic, and social circumstances under which different
generations were educated and socialized. While our conclusions are necessarily tentative, this
study answers some important questions, raises others, and suggests future research directions
that have the potential to further enhance our understanding of how values and behaviors develop
and change in response to institutional upheaval.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interests with respect to their authorship or the publica-
tion of this article.
Financial Disclosure/Funding
The authors declared that they received no financial support for their research and/or authorship of this
article.
Note
1. Czechoslovakia existed from 1918 until 1992. On January 1, 1993, it peacefully split into the Czech
Republic and Slovakia. Hence, we use the term Czechoslovakia when referring to events prior to
January 1993.
References
Abramson, P. R., Inglehart, R., Duch, R. M., & Taylor, M. A. (1994). Education, security, and postmate-
rialism: A comment on Duch and Taylor’s “Postmaterialism and the economic condition”—comment/
reply. American Journal of Political Science, 38(3), 797.
Allen, M. W., Ng, S. H., Ikeda, K. I., Jawan, J. A., Sufi, A. H., Wilson, M., et al. (2007). Two decades of
change in cultural values and economic development in eight East Asian and Pacific Island nations.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38, 247-269.
Alwin, D. F., & Jon, A. K. (1991). Aging, cohorts, and the stability of sociopolitical orientations over the
life span. The American Journal of Sociology, 97(1), 169-195.
Aulakh, P. S., Kotabe, M., & Teegen, H. (2000). Export strategies and performance of firms from emerg-
ing economies: Evidence from Brazil, Chile, and Mexico. Academy of Management Journal, 43(3),
342-361.
Bardi, A., & Goodwin, R. (in press). The dual route to value change: Individual processes and cultural
moderators. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology.
Bardi, A., & Schwartz, S. H. (1996). Relations among sociopolitical values in Eastern Europe: Effects of
the communist experience? Political Psychology, 17(3), 525-549.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological
research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 51(6), 1173-1182.
Caprara, G. V., Caprara, M., & Steca, P. (2003). Personality’s correlates of adult development and aging.
European Psychologist, 8(3), 131-147.
Cepl, V. (1997). The transformation of hearts and minds in Eastern Europe. Cato Journal, 17(2), 229-234.
Danis, W. M. (2003). Differences in values, practices, and systems among Hungarian managers and
Western expatriates: An organizing framework and typology. Journal of World Business, 38(3),
224-244.
Danis, W. M., & Parkhe, A. (2002). Hungarian-Western partnerships: A grounded theoretical model of
integration processes and outcomes. Journal of International Business Studies, 33(3), 423-455.
at GEORGIA STATE UNIVERSITY on March 2, 2011jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
304 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 42(2)
Danis, W. M., & Shipilov, A. V. (2004). Business goals in market versus transition economies: An explor-
atory comparison of Hungarian and U.S. MBA students. Thunderbird International Business Review,
46(3), 275-291.
Egri, C. P., & Ralston, D. A. (2004). Generation cohorts and personal values: A comparison of China and
the United States. Organization Science, 15(2), 210-220.
Egri, C. P., Ralston, D. A., Murray, C. S., & Nicholson, J. D. (2000). Managers in the NAFTA countries:
A cross-cultural comparison of attitudes toward upward influence strategies. Journal of International
Management, 6(2), 149-171.
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1998). Prosocial development. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child
psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., Vol. 3, pp. 701-778). Hoboken,
NJ: John Wiley & Sons Inc.
Fontaine, J. R. J., Poortinga, Y. H., Delbeke, L., & Schwartz, S. H. (2008). Structural equivalence of the
values domain across cultures: Distinguishing sampling fluctuations from meaningful variation. Journal
of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39(4), 345-365.
Garten, J. E. (1997). The big ten: The big emerging markets and how they will change our lives. New York:
Basic Books.
Hitt, M. A., Dacin, M. T., Levitas, E., Arregle, J.- L., & Borza, A. (2000). Partner selection in emerging
and developed market contexts: Resource-based and organizational learning perspectives. Academy of
Management Journal, 43(3), 449-467.
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences: International differences in work-related values. Beverly
Hills, CA: Sage.
Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions, and organizations
across nations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Holt, D. H., Ralston, D. A., & Terpstra, R. H. (1994). Constraints on capitalism in Russia: The managerial
psyche. California Management Review, 36(3), 124-141.
Hoskisson, R. E., Eden, L., Lau, C. M., & Wright, M. (2000). Strategy in emerging economies. Academy of
Management Journal, 43(3), 249-267.
House, R. J., Hanges, P. J., Javidan, M., Dorfman, P. W., & Gupta, V. (2004). Culture, leadership, and
organizations: The globe study of 62 societies. London: Sage Publications.
Inglehart, R. (1971). The silent revolution in Europe: Intergenerational change in post-industrial societies.
The American Political Science Review, 65(4), 991-1017.
Inglehart, R. (1997). Modernization and postmodernization: Cultural, economic, and political change in 43
societies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Inglehart, R., & Abramson, P. R. (1994). Economic security and value change. The American Political
Science Review, 88(2), 336-354.
Inglehart, R., & Carballo, M. (1997). Does Latin America exist? (and is there a Confucian culture?): A
global analysis of cross-cultural differences. PS, Political Science & Politics, 30(1), 34-47.
Inglehart, R., & Welzel, C. (2005). Modernization, cultural change, and democracy: The human develop-
ment sequence. New York and Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Inkeles, A. (1997). National character: A psycho-social perspective. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction
Publishers.
Johnson, G., Smith, S., & Codling, B. (2000). Microprocesses of institutional change in the context of
privatization. Academy of Management Review, 25(3), 572-580.
Khanna, T., & Palepu, K. G. (2006). Emerging giants: Building world-class companies in developing coun-
tries. Harvard Business Review, 84(10), 60-69.
Kipnis, D., & Schmidt, S. M. (1988). Upward-influence styles: Relationship with performance evaluations,
salary, and stress. Administrative Science Quarterly, 33(4), 528-542.
Knafo, A., & Schwartz, S. H. (2001). Value socialization in families of Israeli-born and soviet-born adoles-
cents in Israel. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32(2), 213-228.
at GEORGIA STATE UNIVERSITY on March 2, 2011jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Danis et al. 305
Korsgaard, M. A., & Roberson, L. (1995). Procedural justice in performance evaluation: The role of instru-
mental and non-instrumental voice in performance appraisal discussions. Journal of Management, 21(4),
657-669.
Leung, K., & Bond, M. H. (1989). On the empirical identification of dimensions for cross-cultural compari-
sons. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 20(2), 133-151.
Mannheim, K. (1952). The problem of generations. In P. Kecskemeti (Ed.), Essays on the sociology of
knowledge (pp. 276-320). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Martin, R. (1999). Transforming management in Central and Eastern Europe. New York: Oxford University
Press.
North, D. C. (1990). Institutions, institutional change and economic performance. Cambridge, UK and
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Ohmae, K. (1995). The end of the nation state: The rise of regional economies. New York: Free Press.
Olson, J. E., Frieze, I. H., Wall, S., & Zdaniuk, B. (2006). Economic influences on ideals about future
jobs in young adults in formerly socialist countries and the United States. Cross-Cultural Research,
40(4), 352.
Osland, J. S., Kolb, D. A., Rubin, I. M., & Turner, M. E. (2006). Organizational behavior: An experiential
approach (8th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Peng, M. W. (2003). Institutional transitions and strategic choices. Academy of Management Review, 28(2),
275-296.
Podasakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J.- Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in
behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 88(5), 879-903.
Ralston, D. A., Egri, C. P., Stewart, S., Terpstra, R. H., & Kaicheng, Y. (1999). Doing business in the 21st
century with the new generation of Chinese managers: A study of generational shifts in work values in
China. Journal of International Business Studies, 30(2), 415.
Ralston, D. A., Egri, C. P., Wagenheim, F., Naoumova, I., Fu, P. P., de la Garza Carranza, M. T., et al.
(2005). How do you climb the corporate ladder? A multi-regional analysis of the ethical preferences for
influencing superiors. Paper presented at the Academy of Management, Honolulu, Hawaii.
Ralston, D. A., Gustafson, D. J., Terpstra, R. H., Holt, D. H., Cheung, F., & Ribbens, B. A. (1993). The
impact of managerial values on decision-making behaviour: A comparison of the United States and
Hong Kong. Asia Pacific Journal of Management, 10(1), 21.
Ralston, D. A., Hallinger, P., Egri, C. P., & Naothinsuhk, S. (2005). The effects of culture and life stage on
workplace strategies of upward influence: A comparison of Thailand and the United States. Journal of
World Business, 40(3), 321-337.
Ralston, D. A., Holt, D. H., Terpstra, R. H., & Kai-Cheng, Y. (1997). The impact of national culture and
economic ideology on managerial work values: A study of the United States, Russia, Japan, and China.
Journal of International Business Studies, 28(1), 177-208.
Ralston, D. A., Pounder, J., Lo, C. W. H., Wong, Y.- Y., Egri, C. P., & Stauffer, J. (2006). Stability and
change in managerial work values: A longitudinal study of China, Hong Kong, and the U.S. Manage-
ment and Organization Review, 2(1), 67-94.
Ralston, D. A., Thang, N. V., & Napier, N. K. (1999). A comparative study of the work values of north and
south Vietnamese managers. Journal of International Business Studies, 30(4), 655.
Ralston, D. A., Vollmer, G. R., Srinvasan, N., Nicholson, J. D., Tang, M., & Wan, P. (2001). Strategies of
upward influence: A study of six cultures from Europe, Asia, and America. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology, 32(6), 728-735.
Redding, G. (2005). The thick description and comparison of societal systems of capitalism. Journal of
International Business Studies, 36(2), 123-155.
Roberts, B. W., & DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The rank-order consistency of personality traits from
childhood to old age: A quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 126(1),
3-25.
at GEORGIA STATE UNIVERSITY on March 2, 2011jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
306 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 42(2)
Roberts, B. W., Walton, K. E., & Viechtbauer, W. (2006). Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits
across the life course: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 132(1), 1-25.
Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. New York: Free Press.
Ronen, S., & Shenkar, O. (1985). Clustering countries on attitudinal dimensions: A review and synthesis.
Academy of Management Review, 10(3), 435-454.
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical advances and empiri-
cal tests in 20 countries. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 1-65).
San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Schwartz, S. H. (1994a). Are there universal aspects in the structure and contents of human values? The
Journal of Social Issues, 50(4), 19-45.
Schwartz, S. H. (1994b). Beyond individualism/collectivism: New cultural dimensions of values. In U. Kim,
H. C. Triandis, C. Kagitcibasi, S.- C. Choi, & G. Yoon (Eds.), Individualism and collectivism: Theory,
method, and applications (Vol. 18, pp. 85-119). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Schwartz, S. H. (2007). Basic human values: An overview. The Hebrew University of Jerusalem. Retrieved
from http://www.fmag.unict.it/Allegati/convegno%207-8-10-05/Schwartzpaper.pdf
Schwartz, S. H., & Bardi, A. (1997). Influences of adaptation to communist rule on value priorities in East-
ern Europe. Political Psychology, 18(2), 385-410.
Schwartz, S. H., Bardi, A., & Bianchi, G. (2000). Value adaptation to the imposition and collapse of com-
munist regimes in east-central Europe. In J. Duckitt & S. A. Renshon (Eds.), Political psychology:
Cultural and cross cultural foundations (pp. 217-237). New York: New York University Press.
Schwartz, S. H., & Bilsky, W. (1987). Toward a universal psychological structure of human values. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(3), 550-562.
Schwartz, S. H., & Bilsky, W. (1990). Toward a theory of the universal content and structure of values:
Extensions and cross-cultural replications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(5), 878-891.
Smith, P. B., & Schwartz, S. H. (1997). Handbook of cross-culture psychology (Vol. 30). Boston: Allyn
and Bacon.
Sobotka, T., Zeman, K., & Kantorová, V. (2003). Demographic shifts in the Czech Republic after 1989: A
second demographic transition view. European Journal of Population, 19(3), 249-277.
Soulsby, A., & Clark, E. (1996). The emergence of post-communist management in the Czech Republic.
Organization Studies, 17(2), 227-247.
Terpstra-Yong, J., & Ralston, D. A. (2002). Moving toward a global understanding of upward influence
strategies: An Asian perspective with directions for cross-cultural research. Asia Pacific Journal of
Management, 19(2,3), 373-404.
Terpstra, R. H., Ralston, D. A., Jesuino, J. C., & Cheung, I. (2002). Differences in strategies of upward
influence: A comparative study of managers from China, Macao and Portugal. International Journal of
Business, 7(2), 67-86.
Trompenaars, F. (1994). Riding the waves of culture, understanding diversity in global business. Chicago:
Irwin.
Wade-Benzoni, K. A. (2002). A golden rule over time: Reciprocity in intergenerational allocation deci-
sions. Academy of Management Journal, 45(5), 1011-1028.
Whitley, R. (2000). The institutional structuring of innovation strategies: Business systems, firm types and
patterns of technical change in different market economies. Organization Studies, 21(5), 855-886.
World Bank. (1996). From plan to market: World development report. Washington, DC: Author.
World Bank. (2008). World development indicators. Washington, DC: Author.
Yukl, G., & Tracey, J. B. (1992). Consequences of influence tactics used with subordinates, peers, and the
boss. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 525-535.
at GEORGIA STATE UNIVERSITY on March 2, 2011jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
... These SNS data provide a wide range of opportunities to identify cognitive and psychological attributes such as basic human values (aka values) [8], personality [16], and behavior [21]. [10,14]. In this paper, we predict users' future value scores, which might be changed by the influence of friends in an egocentric network such as Facebook 5 . ...
... Value of a user might be changed her values for different offline behaviors such as life experiences, life events, technological change, social structure, and life style of others [10] [15] [10]. In a previous study [20], we find that value change of an individual reflects in their social media usage behavior. ...
... Value of a user might be changed her values for different offline behaviors such as life experiences, life events, technological change, social structure, and life style of others [10] [15] [10]. In a previous study [20], we find that value change of an individual reflects in their social media usage behavior. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Basic human values represent a set of values such as security, independence, success, kindness, and pleasure, which we deem important to our lives. Each of us holds different values with different degrees of significance. Existing studies show that values of a person can be identified from their social network usage. However, the value priority of a person may change over time due to different factors such as life experiences, influence, social structure and technology. Existing studies do not conduct any analysis regarding the change of users' value from the social influence, i.e., group persuasion, form the social media usage. In our research, first, we predict users' value score by the influence of friends from their social media usage. We propose a Bounded Confidence Model (BCM) based value dynamics model from 275 different ego networks in Facebook that predicts how social influence may persuade a person to change their value over time. Then, to predict better, we use particle swarm optimization based hyperparameter tuning technique. We observe that these optimized hyperparameters produce accurate future value score. We also run our approach with different machine learning based methods and find support vector regression (SVR) outperforms other regressor models. By using SVR with the best hyperparameters of BCM model, we find the lowest Mean Squared Error (MSE) score 0.00347.
... Values represent one's attitudes, opinions, thoughts, and goals in life. Values of an individual might change over time due to the influence of her friends [10,14]. In this paper, we predict users' value dynamics (change of value scores over time) based on her social media usage (e.g., statuses and comments) and her friends' influence in an egocentric network such as Facebook 6 . ...
... The value of a user might be changed due to different offline behaviors such as life experiences, life events, technological change, social structure, and life style of others [15] [10]. In previous studies [21,20], we find that value change of an individual reflects in their social media usage behavior, where authors capture the opinion change over time from social media. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Basic human values represent a set of values such as security, independence, success, kindness, and pleasure, which we deem important to our lives. The value priority of a person may change over time due to different factors such as life experiences, influence, social structure and technology. In this study, we show that we can predict the value change of a person by considering both the influence of her friends and her social media usage. This is the first work in the literature that relates the influence of social media friends on the human value dynamics of a user. We propose a Bounded Confidence Model (BCM) based value dynamics model from 275 different ego networks in Facebook that predicts how social influence may persuade a person to change her value over time. Then, to predict better, we use a particle swarm optimization based hyperparameter tuning technique. We observe that these optimized hyperparameters produce more accurate future value score. We also run our approach with different machine learning based methods and find support vector regressor (SVR) outperforms other regressor models. By using SVR with the best hyperparameters of BCM model, we find the lowest Mean Squared Error (MSE) score as 0.00347.
... The logic of state socialism, which promotes a "preference for security rather than risk", leads individuals to adopt more riskaversive behaviors (Wyrwich 2013). Consequently, these workers tend to prefer more secure jobs and steady returns (Alas and Rees 2006), try to avoid positions that endanger their personal interests (Danis et al. 2011), maintain a low profile (Schwartz and Bardi 1997), experience inner fear of anti-capitalist sentiment (Pop-Eleches and Tucker 2014) and also express feelings "we are lost generation" (Hollander 1994). Therefore, the personenvironment misfit within privatized family firms makes it challenging to fully leverage their origin-imprinted workforce to drive technological breakthroughs, impart new knowledge, and foster innovation. ...
Article
Full-text available
In transforming emerging economies, many state-owned enterprises (SOEs) underwent privatization, transferring property rights from the state to private entities. This transition not only facilitated the establishment of entrepreneurial family firms but also encouraged the emergence of privatized family firms as property rights were transferred to individuals and families. Consequently, the roots of property rights in these settings can be traced back to either direct establishment or privatization. In this study, we examine how these origin imprints influence corporate innovation. By analyzing a dataset of A-share Chinese listed non-financial family firms spanning from 2005 to 2021, we find that pre-privatization organizational imprints which primarily focus on societal well-being, tend to persist within these privatized family firms, resulting in a lower degree of corporate innovation compared to their entrepreneurial counterparts. Moreover, additional subsample analysis indicates that the adverse impact of privatized family firms on corporate innovation is intensified by strong political connections while mitigated by a well-developed institutional environment in the region. Our results are robust to various econometric methods, alternative explanations, and approaches to address endogeneity concerns such as the two-stage least squares (2SLS), Generalized Method of Moments (GMM), and propensity score matching (PSM) techniques. Overall, this study highlights a source of heterogeneity within the family firms and reveals how organizational imprints inherited from a pre-privatization economic regime can diminish the positive effects usually associated with family ownership.
... Values may change according to physiological changes (Schwartz, 2005;Schwartz & Bardi, 1997) and adaptation to new life situations (Bardi et al., 2014;Danis et al., 2011;Ferriman et al., 2009;Kohn & Schooler, 1982). Given that adolescence is a time of rapid change in several aspects of personality (Meeus, 2011;Steinberg et al., 2018), it is possible that the importance that individuals assign to values changes more quickly during this period than in other stages of life. ...
Article
Adolescence is critical for values development since transitions in several domains occur during this period. Building upon Schwartz’s circumplex model, this study investigated stability and change of values during adolescence and their environmental precursors (i.e., parental rejection and community violence exposure). A sample of 773 Italian high school students (342 males; M age [Time 1] = 14.19, SD = 0.56) was assessed using self-report questionnaires across four waves. Rank-order stability coefficients were moderate for all values. Latent growth curve analyses revealed a significant linear increase in self-enhancement; all the other values displayed a quadratic shape in change over time, with a decline in the first part of the curve and a subsequent increase across time points. Overall, significant associations were found between environmental factors and the baseline levels of values. Value change over time was not significantly predicted by environmental factors except for openness to change, which was associated with paternal rejection. Specifically, high paternal rejection was linked with a faster decrease and a slower increase of openness to change over time. These findings highlight the importance of longitudinal studies when examining value change over time. Also, they suggest that environmental factors may play a significant role in shaping adolescent values.
... Regarding actors and institutions and concerning civil society, beyond the driver of compliance with the SDG legislation, which is specific to Latvia (e.g., the requirements of the emissions legislation in relation to SDGs 7, 13, and 15) [116] and other countries (e.g., the LANDFILL tactical tool for the SDGs) [109], there are barriers, such as a lack of knowledge and awareness around the SDGs caused by individuals not being fully aware of the importance of the SDGs for sustainable development [120], [121] and a lack of networking skills due in the Latvian context to the former Soviet system, which encouraged the expression of conservation values through resistance to change and self-reinforcement [122] and which can generally hinder the adoption of the SDGs by blocking knowledge sharing and facilitating individual conflicts [123]. At the firms and organizations level, despite the positive developments of Latvian enterprises in relation to SDGs 7,8,9,14,and 15 [114] and the support of technology investors and social entrepreneurs, they still face institutional voids, as other countries [124], [125]. ...
Article
Research examining the drivers and barriers of implementing sustainable development goals (SDGs) in national innovation ecosystems (NIEs) is scarce. This study proposes a framework of drivers and barriers to NIEs for SDGs implementation used in the Latvian context. An exploratory qualitative approach based on the Latvian case study is used to generate in-depth information on the drivers and barriers of NIEs for implementing SDGs. Our results show that at the NIE level, drivers for actors and institutions, such as legislative compliance, eco-innovations, cross-sector cooperation, and talent pools, are hindered by some barriers, such as lack of networking skills, institutional voids, totalitarian power, and academic rigidity. Despite attempts at technological convergence that facilitated distance learning, remote work, and telehealth solutions during the pandemic, the lack of collaborative platforms hinders the development of artifacts. Sustainable innovation is hindered by a lack of resources and openness that also blocks open innovation. Beyond coevolution and co-specialization initiatives, friction between the public and private sectors hinders complementary and substitute relations. Our results provide a systemic and holistic NIE tool to analyze the drivers and barriers of the SDGs.
... However, certain life experiences can reorient and restructure the importance of certain values (Rokeach & Ball-Rokeach, 1989). One such experience is adaptation, which occurs when big life transitions, for example, becoming a parent (Ferriman et al., 2009), experiencing a change in job conditions (Kohn & Schooler, 1982), or witnessing a major political shift (Danis et al., 2011), require a person to confront their default values (Bardi & Goodwin, 2011). When something major like this happens, people demote those values that do not align with the new context and elevate values that are encouraged by the new context (Bardi & Goodwin, 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
The values people hold tend to be relatively enduring. An important exception appears to be values adaptation in response to major, life‐altering situations. Major events can act as triggers for people to adapt their values based on the new context. In particular, collective traumas—such as the COVID‐19 pandemic—may incite immediate values change. The aim of the current paper is to compare business school students' value orientations before and after the COVID‐19 global pandemic outbreak. We investigated responses from two comparable samples of business students: one surveyed before and one surveyed after the outbreak of the COVID‐19 pandemic. The subjects' individual value orientations were aggregated and analyzed by comparing the distribution of the first group's pre‐COVID‐19 outbreak responses with the second group's post‐COVID‐19 outbreak responses regarding the importance given to values in the Rokeach Values Survey. We further explored specific demographic differences in personal versus social orientations and competence versus moral orientations for our samples. Results confirm differences in business school students' pre‐COVID‐19 outbreak versus post‐COVID‐19 outbreak value orientations, with the post‐COVID‐19 outbreak sample reporting greater attention to social values, as predicted, and competence values, not as predicted. Implications of our findings are discussed.
... Finally, age may influence upward influence behaviours. Age is conceptualised in terms of sociocultural generations (Ralston, Hallinger, Egri, & Naothinsuhk, 2005) and generational cohorts (Danis et al., 2011), which extend age beyond birth year to encompass cultural, economic, political, and social situations. When selecting upward influence tactic, timing, and presentation of tactic, employees should be aware of gender, cultural, and age differences. ...
Chapter
This study extends work from home (WFH) literature by recasting WFH performances that emphasise agents’ manipulation of scene. Drawing on the dramatist paradigm, the study uses Burke’s pentadic criticism to code the social media application Pinterest’s ‘work from home’ and ‘home of- fice’ pinboards for act, agent, agency, scene, and purpose. Pinterest is a social media application that users post pins (images with verbal tags and link to external sites, especially blog sites) and collect pins by subject on an electronic pinboard, which other users can like, follow, and share. Analysis of WFH pins reveals that the agent–scene ratio saturates pins emphasising the agency of WFH agents to control their scenes or home office spaces. The idealism of the agent–scene ratio in pins further demonstrates an un- realistic approach to popular culture’s shift to WFH and a romanticisation of WFH as idyllic working conditions. Scripts for employees and employers are explored to equip them with the rhetorical resources to more closely align their agent–scene ideals with the scene–agent realities. Keywords: Agent–scene ratio; dramatism; Kenneth Burke; pentadic criticism; Pinterest; work from home
... The literature recognises that values in transition economies shift in the face of prevailing change currents (Schwartz & Bardi 1997), and that new values emerge (Danis et al 2011). ...
Thesis
Full-text available
The modern conceptualisation of the psychological contract recognises a tacit mental representation or schema, spanning all aspects of an employee's perception of work. Reciprocity is a normative force in contract functioning. For over 500 years, the Czech Republic was subject to the rule of other nations. The failed totalitarianism of the most recent Soviet hegemony precipitated the Velvet Revolution and Czech adoption of the market economy in 1989. Some commentators have argued that unproductive work attitudes remain as a legacy of the command system. Following the phenomenological paradigm and constructivist epistemology, the research uses concepts from Personal Construct Psychology to compare the work constructs of Czech and non-Czech staff within the Czech and UK subsidiaries of the same company, examining antecedent effects of culture and individual experiences on psychological contract formation and development. The findings show that the two nationalities construe work along broadly similar lines, prioritizing its social qualities. Czech constructs seem to be simpler than those of non-Czechs, apparently lacking the value placed on personal ambition and achievement by the comparator group. Czechs do, however, appear to value independence much more than non-Czechs, with young Czechs also seemingly expecting social justice and the right to self-determination. The findings make a strong case for suggesting that these values have their origins in Czech culture and history, implying that both influence the work dispositions of Czechs and may plausibly be psychological contract antecedents. The conclusions call for a wider conceptualisation of the psychological contract, specifically in its anticipatory (pre-work) form, and suggest that existing theory might benefit from giving greater consideration and prominence to the social properties of work. Suggestions for further research and business applications are included. ii Stones taught me to fly, Love taught me to lie, Life taught me to die…. Damien Rice, Cannonball iii
Article
The present study describes ten personal values expressed by men and women from two countries, and it explores the relationships between two opposing values, Achievement and Benevolence, specifying Stimulation as a mediator between them. It is further explored whether such a mediation model could be further qualified by age and Gender as moderators. The 40-item Portraits Values Questionnaire (PVQ), measuring ten basic values, was administered to 1,000 young adults from two countries. Hierarchical regression methods were applied to examine mediation and moderation mechanisms.Minor gender and country differences emerged for some of the ten basic values. An indirect relationship among the three selected values was identified. Stimulation was found to operate as a mediator between achievement and benevolence. A conditional process model was established with Gender moderating the Achievement – Stimulation path (men had a steeper slope than women), whereas age moderated the Stimulation – Benevolence path (younger individuals had a steeper slope than older ones). Gender also moderated the Achievement – Benevolence path (men had a steeper slope than women). For men, the association between achievement and stimulation was stronger than for women. For the younger persons, the association between stimulation and benevolence was stronger than for older ones. For women, the level of benevolence was independent of their achievement level. The present analyses shed new light on indirect and differential associations among personal values, adding a novel perspective to research on cognitive mechanisms involved in the ten basic values’ becoming. References Bardi, A., & Schwartz, S.H. (2003). Values and behaviour: Strength and structure of relations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(10), 1207–1220. Berlyne, D. Conflict, arousal and curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill. 1960. Burgoyne, C.B. & Lea, S.E.G. (2006). Money is material. Science, 314(5802), 1091–1092. Hayes A. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: a regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press. Kluckhohn, C. (1951). Values and value-orientations in the theory of action: An exploration in definition and classification. In: Toward a General Theory of Action, (pp. 388-433). T. Parsons & E. Shils (Eds.), Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Korman, A. (1974). The Psychology of Motivation. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Maio, G. R., Hahn, U., Frost, J., & Cheung, W. (2009). Applying the value of equality unequally: Effects of value instantiations that vary in typicality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(4), 598–614. Maio, G.R., Pakizeh, A., Cheung, W.Y. & Rees, K.J. (2009). Changing, priming, and acting on values: effects via motivational relations in a circular model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(4), 699–715. Maslow, A.H. (1965). Eupsychian management. Homewood, IL: Dorsey. Parks-Leduc, L., Feldman, G., & Bardi, A. (2015). Personality traits and personal values: A meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 19(1) 3–29. Parsons, T. & Bales, R. F. (1953). Family, Socialization, and Interaction Process. Glencoe: Free Press.Prince-Gibson, E., & Schwartz, S. H. (1998). Value priorities and gender. Social Psychology Quarterly, 61, 49-67. Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 1-65. Schwartz, S. H. & Sagiv, L. (2000). Value Priorities and Subjective Well-Being: Direct Relations and Congruity Effects. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 177-198. Schwartz, S. H., Melech, G., Lehmann, A., Burgess, S., Harris, M., & Owens, V. (2001). Extending the cross-cultural validity of the theory of basic human values with a different method of measurement. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 32, 519-542. Vohs, K.D., Mead, N.L. & Goode, M.R. (2006). The psychological consequences of money. Science, 314 (5802), 1154–1156. Wade, M.D., Liu, L.A. & Vacek, J. (2011). Values and Upward Influence Strategies in Transition: Evidence From the Czech Republic. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42 (2), 288–306.
Article
This study examines market reaction to CEO appointments at state-owned enterprises (SOEs) listed on the Warsaw Stock Exchange between 2000 and 2015. We find evidence that the market reaction to CEO turnover at SOEs is nonmonotonic. A negative market reaction dominates in a short window, whereas a positive market response prevails in a long window. In addition, we observe a significant inverted U-shaped relationship between the level of state ownership and shareholder reaction. Shareholders prefer CEO replacements at SOEs whose financial results are poor. We report a negative impact on abnormal returns if the CEO is in the pre-transition generation.
Article
Full-text available
This paper presents a theory of potentially universal aspects in the content of human values. Ten types of values are distinguished by their motivational goals. The theory also postulates a structure of relations among the value types, based on the conflicts and compatibilities experienced when pursuing them. This structure permits one to relate systems of value priorities, as an integrated whole, to other variables. A new values instrument, based on the theory and suitable for cross-cultural research, is described. Evidence relevant for assessing the theory, from 97 samples in 44 countries, is summarized. Relations of this approach to Rokeach's work on values and to other theories and research on value dimensions are discussed. Application of the approach to social issues is exemplified in the domains of politics and intergroup relations.
Article
Full-text available
In this article, we attempt to distinguish between the properties of moderator and mediator variables at a number of levels. First, we seek to make theorists and researchers aware of the importance of not using the terms moderator and mediator interchangeably by carefully elaborating, both conceptually and strategically, the many ways in which moderators and mediators differ. We then go beyond this largely pedagogical function and delineate the conceptual and strategic implications of making use of such distinctions with regard to a wide range of phenomena, including control and stress, attitudes, and personality traits. We also provide a specific compendium of analytic procedures appropriate for making the most effective use of the moderator and mediator distinction, both separately and in terms of a broader causal system that includes both moderators and mediators. (46 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
This study assesses the impact of business ideology and national culture on work values of managers in the United States, Russia, Japan, and China. The convergence/divergence/crossvergence framework is used to discuss the findings. Implications for international businesses are drawn regarding the applicability of multilocal or global strategies for corporate culture.
Article
How do organizations make strategic choices during the time of fundamental institutional transitions such as those sweeping numerous emerging economies? To answer this question, a two-phase model of institutional transitions is developed in this article. I focus on the longitudinal process to move from a relationship-based, personalized transaction structure calling for a network-centered strategy to a rule-based. Impersonal exchange regime suggesting a market-centered strategy. I then identify the points of inflection; predict strategic choices for incumbent, entrepreneurial, and foreign firms; and delineate their performance implications.