Content uploaded by Lawrence J. Schweinhart
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Lawrence J. Schweinhart on Jun 16, 2014
Content may be subject to copyright.
67
Outcomes of the High/Scope Perry
Preschool Study and Michigan School
Readiness Program
Lawrence J. Schweinhart
*
Evaluations of ongoing early childhood development (ECD) programs yield lessons for
improving the design of ECD programs, certainly in industrialized countries and probably in
developing countries. Several U.S. programs are known worldwide for demonstrating the
extraordinary value of high-quality preschool education.
Two longitudinal evaluation studies, in particular, show the beneficial effects of ECD
programs on young children who are living in poverty and otherwise potentially vulnerable to
failure in school. In addition, a multicountry early childhood study found relationships between
early childhood practices and child outcomes that were the same in all the countries studied.
In the High/Scope Perry Preschool Study, researchers followed 123 low-income children,
who entered the ECD program at ages 3–4 years, through age 40. In the Michigan School
Readiness Program Evaluation, researchers tracked 596 children, who entered the program at age
4 years, through age 10. These two different studies offer similar and complementary lessons for
designing effective ECD programs.
Measuring ECD Outcomes
High/Scope Perry Preschool Study
Michigan School Readiness Program Evaluation
Lessons for Program Design
Measuring ECD Outcomes
High/Scope Perry Preschool Study
The High/Scope Perry Preschool Study is a scientific experiment identifying the short- and long-
term effects of a high-quality preschool education program for young children living in poverty
*
Lawrence J. Schweinhart, Ph.D., is President, High/Scope Educational Research Foundation, Ypsilanti, Michigan,
U.S.A.
Table of Contents
68
(Schweinhart and others 2005). In 1962–67, David Weikart and colleagues operated the
High/Scope Perry Preschool program for young children in the Ypsilanti, Michigan, school
district (Weikart and others 1970). The aim was to help the children avoid school failure and
related problems.
Study Design
For the evaluation study, the researchers:
Identified 123 African American children ages 3–4 years who were living in poverty and
were at high risk of school failure
Assigned the children randomly to two groups: 58 children to a high-quality preschool
“program group” and 65 children to a “no-program group”
Collected data on both groups—annually from age 3 through age 11 and at ages 14, 15,
19, 27, and 40
Compared the outcomes for children who did and did not participate in the ECD
program.
The researchers defined “children living in poverty” as those whose parents had little
schooling (9
th
grade, on average) and low occupational status (i.e., unemployed or in unskilled
jobs) and who lived in high-density households (i.e., 1.4 persons per room). The program group
consisted of several classes of 20–25 children who met daily with certified teachers. The children
participated in their own education by planning, doing, and reviewing their activities. The
teachers also made weekly home visits.
Evaluation Results
Figure 1 presents the evaluation results over time and chronologically for the program group
and no-program group. All comparisons are statistically significant at p < 0.05. The missing-data
rate across all measures was only 6 percent.
Because the study was based on random assignment and the characteristics of the children in
the two groups were almost exactly alike—
Preschool experience is the best explanation for the differences in the children’s
subsequent performance over time.
The figure shows that, compared with children in the no-program group, more children in
the program group―
Were ready for school at age 5 (67 percent versus 28 percent).
Table of Contents
69
Were committed to school at age 14 (61 percent versus 38 percent)—a higher percentage
did their homework and talked with their parents about school.
Attained a basic level of achievement at age 14 (49 percent versus 15 percent).
Graduated from high school (77 percent versus 60 percent).
Beyond High School. The benefits of participating in the ECD program extended beyond high
school. The findings for the program group compared with the no-program group are as follows:
The program group had higher median annual earnings at age 27 ($12,000 versus
$10,000) and age 40 ($20,800 versus $15,300).
More were employed at age 27 (69 percent versus 56 percent) and age 40 (76 percent
versus 62 percent).
More owned their own homes at age 27 (27 percent versus 5 percent) and age 40 (37
percent versus 28 percent).
More raised their own children (57 percent versus 30 percent).
In addition:
Table of Contents
70
Fewer were arrested five or more times by age 40 (36 percent versus 55 percent).
Fewer were arrested for violent crimes (32 percent versus 48 percent), property crimes
(36 percent versus 58 percent), and drug crimes (14 percent versus 34 percent).
Fewer were sentenced to prison or jail by age 40 (28 percent versus 52 percent).
Gender-specific Program Effects. The findings indicated that females and males gained
different advantages from participating in the ECD program. Females’ advantages were in
educational placement: fewer program females than no-program females were retained in grade
(21 percent versus 41 percent), fewer were treated for mental impairment (8 percent versus 36
percent), and fewer dropped out of high school (12 percent versus 54 percent). Males’
advantages were in reduced crime: fewer program males than no-program males were arrested
five or more times by age 40 (45 percent versus 69 percent).
Causal Model. The data show consistent effects of participation in the ECD program from ages 4
to 40 years. The researchers documented a causal model that tracks cause–effect paths from the
children’s preschool experience and pre-program intellectual performance to their post-program
intellectual performance, school achievement and commitment to schooling, and then on to their
educational attainment, adult earnings, and lifetime arrests.
Cost-Benefit Analysis. One of the most well-known findings of the High/Scope Perry Preschool
Study is that the preschool program had a large return on investment. A cost-benefit analysis
indicates that (in constant U.S. dollars, 2000, discounted at 3 percent)―
The economic return to society for the program was $258,888 per participant on
an investment of $15,166 per participant—or, $17.07 per dollar invested.
This return benefited both the general public and the participant. Of the total return,
$195,621 was a return to the general public, and $63,267 was a return to the participant. The
distribution of the public return was calculated as follows:
88 percent represented savings from crime, whereas up to 7 percent represented savings
from special education and welfare, as well as increased funds from taxes on higher
earnings.
Remarkably, 93 percent of the public return was attributed to males because of the
program’s substantial reduction in crime committed by males, and only 7 percent of the
public return was attributed to females.
Figure 2 graphically portrays the costs and benefits.
Table of Contents
71
¾ The full report of the High/Scope Perry Preschool Study, entitled Lifetime Effects: The
High/Scope Perry Preschool Study through Age 40, is available from the High/Scope Press.
Summaries are available at:
<http://www.highscope.org/NewsandInformation/PressReleases/PerryP-Age40.htm>.
Michigan School Readiness Program Evaluation
The Michigan School Readiness Program (MSRP) is Michigan’s preschool program for 4-year
olds who are at risk of school failure. The program is intended to help these children get ready
for school. The program serves approximately 22,000 children each year who qualify for the
program by having two risk factors (e.g., parents with low income, living in a single-parent
family).
The High/Scope Foundation, as commissioned by the Michigan State Board of Education,
has led evaluation of the MSRP for a decade (Xiang and Schweinhart 2002).
Study Design
Evaluation of the MSRP consists of two major efforts:
Local program evaluations conducted by MSRP grantees, with support from the
High/Scope Foundation.
A state evaluation conducted by the High/Scope Foundation at selected sites—following
596 children ages 5–10 who participated in the MSRP in 1995–96 at six sites in and
around the Michigan cities of Detroit, Grand Rapids, Grayling, Kalamazoo, Muskegon,
and Port Huron.
Table of Contents
72
The state evaluation serves as a scientific model for ECD evaluation. It was conducted by
trained data collectors and included a comparison group of children who have not participated in
the MSRP.
For each site, evaluation researchers tracked and compared children who did and did not
participate in the preschool program. In this quasi-experimental model, the two groups of
children were similar in age, mothers’ and fathers’ schooling, presence of the father in the home,
number of persons per household, and household income
.
Evaluation Results
The effects of participating or not participating in the preschool program were documented for
the program and no-program children as they entered kindergarten and at age 10 (4
th
grade).
On Entering Kindergarten:
Observers rated the graduates of the preschool program significantly better in language
and literacy, creative representation, music and movement, initiative, and social relations,
compared with their no-program classmates.
Elementary school teachers rated the program children significantly more ready for
school (i.e., they were more interested in school and were more likely to take initiative,
have good attendance, and retain learning), compared with their no-program classmates.
The parents of the program children became significantly more involved in their
children’s school activities and talked with the elementary school teachers more often,
compared with the parents of the no-program children.
Figure 3 presents the observers’ findings.
By Age 10:
Fewer program children had repeated a grade, compared with their no-program
classmates of similar background (14 percent versus 22 percent).
More program children passed Michigan’s 4
th
grade reading and mathematics tests,
compared with the no-program children (44 percent versus 36 percent for reading, 55
percent versus 47
percent for mathematics).
Figure 4 presents these findings.
The benefits of the MSRP to the state of Michigan are significant. For example, extending
the 14 percent of fewer program children repeating a grade by age 10 to the total number of
children participating in the MSRP each year, the program is preventing an estimated 2,100
children in Michigan each year from having to repeat a grade by age 10. This benefit alone—
reduced grade repetition—potentially saves Michigan an estimated $13.6 million annually.
Table of Contents
74
Lessons for Program Design
The two evaluation studies—of the High/Scope Perry Preschool Program and the MSRP—yield
important lessons. Ten lessons are suggested below, both for designing preschool programs and
supporting early childhood development.
1. Evaluation Is Critical for Determining the Effectiveness and Value of an ECD
Program
This lesson is most obvious. Without evaluation, individuals can only assume or guess whether a
program is worth the money spent on it.
2. The Best Program Directors Understand Evaluation and Are Immersed in the
Evaluation Process
The High/Scope Perry Preschool Study has been criticized at times because the program director
was also the first evaluation director. However, instead of compromising the objectivity of the
evaluation, the program director’s knowledge and desire to achieve the measured outcomes for
the children may well have been the major reason for the success of the program and the
evaluation. ECD programs in developing countries may be better served by having program
directors who understand and are immersed in the evaluation.
3. Experimental Designs Are Preferable to Quasi-Experimental Designs
Although experimental studies are more difficult to implement because of practical
considerations, they yield much more trustworthy results because the two groups—program and
no-program—are exactly alike except for the program’s effect.
Both the High/Scope Perry Preschool and the MSRP evaluations measured child
performance in two groups, one that participated in the program and one that did not. The no-
program group could be considered counterfactual, because the group’s performance is an
estimate of how well the program group would have done without the program. The difference
between the two groups’ performance is an estimate of the program’s effect.
Establishing this effect with certainty is more difficult in quasi-experimental studies than in
experimental studies because of the possibility of selection bias. In quasi-experimental studies,
the two selected groups may differ in a variety of ways and the difference in their performance
may be due to factors other than the program’s effect.
In an experimental study, such as the High/Scope Perry Preschool Study, a sample of
children is randomly assigned into groups.
In a quasi-experimental study, such as the MSRP evaluation, children in the population
are selected randomly to represent two pre-existing groups.
Table of Contents
75
4. The Longer the Follow-Up, the More Can Be Said about the Extent and Duration of
a Program’s Effects
In both the High/Scope Perry and MSRP studies, data were collected over a number of years—
through age 40 in the High/Scope Perry study, and through age 10 in the MSRP study. Practical
considerations make long-term follow-up difficult. The missing-data rate of only 6 percent
across all measures in the High/Scope Perry study is a far-from-typical accomplishment.
Tracking individuals over time is a difficult problem anywhere, especially in developing
countries.
5. Poverty and Its Effects May Not Be the Same Everywhere
Both High/Scope Perry and MSRP focused on children who were living in poverty and at special
risk of school failure. Whereas the evidence for positive effects of ECD programs targeted to
these children is substantial, there is little evidence for effects on children who do not live in
poverty or are at special risk of school failure. With their focus on at-risk children living in
poverty, the studies may have applicability in developing countries.
However, both studies were conducted in the United States, so the children in the study were
living in the midst of U.S. conditions, rather than conditions of developing countries. Although
the absolute poverty of the children in the studies may be at the same level or even higher than
that of most children in developing countries, the relative poverty of the U.S. children (i.e., their
income levels relative to others in their community) is far worse.
The effects of this poverty may differ. For example, if relative poverty motivates economic
initiative, then the children in the U.S. studies would be more motivated than would most
children in developing countries. But, if relative poverty generates greater discouragement, the
children in the U.S. studies would be less motivated than would most children in developing
countries.
6. High-Quality ECD Programs Have Certain Key Characteristics
To obtain the results achieved by the High/Scope Perry Preschool Program, an ECD program
must have characteristics similar to this program. Much of High/Scope’s success is attributable
to its preschool teachers. A quality ECD program has teachers who:
Are educationally qualified and trained in participatory education
Help children participate in their own education—by having them plan, do, and
review their own activities
Hold daily classes for children ages 3–4, including those at risk of school failure
Fulfill a 1:8 ratio of adults to children
Visit with families frequently to discuss their children’s development with them.
Table of Contents
76
IEA Pre-Primary Study
The results of this large, multinational, longitudinal study, which was sponsored by the
International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement (IEA), complement the
High/Scope findings and are especially relevant to developing countries. The study included
more than 5,000 children ages 4–7 years who were followed in nearly 2,000 settings across 15
countries in Africa, Asia, Europe, and North America (Montie, Xiang, and Schweinhart 2006).
The purpose was to identify how characteristics of pre-primary settings in various communities
affect children’s language and intellectual development at age 7.
In the study, four characteristics of preschool programs that predicted children’s abilities
later were:
Having free choice in participatory learning activities
Engaging in few whole-group activities
Having many, varied materials available
Having teachers with higher general levels of schooling.
Any limitations in the applicability of U.S. studies of ECD programs to developing countries
are not shared by the IEA Pre-primary Project. This project shows what works in a great variety
of countries.
¾ A summary of the IEA Pre-Primary Study is available at <http://www.iea.nl/ppp.html> or
<http://www.highscope.org/Research/international/iea_preprimary.htm>.
7. Evaluations Must Be Well-designed and Include Program-sensitive Outcome
Measures
ECD programs cannot be known to be successful unless they receive good evaluations. An
effective evaluation assesses three components of ECD programs:
Program implementation—to ensure that an ECD curriculum is being implemented as
expected
Teacher and child engagement—to ensure that teachers are actively engaged with
children and that children are actively engaged in learning
Child outcomes—to ensure that the program contributes to the children’s intellectual,
social, and physical development and motivation.
The evaluation must demonstrate that the children participating in the program are
developing better than they would without the program. Each component of the evaluation must
be carefully designed and incorporate program-sensitive measures. For example―
Table of Contents
77
For program implementation and teacher and child engagement, the evaluation design
should include visits by trained observers (data collectors) to see the programs in action.
To assess program implementation, data collectors would observe the program and
interview teachers. To assess teacher and child engagement, the data collectors would use
a systematic observation technique.
For child outcomes, the evaluation design should include assessments of children before
and after their participation in the program. Assessments would be made across a variety
of measures and compared to a standard. This standard for comparison may be the norms
for an assessment tool which provides norms, or the performance of a comparison group.
In a quasi-experimental study, the comparison group is a pre-existing group; in an
experimental study, the comparison group is a randomly assigned control group.
8. Child Assessment Tools Must Be Suitable to the Country and Culture
of the Children Who Are Being Assessed
A variety of tools are available for assessing child outcomes. The evaluation could include
systematic observation tools (e.g., the High/Scope Child Observation Record) and/or tests (e.g.,
the Woodcock-Johnson Achievement Test). Assuring that the tools which are used are suitable to
the children’s country or culture is often challenging—many instruments are developed in the
United States and may or may not be translated into various languages. The designers of an
evaluation study must closely scrutinize the instruments to be used to verify that they are aligned
with the program’s goals and the children’s culture.
9. Quality ECD Programs that Contribute to Children’s Development Are the
Fountainhead of Quality Education and, Hence, a Major Building Block in a
Country’s Economic Development
ECD programs may founder if they do not receive good evaluations. Providing custodial care to
children is not sufficient and does not fulfill the potential of ECD programs to contribute to
children’s development.
In his book The World Is Flat, Thomas Friedman (2005) makes a strong case that initiative
and nimble problem-solving are the keys to any country’s economic success in the world market.
Learning specific skills may be useful in one context, but irrelevant in a different context.
However, initiative and problem-solving are always useful, because they help people adapt to
changing contexts.
The High/Scope Perry Preschool Study has shown that a good ECD program
that focuses on initiative and problem-solving can be the first step toward
extraordinary economic and social benefits.
Table of Contents
78
10. Long-Term, Widespread Benefits and Returns Are Possible with a Variety of
High-Quality ECD Efforts
The High/Scope Perry Preschool Study is the first study to identify many long-term effects of an
ECD program—including economic return on investment—but it is not the only study to do so.
Three additional U.S. studies have documented important long-term benefits, including
economic return on investment, that can come from a variety of quality ECD programs.
In the North Carolina Abecedarian Project, Campbell and colleagues (2002) underscored
the point that high-quality childcare has long-term effects far exceeding those of typical
or low-quality childcare.
In the Chicago Child–Parent Centers study, Reynolds and colleagues (2001) noted the
long-term effects of this large-city service program.
In Elmira, New York, Olds and colleagues (1998) showed widespread, long-term effects
from a nurse home-visiting ECD program. This program is the one of few home-visiting
programs in the United States to show such strong effects, a finding that conveys the
value that skilled professionals can bring to ECD programs.
Web Resources [as of November 2006]
High/Scope Educational Research Foundations: <http://www.highscope.org>
Lawrence Schweinhart’s e-mail: <lschweinhart@highscope.org>
References
Campbell, F. A., C. T. Ramey, E. P. Pungello, J. Sparling, and S. Miller-Johnson. 2002. Early
Childhood Education: Young Adult Outcomes from the Abecedarian Project. Applied
Developmental Science 6:42–57.
Friedman, T. L. 2005. The World Is Flat: A Brief History of the Twenty-First Century. New
York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
Montie, J. E., Z. Xiang, and L. J. Schweinhart. 2006. Preschool Experience in 10 Countries:
Cognitive and Language Performance at Age 7. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 21.
Summary: <http://www.highscope.org/Research/international/iea_preprimary.htm>.
Olds, D. L., C. R. Henderson, Jr., R. Cole, J. Eckenrode, H. Kitzman, D. Luckey, L. M. Pettit, K.
Sidora, P. Morris, and J. Powers. 1998. Long-Term Effects of Nurse Home Visitation on
Children’s Criminal and Antisocial Behavior: 15-Year Follow-Up of a Randomized Trial.
Journal of the American Medical Association 280:1238–44.
Reynolds, A. J., J. A. Temple, D. L. Robertson, and E. A. Mann. 2001. Long-Term Effects of an
Early Childhood Intervention on Educational Achievement and Juvenile Arrest: A 15-Year
Table of Contents
79
Follow-Up of Low-Income Children in Public Schools. Journal of the American Medical
Association 285: 2339–46.
Schweinhart, L. J., J. Montie, Z. Xiang, W. S. Barnett, C. R. Belfield, and M. Nores. 2005.
Lifetime Effects: The High/Scope Perry Preschool Study through Age 40. Ypsilanti, Mich.:
High/Scope Press. Summaries:
<http://www.highscope.org/Research/PerryProject/perrymain.htm>
Weikart, D. P., D. Deloria, S. Lawser, and R. Wiegerink. 1970. Longitudinal Results of the
Ypsilanti Perry Preschool Project. Ypsilanti, Mich.: High/Scope Press.
Xiang, Z., and L. J. Schweinhart. 2002. Effects Five Years Later: The Michigan School
Readiness Program Evaluation through Age 10. Ypsilanti, Mich.: High/Scope Press.
<www.highscope.org/Research/MsrpEvaluation/msrp-Age10-2.pdf>
Table of Contents