ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

This article describes a study that examined the development of expository writing in children and adolescents, using a task that could easily be administered by the speechlanguage pathologist to students in their classrooms at school. The task is presented and methods for analyzing and interpreting the results using a computer program are described. Excerpts from essays that were written by children and adolescents with typical development are discussed.
Content may be subject to copyright.
100
Unless otherwise noted, the publisher, which is the American Speech-Language-
Hearing Association (ASHA), holds the copyright on all materials published in
Perspectives on Language Learning and Education, both as a compilation and as
individual articles. Please see Rights and Permissions for terms and conditions of
use of Perspectives content: http://journals.asha.org/perspectives/terms.dtl
Expository Writing in Children and Adolescents: A
Classroom Assessment Tool
Marilyn A. Nippold
University of Oregon
Eugene, OR
Lei Sun
Los Angeles Unified School District
Los Angeles, CA
Abstract
This article describes a study that examined the development of expository writing in
children and adolescents, using a task that could easily be administered by the speech-
language pathologist to students in their classrooms at school. The task is presented and
methods for analyzing and interpreting the results using a computer program are
described. Excerpts from essays that were written by children and adolescents with
typical development are discussed.
Speech-language pathologists (SLPs) who work with children and adolescents in the
schools frequently conduct evaluations to identify students with language disorders, qualify
them for speech-language services, and plan interventions. Although norm-referenced
standardized tests such as the Test of Language Development: Primary–Fourth Edition
(TOLD:P-4; Newcomer & Hammill, 2008), the Test of Language Development: Intermediate –
Fourth Edition (TOLD:I-4; Hammill & Newcomer, 2008), and the Clinical Evaluation of
Language Fundamentals–Fourth Edition (CELF-4; Semel, Wiig, & Secord, 2003) can be helpful
in this process, they do not provide detailed information about how a child or adolescent
actually uses language to communicate in natural settings such as the classroom. To obtain
this type of data, it is necessary to collect and analyze spoken and written language samples
(Nippold, 2010b).
The profession of speech-language pathology has a strong tradition of using language
samples to gain information about language development (Heilmann, Miller, & Nockerts, 2010;
Nippold, 2010b). Fortunately, normative databases are available for children and adolescents
speaking in conversational, narrative, and expository genres (Miller, 2009; Miller & Iglesias,
2008). In contrast to spoken language, however, less information is available on written
language. Because written language is critical for school success, it is important to establish
normative databases in this modality. Expository discourse, the use of language to convey
information, is particularly worthy of attention because it is the predominant genre used in the
classroom beginning in fourth grade and continuing through high school (Nippold & Scott,
2010). During these years, students are expected to use expository discourse when writing
term papers, essays, and reports that focus on complex topics in disciplines such as biology,
economics, history, and social studies. Success with expository discourse requires, among
many things, a more sophisticated level of syntactic development than do other genres, a
research finding that has been replicated across many languages (Berman & Verhoeven, 2002;
101
Heilmann et al., 2010; Nippold, 2009; Nippold, Hesketh, Duthie, & Mansfield, 2005; Nippold,
Mansfield, Billow, & Tomblin, 2008, 2009; Scott & Windsor, 2000; Verhoeven et al., 2002).
However, students with language disorders frequently exhibit syntactic deficits in expository
discourse (Nippold et al., 2008, 2009; Scott & Windsor, 2002). This suggests that syntactic
development in expository writing should be evaluated.
Thus, the present study was designed to begin to establish a normative database in
expository writing for children and adolescents. An essay task was employed that could be
administered easily to students in their classrooms at school. To simplify the process of
analyzing the essays, we elected to use the computer program, Systematic Analysis of
Language Transcripts (SALT; Miller & Iglesias, 2008), because once an essay has been entered
into SALT, the program automatically calculates and reports numerous variables. One key
variable is mean length of T-unit (MLTU), a measure of syntactic development. A T-unit, similar
to an utterance, consists of one main clause and any subordinate clauses that are attached to
it (Hunt, 1970). The sentence “Friendship is a relationship between people who spend time
together” is a T-unit that contains one main clause (Friendship is a relationship between
people) and one subordinate clause (who spend time together). Studies have shown that MLTU
gradually increases during childhood, adolescence, and into adulthood (Berman & Verhoeven,
2002; Hunt, 1970; Loban, 1976; Nippold et al., 2005). Other key variables reported by SALT
include total words (TW) and total T-units (TTU) produced. Because language productivity as
measured by TW and TTU partially may reflect a student’s knowledge of the topic (Nippold,
2010b), these variables, which can be obtained quickly and easily, are important to examine as
well.
The essay task focused on friendship. This topic was chosen because peer relationships
gradually become more important to young people as they make the transition from childhood
to adulthood (Santrock, 1996; Schickedanz, Schickedanz, Forsyth, & Forsyth, 2001). Given
that children and adolescents are more likely to use complex language when discussing
subjects that interest them and about which they are knowledgeable (Nippold, 2009, 2010a), it
was thought that the topic of friendship might elicit strong expository writing from the
participants in the study. It also was thought that the task might reveal differences in syntactic
development and in language productivity. Thus, we compared children (fifth graders) to
adolescents (eighth graders) on the basis of MLTU, TW, and TTU. Research also has shown that
during the years between late childhood and early adolescence, girls spend more time
socializing with friends than do boys (Raffaelli & Duckett, 1989). For this reason, it also was
important to examine the possibility of a gender difference to determine if the task was more
appropriate for girls than for boys.
In sum, the present study posed these questions:
1. Would adolescents outperform children on an expository writing task in terms of
a. Syntactic development?
b. Language productivity?
2. Would boys and girls at both grade levels differ in their performance on the task?
Methods
The participants included 40 fifth-grade children (23 boys; 17 girls) and 40 eighth-grade
adolescents (20 boys; 20 girls) who were attending public schools in a rural town in western
Oregon, populated by middle- to lower-income families. The fifth graders (mean age=11;1,
range=10;5-11;8) attended an elementary school that fed into the middle school that the eighth
graders (mean age=14;1, range=13;4-14;10) attended. According to teacher report, all
participants were considered to be typical achievers who spoke Standard American English as
their primary language. None were receiving special education services at the time of the study.
102
The students were asked to write an expository essay on the topic of friendship. The
task was administered in large-group fashion in their classrooms at school during language
arts or English. The investigators provided each student with a booklet that contained a set of
written instructions and a 7-point outline, shown in Figure 1, and four pages of lined paper for
writing the essay. The outline prompted the students to address a number of key issues
concerning the nature of friendship and helped them to organize their thoughts. Before the
students began writing, one investigator read the instructions and outline aloud to the class
while the other investigator walked around the classroom to ensure that all students were
following along. Students were allowed 20 minutes to complete their work. After time was
called, their booklets were collected.
Figure 1. Expository Writing Task: “The Nature of Friendship” (Nippold, 2010b, p. 48)
Each student’s hand-written essay was typed verbatim, segmented into T-units, and
entered into SALT (Miller & Iglesias, 2008) by one of the investigators. The other investigator or
a research assistant double-checked each essay. Any discrepancies in content or segmenting
were discussed and resolved. Very few discrepancies occurred. The following dependent
variables, reported by SALT, were recorded for each participant: MLTU, TW, and TTU.
Results
103
Table 1. Performance of the groups on the expository writing task (n=40 per group).
Fifth Grade Eighth Grade
Boys Girls All Boys Girls All
Mean Length of T-Unit (MLTU)
Mean 11.76 13.11 12.33 15.09 13.97 14.53
SD 2.95 2.97 3 3.51 2.65 3.13
Range 5.80-17.60 7.74-17.67 5.80-17.67 8.94-24.50 7.39-18.08 7.39-24.50
Total Words (TW)
Mean 112.43 176.29 139.58 181.75 235.05 208.40
SD 39.08 65.38 60.31 70.41 50.10 66.08
Range 29-178 89-346 29-346 48-339 133-321 48-339
Total T-Units (TTU)
Mean 9.61 14.12 11.53 13.10 17.25 15.18
SD 2.78 6.2 5.02 7.77 4.18 6.5
Range 5-16 7-31 5-31 3-33 11-25 3-33
Table 1 reports the mean raw scores, standards deviations, and ranges obtained on the
expository writing task. The data were analyzed using a 2 (grade) x 2 (gender) analysis of
variance (ANOVA) for each dependent variable. Effect sizes were computed using the eta (η)
coefficient (Meline & Schmitt, 1997), and based on Cohen’s (1969, p. 276) recommendation,
they were interpreted as small (η=.10-.23), medium (η=.24-.36), or large (η=.37-.71).
For mean length of T-unit (MLTU), a statistically significant main effect was obtained for
grade [F (1, 76)=10.43, p=.0018, η=.35] but not for gender [F (1, 76)=.15, p=.6996, η=.04]. The
effect size for grade was medium. For total words (TW), statistically significant main effects
were obtained for grade [F (1, 76)=29.52, p<.0001, η=.53] and gender [F (1, 76)=24.89,
p<.0001, η=.50], and both effect sizes were large. Similarly, for total T-units (TTU), statistically
significant main effects were obtained for grade [F (1, 76)=8.95, p=.0037, η=.32] and gender [F
(1, 76)=14.00, p=.0004, η=.39]; the effect sizes were medium and large, respectively. No
interactions between grade and gender were statistically significant: MLTU [F (1, 76)=3.13,
p=.0807, η=.20]; TW [F (1, 76)=0, p=1, η=0]; TTU [F (1, 76)=0, p=1, η=0]. Thus, the eighth
graders outperformed the fifth graders on syntactic development (MLTU) and language
productivity (TW, TTU). Moreover, at both grades, girls outperformed boys on language
productivity (TU, TTU) but not on syntactic development (MLTU).
Discussion
The purpose of the study was to begin the process of establishing a normative database
for expository writing in children and adolescents. The task, an essay on the nature of
friendship, successfully elicited expository writing in fifth grade children and eighth grade
adolescents, revealing developmental gains in syntax and language production, with the
adolescents producing longer T-units and a greater number of words and T-units than the
children. At both grades, however, girls wrote significantly more than boys, perhaps reflecting
differences between the sexes in topic knowledge, interest, and/or motivation. This suggests
that separate sets of normative data for boys and girls should be established for this particular
writing task.
104
It is interesting to consider how sentences change in relation to age. Prior
developmental research has established that during childhood, adolescence, and into
adulthood, people gradually produce a greater number of subordinate clauses, resulting in
longer, denser, and more informative sentences (Nippold, 2007). This pattern can be illustrated
by comparing sentences that were produced by participants in the study who were addressing
the same points in their essays. Consider the excerpts shown in Figure 2. Writer #1, a fifth
grade girl and Writer #2, an eighth grade girl, were addressing Questions 1 and 2 (What is
friendship? Why is it important to people?). Writer #1 produced 3 T-units and 4 subordinate
clauses, whereas Writer #2 produced 6 T-units and 10 subordinate clauses, evidencing greater
productivity and syntactic complexity. Similar patterns can be seen by comparing Writer #3, a
fifth grade boy, to Writer #4, an eighth grade boy, who were addressing Questions 6 and 7
(“What kinds of actions can damage friendships? How can people remain good friends over
time?). Writer #3 produced only 2 T-units and 4 subordinate clauses, whereas Writer #4
produced 6 T-units and 9 subordinate clauses. Additionally, the two older writers were more
likely to embed subordinate clauses within other subordinate clauses, a phenomenon called
hierarchical complexity (Nippold, 2010b). For example, Writer #2’s most complex T-unit
contained 4 subordinate clauses, 2 of which (that you trust, can count on) were embedded
within the first (when there is someone); moreover, the fourth subordinate clause (whenever
you need them) in that T-unit was embedded within the third (can count on) subordinate
clause, creating multiple layers of complexity. Similarly, Writer #4’s most complex T-unit
contained 3 subordinate clauses, 2 of which (that you know, they won’t like) were embedded
within the first (if you intentionally do something). In these examples, the two older writers (#2
and #4) also seemed to express a greater understanding of the topic of friendship than the two
younger ones (#1 and #3), a pattern that can be expected to emerge in view of research in
socio-emotional development (Santrock, 1996; Schickedanz et al., 2001). A question for future
research would be to determine to what extent these age-related gains in expository writing
reflect growth in topic knowledge, for research (Nippold, 2009) has shown that when students
are discussing complicated topics about which they are knowledgeable, their language use is
more sophisticated than when they are discussing simpler, more familiar topics.
105
Figure 2. Excerpts from essays on friendship written by fifth and eighth grade students in the present study
(subordinate clauses have been highlighted with underlining)
Given the positive findings of this study, it is reasonable to propose that the task be
used as a classroom assessment tool. To obtain local normative data, SLPs and classroom
teachers in elementary and middle schools could work together to administer the task to
typical students (both boys and girls) at various grade levels in their district. The handwritten
essays could be typed, segmented into T-units, and entered into SALT (perhaps with the
assistance of university students majoring in communication sciences and disorders) and
examined for MLTU, TW, and TTU—variables that provide valuable information that is quickly
and easily obtained. Then, the results for each variable (means, standard deviations, and
ranges) could serve as a useful reference point for comparing students who struggle with
expository writing because of a language disorder. Once a student with a language disorder has
completed the task, the SLP could evaluate the essay by entering it into SALT, and examining it
for these variables, noting how far below the mean for the student’s grade and gender he or she
falls. It also may be useful to examine the essay informally for content, clarity, and topic
knowledge. Based on these formal and informal analyses, the SLP will have a clear
106
understanding of the student’s strengths and weaknesses in expository writing, and be able to
plan appropriate, individualized treatment with confidence.
Marilyn A. Nippold, PhD, is a professor and ASHA Fellow in the Communication Disorders
and Sciences Program at the University of Oregon in Eugene, Oregon. Her research and clinical
interests focus on School-age and adolescent language development and disorders.
Lei Sun, PhD, is a speech-language pathologist in Los Angeles Unified School District Los
Angeles, California. She specializes in school-age and adolescent language development and
disorders.
References
Berman, R. A., & Verhoeven, L. (2002). Cross-linguistic perspectives on the development of text-
production abilities: Speech and writing. Written Language and Literacy, 5(1), 1-43.
Cohen, J. (1969). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York, NY: Academic Press.
Hammill, D. D., & Newcomer, P. L. (2008). Test of Language Development: Intermediate (4
th
ed.). Austin,
TX: PRO-ED.
Heilmann, J. J., Miller, J. F., & Nockerts, A. (2010). Using language sample databases. Language, Speech,
and Hearing Services in Schools, 41, 84-95.
Hunt, K. W. (1970). Syntactic maturity in school children and adults. Monographs of the Society for
Research in Child Development, 35(1), 1-67.
Loban, W. (1976). Language development: Kindergarten through grade twelve. Urbana, IL: National
Council of Teachers of English.
Meline, T., & Schmitt, J. F. (1997). Case studies for evaluating statistical significance in group designs.
American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 6, 33-41.
Miller, J. F. (2009). New database: Helps identify, monitor older students. Advance for Speech-Language
Pathologists & Audiologists, 19(8), 4-7.
Miller, J. F., & Iglesias, A. (2008). Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT), English & Spanish
(Version 9) [Computer software]. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin-Madison, Waisman Center,
Language Analysis Laboratory. http://www.languageanalysislab.com/
Newcomer, P. L., & Hammill, D. D. (2008). Test of language development: Primary (4th ed.). Austin, TX:
PRO-ED.
Nippold, M. A. (2007). Later language development: School-age children, adolescents, and young adults
(3rd Ed.). Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
Nippold, M. A. (2009). School-age children talk about chess: Does knowledge drive syntactic complexity?
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 856-871.
Nippold, M. A. (2010a). Explaining complex matters: How knowledge of a domain drives language. In M.
A. Nippold & C. M. Scott (Eds.), Expository discourse in children, adolescents, and adults: Development
and disorders (pp. 41-61). New York, NY: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis.
Nippold, M. A. (2010b). Language sampling with adolescents. San Diego, CA: Plural.
Nippold, M. A., Hesketh, L. J., Duthie, J. K., & Mansfield, T. C. (2005). Conversational versus expository
discourse: A study of syntactic development in children, adolescents, and adults. Journal of Speech,
Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 1048-1064.
Nippold, M. A., Mansfield, T. C., Billow, J. L., & Tomblin, J. B. (2008). Expository discourse in adolescents
with language impairments: Examining syntactic development. American Journal of Speech-Language
Pathology, 17, 356-366.
Nippold, M. A., Mansfield, T. C., Billow, J. L., & Tomblin, J. B. (2009). Syntactic development in
adolescents with a history of language impairments: A follow-up investigation. American Journal of
Speech-Language Pathology, 18, 241-251.
107
Nippold, M. A., & Scott, C. M. (2010). Overview of expository discourse: Development and disorders. In M.
A. Nippold & C. M. Scott (Eds.), Expository discourse in children, adolescents, and adults: Development
and disorders (pp. 1-11). New York, NY: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis.
Raffaelli, M., & Duckett, E. (1989). “We were just talking…”: Conversations in early adolescence. Journal
of Youth and Adolescence, 18, 567-582.
Santrock, J. W. (1996). Adolescence: An introduction (6th Ed.). Madison, WI: Brown & Benchmark.
Schickedanz, J. A., Schickedanz, D. I., Forsyth, P. D., & Forsyth, G. A. (2001). Understanding children
and adolescents (4th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Scott, C. M., & Windsor, J. (2000). General language performance measures in spoken and written
narrative and expository discourse in school-age children with language learning disabilities. Journal of
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 324-339.
Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (2003). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals (4
th
ed.). San
Antonio, TX: Pearson.
Verhoeven, L., Aparici, M., Cahana-Amitay, D., van Hell, J., Kriz, S., & Viguie-Simon, A. (2002). Clause
packaging in writing and speech: A cross-linguistic developmental analysis. Written Language and
Literacy, 5(2), 135-162.
... Expository discourse, on the other hand, develops through adolescence into adulthood (Berman and Verhoeven 2002;Berman and Katzenberger 2004;Tolchinsky, Johansson, and Zamora 2002). Productivity, for instance, has been shown to increase greatly between late childhood and early adolescence (Berman and Verhoeven 2002;Nippold and Sun 2010;Ravid, Dromi and Kotler 2010). Moreover, expository text construction at the global level develops more slowly than narrative text construction (Berman and Nir-Sagiv 2007). ...
... Productivity was calculated in terms of the total number of words produced, a measure that is commonly used in this line of research (Berman and Verhoeven 2002;Nippold and Sun 2010;Scott and Windsor 2000). The results are presented in Table 2. To assist the reader visualize the data, Figure 1 shows the development in text length for all texts across our sample. ...
... However, with age, expositions became longer and reached the narrative in length. This gradual increase, which is in accordance with the findings of previous studies (Berman and Verhoeven 2002;Nippold and Sun 2010;Ravid, Dromi and Kotler 2010), may be a reflection of the learners' accumulating knowledge on the subject. In addition to this, it may have resulted from the increased cognitive demands that exposition imposes on younger children in terms of discourse planning. ...
Article
Full-text available
Expository discourse, i.e., the use of language to communicate information about a topic, calls upon users’ advanced language skills, thus offering a unique opportunity to study later language development. Furthermore, exposition is of enormous importance in education, closely related to academic success. Nevertheless, research examining the developing ability of students to produce expository texts is relatively recent, and for Modern Greek quite limited. Relevant studies have indicated that the development of the linguistic skills necessary for mature expository text production is a process that develops gradually, all through adolescence into adulthood. This cross-sectional study investigates the developmental trajectories of two expository and one narrative text in Modern Greek, and probes into productivity and syntactic complexity at both clausal and subclausal level. Three written texts were elicited from students aged 10, 13, and 16 years, as well as adults aged 25–35 years. Results revealed that the younger participants produced shorter expository than narrative texts. However, with age, expositions became longer and reached the narrative in length. With regard to syntactic complexity, expository writing gave even young learners the opportunity to unfold more advanced language skills than narrative writing. However, age did not lead to an increase in subordinate clause use in either of the expository texts. Subclausal syntactic complexity proved more sensitive in capturing age-related progress in these texts. Overall, it was revealed that the development of mature expository skills is complex, continues into adulthood and has distinct characteristics for each expository text. Finally, in narratives, syntactic complexity increased with age at both the clausal and subclausal level. These results are discussed in terms of the cognitive demands each text type creates on language users, and future research directions are suggested.
... We know that in addition to reader competencies that depend on text genre (Best, Floyd & McNamara, 2008), text production abilities are also highly dependent on text genre (Berman & Nir-Sagiv, 2009;Brandes & Ravid, 2016. A plethora of developmental psycholinguistic studies have examined narrative versus expository writing across the school years (Berman, 2008;Berman & Katzenberger, 2004;Nippold & Scott, 2010;Nippold & Sun, 2010;, focusing on text content, discourse structure, lexicon, and morphosyntax. These studies indicate that growing familiarity with different text types and genres enables writers to employ various genre-appropriate complex syntactic constructions by adolescence (Aparici, Rosado, & Perera, 2016;Brimo & Hall-Mills, 2019;Ravid, 2005Ravid, , 2013Ravid, Dromi & Kotler, 2009). ...
... They are characterized by complex syntax (both subordinating and coordinating), heavy NPs, non-dynamic constructions, passive voice, non-finite verbs, and epistemic hypothetical constructions. Thus, it takes the whole span of adolescence to achieve writing skills in expositories (Cutillas & Tolchinsky, 2017;Nippold & Sun, 2010;Oblinghouse & Wilson, 2013;Ravid & Levie, 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
Writing is the highway to learning, processing, organizing, storing and retrieving information during the school years. Gaining command of the structures and functions of texts of different genres is one of the main goals of linguistic literacy in education. The texts that school-goers construct provide optimal hunting grounds for unveiling their linguistic abilities during the genre-specific construction of different text types. The current study examines the linguistic constructions typical of expository texts, characterized by argumentation or persuasion regarding social / conceptual issues, versus informative texts, describing factual (or less controversial) phenomena. This examination constitutes a window on Hebrew-speaking students’ developing writing abilities in elementary, middle and high school, compared with adults. Participants were 547 students and educated adults with post-high school education. Each participant wrote an informative text and an expository text, yielding a total of 1,094 texts. Three types of variables were examined: lexical, morpho-syntactic and syntactic. We found that complex lexicon and syntax in discourse increased in prevalence across the school years, with specific structures being genre-typical as befits the stance and character of the two genres under investigation. We also found that it was only in adulthood that all of these components were employed in optimal fashion, at the culmination of the period of Later Language Development. Our results imply that informative and expository texts are indeed distinct in their characteristics, and that it takes many years of internal linguistic and cognitive development, on the one hand, and schooling instruction and experience, on the other hand, to achieve qualitative academic writing in non-experts.
... Expository discourse begins to develop later than conversation and narration. Procedural description, persuasion, negotiation and explanation are all forms of expository discourse (Nippold et al., 2007;Nippold and Sun, 2010). Expository discourse emerges within conversations in the preschool period (Cabell et al., 2011) but becomes more prevalent in children's experiences once schooling begins and is increasingly more frequent in their spoken language at that time (Nippold and Sun, 2010). ...
... Procedural description, persuasion, negotiation and explanation are all forms of expository discourse (Nippold et al., 2007;Nippold and Sun, 2010). Expository discourse emerges within conversations in the preschool period (Cabell et al., 2011) but becomes more prevalent in children's experiences once schooling begins and is increasingly more frequent in their spoken language at that time (Nippold and Sun, 2010). In narration, children begin to . ...
Article
Full-text available
The COVID-19 pandemic has created novel challenges in the assessment of children’s speech and language. Collecting valid data is crucial for researchers and clinicians, yet the evidence on how data collection procedures can validly be adapted to an online format is sparse. The urgent need for online assessments has not decreased the barriers that clinicians face during implementation. The current study emerged from the project ‘French/English Discourse Study – Canada’ (FrEnDS-CAN), which focuses on a variety of discourse skills in typically developing monolingual and bilingual school-aged children. The present study describes the adapted procedures and compares the outcomes of online and in-person testing using discourse samples and standardized vocabulary testing for monolingual and bilingual children. For the present analyses, 127 (103 in-person, 24 online) English monolinguals and 78 (53 in-person, 25 online) simultaneous French-English bilinguals from ages 7-12 were studied. Several measures were analyzed : receptive vocabulary, conversational discourse, expository discourse, and narrative discourse. Across these measures, productivity, proficiency, and syntactic complexity were calculated . Bayesian statistics and MANOVAs were run to assess the comparability of the two contexts. This study found that there were no differences across testing contexts for receptive vocabulary or narrative discourse. However, some differences were found for conversational and expository discourse. The results from the study contribute to understanding how clinical assessment can be adapted for online format in school-aged children.
... Este modelo lingüístico, sistémico y funcional propone un sistema de la valoración 1 para el análisis de la evaluación textualizada, compuesto por tres subsistemas: la actitud, el compromiso y la graduación (Martin & White, 2005). En el presente capítulo mostramos el análisis del funcionamiento de las selecciones lingüísticas valorativas que se encuentran dentro del primero de esos subsistemas, el de actitud, en la estructura argumentativa de breves ensayos escritos por los estudiantes de bachillerato de nuestro estudio, quienes se encuentran todavía en las etapas tardías del desarrollo lingüístico-discursivo Nippold et al., 2005;Nippold & Sun, 2010;Nippold & Ward-Lonergan, 2010). ...
Book
Full-text available
A lo largo de las últimas cuatro décadas, los estudios psicolingüísticos han confirmado cada vez más que el desarrollo lingüístico no termina en la infancia temprana, sino que continúa durante los años escolares, y va más allá de la adolescencia e incluso ya entrada la adultez. Los estudios sobre la adquisición del lenguaje han mostrado que durante las etapas tardías se adquiere un sinnúmero de habilidades en todos los niveles lingüísticos (fonología, morfología, sintaxis, semántica, pragmática y discurso). Lo anterior se debe a que, cuando ingresan al sistema educativo, niñas y niños se enfrentan a nuevas experiencias comunicativas que les exigen apropiarse de capacidades lingüísticas cada vez más complejas. En el presente libro se conjuntan nueve investigaciones psicolingüísticas que buscan dar cuenta del desarrollo tardío del lenguaje en poblaciones hispanohablantes. Para ello se recopilan estudios realizados en tres países (Argentina, Chile y México) organizados en cinco ejes temáticos: lectura, escritura, discurso narrativo, discurso argumentativo y lenguaje no literal. Mediante estos trabajos de investigación se busca presentar un panorama general de los estudios recientes sobre la manera en que niños y adolescentes hispanohablantes continúan su desarrollo lingüístico más allá de la infancia. Este libro se convierte por ello en una contribución importante para la psicología, la lingüística y la educación en nuestra lengua.
... studies, and mostly on homogeneous middle-class samples (Christie & Derewianka, 2008;Nippold & Sun, 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
Mid-adolescence is a period of considerable potential growth in the language for academic writing. Yet, to date, few writing studies explore language development during this period and even fewer focus on longitudinal or diverse samples. In this study, we examined the development of language skills for academic writing in a socio-economically diverse sample followed from sixth to seventh grade ( n = 124). In each grade, participants wrote summaries of a science text. Subsequently, summaries were scored for writing quality (WQ) and analyzed for productive language skills (lexico-syntactic and discourse features). Participants completed a receptive academic language assessment and a test that measured reading comprehension of the source text. First, we examined if WQ or productive language skills changed over time. Next, we tested if Grade 6 productive and receptive language skills predicted Grade 7 WQ. Results revealed syntactic growth over time. Grade 6 use of connectives and receptive language skills emerged as predictors of Grade 7 WQ.
Article
Full-text available
With this article I explore the relationship between core academic language skills (CALS) – a construct representing a set of high-utility language skills needed to manage the linguistic features prevalent in academic texts across content areas – and schooling outcomes. There has long been an understanding that there is a distinction between academic language and colloquial language, originally described by Jim Cummins (1976) as cognitive academic language proficiency (CALP) and basic interpersonal communication skills (BICS). The construct has only recently been operationalised (Uccelli, Phillips Galloway, Barr, Meneses & Dobbs, 2015) as the individual skills and competencies that underlie CALP. The CALS construct describes an empirically testable set of competencies that address this need. This has been used in the development of an assessment instrument aimed at South African learners – the CALS-I-ZA (MacFarlane, Barr & Uccelli, 2022) – and I investigate whether a measurable link exists between this assessment and schooling outcomes for a sample of Grade 6 learners in 2 public schools in the Gauteng province of South Africa. Schooling outcomes have been measured using the Gauteng Provincial Common Assessments – a provincial examination intended to measure schooling outcomes on a comparable assessment instrument. The study reported on here shows a moderately strong correlation between the CALS I ZA and the provincial common examinations (r = 0.64 and r = 0.65). This predictive relationship between CALS and schooling outcomes leads to an argument for direct instruction in CALS as an embedded feature of pedagogy in South Africa.
Article
The article applies the multiple language-levels (MLL) model to alter identification perspectives in writing assessment from language abilities that are “missing” in students with specific learning disabilities (SLDs) to writing assessment as the discovery of more complex language strengths foundational to success across disciplines. The compositions of 19 students in grades 4–9 who were classified with SLDs and struggled with academic writing were examined. The writing assignment was to explain how astronauts wrote about space exploration. Highlighting four case examples, the MLL approach uncovered patterns of strengths at the global (big picture) and local (close-up) levels of composing. Classroom implications and treatment suggestions are provided.
Article
Analytical writing poses particularly challenging, yet often overlooked, language demands that need attention in educational research and practice. In this article, I discuss the Core Analytical Language Skills (CALS) construct and its relevance for school reading and writing. CALS refer to the set of learners’ school-relevant language resources that are of high utility to understanding analytical texts across content areas. After a brief review of the relations between mid-adolescents’ language and their school reading and writing proficiencies, I offer illustrative examples of individual differences in middle-schoolers’ analytical writing and CALS. I argue, on the basis of recent but extensive empirical evidence, that without understanding and addressing the immense variability in the language resources that students bring to school and the language demands of reading- and writing-to-learn tasks and texts, schools run the risk of maintaining and even exacerbating the inequalities that exist in the larger society.
Article
Language learning is context-dependent and requires learners to employ different sets of language skills to fulfill various tasks. Yet standardized English as a foreign language assessments tend to conceptualize English proficiency as a unidimensional construct. In order to distinguish English proficiency as separate context-driven constructs, I adopted a register-based approach to investigate academic English proficiency (i.e., specific set of language skills that support academic literacy) and general English proficiency (i.e., wide range of language skills undifferentiated by context that are measured by traditional assessments) as separate predictors of overall essay quality. In the study, students completed a general English proficiency assessment and an academic language proficiency assessment, and essays were coded for academic writing features at the lexical, syntactic, and discourse levels. Beyond the contribution of academic writing features and general English proficiency, academic English proficiency emerged as a significant contributor to essay quality. Findings suggest that academic English proficiency scores more precisely identified a subset of academic language skills that is relevant to essay writing. The article concludes by discussing implications for strategic writing instruction that articulates the key expectations of academic writing used in and beyond school contexts.
Article
Competent writing eludes many school children and adolescents, especially those with developmental language and reading disorders. Language sample analysis (LSA) of writing is a powerful but underused assessment tool that can also inform instruction. Similar to LSA of oral language, writing has been analyzed at word, sentence, and text levels using measures that include lexical diversity, sentence length, clause density, grammaticality, productivity, and text organization and content. Unique observations of writing include spelling and literate semantic and syntactic features. This article offers a review of analytic writing measures in the LSA literature from perspectives that include developmental change, language ability differences, relation to quality ratings, practical utility, and effects of genre and task. Writing samples from two 12-year-old students, with and without a language disorder, are used to illustrate application of these measures and suggest potential instruction targets.
Article
Full-text available
Marilyn A Nippold, Ph.D., is internationally recognized for her scholarship in later language development. Her research serves as the foundation for understanding the protracted course of language development in school-age children, adolescents, and young adults, and for recognizing the difficulties that young people sometimes encounter in attaining linguistic proficiency. A Fellow of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASHA) and an Endowed Professor, Dr. Nippold has taught in the Communication Disorders and Sciences program at the University of Oregon for over 20 years. She has published two books and 70 articles and chapters, and has made over 100 presentations at state, national, and international conferences. Dr. Nippold's recent research projects have examined morphological analysis, figurative language comprehension, literacy, and spoken and written language development in school-age children, adolescents, and adults. She also has conducted crosscultural and crosslinguistic studies of later language development with colleagues in New Zealand, Australia, and Israel.
Article
Full-text available
We present a tutorial for evaluating statistical significance in research reports when t, F, or X² is the primary statistic. The article is intended to help speech-language pathologists evaluate research conclusions so that they can decide if the result is meaningful for their practice. To this end, we explain the value of "practical significance" and describe the metric known as "effect size." The use of effect-size measures is illustrated in a variety of case studies that are drawn from the communication disorders literature.
Article
Full-text available
We present a tutorial for evaluating statistical significance in research reports when t, F, or χ2 is the primary statistic. The article is intended to help speech-language pathologists evaluate research conclusions so that they can decide if the result is meaningful for their practice. To this end, we explain the value of "practical significance" and describe the metric known as "effect size." The use of effect-size measures is illustrated in a variety of case studies that are drawn from the communication disorders literature.
Article
Full-text available
This article analyses clause packaging in the written narrative and expository texts in Dutch, English, French, Hebrew, and Spanish by grade-school children and adults. A “clause package“ is defined as a text unit consisting of clauses linked by syntactic, thematic, and discursive criteria. Our study attempts to arrive at an understanding of cross-linguistic universals in the distribution of clause-package types as a function of Genre and Modality in both children and adults. To this end, we distinguish finite vs. nonfinite coordination, subordination, and relativization; we compare their distribution in the five languages; and we examine the semantics of the conjunctions occurring in narrative vs. expository texts. Results show that, in all languages, children tend to use more co-ordinate devices than adults, whereas adults use more adverbial, complement, and relative clauses. A clear interaction was found between Genre and type of clause packaging: For both children and adults, coordination was the preferred device in narrative, whereas subordination occurred more often in the expository texts.
Article
Full-text available
The studies reported in this volume of WL&L (5: 1–2, 2002) all derive from a joint project entitled “Developing literacy in different contexts and in different languages”, funded by the Spencer Foundation, Chicago. The study encompasses seven languages — Dutch, English, French, Hebrew, Icelandic, Spanish, and Swedish — for which data were collected in Europe, Israel, and the US by graduate research assistants in education, linguistics, and psychology, under the supervision of a project director in each country — each of whom is listed as a first or “lead” author in the articles which follow the introduction to this collection.
Article
Full-text available
Expository discourse, the use of language to convey information, is frequently called on in learning environments where individuals are expected to listen, speak, read, write, and analyze new concepts. In schools, for example, this occurs as students learn about challenging subjects such as economics, astronomy, and mathematics. This book was designed to provide information on the use and understanding of expository discourse in spoken and written language in school-age children, adolescents, and young adults. Recently, researchers have been investigating the development of this genre in typical students and in those with language disorders. Although there are many books that address the development of narrative discourse in both types of students, by comparison, contributions on expository discourse are sparse. This book has been designed to address this knowledge gap. Chapters 2 through 6 discuss the development of expository discourse, covering comprehension and production, and chapters 7 through 11 discuss the challenges faced by students with developmental or acquired language disorders, focusing on the nature of their problems and appropriate methods of assessment and intervention, given what has been learned from research. In this edited volume, invited authors include professors, research associates, and clinicians at universities in the United States, France, Israel, and New Zealand. Representing international perspectives, it contains information on speakers of English in addition to speakers of other languages, including French and Hebrew. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
This paper explores young adolescents' experience of talk, examining changes in boys' and girls' patterns of communication with family and friends. The data consist of immediate self-reports provided by 401 5th-9th grade students during the course of one week of their normal lives. Results indicate that while time spent talking to friends increased dramatically across this age period, especially for girls, talk with family members remained stable. Analysis of topics of conversation suggests that older children turned to friends for discussions of age-related concerns while continuing to discuss daily issues with family members. Talk with friends did not appear to replace talk with family members but rather represented a new facet of the social world, supplementing existing family relationships.