ArticlePDF Available

Promoting Children's Social and Emotional Development Through Preschool Education

Authors:
  • Tools of the Mind
  • Tool of the Mind

Abstract

must be able to understand the feelings of others, control their own feelings and behaviors, and get along with their peers and teachers. Children need to be able to cooperate, follow directions, demonstrate self-control, and "pay attention." Unfortunately, many students preschool experiences do not fully support their social and emotional development. This policy brief describes the importance of social and emotional development for children in their earliest years and as they grow older and describes the characteristics of those preschool education programs that best support these aspects of development.
NIEER
Promoting Children’s Social
and Emotional Development
Through Preschool Education
by Judi Boyd, W. Steven Barnett, Elena Bodrova,
Deborah J. Leong, and Deanna Gomby
Children need a combination of intellectual skills, motivational
qualities, and socioemotional skills to succeed in school.1They
must be able to understand the feelings of others, control their
own feelings and behaviors, and get along with their peers and
teachers. Children need to be able to cooperate, follow directions,
demonstrate self-control, and “pay attention.” Unfortunately,
many students preschool experiences do not fully support their
social and emotional development. This policy brief describes
the importance of social and emotional development for children
in their earliest years and as they grow older and describes the
characteristics of those preschool education programs that
best support these aspects of development.
XX, Issue XX
Series edited by
Ellen Frede and
W. Steven Barnett
Preschool Policy Brief
What We Know:
• Kindergarten teachers say that about 20
percent of children entering kindergarten
do not yet have the necessary social and
emotional skills to be “ready” for school.
Of very low-income children, as many as
30 percent may not have the necessary skills.
• Social and emotional development is
important both in its own right and
because aspects of it facilitate cognitive
development.
• When children are young, the adults
around them (parents, other adult care-
givers, preschool teachers) are the most
important influences on their social and
emotional development.
• High-quality preschool education can sup-
port early development in ways that yield
long-term social and emotional benefits. A
significant part of the long-term economic
pay-off to public investments in high-quali-
ty preschool programs can come from their
social outcomes, including the prevention
of crime and delinquency.
Policy Recommendations:
• Establish as a key goal of preschool
education programs enhancing social
and emotional development, without
de-emphasizing cognitive development.
Both domains are important, and neither
should be sacrificed for the other.
• Include in learning standards the
outcomes that preschool programs
are expected to achieve for social
and emotional development.
• Expand access to high-quality preschool
education so that more children can benefit
from experiences that will improved their
social and emotional development.
Ensure that preschool education programs
are high-quality because only high-quality
programs adequately support children’s
social and emotional development.
• Provide administrators and teachers with
technical assistance and training to help
them implement effective curricula and
teaching practices supporting social and
emotional development.
National Institute for
Early Education Research
Contact Us:
120 Albany Street
Suite 500
New Brunswick, NJ 08901
Tel 732 932-4350
Fax 732 932-4360
www.nieer.org
March 2005
[2]Promoting Children’s Social and Emotional Development Through Preschool
Knowing the ABCs is not enough. To be prepared for school, children also must be excited and curious
about learning and confident that they can succeed (motivational qualities). They must be able to
understand the feelings of others, control their own feelings and behaviors, and get along with their peers
and teachers (socioemotional skills). Indeed, kindergarten teachers rate these motivational and socioemo-
tional skills as more important to school success than being able to hold a pencil or read.2They want chil-
dren to be ready for learning—to be able to cooperate, follow directions, demonstrate self-control, and
“pay attention.”
Unfortunately, kindergarten teachers report that many
of their students are not socially or emotionally prepared
for the challenges of the new environment.3Kindergarten
teachers rate about 20 percent of all entering
kindergarteners and 30 percent of very low-income
entering kindergarteners as having poor social
development.4They enter kindergarten unable to
learn because they cannot pay attention, remember
information on purpose, or function socially in a
school environment.5The result is growing numbers
of children who are hard to manage in the classroom.6,7
These children cannot get along with each other,
follow directions, or delay gratification. They show
belligerence and aggression in the classroom and
on the playground. The problems begin before
kindergarten: In some studies as many as 32 percent
of preschoolers in Head Start programs have behavioral
problems.8
These missing social and emotional skills mean that teachers spend too much of their time trying to rein
in unmanageable children and too little time teaching.9Early childhood teachers report that they are
extremely concerned about growing classroom management problems,10, 11 and that they are ill-equipped
to handle them.12 Kindergarten teachers report that more than half of their students come to school
unprepared for learning academic subjects.13 If these problems are not addressed, the result can be growing
aggression, behavioral problems and, for some, delinquency and crime through the school years and into
adolescence and adulthood.
The core features of emotional
development include the ability to
identify and understand one’s own
feelings, to accurately read and
comprehend emotional states in others,
to manage strong emotions and their
expression in a constructive manner,
to regulate one’s own behavior, to
develop empathy for others and to
establish and sustain relationships.
—National Scientific Council on the
Developing Child (2005)
[3]Promoting Children’s Social and Emotional Development Through Preschool
Social and Emotional Development: Definitions and Importance
Social and emotional development involves the acquisition of a set of skills. Key among them are
the ability to:
• identify and understand one’s own feelings,
• accurately read and comprehend emotional states in others,
• manage strong emotions and their expression in a constructive manner,
• regulate one’s own behavior,
• develop empathy for others, and
• establish and sustain relationships.14
Each of these skills develops on its own timetable, but the skills build on one another. Very young chil-
dren, for example, have to learn to understand and recognize their own feelings, but then they gradually
learn to associate verbal labels to those feelings, to learn that others have feelings too, and to begin to
empathize with others. As children grow older, they learn to manage their emotions—to shake off feelings
of anxiety, sadness, or frustration, and to delay gratification in order to achieve a goal.15 As adults, those
skills help differentiate the mediocre salesman from the successful one who can read the emotional
response of a prospective client. They help athletes persevere until they win their gold medals. They help
spouses empathize with one another to de-escalate arguments, and they impel good citizens to shy away
from injuring others because they can understand how such actions would cause pain.
One of the most important skills that children develop is self-regulation—the ability to manage one’s
behavior so as to withstand impulses, maintain focus, and undertake tasks even if there are other more
enticing alternatives available. Self-regulation underlies the ability to undertake every task, so that it has
implications for not just how children get along with one another but also how they can focus and learn
in the classroom. (See sidebar, p. 4.)
In short, these skills help promote a range of positive behaviors, beginning before children enter kinder-
garten and extending into adult life. Not surprisingly, when social and emotional development goes awry,
the result can be problems in school and later life.
Problems in Social and Emotional Development: The Beginnings of Aggression
Persistent physical aggression, high-school dropout rates, adolescent delinquency, and antisocial behavior
have all been associated with early childhood conduct problems.16 The preschool years are a “sensitive
period” for learning to regulate development of aggression.17 Children who exhibit high levels of physical
aggression in elementary school are at the highest risk of engaging in violent behavior as adolescents.
Researchers believe that children with difficult, disruptive behavior (poor social and emotional skills) are
at risk for these later problems for at least three reasons: (1) teachers find it harder to teach them, seeing
them as less socially and academically competent, and therefore provide them with less positive feedback;
(2) peers reject them, which cuts off an important avenue for learning and emotional support; and (3)
children faced with this rejection from peers and teachers tend to dislike school and learning, which leads
to lower school attendance and poorer outcomes.18, 19
Because difficult behavior exhibits itself early—even before children begin kindergarten—the pattern of
rejection and negative experiences begins early, too.20 The early experience of rejection can have lasting
emotional and behavioral impacts beyond elementary school, creating a downward spiral that becomes
increasingly difficult to reverse.
[4]Promoting Children’s Social and Emotional Development Through Preschool
Social-Emotional Self-Regulation: A Key to School Readiness
One of the most important skills that children must develop is self-regulation. Self-regulation is
a deep, internal mechanism that underlies the mindful, planful, and thoughtful behaviors of all
children. It underlies performance in all domains, from reading to getting along well with others.
Self-regulation is the capacity to control one’s impulses both to stop doing something that is unnec-
essary (even if one wants to continue doing it), and to start doing something that is needed (even if
one does not want to do it). This ability to inhibit one response and to enact another on-demand is
a skill used in thinking as well as social interactions. The child who does not have self-regulation at 5
years of age is the child who cannot follow the teacher’s directions at age 6 or who cannot plan how
to solve a problem at age 7. The child without self-regulation of emotions at age 4 will not be able to
control his temper at 5 and will have negative peer interactions at age 7.
Self-Regulation and School Readiness
Self-regulation is necessary for positive social relations with others and for successful learning. To
learn anything in a school setting, a child has to ignore the child next to him who is fun to play with
and make his mind concentrate on the story the teacher is reading. The abilities to pay attention and
to remember things on purpose are also part of self-regulation.21
The role of self-regulation in school success—from preschool and kindergarten to middle and high
school—has now been documented in a number of studies.22 Levels of self-regulation actually predict
school success in first grade over and above children’s cognitive skills and family background.23
Cognitive self-regulation is linked with students’ achievement in school.24 Children lacking emotional
self-regulation are at higher risk for disciplinary problems and are less likely to make a successful
transition from preschool to kindergarten.25 Emotional self-regulation seems to play a part in child
resiliency and later adjustment.26 Children who did not learn self-regulation in preschool can turn
into bullies with aggressive habits of interaction that are difficult to break in later years.27, 28
New studies demonstrate that there is a physiological basis for the development of self-regulation.
Brain research shows that self-regulation is linked to maturation of the prefrontal cortex area of the
brain, which occurs during the preschool years.29, 30 Both emotional and cognitive self-regulation seem
to have the same neural roots, making it possible for children to take control of both their thinking
and their feelings as they grow older and their brains develop. Based on other brain research, we
believe that preschoolers must practice self-regulation if they are to develop finely tuned skills.
Generally, if children do not practice deliberate and purposeful behaviors, traces in the brain are not
reinforced (“use it or lose it” principle). So, if preschoolers do not practice self-regulation enough,
the related brain areas will not be fully developed, and the end result may be adults who still act like
they are in their “terrible twos.”
Practice Makes Perfect
Evidence indicates that self-regulation and impulse control does not emerge spontaneously, but is
learned.31 Most important, it can be learned not just in families, but also in preschool classrooms.
In fact, in many good quality programs, children do learn self-regulation. In these high-quality pre-
school programs, teachers set up the preschool environment so that children begin to think ahead,
to plan their activities, and to think about and use strategies to solve social problems.32
[5]Promoting Children’s Social and Emotional Development Through Preschool
Without intervention, the troubles born out of problems in social and emotional development create
high costs for society in terms of juvenile delinquency and adult crime. Close to 2.3 million juveniles
were arrested in 2002,33 more than 134,000 juveniles were held in residential facilities in 1999, and about
12,000 juveniles were incarcerated in adult jails or state prisons as of mid-year 2000.34 At year-end 2003,
6.9 million persons (3.2 percent of all U.S. adults) were on probation, parole, or in prison or jail.35
How Social and Emotional Skills Develop
Promoting social and emotional development and preventing problems caused by maladaptive develop-
ment is clearly important to individuals and to society, but how do those socio-emotional skills develop?
They begin with the relationships children form with the people around them, including parents, care-
givers, and peers.
The Role of Parents
Parents and families play an enormous role in shaping a child’s social and emotional development. Early
relationships with parents lay the foundation on which social competency and peer relationships are
built. Parents who support positive emotional development interact with their children affectionately;
show consideration for their feelings, desires and needs; express interest in their daily activities; respect
their viewpoints; express pride in their accomplishments; and provide encouragement and support dur-
ing times of stress.36 This support greatly increases the likelihood that children will develop early emo-
tional competence, will be better prepared to enter school, and less likely to display behavior problems at
home and at school.37 This is why many preschool programs include a focus on parent involvement and
parenting education.
The Role of Teachers/Early Childhood Educators
Most children spend many hours each week in the care of someone other than their parents. These care-
givers play the same role in promoting social and emotional development as do parents when children
are young. Just as parents who are warm and responsive are more likely to promote strong social and
emotional skills in their children, so too are early childhood educators and teachers, which means that
the classroom environment must enable teachers the time to focus on individual children. Just as it is
important for a consistent attachment to form between a parent and child, so too is such an attachment
important for caregiver and child. That means that staff turnover in preschool programs should be kept
to a minimum.38, 39
The Role of Peers
Emotionally healthy children engage in positive play behaviors, develop mutual friendships, and are more
likely to find acceptance from their peers. Through their play, they learn how to work in teams and coop-
erate with others. Their behavior and interactions influence the way in which teachers perceive them and
the way they are treated by their peers. As early as preschool, the relationships children develop with one
another can have a lasting impact on academic achievement, because they can contribute to more positive
feelings about school and eagerness to engage in classroom activities, which can, in turn, lead to higher
levels of achievement.40 Conversely, early rejection by peers has been associated with persistent academic
and social difficulties in elementary school.41 That is why it is important to have skilled preschool teachers
who can intervene when they see children having difficulties with peers and help the children learn how
to resolve conflicts, regulate emotion, and respond to the emotions of others.42
[6]Promoting Children’s Social and Emotional Development Through Preschool
Evidence that Preschool Influences Social and Emotional Development
Convincing evidence exists that high-quality preschool positively affects social-emotional development.
Across hundreds of studies of immediate and short-term outcomes, impacts of early education on factors
such as self-esteem, motivation, and social behavior are positive, and range from about .25 to .40 of a
standard deviation—a meaningful impact.43 Other studies demonstrate that quality preschool produces
long-term benefits in terms of improved classroom behavior and social adjustment and decreased future
crime and delinquency. These successful programs all deliver high-quality center-based early childhood
education services, but they differ in some ways, too. Some focus on 3- and 4-year-olds only, while others
serve children from birth to 5, and some offer parent education or family support services in addition to
center-based early childhood education.
A small group of studies suggest that too much time in an early childhood program, particularly in a
low-quality setting, may actually increase children’s aggression levels slightly, but the bulk of the positive
evidence for preschool is compelling and derives from a variety of studies, beginning with demonstration
programs that were implemented decades ago and continuing to present-day demonstration programs
and large-scale programs, both in the United States and abroad.
Evidence from Demonstration Projects
Some of the strongest evidence for the benefits that preschool programs can produce on children’s social
and emotional development is derived from demonstration projects begun in the 1960s and 1970s.
The High/Scope Perry Preschool Project. The High/Scope Perry Preschool Project,44 initiated more than
40 years ago, was one of the first studies to provide a clear picture of the effects of early, high-quality
preschool on educational, social, academic, and economic outcomes. Three- and 4-year-olds from
low-income families (n=123) were randomly assigned to either a program or no-program group. Those
in the program group experienced a well-designed preschool program, which included weekly home vis-
its that encouraged parent-child interaction.
A recently released report of outcomes through age 40 confirms the economic benefits of investing in
the education of young children.45 Throughout their school years, the children from the program group
outperformed the control group on achievement tests, had better attitudes about school, and were more
likely to graduate from high school. As adults, the preschool participants attained higher levels of educa-
tion and were more likely to vote in elections, find and maintain employment, and own their own homes,
than children in the control group. The program group also averaged significantly fewer criminal arrests,
including fewer arrests for drug-dealing crimes, and relied less on welfare or other social services as adults.
From an economic standpoint, the program benefited the general economy with a 17 to 1 return on the
original investment. This includes savings in the costs of crime, special education or retention in school,
and welfare, as well as increases in taxes paid by those earning higher incomes.46 Much of that return on
investment is attributable to decreased costs of crime—an outcome clearly linked to social and emotional
development. These data provide convincing evidence that providing more funding for preschool programs
today will result in substantial social and economic gains in the future.
The Syracuse University Family Development Research Program. This program offered education, nutrition,
health and safety, and human service resources to low-income, primarily African-American families
(n=108) from 1969-1975. Services included weekly home visits, high-quality child care (one-half day five
[7]Promoting Children’s Social and Emotional Development Through Preschool
days per week for children 6 to15 months of age, and full-day care five days per week for children 15 to
60 months of age); and weekly parent group meetings. Services began prenatally and continued until
children reached elementary school age. At follow-up, when children were 13-16 years old, 6 percent
of the intervention group versus 22 percent of the matched comparison group children had been
processed as probation cases (juvenile delinquency) by the County Probation Department, and the
cases for the youth in the comparison group were much more severe and chronic.47
The Houston Parent Child Development Center. Launched in 1970, the Houston Parent-Child Develop-
ment Center was designed to promote social and intellectual competence in children from low-income
Mexican-American families. Families received two years of services, beginning when children were one
year of age. In the first year of enrollment, families received biweekly 90-minute in-home visits that
focused on parent-child interaction. Fathers and siblings participated in periodic weekend sessions on
issues such as decision making and family communication. In the program’s second year, the mother and
child came to the project center four mornings per week. While the child participated in a nursery school,
the mother attended classes on child management, child development, and family communication. Five
to eight years after the end of program services, teachers rated control group children as more obstinate,
impulsive, disruptive, and involved in fights than program group children (study n= 132). Program
group children were rated as more considerate and less hostile.48 Caution is suggested by the failure to
find similar long-term effects on behavior in two studies of two other PCDCs using somewhat different
approaches with somewhat different populations.49 In addition, the Houston PCDC’s effects on behavior
problems appear to be limited to earlier cohorts, perhaps because program implementation suffered diffi-
culties in later years. These results indicate that outcomes can be expected to vary with the design and
delivery of a program, suggesting that effective policies and practice should stick closely to those models
found to be most effective.
Findings Pooled Across Many Studies: The Consortium for Longitudinal Studies.50 The Consortium for
Longitudinal Studies combined data from 11 studies (including the Perry Preschool Project) begun in the
1960s and 1970s to assess the long-term effects of early childhood education programs. More than 3,500
low-income, predominantly African-American children were initially enrolled in early childhood programs,
and more than 1,100 were followed to young adulthood. Findings confirmed the well-established benefits
of preschool attendance for cognitive development and school competence, but they also suggested that
early education can affect children’s future goals and aspirations. At 10 to 19 years of age, children in
both program and control groups had high educational and occupational aspirations and equivalent eval-
uations of their own school performance, but children who had attended preschool were far more likely
to express pride in a school- or work-related achievement. Four years later, at ages 14 to 23 years, those
participants with higher “achievement orientation” were found to have better employment status and
higher educational attainment. Preschool attendance was also significantly associated with higher occupa-
tional aspirations and expectations for post-high school participants.
[8]Promoting Children’s Social and Emotional Development Through Preschool
Evidence from Large-Scale, Publicly Funded Programs
These positive findings do not just apply to demonstration programs or to programs begun in the 1960s
and 1970s. Similar results have emerged from federally funded programs begun in the 1980s and 1990s.
Chicago Child-Parent Centers. Since 1985, the Chicago Child-Parent Centers (CPC) have provided children
from low-income families with preschool and kindergarten programs, continued intervention in early
elementary school, and family support services. The Chicago Longitudinal Study has followed the devel-
opment of more than 1,500 children who participated in CPC and has documented positive short- and
long-term social and academic outcomes.51
Children who participated in CPC demonstrated greater cognitive achievement, better social adjustment,
less frequent grade retention, and lower crime rates as adolescents than the control group. In addition, a
cost-benefit analysis revealed a substantial return on the original investment in the form of reduced crime
rates, costs to crime victims, and school remedial services, as well as participants’ increased earnings
capacity by age 21.52 Much of these benefits are attributable to crime prevention (social outcomes). These
results demonstrate the long-term benefits of providing early childhood programs that nurture and care
for the emotional and academic needs of children and families.
Early Head Start. Evidence from another large-scale, federally-funded program, Early Head Start, also
supports the benefits of high-quality early childhood services. Early Head Start is a federally-funded pro-
gram that provides comprehensive educational, health, and social services to low-income families across
the country. Through either center-based child development services, home visits, or a combination of
the two, children ages birth to 3 receive early childhood education and parents learn how to meet their
children’s emotional needs and provide nurturing learning environments. Results of a rigorous random-
ized trial evaluation of the program53 documented that children in EHS enjoyed more positive interactions
with their parents and their parents showed more emotional support and less negativity toward them
than did their control group counterparts. By age 3, Early Head Start children were more likely to behave
in ways that maintained interaction with their parent, were more attentive to objects during play, and
were reported by parents to be less aggressive. Further analyses revealed that these effects were primarily
found in EHS program sites that employed a combination of center- and home-based services, suggesting
that it is very important for early childhood education services to partner with parents.
International Evidence
Mauritius: Evidence of the power of preschool education programs to promote social and emotional
development comes from other nations as well. A sample of children, randomly selected from the local
population on the island of Mauritius (located off the southeastern coast of Africa), participated in a 2-
year preschool program (from ages 3 to 5). The program included three components: (1) educational
activities focusing on verbal and conceptual skills, visuospatial coordination, memory, and sensation and
perception; (2) nutrition (milk, juice, hot meal with fish, chicken or mutton, and salad provided each
day); and (3) 2 1/2 hours of physical exercise each day. Adult-child ratios were 1:5.5.
When compared with a control group of children who had experienced usual community care (adult-
child ratio of 1:30; no lunch or structured exercise periods, and a traditional curriculum), benefits were
seen which were maintained into adulthood. The preschool group had better scores on measures of
mental health and antisocial behavior at age 17 and lower rates of criminal behavior at age 23, compared
[9]Promoting Children’s Social and Emotional Development Through Preschool
to the control group. These benefits were especially pronounced for children with signs of malnutrition
at age 3. The authors argue that the combination of services is important, perhaps because they result in
lasting changes in brain development.54 This suggests a need for a high-quality preschool environment that
pays attention to the needs of the whole child—social, emotional, and physical health and development.
Jamaica: Relatively few studies have investigated the long-term effects of preschool programs on such
internalizing behaviors as anxiety and depression as opposed to aggression and other externalizing
behaviors. The EPPE study discussed later in this brief is one of the few to look at such effects on a large
scale for preschool education programs generally. Follow-up of a randomized trial originally conducted
with 9-24 month old growth-stunted children in Jamaica provides evidence on the effects of educational
stimulation on these and other outcomes when the children are 17-18 years old.55
Children (n=127) were randomly assigned to four groups: no-treatment, dietary supplementation, home
visits, or both. Dietary supplementation (one kg of milk-based formula) and home visits were provided
weekly for two years. The home visits were one-hour sessions working with mother and child to enhance
mother-child interaction in play. Mothers were encouraged to talk with their children and to use praise
and positive reinforcement rather than physical discipline, and were taught play techniques designed to
foster positive child development. Mothers also were provided with toys and picture books and encour-
aged to play with their children between visits.
Follow-up at age 17-18 (n=103) showed significant effects of the educational intervention but not of
dietary supplementation. Participants in the home visits reported less anxiety and depression and better
self-esteem. Parents reported that they had fewer attention problems. In addition, there was some
evidence that they were less likely to have been expelled from school (2% v. 11%, p =.08). No significant
effects were found for self-reported anti-social behavior or parental report of hyperactivity and
oppositional behavior.
Complex Findings Including Negative Effects in Some Circumstances
Several studies find evidence that children who spend long hours in child care exhibit somewhat higher
levels of aggressive behavior in the first few years of school. This effect is small, and its practical importance
is unclear. The problem may be largely avoidable by providing better education in child care, though this
aspect of curricular improvement does not appear to be captured by commonly used measures of child
care quality. Moreover, the broadest research indicates that even when this mild negative effect is present
it is accompanied by positive effects on other aspects of social and emotional development as well as
positive effects on cognitive development. There is some evidence that typical child care over the first
5 years of life can have modest negative effects on social and emotional development that persists
into elementary school, in the form of behavior problems, less social competence and poorer schoolwork
habits.56
The NICHD Study of Early Child Care.57 The NICHD Study of Early Child Care followed the development
of over 1,200 children from 10 sites across the country. Families were recruited for participation through
hospital visits shortly after the birth of a child in 1991, and the children’s social behavior was subsequently
assessed at 15, 24, 36, and 54 months, as well as early in their kindergarten year. Mothers, teachers, and
child care providers rated children’s behavior, and researchers observed the children’s interactions with
peers. Results suggested that a small percentage (less than 20 percent) of the children who spent a lot of
time (at least 30 hours per week) in non-maternal child care arrangements were more noncompliant and
[10]Promoting Children’s Social and Emotional Development Through Preschool
aggressive than their peers at 54 months of age and in kindergarten. This relationship held even when the
effects of quality, type, and stability of child care and maternal sensitivity were controlled for through
statistical modeling. Nevertheless, additional analyses revealed that the persistent effect of duration on
aggression was fairly small—smaller than the effects of children’s socioeconomic status and the maternal
sensitivity of their mothers.
Some researchers have suggested that the NICHD link between aggressive behavior and long hours in
child care may be easily explained and may not be problematic.58 The proportion of children in the high
duration of care group who show higher levels of problem behaviors does not exceed the proportion of
children in the national population as a whole who display the same frequency of these behaviors, so child
care is not increasing the number of aggressive children that otherwise exists in the population at-large.
Instead, it is possible that the increased aggression emerges when children first spend substantial time
in large-group settings. For the children in long-term care, their exposure begins earlier, so the higher
aggression levels emerge earlier. When their agemates are exposed to substantial time in large-group
settings, their levels of aggressive behavior will increase too, so that, in the end, preschool participants
will not display more aggression than agemates.
The Abecedarian Project.59 The Abecedarian project offers some additional evidence from a randomized
trial with a sample of 104 at age 21 follow-up. The Abecedarian Preschool Project provided low-income
African-American children with full-day educational child care from birth to age 5, and has demonstrated
important long-term benefits for children such as higher rates of high school graduation and college
attendance. However, in an early study, teachers rated 59 children from the project to be more aggressive
during the first three years of primary school than control group children. Although the teachers did
not dislike or find the Abecedarian children harder to manage than children from the control group, the
Abecedarian children were more likely to kick, push, and hit in a variety of settings (such as lunchroom
and classroom) than children from the control group. By the third year in public school, the aggression
level of students who had participated in the Abecedarian program began to decline, and the level for
children in the control group began to rise slightly.
There were no differences in aggressive behavior among children in the Abecedarian control group,
although their exposure to child care ranged from none at all to nearly five years. Of course, the sample
size is quite small making it difficult to detect small effects. The study’s author suggests that the program’s
curriculum was an important factor in explaining the difference between treatment and control group
aggression. In the early years of Abecedarian, the program emphasized academic growth in its curriculum
activities. When early results showing elevated aggressive behavior were observed, the program’s designers
changed the curriculum to reinforce prosocial alternatives, and they brought in a consultant to work with
teachers on methods of behavior control. Subsequent cohorts of children enrolled in the program showed
much lower rates of aggressive behavior. It is noteworthy that large positive effects on school success were
found across all cohorts.
The Effective Provision of Preschool Education Project.60 The Effective Provision of Preschool Education
(EPPE) Project involved nearly 3,000 children and 141 centers from five regions in England. Children were
recruited from six types of service settings, such as nursery school, playgroup, or day care. Social and
behavioral development over the preschool period was analyzed by measuring change in social behavior
from entry to the study (primarily 3-year-olds) to start of primary school (primarily 5-year-olds).
[11]Promoting Children’s Social and Emotional Development Through Preschool
Classroom teachers rated the children who attended preschool centers significantly higher on measures of
independence and concentration, cooperation and conformity, and peer sociability compared to children
who remained home. At entry to primary grades, effect sizes (measured in standard deviations) for 1-2
years of preschool attendance ranged from .11 for cooperation and conformity to .36 for peer sociability,
after controlling for child, parent, and home characteristics.
In general, children in higher quality programs benefited more. In particular, developmental gains in
cooperation and conformity were stronger if children were enrolled in programs with highly qualified
staff, or in programs that scored well on “language and reasoning” and “social interaction” subscales of
the Early Childhood Environment Rating Scale (a measure commonly used to assess the quality of early
childhood programs). Children in centers with highly qualified staff also showed reduced anti-social/
worried behavior.
In general, the researchers found that the more time children spent in preschool, the more social benefits
they enjoyed, although it appeared that children who were enrolled in preschool for an extended time
(more than 3 years) showed some increased anti-social/worried behavior. The quality of the program made
a difference, such that the problematic behavior levels were lower in high-quality programs, although
those levels were still higher than for children who spent less time in care. This evidence is consistent with
the findings of the NICHD and Abecedarian studies, but places this one negative result in the context of
other more positive effects on social and emotional development because of the broader set of outcome
measures used.
The Role of Preschool Quality and Curriculum
Successful preschool programs may differ in some ways, but they all are high quality programs, with
well-trained staff who focus attention on the needs of each of their students. In policy terms, this means
that the programs share the following characteristics:61
Small group sizes
A partnership with parents
• A sound curriculum that addresses the needs of the whole child
• High adult-child ratios
• Competitive staff compensation and benefits to attract and retain good staff
• Well-prepared teachers and ongoing professional development
The following sidebar (p. 13) discusses some of these aspects of preschool program quality in greater
detail. In addition to these important program elements, however, the methods of teaching and organizing
student activities are highly influential in the development of social competency. Many child development
experts feel that early childhood programs that employ only didactic methods of instruction may fail
to enhance social and emotional skills.62 This type of instruction does not always provide children with
opportunities to develop problem-solving abilities and may negatively affect their development of social
and emotional skills, which can have long-term consequences for learning.
[12]Promoting Children’s Social and Emotional Development Through Preschool
The Benefits of a Balanced Curriculum
In an attempt to highlight the value of a balanced curriculum, the High/Scope Preschool Curriculum
Comparison Study63 compared the effectiveness of three preschool curriculum models when used with
children at risk for school failure. Children were randomly assigned to participate in programs employing
(1) the High/Scope curriculum model which balances child- and teacher-directed instructional activities,
(2) a direct instruction model in which it is primarily teachers who initiate activities, or (3) a traditional
nursery school program in which classroom activities are the teacher’s responses to the child’s expressed
needs and interests, and teachers encourage children to engage in free play.
The High/Scope curriculum provides children with opportunities to make choices about their activities by
identifying goals and making plans to achieve them. Students are also encouraged to recall or reflect upon
different experiences they have had during the day, taking time to consider ideas and concepts they have
discovered and discuss what they might do to build on or extend what they have learned. The plan-do-review
sequence helps children develop language and social behavioral skills and contributes to the development of
higher-order thinking skills such as making predictions, solving problems, and anticipating outcomes.64 This
model provides autonomy and assists in the development of analytical abilities, abstract thinking, problem
solving skills, self regulation, and metacognitive skills, all of which are essential for success in school.
Adults who had attended the High/Scope program as children experienced fewer social difficulties and were
more likely to participate in volunteer work, vote, and stay married longer than their peers who had partici-
pated in the other programs. Other studies also suggest that a balanced curriculum can have a significant
long-term impact on sociobehavioral outcomes,65 including more prosocial behavior and better relationships
with peers in early elementary school.66 These suggest that more attention to how children are taught and
the kinds of relationships children and adults have in preschool programs may be the key to avoiding even
small negative effects on aggression and maximizing positive effects on social and emotional development
and behavior.
The High/Scope curriculum study is just one small experiment, but the field is decidedly lacking in ran-
domized trials investigating the effects of curriculum on social and emotional development on long-term
social and emotional development. Indeed, the experimental literature has tended to focus on highly spe-
cific interventions for children identified as having serious problems rather than on a whole child
approach for the general population.67 The preschool education programs found to be the most effective in
preventing antisocial behavior and delinquency are quality programs with balanced curricula that focus on
the needs of the children,68 provide opportunities for peer interactions during play and produce high levels
of teacher-child closeness.69
Understanding the impact of quality preschool education is the first step, but providing programs
that foster healthy emotional development requires foresight, planning and the support of politicians,
communities and families.
[13]Promoting Children’s Social and Emotional Development Through Preschool
Quality Preschool Programs: Definitions and Evidence for Benefits
Preschool programs that maintain high standards of quality provide children, especially those at risk,
with skills that will assist them in their social and academic adjustment to elementary school. High-
quality preschool is organized in ways that allow children to form close, sustained relationships with
teachers and encourage positive interactions with peers. Small group sizes and high adult-child ratios,
competitive staff compensation and benefits, professional development, and other aspects of the
program are geared toward fostering strong relationships and reducing teacher turnover. These com-
ponents have been associated with positive social and emotional outcomes for children, including
greater compliance, sociability, attention, self-regulation, and peer relations as well as lower rates
of negative affect and behavior problems.70
A program’s quality may also be determined by the qualifications required of the teachers and staff.
Teachers with four-year degrees and specialization in early childhood are better prepared to develop
meaningful relationships with their students and create safe, nurturing climates that support children’s
emotional well being. Children cared for by teachers who are highly involved and invested during
their preschool years have been found to be less likely to display behavior problems in kindergarten
and demonstrate increased social skills through elementary school.71 High teacher-student ratios allow
for the development of these relationships, which provide stability in transitions to new classrooms,
contribute to increased social skills, and improve emotional stability.72
The National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) has conducted extensive
research that has contributed to a better understanding of the relationship between preschool quality
and child outcomes. The research has shown that higher quality preschool programs defined by high
teacher-student ratios, group sizes and higher teacher qualifications results in more responsive teaching
and fewer behavior problems from children.73 Children who attend higher quality preschool have also
been reported to have fewer behavior problems, closer relationships with their mothers, and to be
better prepared for school.74
[14]Promoting Children’s Social and Emotional Development Through Preschool
Conclusions and Recommendations
In sum, high-quality preschool education is designed to enhance children’s social and emotional devel-
opment. Such programs provide children with highly qualified teachers, small class sizes with high
teacher-student ratios, opportunities to pursue their interests and interact with their peer, and activities
intentionally designed and implemented to educate the whole child. Among the benefits found from
such programs; children are more likely to graduate from high school, continue with higher education,
and have lower rates of teen pregnancy, special education placement, disruptive behavior, and arrests.
They are more likely to give back to their neighborhoods as adults by participating in volunteer work
and contribute more to their communities through higher employment rates and earnings, higher voting
rates, increased church attendance, and home ownership.75 These benefits and associated economic
returns have only been found for programs that are high-quality. Benefits have been found for larger
scale programs including the Chicago Child Parent Centers, Head Start and Early Head Start. By contrast,
length of attendance in typical child care has been associated with modest negative outcomes.
As policymakers design programs and make decisions regarding early education, they should invest in
programs that support development of the whole child, including academic, social, and emotional skills.
Recommendations for policymakers are as follows:
1. Include enhanced social and emotional development as a key goal of preschool education programs.
This does not mean that enhanced cognitive development should be de-emphasized. Both aspects of
children’s development are important, and one need not be sacrificed to support the other.
2. Standards should spell out the outcomes that preschool education programs are expected to achieve
for social and emotional development.
Performance standards for preschool education programs should include explicit mention of social and
emotional development. The new draft of National Association for the Education of Young Children
accreditation standards include discussion of promotion of social and emotional skills and can provide
guidance for program administrators who are considering launching new preschool programs.76
Then, when policymakers require that progress be measured by indicators, those indicators should map
onto the performance standards and should therefore also include measures of social and emotional
development. Seventeen states have launched school readiness indicator projects, and their work can
provide examples of such indicators.77
3. Expand access to high-quality preschool education programs so that more children can benefit
from educational experiences that will improve their socio-emotional development.
Nationally, only a few states have committed to funding preschool for all 4-year-olds whose parents
wish them to attend.78 High-quality infant and toddler programs, even for the most disadvantaged
children remain rare. Substantial new investments should be made to increase access to high-quality
child care and preschool education programs designed specifically to enhance early learning and
development, broadly defined.
[15]Promoting Children’s Social and Emotional Development Through Preschool
4. Ensure that all preschool programs are high-quality because only educational programs will support
children’s social and emotional development. Such programs have strong leadership, well-prepared
teachers, a balanced curriculum, reasonable class sizes and ratios, and partnerships with parents.
5. Provide administrators and teachers with technical assistance and training to help them implement
effective curricula and teaching practices that support children’s social and emotional development.
Studies demonstrate that, beyond the traditional aspects of program quality, the content of the curricu-
lum and the teaching practices that teachers employ are critical determinants of a program’s ability to
benefit children’s social and emotional development. Policymakers should ensure that resources are
available to help teachers put into practice the best approaches for promoting children’s social and
emotional development.
The Last Word
A child’s ability to learn and to function as a contributing member of society rests heavily on the devel-
opment of social competency and emotional health that begins at birth and is greatly influenced during
the preschool years. Preschool programs that pursue the highest standards of quality will contribute
substantially to this development. And while it may be a difficult and costly responsibility to promote
and maintain such standards, the benefits far outweigh the costs. If we value our children and their
future, we would be wise to make every effort to provide access to quality preschool programs and
endorse such a prescient investment in the social and economic future of our country.
[16]Promoting Children’s Social and Emotional Development Through Preschool
Endnotes
1Thompson, R.A. (2002). The roots of school readiness in social and emotional development. Set for
Success: Building a strong foundation for school readiness based on the social-emotional development of young
children. 1(1), 8-29. Kansas City, MO: The Ewing Marion Kauffman Foundation.
2National Center for Educational Statistics (NCES) (1995). Readiness for Kindergarten: Parent and
Teacher Beliefs. Retrieved, March 23, 2005 http://nces.ed.gov/pubs93/web/93257.asp
3Rimm-Kaufmann, S.E., Pianta, R.C., & Cox, M.J. (2000). Teachers’ judgments of problems in the
transition to kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 15(2), 147-166.
4Child Trends. (2003). Kindergarteners’ social interaction skills. Retrieved March 15, 2005 from
http://www.childtrendsdatabank.org/pdf/47_PDF.pdf .
5Bronson, M.B. (2000). Self-regulation in early childhood. New York: NY: Guilford Press.
6Ravner, C. C. & Knitzer, J. (2002). Ready to Enter: What research tells policymakers about strategies to
promote social and emotional school readiness among three- and four-year-old children. New York, NY:
National Center for Children in Poverty, Mailman School of Public Health, Columbia University.
7West, J., Denton, J. & Reaney, L.M. (2001). The kindergarten year: Findings from the Early Childhood
Longitudinal Study Kindergarten class of 1999-1999. Washington, DC: US Department of Education,
National Center for Education Statistics.
8West, J., Denton, K. & Reaney L. M. (2001).
9Wallis, C. (2003, December 15). Does kindergarten need cops? Time Magazine, 162(24).
10 Ladd, G., Birch, S., & Buhs, E. (1999). Children’s social and scholastic lives in kindergarten: Related
spheres of influence? Child Development, 70, 1373-1400.
11 Greenwood, C.R. (1991). Longitudinal analysis of time, engagement, and achievement in at-risk versus
non-risk students. Exceptional Children, 57, 521-534.
12 Arnold, D. H., McWilliams, L. & Arnold, E. H. (1998). Teacher discipline and child misbehavior in day
care: Untangling causality with correlational data. Developmental Psychology, 34, 276-278.
13 Rimm-Kaufman, S., Pianta, R. C. & Cox, M. (2000). Teachers’ judgments of problems in the transition
to school. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 15, 147-166.
14 National Scientific Council on the Developing Child (2005). Children’s emotional development is
built in to the architecture of their brains (Working Paper II). Retrieved March 15, 2005 from
http://www.developingchild.net/papers/workingpaperII.pdf
15 Goleman, D. (1994). Emotional intelligence: Why it can matter more than IQ. New York: Bantam Books.
16 Nagin, D. S., & Tremblay, R. E., (2001) Parental and early childhood predictors of persistent physical
aggression in boys from kindergarten to high school. Archives of General Psychology, 58, 389-394;
LaCourse, E., Cote, S., Nagin, D. S., Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., & Tremblay, R. E. (2002). A longitudinal-
experimental approach to testing theories of antisocial behavior development. Development and
Psychopathology, 14(4), 909-924.
17 Tremblay, R.E. Development of physical aggression from early childhood to adulthood. In: Tremblay
R.E., Barr R.G., Peters R.DeV., eds. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online]. Montreal,
Quebec: Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development; 2002:1-6. Available at http://www.
excellence-earlychildhood.ca/documents/TremblayANGxp.pdf. Accessed December 21, 2006.
18 Raver, C.C. (2002). Emotions matter: Making the case for the role of young children’s emotional
development for early school readiness. Social Policy Report, 16(3), 13-19.
[17]Promoting Children’s Social and Emotional Development Through Preschool
19 Tremblay, R. E., Pihl, R. O. Vitaro, F., & Dobkin, P. L. (1994). Predicting early onset of male antisocial
behavior from preschool behavior. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51(9), 732-739.; Crockenburg, S. C.
(2003). Rescuing the Baby from the Bathwater: How gender and temperament (may) influence how child
care affects child development. Child Development, 74(4), 1034-1038.
20 Tremblay, R.E. et al (1994).
21 Blair, C. (2003). Self-regulation and school readiness. Champlaign, IL: ERIC Clearinghouse on
Elementary and Early Childhood Education. (Eric Identifier No. ED477640).
22 Huffman, L. C., Mehlinger, S. L., & Kerivan, A. S. (2001). Risk factors for academic and behavioral
problems in the beginning of school. In Off to a good start: Research on the risk factors fro early school
problems and selected federal policies affecting children’s social and emotional development and their
readiness for school. Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Carolina, FPG Child Development Center.
23 Ravner, C. C. & Knitzer, J. (2002). Ready to Enter: What research tells policymakers about strategies to
promote social and emotional school readiness among three- and four-year-old children. New York, NY:
National Center for Children in Poverty, Mailman School of Public Health, Columbia University.
24 Lyon, G. R. & Krasnegor, N. A. (1996). Attention, memory, and executive function. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
25 Huffman, L. C., Mehlinger, S. L. & Kerivan, A. S. (2001).
26 Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A., Reiser, M., Cumberland, A., Shepard, S. A., Valiente, C.,
Losoya, S. H., Guthrie, I. K., & Thompson, M. (2004). The relations of effortful control and impulsivity
to children’s resiliency and adjustment. Child Development, 75(1), 25-46.
27 Nagin, D. S. &Tremblay, R. E. (1999). Trajectories of boys’ physical aggression, opposition and hyper-
activity on the path to physically violent and nonviolent juvenile delinquency. Child Development, 70(5),
1181-1196.
28 Shonkoff, J. P. & Phillips, D. A. (Eds.) (2000). From Neurons to Neighborhoods: The science of early
childhood development. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
29 Blair, C. (2003).
30 Shonkoff, J. P. & Phillips, D. A. (Eds.) (2000).
31 Nagin, D. S. &Tremblay, R. E. (1999).
32 Hyson, M., C. Copple, and J. Jones, Bringing developmental theory and research into the early childhood
classroom: Thinking, emotions, and assessment, in Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Child psychology in
practice, K.A. Renninger and I.E. Sigel, Editors. In press, Wiley: New York.
33 Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention (2002). Juvenile arrests. Retrieved March 15,
2005 from http://ojjdp.ncjrs.org/ojstatbb/crime/qa05101.asp?qaDate=20040801 .
34 Sickmund, M. (June 2004). Juveniles in corrections. Juvenile Offenders and Victims National Report
Series Bulletin. NCJ 202885. Retrieved March 15, 2005 from
http://www.ncjrs.org/pdffiles1/ojjdp/202885.pdf
35 Bureau of Justice Statistics (2005). Corrections statistics. Retrieved March 15, 2005 from
www.ojp.usdoj.gov/bjs/correct.htm
36 Moore, S.G. (1992). The Role of Parents in the Development of Peer Group Competence. Urbana, IL:
ERIC Digest.
[18]Promoting Children’s Social and Emotional Development Through Preschool
37 Sammons, P., Sylva, K., Melhuish, E., Siraj-Blatchford, I., Taggart, B., & Elliot, K. (2003). Measuring the
impact of pre-school on children’s social/behavioural development over the preschool period (Technical paper
8b). London: Institute of Education, University of London.; Raine, A., Mellingen, K., Liu, J., Venables, P.,
Mednick, S.A. (2003). Effects of environmental enrichment at ages 3-5 years on schizotypal personality
and antisocial behavior at ages 17 and 23 years. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(9), 1627-1635;
NICHD Early Childcare Research Network, (2002). Early child care and children’s development prior to
school entry: Results from the NICHD study of early child care. American Educational Research Journal,
39(1), 133-164.
38 Thompson, R.A. (2002).
39 National Scientific Council on the Developing Child. Young children develop in an environment of
relationships. Working paper #1. Retrieved March 15, 2005 from
http://www.developingchild.net/papers/paper_1.pdf
40 Ladd, G.W., Birch, S. H., & Buhs, E.S. (1999). Children’s social and scholastic lives in kindergarten:
Related spheres of influence? Child Development, 70(6), 1373-1400.
41 O’Neil, R., Welsh, M., Parke, R. D., Wang, S., & Strand, C. (1997). A longitudinal assessment of the
academic correlates of early peer acceptance and rejection. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 26(3),
290-303.
42 If teachers are not skilled enough themselves, many locales are investing in early childhood mental
health consultants who can either intervene directly with the children or coach the preschool teachers.
See for example: Casas, P. Toward the ABCs: Building a healthy social and emotional foundation for learn-
ing and living. Chicago, IL: Ounce of Prevention Fund. www.ounceofprevention.org.
43 Barnett, W.S. (2002) Early childhood education. In A. Molnar (Ed.) School Reform Proposals: The
Research Evidence (pp.1-26). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.
44 Peisner-Feinberg, E.S., Burchinal, M.R., Clifford, R.M., Culkin, M.L, Howes, C., & Kagan, S.L., et al
(2000). The children of the cost, quality, and outcomes study go to school: Technical Report. Chapel Hill:
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Frank Porter Graham Child Development Center.
45 Schweinhart, L.J. (2004). The High/Scope Perry Preschool Study Through age 40: Summary, Conclusions,
and Frequently Asked Questions. Retrieved March 15, 2005 from
http://www.highscope.org/Research/PerryProject/PerryAge40SumWeb.pdf
46 Barnett, W.S. (1995). Long-term effects of early childhood programs on cognitive and school out-
comes. The Future of Children, 5(3), 25-50.; Schweinhart, L.J & Wallgren, C.R. (1993). Effects of a
Follow Through program on school achievement. Journal of Research in Childhood Education. Vol 8(1),
43-56.
47 Lally, J.R., Mangione, P.L., & Honig, A.S. (September, 1987). The Syracuse University Family
Development Research Program: Long-range impact of an early intervention with low-income children and
their families. Sausalito, CA: The Center for Child & Family Studies, Far West Laboratory.
48 Johnson, D. L. & Walker, T. (1987). Primary prevention of behavior problems in Mexican-American
children. American Journal of Community Psychology, 15(4), 375-385.
49 Johnson, J. (2006). Parent-Child Development Center Follow-Up Project: Child behavior problem
results. Journal of Primary Prevention, 27(4), 391-407.
50 Royce, J. M., Darlington, R. B., & Murray, H. W. (1983). Pooled analyses: Findings across studies. In
Consortium for Longitudinal Studies, (Ed.). As the twig is bent…lasting effects of preschool programs.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
[19]Promoting Children’s Social and Emotional Development Through Preschool
51 Reynolds, A. J., Temple, J., Robertson, D., Mann, E., (2001). Long-term effects of an early childhood
intervention on educational achievement and juvenile arrest: A 15-year follow-up of low-income children
in public schools. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association, 285(18), 2339-2346.
52 Reynolds, A., Temple, J., Robertson, D. & Mann, E. (2001) Age 21 cost-benefit analysis of the Title I
Chicago Child Parent Center Program, Madison, WI: Waisman Institute, University of Wisconsin;
Reynolds, A. J. (1994). Effects of a preschool plus follow-on intervention for children at risk.
Developmental Psychology, 30(6), 787-804; Reynolds, A.J. (1997). The Chicago Child-Parent Centers: A
longitudinal study of extended early childhood intervention, Discussion Paper no. 1126-97. Madison, WI:
Institute for Research on Poverty.
53 Love, J., Kisker, E., Ross, C. Schochet, P, Brooks-Gunn, J., Paulsell, D. et al. (2002). Making a difference
in the lives of infants and toddlers and their families: The impacts of Early Head Start. Volume 1: Final
Technical Report. Washington, DC: US Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for
Children and Families, Office of Planning, Research and Evaluation Administration on Children, Youth
and Families, Head Start Bureau. Retrieved March 15, 2005 from
http://www.acf.hhs.gov/programs/opre/ehs/ehs_resrch/reports/impacts_vol1/impacts_vol1.pdf
54 Raine et al (2003).
55 Walker, S.P., Change, S.M., Powerll, C.A., Simonoff, E., & Grantham-McGregor, S.M. (2006). Effects
of psychosocial stimulation and dietary supplementation in early childhood on phychosocial functioning
in late adolescence: follow-up of randomised controlled trial. British Medical Journal,
BMJdoi:10.1136/bmj.38897.555208.2F (published 28 July 2006) Available at:
http://www.bmj.com/cgi/reprint/333/7566/472. Accessed December 21, 2006.
56 Belsky, J. (2006) Effects of child care on child development in the USA. In Jef J. van Kuyk (Ed.), The
quality of early childhood education: Report of a scientific conference 2006 (23-32). Arnhem, The
Netherlands: Cito.
57 National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care Research Network.
(2003). Does amount of time spent in child care predict socioemotional adjustment during the transition
to kindergarten? Child Development,74(4), 976-1005.
58 Todd, C.M. (2001). The NICHD Child Care Study Results: What do they mean for parents, child-care
professionals, employers and decision makers? Washington, DC: USDA/CSREES: Extension CARES for
America’s Children and Youth Initiative.
59 Haskins, R. (1985). Public school aggression among children with varying day-care experience. Child
Development, 56(3), 689-703.
60 Sammons P. et al (2003).
61 Frede, E.C. (1998). Preschool program quality in programs for children in poverty. In W.S. Barnett and
S.S. Boocock (Eds.) Early care and education for children in poverty: Promises, programs, and long-term out-
comes (77-98). Buffalo, NY: SUNY Press.
62 Schweinhart, L.J. & Weikart, D.P. (1997). The High/Scope preschool curriculum comparison study
through age 23. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 12, 117-143; Stipek, D., Feiler, R., Daniels, D.,
Milburn, S. (1995). Effects of different instructional approaches on young children’s achievement and
motivation. Child Development, 66, 209-223; Sammons, P. et al. (2003); Stipek, D. (1998). Good begin-
nings: What difference does the program make in preparing young children for school? Journal of
Applied Developmental Psychology, 19(1), 41-66.
63 Weikart, D. P. (1998). Changing early childhood development through educational intervention.
Preventive Medicine: An International Journal Devoted to Practice & Theory, Vol 27(2), 233-237.
[20]Promoting Children’s Social and Emotional Development Through Preschool
64 Epstein, A. (2003). How planning and reflection develop young children’s thinking skills. Beyond the
Journal: Young Children on the Web. http://www.journal.naeyc.org/btj/200309/Planning&Reflection.pdf
65 Stipek, D., Feiler, R., Daniels, D., Milburn, S. (1995).; Sammons, P. et al. (2003).
66 Peisner-Feinberg et al. (2000).
67 Bierman K.L. Programs and services proven to be effective in reducing aggression in young children.
Comments on Webster-Stratton, Domitrovich and Greenberg, and Loehman. In: Tremblay R.E., Barr
R.G., Peters R.DeV., eds. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online]. Montreal, Quebec:
Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development; 2003:1-6. Available at: http://excellence-early-
childhood.ca/documents/BiermanANGxp.pdf. Accessed December 21, 2006.
68 Yoshikawa, H. (1995). Long-term effects of early childhood programs on social outcomes and
delinquency. The Future of Children, 5(3), 51-74.
69 Peisner-Feinberg et al. (2000).
70 Votruba-Drzal, E., Coley, R.L., & Chase-Lansdale, P.L. (2004). Child care and low-income children’s
development: Direct and moderated effects. Child Development, 75(1), 296-312.
71 Peisner-Feinberg, E. S., Burchinal, M. R., Clifford, R.M., Culkin, M.L., Howes, C., Kagan, S.L., &
Yazejian, N. (2001). The relation of preschool child-care quality to children’s cognitive and social devel-
opmental trajectories through second grade. Child Development, 72(5), 1534-1553; Howes, C. (2000).
Social-emotional classroom climate in child care, child-teacher relationships and children’s second grade
peer relations. Social Development, 9(2), 191-204; Howes, C. (1990). Can the age of entry into child care
and the quality of child care predict adjustment in kindergarten? Developmental Psychology, 26(2), 292-303.
72 Votruba-Drzal et al. (2004); Howes, C., & Hamilton, C.E. (1993). The changing experience of child
care: Changes in teachers and in teacher-child relationships and children’s social competence with peers.
Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 8, 15-32; Peisner-Feinberg, E.S., Burchinal, M.R., Clifford, R.M.,
Culkin, M.L., Howes, C., Kagan, S.L., et al. (1999). The children of the cost, quality and outcomes study go
to school: Executive summary. Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Frank Porter
Graham Child Development Center.
73 NICHD (2001). Nonmaternal care and family factors in early development: an overview of the NICHD
study of early child care. Applied Developmental Psychology, 22, 457-492.
74 NICHD (2002). Child-care structure-process-outcome: Direct and indirect effects of child-care quality
on children’s development. Psychological Science, 13(3), 199-206.
75 Peisner-Feinberg et al. (2000); Consortium for Longitudinal Studies. (1983). As the twig is bent: Lasting
effects of preschool programs. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.; Yoshikawa, 1995; Schweinhart, L. J., & Weikart, D.
P. (1997). Lasting differences: The High/Scope Preschool Curriculum Comparison study through age 23
(Monographs of the High/Scope Educational Research Foundation, 12). Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press.
76 National Association for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC) (n.d.). Building new NAEYC
program standards and accreditation criteria. Retrieved March 15, 2005 from
http://www.naeyc.org/accreditation/next_era.asp#standards
77 Rhode Island KIDS COUNT. (February 2005). Getting Ready: Findings from the National School
Readiness Indicators Initiative. A 17 State Partnership.
78 Barnett, W. S., Hustedt, J. T., Robin K. B., & Schulman, K. L. (2004). The state of preschool: 2004 state
preschool yearbook. New Brunswick, NJ: National Institute for Early Education Research, Rutgers
University.
This document was prepared with the support of The Pew Charitable Trusts. The Trusts’ Advancing Quality Pre-Kindergarten
For All initiative seeks to advance high quality prekindergarten for all the nation’s three- and four-year-olds through objective,
policy-focused research, state public education campaigns and national outreach. The opinions expressed in this report are
those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of The Pew Charitable Trusts.
... In linea con queste premesse, a maggio 2022 Indire ha avviato un progetto di ricerca pilota incentrato sulla pratica del Debate in VR, coinvolgendo dieci scuole secondarie di secondo grado 1 variamente distribuite sul territorio nazionale, in partnership con l'azienda H-Farm, con sede a Roncade (TV). 1 Le scuole di Avanguardie Educative partecipanti alla sperimentazione sono: il Convitto Nazionale "Umberto I" di Torino; l'ITE "A. Tosi" di Busto Arsizio; l'IISS "C. A. Dalla Chiesa" di Montefiascone; l'ITT "M. ...
... The selected articles highlighted the potential of VR to stimulate brain plasticity in order to promote rehabilitation/habilitation processes. However, the effects have been observed to be more evident in combination with the integration of VR treatments with other therapeutic modalities such as standard therapies, BCI, and non-invasive brain stimulation (1). Training programs are deficit-specific and therefore aim at the recovery of lost or to-be-acquired abilities. ...
... Affiancando la didattica tradizionale, portata avanti dall'insegnante curriculare, e in base a quanto emerso dal PEI, si è deciso di sviluppare, con l'ausilio delle TIC, una serie di attività nell'ambito delle Scienze, approfondendo un argomento trattato all'interno del programma scolastico e sviluppando alcune tematiche riguardanti l'educazione ambientale. Sebbene sia importante un supporto nella didattica, è altrettanto e forse ancor più rilevante che il gruppo sia in grado di creare un 1 Il prompting consiste nel fornire alla persona uno o più stimoli sotto forma di prompt (aiuti), in modo che questi ultimi rendano possibile il verificarsi di un comportamento desiderato. 2 Il fading è un'operazione di controllo dello stimolo e consiste nella progressiva riduzione degli aiuti. 3 https://didatticapersuasiva.com/didattica/il-tutoring-e-il-peer-tutoring. ...
... As Boyd, Barnett, Bodrova, Leong and Gomby (2005) stated, children need social, emotional, cognitive, and motivational skills to adapt to and succeed at school. In line with the research findings, young children need emotional and self-regulation skills for a successful education in the short and long run. ...
Article
Full-text available
Adjustment to preschool in early childhood refers to children's successful adaptation to the preschool environment. Attitudes towards school, and therefore school achievement in the subsequent years, are directly related to the child's adjustment to preschool. Children’s emotional competence and self-regulation are two important child-level factors affecting children’s adjustment to preschool. This descriptive study aimed to explore whether self-regulation acts as a mediator between emotional competence and the preschool adjustment of children. The mediation model was evaluated using linear and hierarchical regression analyses, along with a Sobel test. A total of 628 children (294 female; 334 male) from Istanbul participated in the study. Teachers (n= 53) who had known the children for at least two months before the study provided data. The study's results indicated that emotional competence and self-regulation are significant predictors of school adjustment. Notably, self-regulation partially mediates the association between emotional competence and preschool adjustment. Consequently, there exists an indirect link between emotional competence and preschool adjustment. The study highlighted the crucial role of self-regulation in mediating the relationship between emotional competence and school adjustment in preschool children. Findings suggested that strong emotional and self-regulation skills facilitate adaptation to preschool, whereas deficiencies in these areas lead to adjustment difficulties.
... Self-regulation of learning refers to the willingness to manage one's behavior, resist impulses, maintain concentration, and undertake tasks, even in the presence of more appealing alternatives (Boyd et al., 2005;Beytekin and Kadi, 2014;Akyol, 2016;Solmaz, 2017). It involves being aware of one's reasoning, being sensitivity to feedback, and evaluating the effectiveness of actions (Marzano and Kendall, 2006). ...
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this study was to investigate whether, within a practice-based curriculum, feedback on the assessment tasks provided during campus coursework offers opportunities to promote lifelong learning dispositions in pre-service teachers. For this, pre-service teachers (n = 231) completed a validated questionnaire regarding lifelong learning dispositions. Then, feedback from assessment tasks (n = 14) was analyzed to identify claims related to curiosity, motivation, perseverance, and self-regulation of learning. Finally, in-depth interviews were conducted with pre-service teachers (n = 8) to explore their perspectives on feedback and lifelong learning dispositions. Data triangulation was used to confirm and add depth to the findings. Feedback on assessment tasks provided during campus course work promotes lifelong learning dispositions when: (i) tied to authentic tasks, (ii) is provide not only by teacher educators but also by peers, (iii) incorporates both positive and negative comments, along with practical advice. The implication of findings for teacher education is discussed.
Conference Paper
Over the past few decades, the use of extended reality environments for the purpose of teaching and learning has become increasingly popular. Such environments provide an opportunity for perceptual presence and immersion through multisensory experience and interaction and thus mimicking the real-world [1], [2]. Extended reality (XR) encompasses environments and technologies such as Augmented Reality (AR), Virtual Reality (VR), and Mixed Reality (MR) [3], [4]. AR overlays information such as images, texts, video and sounds on the real world or the user's viewing device [5]. VR provides an environment for sensory immersion and simulates the real world. It offers an interactive environment where users are immersed in a virtual environment and cannot see the real environment around them [5], [6]. MR uses technology to influence the human perception of an experience. It provides a view of the physical world with an overlay of virtual information where physical and virtual elements can interact [4], [7]. XR environments can help students practice and develop skills that might be difficult to learn in a traditional teaching and learning environment [8]. They also provide an opportunity for students to “see” concepts/structures that are difficult to see and/or are invisible [9] or interact with materials that might be hazardous in real life [10], [11]. Review studies have indicated other benefits of virtual environments such as improved learning outcomes, cognitive, psychomotor and affective skills, as well as enhanced enjoyment, engagement and motivation [12]-[15]. di Lanzo et al. reviewed the use of virtual reality specifically in engineering education [16]. Findings indicated that virtual reality can be beneficial for cognitive and skill-based learning outcomes. However, shortcomings with respect to evaluation metrics and small sample size were noted. The authors further argue that those shortcomings can even question the reported benefits of using VR in engineering education. Despite recent trends in integrating XR environment in education, there is an absence of theoretical frameworks in studies of XR applications for learning [17]-[20]. Learning theories and educational frameworks, however, can play a key role in supporting educational technology implementation. This paper, thus, seeks to identify, and synthesize theoretical frameworks that support the design and/or implementation and evaluation of XR as a guide for faculty, educational researchers and instructional designers. It will present a systematic review that addresses the following questions: 1. What theoretical frameworks/technological factors are used in educational XR studies? 2. How are theoretical frameworks/technological factors applied in educational XR studies? 3. How is the effectiveness of XR evaluated?
Preprint
Full-text available
The protection of the natural environment requires ecologically sustainable behavior from current and future generations. Therefore, the present study aimed to conceive an immersive virtual reality experience to foster ecologically sustainable behavior in elementary schools. The findings were obtained in a three-stage iterative process using twelve qualitative interviews with experts from the fields of sustainability, psychology, didactics, and pedagogy, as well as elementary school teachers. The findings suggest that immersive virtual reality learning experiences should be embedded holistically into iterative learning loops to achieve transformative learning. Accordingly, we have developed a conceptual framework for designing transformative learning processes. This framework distinguishes between different learning phases and includes didactic tools, psychological success factors, and recommendations for action. These allow us to formulate concrete measures for each learning phase. In addition, we have conceived an immersive virtual reality experience suitable to reduce the psychological distance to the environmental changes caused by meat consumption. It shall illustrate how immersive virtual reality can be designed and embedded into holistic learning processes to achieve transformative learning.
Article
Full-text available
Play is universal. Children of every culture engage in play. Research indicates that children learn best in an environment which allows them to explore, discover, and play. Play is an important part of developmentally appropriate child care programs. It is also closely tied to cognitive, physical, social, emotional, linguistic, and creative development. By the method of documentary analysis, this study explores why is play important for young children's development? Based on this exploration, the reasons are as follows: (1) play is important to healthy brain development, (2) play favors young children’s physical development, (3) play is important to young children’s social and emotional development, (4) play can make important contributions to the development of language, and (5) play benefits young children’s creative development. Hopefully such an exploration will make people understand the importance of play in young children's development.
Article
Children’s’ literacy development starts very early in life through participation and experience in the home well before school. It grows more consistently during pre-school and school years as children explore their socio-cultural world more and more. Early years represent children’s important formative stages of growth when they learn the language and the world that they will grow to establish a life-long relationship with. When children learn and develop their Mother tongue or primary language therefore, they are learning the foundation of learning itself, a process of language and meaning making that is a continuous process from birth, through infancy and childhood to adolescence an d adult life. The majority of young children in Sub-Saharan Africa are unable to readily access sustainable early literacy development and childhood care and education and their educational achievement trajectories are hugely compromised. Lack of viable parental participation in their literacy development, encouragement and support from teachers and availability of learning materials at school are some of environmental factors that make up their early social world. Meanwhile, rural homes and communities are well endowed with cultural resources and a stock of practices, knowledge, and skills relevant to the promotion of literacy development waiting to be tapped into. This paper uses analysis of data from two qualitative and one mixed methods studies in three different countries to discuss the factors that influence literacy development based on the interplay of language (learning), literacy (development) and school education out comes in low resource communities and how best they could be overcome. The paper concludes that low performance in general is a result of a combination factors that require concerted efforts at all levels if the situation can be reversed.
Article
The term “kindergarten readiness” lacks a formal definition. The need for a single, widely accepted definition for this term is necessary for teachers and educational leaders to prepare children and their families for the start of formal education. This study was conducted to understand the perceptions of preschool and kindergarten teachers concerning typically developing students on day 60 of kindergarten in the state of North Carolina. Focus groups were used to determine particular skills that a kindergartener should exhibit by that point in school in order to be successful during that year. Both groups of teachers were knowledgeable about the developmental continuum kindergartners should follow in order to achieve needed skills according to the North Carolina Early Learning Inventory. A second finding was that kindergarten teachers scored students lower than preschool teachers on the anticipated ability of their students to achieve a skill. All teachers considered some skills were introduced in the inventory prematurely. Participants within this study perceived school readiness as skills that students should exhibit on day 60 of kindergarten that would allow for them to have a successful kindergarten year. Some skills that would be typical on day 60 of kindergarten are that students are: able to control their emotions and understand the emotions of others, understand how to behave in familiar environments when routines and procedures do not change, and are able to discriminate the sound that an alliteration and rhyme make but may not be able to explain the reason for the alliteration or rhyme.
Article
Full-text available
Effects of early child care on children's functioning at the age of 41/2 years wee a examined in the NICHD (National Institute of Child Health and Human Development) Study of Early Child Care, a prospective longitudinal study of more than 1,000 children. Even after controlling for multiple child and family characteristics, children's development was predicted by early child-care experience. Higher-quality child care, improvements in the quality of child care, and experience in center-type arrangements predicted better pre-academic skills and language performance at 41/2 years. More hours of care predicted higher levels of behavior problems according to caregivers. Effect sizes associated with early child-care experiences were evaluated in relation to effect sizes obtained for two other well-recognized influences on early development: parenting and poverty. The findings indicated the importance (and relative independence) of quantity, quality, and type of child care for children's development just prior to the time that children initiate formal schooling.
Article
Full-text available
In the most comprehensive US study to date about connections among child care experiences, family factors, and children's early development, 1100 children have been followed from birth through age 7. The study's intent is to describe family and child care/school contexts of children's development and examine associations between contextual variations and children's developmental outcomes in social-emotional, cognitive, and physical health domains. By age 3, over 90% of the children had experienced regular nonmaternal care, and over 50% were regularly spending over 30 hours/week in care. Economic factors, family characteristics, and maternal attitudes all influenced the amount and nature of early nonmaternal care. Observed quality of nonmaternal care consistently predicted social-emotional and cognitive-linguistic outcomes during the first 3 years of life. Amount of time spent in nonmaternal care predicted some social-emotional outcomes. Type and stability of nonmaternal care had limited predictive value. Family factors, including maternal sensitivity, quality of home environment, and income, were more consistent predictors of children's outcomes than any aspect of early nonmaternal care experiences. (C) 2001 Elsevier Science Inc. All rights reserved.
Article
Full-text available
Opinions vary widely as to what young children should know or be capable of doing to be ready for kindergarten. This paper looks at the beliefs held by two groups who play critical roles in the early education of children: parents of preschoolers and kindergarten teachers. Two surveys that were sponsored by the U.S. Department of Education and were conducted in the spring of 1993 asked parents and teachers to rate several attributes and attitudes in terms of their importance to any child's readiness for kindergarten. Parents and teachers agreed that it was very important or essential that the children be able to communicate their needs and wants verbally, and that the children be enthusiastic and curious in approaching new activities. There was considerable disagreement on other characteristics, however. A majority of the parents believed that for a child to be ready to enter kindergarten, knowing the letters of the alphabet, being able to count to 20 or more, and being able to use pencils and paint brushes were very important or essential, whereas few kindergarten teachers shared these beliefs. Four tables and seven endnotes are included. Contains 16 references. (MDM)
Chapter
What is the role of sleep in children's behavioral, emotional, and cognitive regulation? This chapter considers theoretical and conceptual links between sleep and self-regulation, with special attention to sleep and self-regulation in early childhood. We selectively review the growing body of research on associations between sleep and self-regulation, mentioning some methodological issues. We also consider how child characteristics and sociocontextual factors may interact with sleep in the development of self-regulation in early childhood. We provide some relevant empirical examples from our own research.
Article
Cognitive competencies and motivation were assessed at the beginning and the end of the year for 228 preschoolers and kindergartners and again at the end of the next year (kindergarten or first grade) for 93 of the participants. Participants were in classrooms classified as either emphasizing basic skills in a less positive social climate or de-emphasizing basic skills in a more positive social climate. Cognitive competencies were assessed with two achievement tests (one for letters/reading and another for numbers/math) and six subscales from the McCarthy test. Motivation (perceptions of competence, attitudes toward school, anxiety, affect, risk taking, expectations for success, independence, and persistence) was assessed in an experimental setting and by observing children's behavior in their classroom. The results showed primarily negative effects on both cognitive and motivation outcomes of preschool programs emphasizing basic skills using structured, teacher-directed approaches in a relatively negative social climate. For kindergartners both positive and negative achievement and motivation outcomes were associated with both types of classrooms.
Article
With data from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care, we used structural equation modeling to test paths from structural indicators of child-care quality, specifically caregiver training and child-staff ratio, through a process indicator to child outcomes. There were three main findings: (a) Quality of maternal caregiving was the strongest predictor of cognitive competence, as well as caregivers' ratings of social competence; (b) quality of nonmaternal caregiving was associated with cognitive competence and caregivers' ratings of social competence; and (c) there was a mediated path from both caregiver training and child-staff ratio through quality of nonmaternal caregiving to cognitive competence, as well as to caregivers' ratings of social competence, that was not accounted for entirely by family variables. These findings provide empirical support for policies that improve state regulations for caregiver training and child-staff ratios.