Content uploaded by Lilia Cortina
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Lilia Cortina on Jul 17, 2014
Content may be subject to copyright.
UNSEEN INJUSTICE: INCIVILITY AS MODERN
DISCRIMINATION IN ORGANIZATIONS
LILIA M. CORTINA
University of Michigan
This article advances a theory of incivility as a veiled manifestation of sexism and
racism in organizations. To support this argument, I draw from social psychological
research on modern discrimination. The result is a multilevel model of selective
incivility, with determinants at the level of the person, organization, and society.
Selective incivility could be one mechanism by which gender and racial disparities
persist in American organizations, despite concerted efforts to eradicate bias. I dis-
cuss scientific and practical implications.
Recent years have seen increasing scholar-
ship on subtle, nonphysical manifestations of
interpersonal mistreatment in the workplace, in-
cluding general incivility. This term encom-
passes low-intensity conduct that lacks a clear
intent to harm but nevertheless violates social
norms and injures targeted employees (Anders-
son & Pearson, 1999; Cortina, Magley, Williams,
& Langhout, 2001). In this paper I extend the
notion of incivility by examining it through
lenses of gender and race. The central argument
is that incivility, in some cases, is not “general”
at all but instead represents contemporary man-
ifestations of gender and racial bias in the work-
place. That is, with the rise of taboos, policies,
and laws prohibiting discrimination against
specific social groups, blatant intentions and
efforts to alienate women and minorities from
organizational life are no longer tolerated. How-
ever, one can mask discrimination (even without
realizing it) behind everyday acts of incivility
and still maintain an unbiased image. This
would be consistent with research demonstrat-
ing that prejudices persist in covert forms within
society in general (e.g., Dovidio & Gaertner,
1998) and within organizations in particular
(e.g., Brief, Dietz, Cohen, Pugh, & Vaslow, 2000).
I begin the article with a recap of theory and
findings from research on workplace incivility.
Next is an in-depth review of theories of modern
discrimination, focusing on cognitive, affective,
social, and organizational antecedents. An inte-
gration of these two bodies of literature then
supports the proposition that incivility can con-
stitute a particularly insidious, behavioral man-
ifestation of modern/contemporary/covert sex-
ism and racism. The result is a multilevel model
of incivility as modern discrimination in organi-
zations. In the latter half of the paper, I address
implications of this “selective incivility” model
for research, policy, and practice in organiza-
tions. The following definition of workplace dis-
crimination frames this work: “Unfair employ-
ment discrimination [occurs] when persons in a
‘social category’...areputatadisadvantage in
the workplace relative to other groups with com-
parable potential or proven success” (Dipboye &
Halverson, 2004: 131).
This article’s novel contributions are fourfold.
First, by building bridges with social psycholog-
ical scholarship on discrimination, I extend the
organizational literature on antisocial work be-
havior to address issues of race and gender.
Most studies of workplace aggression, deviance,
bullying, and so forth to date have focused al-
most exclusively on “general” conduct irrespec-
tive of social categories, without recognizing
that antisocial work behavior may often reflect
bias against members of undervalued social
groups. A second contribution of the present
work is to the social psychology literature. A
common criticism of social psychology (e.g.,
Fiske, 2000) is that research on “discrimination”
and “intergroup conflict” has addressed atti-
tudes and affect in great detail, often to the
neglect of action. Attitudes, stereotypes, and
ideologies are certainly important, but a com-
plete understanding of intergroup relations re-
Portions of this paper were presented in April 2004 at the
annual meeting of the Society for Industrial-Organizational
Psychology, Chicago. Many thanks to Kim Lonsway, Abby
Stewart, and Arzu Wasti for their valuable comments on
earlier versions.
姝 Academy of Management Review
2008, Vol. 33, No. 1, 55–75.
55
Copyright of the Academy of Management, all rights reserved. Contents may not be copied, emailed, posted to a listserv, or otherwise transmitted without the copyright
holder’s express written permission. Users may print, download, or email articles for individual use only.
quires attention to intergroup behavior. The cur-
rent article does just that by bringing to light the
specific behavioral experience of selective inci-
vility. The few psychologists who have looked at
actual discriminatory conduct have generally
focused on “formal discrimination”—for exam-
ple, unfair selection decisions (e.g., Brief et al.,
2000; Dovidio & Gaertner, 2000). In contrast, I
take an in-depth look at a form of “interpersonal
discrimination,” which Hebl, Foster, Mannix,
and Dovidio define as “nonverbal, paraverbal,
and...verbal behaviors that occur in social in-
teractions” (2002: 816); this represents a third
contribution of the present article. Finally, I in-
tegrate concepts from organizational and social
psychology, management science, and the law
to propose fruitful new directions for research
and practice in cases of selective incivility in
the workplace.
WORKPLACE INCIVILITY: DEFINITION,
INCIDENCE, AND IMPACT
Andersson and Pearson define workplace in-
civility as “low intensity deviant behavior with
ambiguous intent to harm the target, in violation
of workplace norms for mutual respect. Uncivil
behaviors are characteristically rude and dis-
courteous, displaying a lack of regard for oth-
ers” (1999: 457). They conceptualize this as a spe-
cific form of employee deviance (Robinson &
Bennett, 1995), which, in turn, represents a sub-
set of antisocial employee behavior (Giacolone
& Greenberg, 1997). When unambiguous inten-
tions and expectations to harm the target or
organization are present, definitions of incivility
overlap with psychological aggression (e.g.,
Baron, 2004; Neuman, 2004). However, incivility
differs from psychological aggression when be-
haviors lack clear, conscious intentionality.
That is, although incivility may occasionally
have visibly injurious objectives, it can often be
attributed to other factors, such as the instiga-
tor’s ignorance, oversight, or personality; intent,
whether present or not, is ambiguous to one or
more of the parties involved (Andersson & Pear-
son, 1999; Pearson, Andersson, & Wegner, 2001).
However, workplace incivility, by definition, is
completely distinct from physical aggression
and violence (e.g., VandenBos & Bulatao, 1996).
Incivility is perhaps one of the most pervasive
forms of antisocial behavior in the workplace.
To assess incivility prevalence, my colleagues
and I surveyed employees in a range of work
settings; the incidence rates we uncovered illus-
trate the ubiquity of this phenomenon. For ex-
ample, 71 percent of a court employee sample
(Cortina et al., 2001), 75 percent of a university
employee sample (Cortina & Magley, 2007), and
79 percent of a law enforcement sample (Cor-
tina, Lonsway, & Magley, 2004) reported that
they had encountered some form of uncivil con-
duct at work in recent years. Other researchers
have reported similarly high rates of related
workplace behaviors: for example, “generalized
harassment”—75 percent (Einarsen & Raknes,
1997); “generalized workplace harassment”— 64
percent (Rospenda, 2002); and “rude or disre-
spectful treatment”— 67 percent (Neuman, 2004).
The proliferation of incivility in the workplace
has very real, very negative consequences for
employees, workgroups, and organizations.
Barling and colleagues (Barling et al., 1996;
Barling, Rogers, & Kelloway, 2001) have theo-
rized that experiences of abusive behaviors at
work lead to negative mood, cognitive distrac-
tion, fear, and perceived injustice. Others (e.g.,
Andersson & Pearson, 1999; Cortina et al., 2001;
Miner-Rubino & Cortina, 2004) have added dam-
aged social identity and anger to this list. These
cognitive and affective reactions, in turn, ad-
versely influence targets’ occupational, psycho-
logical, and physical health. Some have
proposed that these negative consequences ex-
tend beyond the targeted employee to affect by-
standers, workgroups, and whole organizations
(Andersson & Pearson, 1999; Miner-Rubino &
Cortina, 2004).
Data are emerging to support theories that
although incivility may be subtle, its effects are
not. Empirical research suggests that employees
targeted with uncivil behavior experience
greater job stress and dissatisfaction, lower cre-
ativity, cognitive distraction, and psychological
distress. Possibly in an attempt to dampen the
increased stressfulness of work, targets also use
more substances. Moreover, incivility disrupts
employee relationships and derails cooperation,
rending the social fabric of the workgroup. Per-
sonnel targeted with pervasive incivility ulti-
mately lose commitment to their organizations
and exit at higher rates (Cortina et al., 2001, 2002;
Lim & Cortina, 2004; Pearson, Andersson, &
Porath, 2000; Pearson et al., 2001; Pearson &
Porath, 2004; Richman et al., 1999; Rospenda,
2002). Even employees who merely observe un-
56 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review
civil treatment (e.g., toward colleagues) show
lower job satisfaction and commitment and
greater job burnout and turnover intentions;
such observer effects emerge even while con-
trolling for negative affectivity, so they cannot
be attributed to a negative dispositional stance
making observers more attuned to negative
stimuli in their environments (Miner-Rubino &
Cortina, 2006). These adverse individual and
collective consequences have financial implica-
tions for employers, who must absorb the costs
of employee distraction and discontentment, job
accidents, substance abuse, sick leave, work
team conflict, productivity decline, and turnover.
Explaining how such a “low-grade” phenome-
non as incivility can have such widespread con-
sequences, my colleagues and I (Cortina & Mag-
ley, 2004; Cortina et al., 2001) have asserted that
many uncivil work behaviors fall into the category
of daily hassles—that is, routine nuisances of ev-
eryday life (e.g., Lazarus, 1999; Lazarus & Folkman,
1984). Daily hassles lack the drama and intensity
of major life events. Nevertheless, chronic, low-key
stressors that repeat over time can “wear down”
an individual, both psychologically and physi-
cally (e.g., Wheaton, 1997). Moreover, targeted em-
ployees may have difficulty developing effective
means of coping with and controlling such ambig-
uous phenomena. Hopelessness and resignation
may result (Deitch et al., 2004). Following Richman
and colleagues’ (1996; Rospenda, 2002) reasoning,
personnel might also find interpersonal hostility
highly unexpected and unnecessary in the work
environment (in contrast to task-based stressors,
which might seem more routine and therefore
more tolerable in that context). For all of these
reasons, daily interpersonal stressors at work can
accumulate to have a greater impact on psycho-
logical and health outcomes than major time-
limited disturbances (e.g., Cortina & Magley, 2004;
Cortina et al., 2001; Deitch et al., 2004; Lazarus,
1999; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984).
In sum, prior research has laid important foun-
dations in defining incivility, delineating its im-
pact, and articulating its relationship to other cat-
egories of generalized hostility in the workplace.
Questions remain about how incivility relates to
specific forms of workplace mistreatment, such as
that based on sex and race.
1
Incivility, sexual ha-
rassment, and racial harassment share certain
features, all entailing behaving in an antisocial
way; degrading, offending, or intimidating tar-
gets; and violating standards of interpersonal re-
spect. In addition, similar motivations may drive
these different forms of abuse, such as instigators’
pursuit of social power/dominance, disregard for
authority and norms, desire for valued resources,
self-presentational goals, and value differences
with the target (Andersson & Pearson, 1999;
Buchanan, 2005; Cleveland & Kerst, 1993; Deitch et
al., 2004; James, Lovato, & Khoo, 1994; Lim & Cor-
tina, 2005; O’Leary-Kelly, Paetzold, & Griffin, 2000;
Pearson & Porath, 2004; Sanchez & Brock, 1996;
Schneider, Hitlan, & Radhakrishnan, 2000; Thacker
& Ferris, 1991).
In addition, perhaps incivility, sexual harass-
ment, and racial harassment are, at times, one
and the same. This may seem illogical, given
that incivility is facially neutral by definition.
That is, “generally” uncivil behaviors have no
overt reference to gender, race, or other social
category. This nevertheless does not rule out the
possibility that incivility sometimes represents
covert manifestations of gender and racial bias
in the workplace. The theory advanced in this
article will elucidate this possibility. Such selec-
tive incivility could be one mechanism by which
gender and racial disparities persist in organi-
zations, despite concerted legislative, judicial,
and organizational efforts to eradicate bias.
CONTEMPORARY PERSPECTIVES ON
DISCRIMINATION
The last four decades have seen sweeping
changes in antidiscrimination laws, practices,
and ideologies in the United States. Owing to
Title VII of the Civil Rights Act of 1964 and re-
lated reforms, blatant employment discrimina-
tion based on gender and race (among other
factors) has become illegal. Women and people
of color now enjoy much greater access to occu-
pational and economic opportunities than in the
past. Along with these tangible changes, atti-
tudes toward women’s paid employment, and
white attitudes toward ethnic minorities, have
become more positive, tolerant, and accepting.
1
Of course, workplace mistreatment can be based on
other social dimensions as well, such as sexual orientation,
age, disability status, and so on. The theory elaborated in
this paper focuses primarily on gender and race; however,
similar arguments could be developed for other character-
istics that divide and stigmatize individuals.
2008 57Cortina
Public expression of sexist and racist beliefs
has undergone a radical decline (Benokraitis,
1997; Brief & Barsky, 2000; Brief et al., 1997, 2000;
Brief & Hayes, 1997; Dipboye & Halverson, 2004;
Dovidio & Gaertner, 1998; Dovidio, Gaertner, &
Bachman, 2001; Operario & Fiske, 1998; Swim,
Aikin, Hall, & Hunter, 1995; Tougas, Brown,
Beaton, & Joly, 1995). The importance of these
changes cannot be overstated.
In spite of progress, gender and racial dispar-
ities endure in American organizations, particu-
larly at the highest levels, where power is most
concentrated (e.g., Benokraitis, 1997; Brief et al.,
1997; Brief & Hayes, 1997; Pettigrew & Martin,
1987; Valian, 1998). This is true across a range of
industries, from the military to the federal gov-
ernment to the Fortune 500 (Dovidio, Gaertner, &
Bachman, 2001). According to Census and De-
partment of Labor statistics, women and ethnic/
racial minorities still receive less pay, face
greater unemployment, and work in lower-
status jobs than their white male counterparts;
this remains true even after controlling for edu-
cation, experience, and skill level (Brief et al.,
1997; Dipboye & Halverson, 2004). In 2002 the
Equal Employment Opportunity Commission
(EEOC) received 84,442 complaints of employ-
ment discrimination; two out of every three al-
legations were based on either gender or race
(U.S. Equal Employment Opportunity Commis-
sion, 2003). Illustrating the striking persistence
of these forms of bias, Benokraitis (1997) noted
that, in the mid 1990s, white men constituted
one-third of the U.S. population. At the same
time, they made up “85% of tenured professors,
85% of partners in law firms, 80% of the U.S.
House of Representatives, 90% of the U.S. Sen-
ate, 95% of Fortune 500 CEOs...and100% of all
US presidents” (1997: 5).
One might wonder how discrimination could
be so robust against several decades of legal,
organizational, and attitudinal reform. Some
suggest that these changes may have been ef-
fective in reducing blatant or “old-fashioned”
discrimination, but subtle discrimination lives
on in the world of work (e.g., Benokraitis, 1997;
Brief & Barsky, 2000; Brief, Buttram, Elliott,
Reizenstein, & McCline, 1995; Deitch et al., 2004;
Dipboye & Halverson, 2004). In fact, contempo-
rary antifemale and antiminority bias is some-
times so ambiguous that instigators are un-
aware of its discriminatory nature, and they
typically have rational, nondiscriminatory ex-
planations for their conduct. As Dipboye and
Halverson explain, “Much of today’s discrimina-
tion takes a more subtle form and has slipped
out of the light into the dark side of the organi-
zation” (2004: 132).
The ambiguity inherent in subtle discrimina-
tion makes it particularly difficult for targets
and managers to recognize, much less control.
Meyerson and Fletcher compellingly summarize
this situation as it relates to gender:
As we enter the year 2000, the glass ceiling re-
mains. What will it take to finally shatter it? Not
a revolution . . . the women’s movement [once]
used radical rhetoric and legal action to drive out
overt discrimination, but most of the barriers that
persist today are insidious—a revolution couldn’t
find them to blast away. Rather, gender discrim-
ination now is so deeply embedded in organiza-
tional life as to be virtually indiscernible. Even
the women who feel its impact are often hard-
pressed to know what hit them (2000: 127).
Similar arguments can be made for persistent
but subtle racial bias in the workplace. The
glass ceiling holds strong in contemporary or-
ganizations, impeding women and employees of
color from advancing to the same levels and at
the same rates as their white male contempo-
raries. Moreover, “glass walls” keep women and
minorities confined to certain occupational cat-
egories (Brief & Barsky, 2000; Rowe, 1990). These
concealed barriers likely take many forms, one
of which may be selective incivility. Theories of
modern discrimination explain how and why
this might be the case.
To account for persistent gender and racial
inequalities in the United States, social psychol-
ogists have identified various forms of modern
discrimination. In the realm of race relations,
these concepts include aversive racism (Dovidio
& Gaertner, 1998; Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986), sym-
bolic racism (Sears, 1988, 1998), and modern rac-
ism (Brief et al., 2000; McConahay, 1986; Mc-
Conahay & Hough, 1976). The gender bias
literature refers to similar phenomena, such as
modern sexism (Swim, Scott, Sechrist, Campbell,
& Stangor, 1995), neosexism (Tougas et al., 1995),
and contemporary sexism (Jackson, Esses, &
Burris, 2001). Although each conceptualization of
contemporary discrimination is slightly differ-
ent, the mythologies behind the behaviors are
well-represented by McConahay’s depiction of
modern racism against blacks:
The principal tenets of modern racism are these:
(1) Discrimination is a thing of the past because
58 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review
Blacks now have the freedom to compete in the
marketplace and to enjoy those things they can
afford. (2) Blacks are pushing too hard, too fast,
and into places where they are not wanted. (3)
These tactics and demands are unfair. (4) There-
fore, recent gains are undeserved and the pres-
tige granting institutions of society are giving
Blacks more attention and the concomitant status
than they deserve (1986: 92–93).
Modern racists see these views as empirical
fact rather than opinion or racist ideology. In
fact, they ostensibly endorse egalitarian values,
publicly condemn racism, and strongly identify
themselves as nonprejudiced. This explicit re-
jection of overt bias— combined with implicit
antiminority (or antifemale) beliefs—yields sub-
tle, often unintentional and unconscious forms
of discrimination. However, to maintain an egal-
itarian identity, modern racists only engage in
discrimination when there is a plausible, non-
racial (or nongendered) explanation for the dif-
ferential treatment—for instance, a business
justification. Absent a reasonable, nonpreju-
diced rationale for disparate conduct, discrimi-
nation does not manifest (Brief et al., 1995, 1997,
2000; James, Brief, Dietz, & Cohen, 2001; Mc-
Conahay, 1986; McConahay & Hough, 1976). This
allows the would-be instigators to protect them-
selves from charges of racism.
Contemporary forms of bias are distinct from
“old-fashioned” sexism and racism, character-
ized by blatant antipathy, beliefs that women
and people of color are inherently inferior, en-
dorsement of pejorative stereotypes, and sup-
port for open acts of discrimination. Such anti-
quated beliefs and overtly discriminatory
conduct are becoming less and less common
(Brief et al., 1997; Dovidio & Gaertner, 1998; Mc-
Conahay, 1986; McConahay & Hough, 1976;
Sears, 1988, 1998; Swim et al., 1995; Tougas et al.,
1995). For this reason, scholars are beginning to
recognize that
singular attention to major discriminatory acts in
the workplace is insufficient for explaining the
experience of discrimination many minority
members experience on the job, and may be an
increasingly inadequate research focus in the fu-
ture as the social and political landscape shifts
toward newer forms of racism and discrimination
(Deitch et al., 2003: 1300 –1301).
Different theories have emerged about the de-
velopmental history of modern discrimination.
One is that this “second-generation” form of
bias has recently arisen to replace old-fash-
ioned sexism and racism, now deemed undesir-
able and, at times, unlawful (Benokraitis & Fea-
gin, 1995; Brief et al., 1997; Forbes, Adams-Curtis,
& White, 2004; Pettigrew & Martin, 1987; Tougas
et al., 1995). A second argument is that prejudice
has had a long history in our society, persisting
over time i n different manifestations, as affected
by each sociohistorical moment. An elaborated
version of this hypothesis is that negative atti-
tudes toward women and ethnic minorities have
continued despite social pressure to renounce
prejudice; rather than going away, the negative
attitudes become stored in memory and change
from explicit to implicit (Dovidio, Gaertner,
Kawakami, & Hodson, 2002). A final possibility is
that subtle, unintentional, unconscious forms of
discrimination are not new; what may be new is
their visibility, now that blatant discrimination
no longer overshadows them. Regardless of
their developmental trajectory, prejudice and
discrimination seem to be alive and well in the
contemporary American workplace.
Research has identified various factors that
fuel discrimination, including cognition, affect,
and the organizational and societal context.
Some of these same driving forces could also
underlie discriminatory forms of workplace in-
civility, so here I highlight studies of each cate-
gory of antecedent. The resulting model follows
recent multilevel trends in the organizational
sciences (e.g., Klein & Kozlowski, 2000), attend-
ing to influences at the level of the individual,
the organization, and society. The review below
is intended to be illustrative rather than exhaus-
tive—to demonstrate how scholarship on bias
and discrimination can inform science and prac-
tice related to incivility in organizations. Figure
1 displays a conceptual model of the ideas that
follow.
Person-Level Explanations
Research on modern discrimination has pri-
marily attended to intraindividual cognitive and
affective events that drive a person to discrimi-
nate.
Cognition. Theories of social cognition lie at
the heart of the literature on modern discrimina-
tion, which focuses in particular on categoriza-
tion and stereotyping. Social categorization re-
fers to the cognitive process by which our minds
place people into social categories based on
salient cues, such as gender, race, and age. This
2008 59Cortina
is argued to be a natural, automatic, and often
unconscious process. This process has various
benefits: easing the cognitive burden, simplify-
ing perception and judgment, and helping us
make sense of an intricate social environment.
Without social categorization, the complexity of
person perception would be overwhelming (e.g.,
Devine & Monteith, 1999; Dovidio, Gaertner, &
Bachman, 2001; Fiske, 2000; Jones, 2002; Operario
& Fiske, 1998; Stone, Stone, & Dipboye, 1992).
Categorization of a person into a particular
group often triggers stereotypes—that is, preva-
lent and overgeneralized knowledge, beliefs,
and expectancies about members of that social
category (e.g., Hilton & von Hipple, 1996; Jones,
2002; Miller & Turnbull, 1986; Stone et al., 1992).
In short, stereotypes are the “cultural baggage”
that social categories carry (Operario & Fiske,
1998: 40). They allow perceivers to understand
(often inaccurately) and make predictions about
others, averting the onerous task of learning
in-depth details about each individual encoun-
tered. Indeed, we tend not to seek further per-
sonal information after placing someone into a
social category, instead basing impression for-
mation largely on stereotypes associated with
that category (e.g., Operario & Fiske, 1998; Stone
et al., 1992).
Stereotypes come in explicit and implicit va-
rieties. Explicit stereotypes operate at the con-
scious level and occur with intention, effort,
awareness, and control. Attitudes and stereo-
types can also be implicit, however, occurring
unintentionally, unconsciously, and effortlessly
(e.g., Banaji & Dasgupta, 1998; Baron & Banaji,
2006; Devine, 1989; Devine & Monteith, 1999;
Greenwald & Banaji, 1995; Jones, 2002; Valian,
1998). The implicit variant of stereotyping is par-
ticularly implicated in modern discrimination.
That is, a modern sexist or racist unknowingly
applies negative stereotypes to women and peo-
ple of color, which can lead the stereotyper to
mistreat members of these social groups. How-
ever, the mistreatment only arises in situations
when there is a plausible, nonprejudiced expla-
nation for the behavior so that the instigator can
maintain a nondiscriminatory self-image.
Stereotypes of outgroup members can also be
“ambivalent,” containing both positive and neg-
ative elements. That is, they need not reflect
uniform antipathy or contempt. Across years of
stereotyping research (e.g., Allport, 1954; Fiske,
Cuddy, Glick, & Xu, 2002; Glick & Fiske, 1999,
2001), scholars have identified two core stereo-
type dimensions: warmth and competence.
Some groups are the object of benevolent or
paternalistic stereotypes, being perceived as
warm but generally incompetent; these include
housewives, the elderly, and the disabled. Oth-
ers, in contrast, are seen as low in warmth but
FIGURE 1
Integrated Model of Incivility As Modern Discrimination in Organizations
60 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review
(overly) high in competence, so they are targeted
with a more hostile, envious stereotype; these
groups include black professionals, career
women, and Asians. Hostility toward these “en-
vied” outgroups becomes amplified when they
achieve success, status, and power, posing a
competitive threat to the ingroup.
Affect. Some scholars claim that, compared to
stereotypic cognition, prejudice is the better pre-
dictor of discriminatory behavior (Fiske, 2000).
Prejudice refers specifically to an affective reac-
tion to people solely because of their member-
ship in a specific social category. Particularly
relevant are the categories of ingroup and out-
group. That is, social categorization processes
lead all people to categorize others as “one of
us” versus “one of them.” This gives rise to pos-
itive affective biases toward ingroup members,
coupled with negative feelings toward individ-
uals in the outgroup (Fiske, 2002; Jones, 2002;
Operario & Fiske, 1998).
The negative affect toward the outgroup can
take different forms, not simply amounting to a
homogeneous feeling of contempt. For example,
research by Fiske and colleagues (2002) has
shown that people tend to feel “paternalistic
prejudice” toward groups stereotyped as warm
but incompetent; this prejudice entails a strong
sense of pity, without admiration or envy.
Groups stereotyped as cold but competent con-
jure up the opposite emotional profile: envy and
admiration, but little pity. Regarding these lat-
ter findings, Fiske et al. wrote that “admiration
for high-competence out-groups . . . coexisted
with envy, suggesting a volatile mix of emotions
that could create hostility when groups feel
threatened” (2002: 897).
Diffuse, unconscious antifemale and antimi-
nority feelings are thought to underlie modern
discrimination (McConahay, 1986; McConahay &
Hough, 1976; Sears, 1988, 1998; Swim et al., 1995;
Tougas et al., 1995). Dovidio, Gaertner, and
Bachman (2001) have emphasized the subtlety of
these negative emotions in aversive racists, who
experience mild fear, disgust, uneasiness, and
indifference when they encounter ethnic minor-
ities. This contrasts with “the open flame of ra-
cial hatred” that fuels traditional racism (2001:
419). In addition, Jackson and colleagues (2001:
49) have shown that an important component of
contemporary sexism is greater feelings of es-
teem (i.e., respect) for men than women—a dif-
ferential, affective “gut reaction” to social
groups. Because this visceral response toward
members of particular social groups is outside
conscious awareness and control, modern sex-
ists and racists can maintain a nonprejudiced
self-concept.
Situational Explanations
A complete understanding of workplace dis-
crimination requires attention to the surround-
ing context—at the level of both the immediate
organization and the larger society. These con-
texts provide the proximal and distal backdrops
of discriminatory cognitions, emotions, and be-
haviors in the workplace. The shaded, concen-
tric ovals surrounding the model in Figure 1
convey the pervasive influence of context on all
aspects of this process. In social psychological
research on modern discrimination, scholars
have, to some extent, discussed the societal con-
text, so this review of situational factors begins
there. Next will come the workplace context,
which is discussed more in the organizational
sciences.
Societal context. All organizations operate
within a larger society/culture, which certainly
affects the unfolding of discrimination. Histori-
cally speaking, racism and sexism have a long
tradition in American history, and people tend to
internalize the values and beliefs of their cul-
ture. Not long ago in this country, women were
denied the right to vote, own property, and ma-
triculate in many institutions of higher educa-
tion; employers could openly fire or refuse to
hire women solely on the basis of their gender.
Likewise, “the periods of slavery and Jim Crow
forced African Americans, unlike any other eth-
nic group, into a legalized second-class citizen-
ship for over 300 years” (Sears, 1998: 79). Other
U.S. ethnic minority groups have also encoun-
tered oppression in policies of mass internment,
forced expulsion from their homelands, discrim-
inatory immigration practices, and antimiscege-
nation laws (Operario & Fiske, 1998).
Although many of these overt discriminatory
practices have since been abandoned, the struc-
ture of society remains such that men and
whites tend to occupy different social roles than
women (Eagly & Steffen, 1984; Eagly & Wood,
1999) and ethnic minorities (Dovidio, Gaertner, &
Bachman, 2001; Sears, 1998), which helps to sus-
tain stereotypes. Moreover, people growing up
in the United States still encounter stereotypic
2008 61Cortina
imagery in cartoons, books, films, and other cul-
tural media (Brief & Barsky, 2000; Fiske, 2002;
Operario & Fiske, 1998). This social heritage
maintains prejudice against women and people
of color. It also follows individuals into their
places of work: “Employees come to the organi-
zation with heavy cultural and social baggage
obtained from interactions in other social con-
texts” (Scott, 1992: 20).
The structure of society also perpetuates un-
equal distributions of power, and asymmetrical
power combined with prejudice sets the stage
for oppression. Powerful people often seek to
preserve the status quo in order to bolster their
own status, maintain access to valued re-
sources, and increase personal and collective
self-esteem (Dovidio, Gaertner, & Bachman,
2001; Fiske, 1993, 2000, 2001, 2002; Jones, 2002;
Operario & Fiske, 1998; Tajfel & Turner, 1986).
Power also gives individuals at the top of the
social structure the tools to translate their bi-
ases into discriminatory conduct. With respect
to race, Operario and Fiske argue that “preju-
dice alone does not determine racism; everyone
has prejudices, because all people prefer their
group over others. History and society confer
power to certain groups, granting them exces-
sive ability to exercise their prejudice” (1998: 49).
In contrast, powerlessness necessitates depen-
dence on and acquiescence to the demands of
the powerful (Fiske, 1993; Jones, 2002; Operario &
Fiske, 1998). These social structural forces pro-
vide prime conditions for discrimination to
thrive, in ways both blatant and subtle.
Organizational context. Turning now to the
more immediate context, the organizational en-
vironment should play an important role in ei-
ther enabling or inhibiting discrimination. It is
also a level of context that seems particularly
malleable—more so than the social structural
context, given the difficulties inherent in effect-
ing change at the broad societal level. However,
social psychologists have largely ignored the
workplace in studies of modern discrimination.
The focus of this review therefore now switches
to field research in organizational psychology.
This literature suggests that several features of
the work environment are especially relevant to
discrimination: policy, leadership, and group
norms.
Many U.S. organizations presently have poli-
cies that reflect Title VII of the Civil Rights Act of
1964—protecting certain classes of employees
(including women and personnel of color) from
discrimination or harassment based on social
category membership. Numerous scholars, how-
ever, emphasize that a good policy is necessary
but not sufficient to inhibit discriminatory and
antisocial work behavior; consistent enforce-
ment of that policy is paramount (e.g., O’Leary-
Kelly et al., 2000; Riger, 1991; Williams, Fitzger-
ald, & Drasgow, 1999). To implement policies
effectively, strong leaders must be present.
Leaders set the tone for the entire organiza-
tion, and employees look to them for cues about
what constitutes acceptable conduct. Organiza-
tions tend to have fewer problems with (blatant)
discrimination when their authority figures es-
tablish clear expectations for respectful behav-
ior, model nondiscriminatory values and con-
duct, take discrimination complaints seriously,
and sanction those who discriminate. Argu-
ments and evidence supporting this claim have
emerged particularly in studies of sexual ha-
rassment (e.g., Fitzgerald, Drasgow, Hulin, Gel-
fand, & Magley, 1997; Hulin, Fitzgerald, & Dras-
gow, 1996; O’Leary-Kelly et al., 2000; Pryor,
Giedd, & Williams, 1995; Williams et al., 1999). In
a similar vein, organizational authorities can
send messages to employees that either pro-
mote or inhibit racial discrimination (Brief et al.,
1995, 1997, 2000).
Another feature of the organizational context
that can influence discrimination is its local so-
cial norms (Dipboye & Halverson, 2004). The
need for belonging and acceptance by ingroup
members is a powerful motivator for human be-
havior (e.g., Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Fiske,
2000), including organizational behavior (Wil-
liams, 1998; Williams & Sommer, 1997). In fact,
personnel often work in groups or teams, facing
pressure to conform to group norms. Even with-
out a formal “team” structure, many organiza-
tions foster a psychological sense of community,
and informal social norms arise (Heller, 1989;
Pretty & MacCarthy, 1991; Sarason, 1974). Group
norms “not only define reality for group mem-
bers but also communicate how members can
obtain the approval and avoid the criticisms of
fellow group members” (Dipboye & Halverson,
2004: 145). As a result of these group processes,
people adapt their cognitions, emotions, and be-
haviors to fit better into the social world of work.
Thus, when coworkers convey expectations for
or model biased behavior, an employee is more
likely to follow suit and engage in discrimina-
62 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review
tion (Brief et al., 2000; Robinson & O’Leary-Kelly,
1998).
Person-Situation Interactional Explanations
With a few exceptions, the organizational con-
text literature reviewed above focuses primarily
on overt discrimination. Drawing on Dipboye
and Halverson’s (2004) reasoning, we find that
the picture becomes more complicated when we
consider how the organizational environment
might influence covert discrimination. The orga-
nization’s implicit or explicit support for dis-
crimination (as indicated by lax enforcement or
absence of a nondiscrimination policy, permis-
sive leader behavior, and sexist or racist norms)
could create an immediate social context that
promotes discriminatory conduct. However, em-
ployees bring their own personal convictions
and affective tendencies to that context. The re-
sult is a person-by-situation interaction that de-
termines whether or not discriminatory behavior
occurs and, if it does, whether it is overt versus
covert and implicit versus explicit. Figure 2 sum-
marizes how person and situation factors jointly
influence discriminatory behavior.
As Figure 2 demonstrates, when we cross the
organizational context and individual tenden-
cies, (at least) four different behavioral possibil-
ities emerge. The lower left quadrant represents
the ideal situation: a nonbiased individual
works for an organization that, likewise, does
not permit biased conduct; that employee will
likely not discriminate against women and mi-
norities. Even if the employee unconsciously
endorses negative beliefs or feelings toward
female or minority coworkers, a strong antidis-
criminatory context should prevent that per-
son from acting on his or her implicit biases
(Brief et al., 2000).
The opposite situation appears in the upper
right quadrant: when a biased employee works
for a bias-tolerant company, the likelihood of
overt discrimination (e.g., sexual and racial ha-
rassment) is high. This employee may be fully
aware of and open about the biased nature of
his or her conduct, and the organization does not
motivate the individual to restrain his or her
prejudices.
Note that in both of these situations the indi-
vidual’s approach (pro or con) to bias is in line
with that of the organizational environment. A
different picture emerges in the case of individ-
uals whose feelings and beliefs about discrimi-
nation are in conflict with those advanced by
the organization (Dipboye & Halverson, 2004).
This disconnect fosters ideal conditions for co-
vert discrimination.
The lower right quadrant of Figure 2 portrays
a situation in which a biased employee works
for an organization that deters discriminatory
conduct. Motivation to avoid sanctions, to re-
main in the organization, and to fit in with the
workgroup could inhibit the employee from ex-
FIGURE 2
Person-by-Organization Influences on Discriminatory Behavior
2008 63Cortina
pressing visible bias (i.e., no blatant sexual or
racial harassment). However, these organization-
al influences might only drive discrimination
“underground.” In other words, in lieu of open,
overt acts of hostility, the employee may express
personal biases against female coworkers and
colleagues of color covertly (Dipboye & Halver-
son, 2004). Concealing discrimination in this
way could be a conscious choice on the part of
the employee.
The upper left quadrant applies to two differ-
ent types of employees, although the behavioral
outcome is the same. In one situation an em-
ployee with no implicit or explicit biases
against women and minorities works for an or-
ganization in which there is pressure (e.g., from
peers) to engage in sexist or racist behavior.
Assuming that the employee is motivated to re-
main with the organization (e.g., for financial or
professional reasons), the conflict between the
individual and the immediate context may yield
discrimination that is quite subtle. That is, the
person is ideologically opposed to sexism and
racism and therefore unlikely to engage know-
ingly in overtly discriminatory behavior. This
person, however, spends forty (or more) hours
per week working in an antifemale and/or anti-
minority climate. His or her fundamental need
for belonging, acceptance, and security could
foster covert discrimination against women
and/or colleagues of color, as a means of “fitting
in” in the discriminatory environment. The bi-
ased conduct would need to be subtle and ra-
tionalizable, and perhaps even unconscious, for
the employee to maintain an unbiased self-
image.
A second possibility for the upper left quad-
rant is that an employee self-identifies as non-
prejudiced but implicitly harbors negative
thoughts and feelings toward women or people
of color. This person also works in a context that
is in some way antifemale or antiminority. This
environment permits and perhaps even encour-
ages the employee to act on implicit biases, but
explicit discrimination (e.g., sexual or racial ha-
rassment) would be aversive to his or her egal-
itarian identity. Instead of overtly discriminat-
ing against undervalued social groups, the
employee could disproportionately target them
with negative behavior that is too subtle and
facially neutral to appear biased. This antiso-
cial conduct would be a concealed, uninten-
tional, unconscious form of discrimination (Dip-
boye & Halverson, 2004).
WORKPLACE INCIVILITY AS MODERN
DISCRIMINATION
How might social psychological concepts of
modern/covert/aversive discrimination inform
our understanding of incivility in the work-
place? These theories suggest different paths to
selective incivility. In one case reasonable, for-
ward-thinking, tolerant employees unknowingly
target women and minorities with dispropor-
tionate incivility, despite being explicitly op-
posed to sexism and racism. That is, the employ-
ees’ implicitly stereotypic attitudes, preference
for ingroup members, motivation to maintain so-
cial power, and so forth could give rise to subtle
biases against the outgroup. Lax antidiscrimi-
nation policies, permissive leadership behavior,
and antisocial models in the workplace could
set the stage for employees to act on those bi-
ases. Cultural traditions of sexism, racism, and
asymmetrical power compound the situation.
These instigators might have plausible, nonra-
cial, and nongendered explanations for the un-
civil conduct (e.g., “I didn’t see you,” “I’m having
a bad day,” “I thought you were done speak-
ing”)—explanations that they themselves be-
lieve.
In other cases selective incivility might not be
so innocent, because “some people may be par-
ticularly motivated to think in stereotypic ways
and may use controlled processing to promote
stereotype use” (Devine & Monteith, 1999: 356,
note 1). For example, an employee may con-
sciously experience blatant antipathy toward
women and minorities and make no attempt to
prevent it from influencing his or her behavior
toward coworkers, but may hide prejudice be-
hind the guise of “general” incivility. This at-
tempt at concealment may be particularly likely
if a biased employee works in an organization
with strong nondiscrimination policies and
norms, enforced by strong leaders. Overt disre-
spect (e.g., harassment) of women and minori-
ties would not be tolerated in such a context, so
the employee must find more discreet and ra-
tionalizable methods of expressing bias; selec-
tive incivility is one means toward this end. That
is, bias may emerge in the form of low-level
deviance that, absent any overtly sexist or racist
content, can be attributed to something less ob-
64 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review
jectionable than prejudice (e.g., instigator over-
sight, personality, work overload). In this exam-
ple the employee can explain the conduct in a
way that has nothing to do with gender or race,
but this would be a deceitful “cover-up” for bias.
A third scenario could be that an employee
has strong egalitarian values, both explicit and
implicit, but works alongside coworkers who ex-
press sexism and racism on a regular basis. The
employee is caught in a quandary: not going
along with the biased conduct could lead to
ostracism from the peer group, which constitutes
a significant part of the employee’s social world,
but participating in blatantly biased behavior
would be antithetical to the employee’s values
and, thus, highly aversive. One means of solv-
ing this dilemma would be to target women and
people of color with subtle disrespect—that is,
incivility. The discriminatory nature of this con-
duct would fit in with group norms, and the
low-level (possibly unconscious) nature of the
deviance might be tolerable for the instigator,
who prides him or herself on being nonsexist
and nonracist.
Given the theories and scenarios reviewed
above, I propose that incivility is not always an
“equal opportunity” form of antisocial work be-
havior. Specifically, in many organizations
women and ethnic minority employees likely
experience more uncivil treatment than men
and whites. This should be especially true for
organizations that lack strong nondiscrimina-
tion policies, have leaders who turn a blind eye
to (or even model) antifemale and antiminority
conduct, and employ individuals who openly ex-
press bias.
In many cases disproportionate incivility to-
ward women and people of color would likely
comprise the most ambiguous forms of disre-
spectful conduct (e.g., interrupting an employee,
failing to include an employee in professional
camaraderie, ignoring an employee). These sub-
tle behaviors could be attributed to many factors
other than race or gender (e.g., instigator over-
sight, target hypersensitivity), making it partic-
ularly difficult to label them as discriminatory.
They are thus means by which personnel can
mistreat women and people of color while main-
taining a nonprejudiced image to themselves
and others. The apparent neutrality of this be-
havior could avert disciplinary actions related
to Title VII violation.
It could be, however, that more transparent
forms of incivility (e.g., rumor spreading, accu-
sations of incompetence, anger outbursts) would
not show intergroup differences. These behav-
iors, when targeted at female and minority em-
ployees disproportionately, might be perceived
as sexist or racist, which would be aversive to
the instigator’s egalitarian self-concept. This
might also trigger organizational or legal sanc-
tions. Therefore, it makes sense that many insti-
gators (those who want to avoid penalties or
labels as sexist/racist) would refrain from bla-
tant forms of discriminatory disrespect.
This profile of findings would be highly con-
sistent with modern understandings of discrim-
ination. For example, aversive racism theory
(e.g., Dovidio & Gaertner, 1998; Dovidio, Gaert-
ner, & Bachman, 2001; Dovidio, Gaertner, Nie-
mann, & Snider, 2001) suggests that discrimina-
tion will not emerge in situations where the
discriminatory nature of the conduct is appar-
ent, either to the instigator or others. This is
because “aversive racists” consciously endorse
values of egalitarianism and justice and con-
demn prejudice both internally and externally.
However, these same individuals implicitly har-
bor negative emotions and cognitions toward
minorities, driving them to discriminate in sub-
tle or rationalizable ways. That is, they discrim-
inate (1) when the biased nature of the behavior
is not obvious or (2) when a negative response
can be attributed to something other than race.
Both of these descriptions fit many manifesta-
tions of workplace incivility.
From the target’s perspective, who would be
most at risk for selective incivility? According to
theories of ambivalent stereotyping (e.g., Fiske
et al., 2002; Glick & Fiske, 1999, 2001) and inter-
group competition (e.g., Jackman, 1994), the most
vulnerable might be professionally and eco-
nomically successful women and minorities—
those who are perceived as highly competent
and advancing in ways that threaten the domi-
nant majority. This is based on research show-
ing that successful outgroups, such as black pro-
fessionals, Asians, career women, business
women, and feminists, are often stereotyped as
(too) competent, ambitious, and hardworking
and, at the same time, interpersonally unpleas-
ant and cold. This can trigger admiration to
some extent, but also envy and hostile competi-
tion from the dominant majority. Again, though,
to avoid appearances of racism and sexism, the
2008 65Cortina
hostility may often take a disguised form, such
as selective incivility.
EVIDENCE FROM THE TARGET’S
PERSPECTIVE
The theories reviewed above explain forces
that may promote incivility as a covert form of
modern discrimination against undervalued so-
cial groups. But is there any empirical evidence
to this effect? With workplace incivility being a
new area of inquiry, the empirical record on this
topic remains limited. Several studies do, how-
ever, suggest that gender bias may underlie
some manifestations of incivility. Fewer data
are available on race and incivility, but findings
from one study do suggest potential racial bias.
Starting first with incivility as subtle sexism,
qualitative data suggest that incivility and gen-
der bias are often one and the same. Specifi-
cally, my colleagues and I (Cortina et al., 2002)
examined the interpersonal experiences of 4,608
attorneys practicing in the federal courts. Re-
spondents who had indicated any recent en-
counter with workplace incivility were asked to
provide brief descriptions of the uncivil conduct
that had had the greatest impact on them. Many
women detailed experiences of incivility that
they attributed to gender, despite the mistreat-
ment not being explicitly gendered on its face.
Some of the behaviors described represented
more overt, active, direct forms of disrespect:
Male judges and attorneys tend to cut short, ig-
nore, or exclude female attorneys.
There is still a big gap in how women attorneys
are treated by male attorneys: extremely aggres-
sive behavior in depositions; failure to listen; re-
peated interruptions in all contexts.
A court clerk apparently did not believe that I
was an attorney even though I had been sitting at
counsel table for two weeks of the trial. He
treated me rudely and kept me from getting ex-
hibits in order in a document-intensive case. I
believe this occurred because I am a young-
looking female.
More often, though, the disproportionately un-
civil treatment of women was more subtle, indi-
rect, and perhaps unintentional and uncon-
scious:
I was plaintiff’s counsel on a motion, but the
magistrate spoke exclusively to the male defense
counsel and deferred to him throughout the in-
chamber hearing. Magistrate treated me like I
wasn’t in the room.
The person in charge of the settlement program
. . . refused to speak to me in that conference, and
would only address my male co-counsel, even
though I had identified myself as lead counsel.
The bankruptcy bar...israther small and exclu-
sive. To be young and female is to be discounted
and ignored and makes it very difficult to estab-
lish yourself. I hated my first 5 years of practice
because of it. Good thing I’m tough.
These previously unpublished quotes illustrate
how incivility can represent gender discrimina-
tion (and sometimes age discrimination) of a
less blatant type. The last examples are consis-
tent with the form of bias that Fiske (2002: 125)
calls “cool neglect,” or withholding “basic liking
and respect” rather than being openly hostile.
The Cortina et al. (2002) study is particularly
well-suited to testing the theory articulated
above. The participants were female attorneys,
who would elicit the “career woman” (and some-
times “feminist”) stereotype, which includes
high competence but low warmth (e.g., Fiske et
al., 2002; Glick & Fiske, 1999, 2001). These women
are making inroads into a prestigious profes-
sion that was once exclusively the province of
men. Men, as members of the dominant majori-
ty,
2
may feel a sense of threat, competition, and
hostility toward these outgroup members who
are “encroaching” on their terrain. In some
cases men could justify uncivil behavior as part
of their job, which mandates “zealous advocacy”
for clients. Note, however, that opposing counsel
were not the only instigators of incivility in
these narratives, since the rude behavior also
came from judges, court clerks, and other court
personnel. Behavior from these various sources
cannot be attributed to trial strategy or the ad-
versarial model of justice.
Corroborating these qualitative findings,
quantitative research also suggests gender dif-
ferences in the experience of incivility. Specifi-
cally, the Cortina et al. (2002) attorney study also
included a survey, in which more women (65
percent) than men (47 percent) described recent
experiences of “general incivility” in the context
2
In the year before my colleagues and I (Cortina et al.,
2002) collected our data, only 23 percent of all lawyers na-
tionwide were women (Eighth Circuit Gender Fairness Task
Force, 1997). Thus, in this profession men clearly remain the
dominant majority.
66 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review
of their work. This gender difference echoes the
results of Bjo¨rkqvist, O
¨
sterman, and Hjelt-Ba¨ck
(1994), who reported that 55 percent of female
compared to 30 percent of male university em-
ployees had faced “work harassment” during
the previous six months. Likewise, in a survey
(Cortina et al., 2001) of 1,180 court employees,
women described greater frequencies of incivil-
ity than did their male colleagues.
The gender differences just reported were
based on composite measures of workplace in-
civility, making it difficult to pinpoint which
types of incivility are more prone to being dis-
criminatory. Extending these findings, my col-
leagues and I (Cortina et al., 2004) have provided
in-depth evidence of both gender- and race-
based disparities in the experience of specific
uncivil behaviors. Our results were based on
survey data from two organizations: (1) a city
government in which women have a strong
presence and (2) a law enforcement agency that
employs sizable numbers of ethnic minorities.
With some exceptions, women and ethnic minor-
ities reported more frequent encounters with
specific uncivil behaviors at work, compared re-
spectively to men and whites. Gender and racial
differences were particularly large for the most
ambiguous behaviors (e.g., “ignored you or
failed to speak to you [e.g., ‘the silent treat-
ment‘],” “doubted your judgment on a matter
over which you had responsibility,” “withheld
information that you needed to do your job cor-
rectly,” “failed to give you an award or recogni-
tion you deserved”). Many of these ambiguous
and “withholding” behaviors could be attrib-
uted to instigator oversight or target hypersen-
sitivity, making it difficult to label them as prej-
udiced. These behaviors are thus means by
which employees may disproportionately tar-
get women and minorities with disrespect
while maintaining a nonprejudiced self-
image.
In contrast, we (Cortina et al., 2004) did not
find gender or racial differences in the experi-
ence of more blatant or overtly disrespectful be-
havior (e.g., “made jokes at your expense,” “re-
fused to work with you,” “targeted you with
anger outbursts or ‘tempter tantrums’”). These
behaviors, when targeted disproportionately at
women and minorities, could be attributed to
prejudice more readily, which could threaten in-
stigators’ self-concept and trigger sanctions
from the employer or court system. Modern rac-
ists and sexists would likely want to avoid such
outcomes.
NEW DIRECTIONS FOR RESEARCH
These empirical studies support the theory of
selective incivility advanced in this article. The
findings are preliminary, however, elucidating
only half of the incivility equation (the target’s
perspective) and rarely addressing issues of
race. Thus, this theory and this evidence raise
intriguing possibilities, to be addressed in fu-
ture research on workplace intersections of in-
civility, sexism, and racism.
Generally speaking, research related to work-
place incivility could benefit from stronger alli-
ances with social psychology, and vice versa.
Given the richness of social and organizational
scholarship on discrimination, the indepen-
dence of these two bodies of literature is strik-
ing. Both arenas offer novel concepts that could
be mutually informative. They also tend to favor
different paradigms: lab experiments dominate
social psychological research, whereas surveys
are more typical in the organizational sciences.
Research informed by multiple conceptual tra-
ditions, diverse methodologies, and questions of
both basic and applied significance could take
studies of workplace incivility (and other dis-
criminatory behaviors) down interesting new
paths.
A second recommendation is that organiza-
tional researchers of antisocial work behaviors
follow the model set by social, feminist, and
cultural psychology and routinely engage is-
sues of gender and race. This includes studies of
not only incivility but also other forms of “gen-
eral” interpersonal mistreatment: bullying
(Hoel, Rayner, & Cooper, 1999), aggression
(Baron, 2004; Neuman, 2004), interpersonal devi-
ance (Robinson & Bennett, 1995), petty tyranny
(Ashforth, 1994), social undermining (Duffy, Gan-
ster, & Pagon, 2002), organizational retaliation
behavior (Skarlicki & Folger, 1997), and counter-
productive work behavior (Spector & Fox, 2002).
To date, gender and race (and other social iden-
tities) have been largely absent from this liter-
ature. A few studies, reviewed above, have ad-
dressed gender and incivility, but there has
been little discussion of incivility and race. In-
deed, the neglect of race in the burgeoning lit-
erature on antisocial work behavior has per-
sisted for much too long, perhaps owing to the
2008 67Cortina
fact that “it has become somewhat taboo to rec-
ognize the role of racism in contemporary Amer-
ican work organizations” (Brief & Hayes, 1997:
100). Importantly, this research should not re-
quire targets to have insight into the discrimi-
natory nature of the conduct they face, owing to
the inherent ambiguity of uncivil situations
(Andersson & Pearson, 1999) and to employees’
reluctance to identify as victims of prejudice
(Magley, Hulin, Fitzgerald, & DeNardo, 1999).
Beyond targets, the perspective of instigators
also deserves further scholarly attention. In fact,
tests of the theory summarized in Figure 1 nec-
essarily imply an instigator focus. This work
will require innovative methods to avoid prob-
lems of socially desirable responding. In partic-
ular, research on aversive racism, neosexism,
and so on shows that individuals responsible for
these “modern” forms of discrimination strongly
identify as nonprejudiced. In other words, they
see their own behavior as reasonable, fair, and
unbiased, so they would not openly admit to
disproportionately targeting female and minor-
ity employees with uncivil conduct. It is there-
fore critical that we develop unobtrusive tech-
niques for assessing selective incivility.
On another front, social psychologists inter-
ested in modern discrimination should consider
the negative interpersonal behaviors discussed
in the organizational literature (e.g., incivility,
aggression, bullying, etc.) as potential behav-
ioral vehicles for subtle sexism and racism. All
of these actions appear, at face value, to have
nothing to do with social categories, stereo-
types, or prejudices. However, as this article il-
lustrates, seemingly neutral insensitivity or ag-
gression toward persons could, in many cases,
serve as a convenient mask for unfair discrimi-
nation against socially undervalued persons.
Studies of this possibility would help address a
deficit that is often lamented in social psychol-
ogy: little research in the bias literature directly
assesses discriminatory behavior. This is a ma-
jor problem, because “thoughts and feelings do
not exclude, oppress, and kill people; behavior
does” (Fiske, 2000: 312). Selective incivility is a
specific behavior that could be the focus of
novel social psychological research.
Questions may arise as to what methods may
be most appropriate to launch this line of in-
quiry. Scholars interested in the antecedents of
incivility could use the social psychological ex-
periment as one research tool, pinpointing the
personal and social conditions under which
individuals are most likely to disrespect oth-
ers—particularly women and people of color.
Techniques developed by organizational psy-
chologists could also benefit social research
on modern discrimination. Particularly promis-
ing for this purpose are new survey methods for
assessing implicit motivation and cognition,
such as the “conditional reasoning” paradigm
(e.g., James, 1998; James & Mazerolle, 2003). In
addition, organizational and social psycholo-
gists could collaboratively translate stereotype-
control strategies developed in the laboratory
into respectful-workplace initiatives, followed
by evaluation of those initiatives in actual orga-
nizations. Finally, despite some discussion of
societal influences on modern discrimination,
empirical attention to this topic has been
sparse. Cross-cultural methods of studying or-
ganizational behavior (e.g., Gelfand, Raver, &
Ehrhart, 2002; Schaffer & Riordan, 2003) could
allow us to understand how social structural
forces impinge on bias in multinational organi-
zations. These are just a few examples of how
social and organizational psychologists—and
management scholars from other disciplines—
could pool their ideas and methodologies to ad-
vance research on incivility and discrimination
in the modern workplace.
IMPLICATIONS FOR POLICY AND PRACTICE
Nonspecific workplace mistreatment—in the
absence of overtly sexual, sexist, or racist con-
duct—is not typically regarded as a Title VII
violation. However, in this article I propose that
employees may at times be differentially tar-
geted with incivility on the basis of their sex or
race, potentially creating disparate work envi-
ronments across social groups. At the very least,
this could interfere with the recruitment and re-
tention of a diverse workforce; at worst, perva-
sive patterns of incivility toward protected
classes of employees could expose the organi-
zation to legal liability. For these reasons it is
critical that organizations and policy makers de-
velop effective strategies for managing this dis-
guised form of workplace discrimination.
Referring again to Figure 1, interventions are
possible for every factor that fuels selective in-
civility, at the level of both person and context.
Indeed, it is important that both levels be tar-
geted, as Figure 2 illustrates. If explicit, internal
68 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review
bias persists in an individual working in a non-
discriminatory context, the person may simply
transmute the bias into covert, rationalizable
forms of discrimination. Likewise, an employee
who fundamentally opposes gender and race
bias, but who works in discriminatory organiza-
tions, may give in to social pressures and dis-
criminate covertly. Given that selective incivil-
ity has antecedents at multiple levels, it seems
only logical that attempts to manage it also take
a multilevel approach. Below is a discussion of
person-level interventions, followed by societal
and organizational actions.
Person-Level Interventions
To reduce the incidence of selective incivil-
ity, some might assume that interventions
should primarily target the organizational
context, with less hope for intrapersonal re-
form. However, the social psychology litera-
ture is replete with ideas on how to do both:
modify the environment so as to influence in-
dividual cognition and affect, which could ul-
timately inhibit discriminatory behavior.
These techniques could potentially be applied
to the management of workplace incivility. A
complete review of this literature is beyond
the scope of this article, but I highlight several
segments as an illustration.
The emphasis in stereotype scholarship on
unconscious cognitive processes, unintentional-
ity, and automaticity may give the false impres-
sion that organizational attempts at influencing
social thought and emotion would be pointless.
On the contrary, research shows that it is possi-
ble to intervene at the outset of these internal
processes, manipulating perceivers’ cognitive
representations of their social worlds. Social
categorization may be automatic, but social cat-
egory boundaries—and the dimensions around
which they organize—are far from fixed. Thus, it
is possible to broaden peoples’ conceptions of
who belongs to their ingroup. This can foster
more positive, respectful attitudes and behavior
toward individuals formerly perceived as out-
group members.
To give a concrete example of this, Gaertner
and Dovidio (2000) developed the common in-
group identity model. This intervention shifts
conceptions of social group membership from
many separate groups to a more inclusive, su-
perordinate group. Experimental and field stud-
ies support the model, with the superordinate
identity created using a range of strategies (e.g.,
spatially integrating people, creating a common
group name, implementing joint evaluation and
reward systems) in a range of settings. The com-
mon group identity cultivates greater interde-
pendence, cooperation, and respect among indi-
viduals who were formerly members of separate
groups (e.g., Dovidio, Gaertner, & Bachman,
2001; Dovidio, Gaertner, Niemann, & Snider,
2001; Dovidio, Kawakami, & Gaertner, 2000;
Gaertner & Dovidio, 2000).
Even when gender- or race-based social cate-
gorization occurs, it does not always yield ste-
reotypic thought, emotion, and behavior. De-
spite being natural and automatic, stereotyping
is not inevitable—it can occur automatically but
does not always occur. For instance, people ap-
parently do not engage in negative stereotypic
thinking when they are motivated to endorse a
positive judgment of the target. To demonstrate
this, research shows that when an employee
receives a positive evaluation by a member of a
stereotyped group, the employee is motivated by
self-interest to respect that person’s opinion; ste-
reotype activation does not occur (e.g., Devine &
Monteith, 1999; Erber & Fiske, 1984; Neuberg &
Fiske, 1987).
Moreover, when it does occur, stereotype acti-
vation does not unavoidably lead to stereotype
application. Although a social perceiver may
initially categorize a person and trigger a ste-
reotype, the perceiver can be motivated to re-
place this automatic categorical processing
with more controlled, intentional, individuated
processing, and this can prevent a discrimina-
tory behavioral response. For example, situa-
tions that involve high stakes, emphasize the
need for accuracy, hold people accountable for
their judgments, relax undue time pressures, or
require cooperation to work toward shared
goals give rise to individuated processing in
lieu of stereotyping (e.g., Devine & Monteith,
1999; Dipboye & Halverson, 2004; Fiske, 2000,
2001, 2002; Greenwald & Banaji, 1995; Valian,
1998). In addition, learning and self-regulation
processes can help individuals self-monitor and
recognize situations in which they are suscepti-
ble to stereotyped responding; they can then
interrupt stereotypic thinking, even if it is acti-
vated, and generate nondiscriminatory behavior
(Brief & Barsky, 2000; Monteith, 1993). To quote
2008 69Cortina
Gordon Allport, social perceivers can sometimes
“put the brakes on prejudice” (1954: 332).
Situational Interventions
Societal context. Legal frameworks make up
an important piece of the fabric that holds civil
society together. However, one might wonder
whether existing law is relevant to uncivil work
behavior: because a defining feature of incivil-
ity is that intentionality is ambiguous, does the
absence of clear discriminatory intent remove
legal liability? The issue of intent factors into
some, but not all, legal definitions of discrimi-
nation. For example, in McDonnel Douglas Corp.
v. Green (1973), the U.S. Supreme Court ruled
that proof of intent to discriminate must be
present before a behavior can be considered
unlawful intentional discrimination or disparate
treatment. However, in Griggs v. Duke Power Co.
(1971), the Supreme Court also ruled that unin-
tentional discrimination or adverse impact is
also unlawful. In other words, if an organization-
al practice has differential, adverse effects on
protected classes of employees, then that prac-
tice may be illegal, even if the effects are unin-
tentional. Likewise, the U.S. Equal Employment
Opportunity Commission (1993) has stated that
practices that have the effect of interfering with
work or creating a hostile work environment for
women and racial minorities constitute unlaw-
ful sexual and racial harassment, respectively,
regardless of the employer’s intent. Thus, when
workplace incivility is disproportionately tar-
geted at women and minorities and interferes
with their work and/or contributes to a hostile
environment, this situation may violate law.
Despite potential legal implications, it is un-
clear whether legal grievance mechanisms
present effective solutions to the problem of se-
lective incivility. These mechanisms require the
target to formally complain about the behavior,
which may not be a realistic expectation. Em-
ployees generally have high thresholds for
whistle-blowing (Miceli & Near, 1992), and iso-
lated acts of subtle discrimination may not seem
“severe” enough to warrant complaint (Dipboye
& Halverson, 2004; Rowe, 1990). As Neuman
notes, “How (and to whom) do you report having
had your feelings hurt, and what reporting sys-
tem captures the time you spend ruminating
about the perceived mistreatment? Furthermore,
would you believe that such an incident is seri-
ous enough to merit a formal report?” (2004: 74).
Moreover, individual instances of misconduct
may not appear discriminatory unless consid-
ered in the aggregate (Crosby, 1984; Crosby,
Iyer, Clayton, & Downing, 2003; O’Leary-Kelly et
al., 2000), particularly compared to conduct tar-
geted at men and whites.
Further reducing complaint likelihood, targets
may experience attributional uncertainty about
incivility—a low-level behavior that lacks clear
intent (Andersson & Pearson, 1999) and has no
overtly gendered or racial content (Lim & Cor-
tina, 2005). They may have difficulty deciding
whether they are experiencing general incivility
or, rather, discrimination based on their social
category membership (Cortina et al., 2002;
Schneider et al., 2000). Even if they do suspect
differential negative treatment, targets may still
hesitate to label their instigators’ behavior as
discriminatory. This would imply that the insti-
gators are prejudiced, which is generally seen
as immoral or evil in the current political cli-
mate of this country. Thus, accusations of prej-
udice could embroil targets in emotionally
charged, painful situations, which they would
likely prefer to avoid (Monin & Miller, 2001;
Swim et al., 2003). Given the subtlety and attri-
butional uncertainty inherent in these situa-
tions, it is even less likely that incivility targets
would file discrimination complaints with man-
agement.
In the rare cases when employees do come
forward with reports of selective incivility, man-
agers (or judges and juries) might not find such
“minor,” seemingly neutral misconduct worthy
of reprimand. This may be especially true when
instigators can provide a plausible, nonracial,
nongendered account for their behavior. To
make matters worse, some research suggests
that people who merely observe one social
group (e.g., women) treated less favorably than
another (men) are less likely to perceive the
treatment as discriminatory, compared with per-
ceptions of the individuals who are the direct
targets of the disparate treatment (Swim et al.,
2003). Managers are even further removed from
potentially discriminatory situations— only
hearing reports about them—so they may be
even less likely to believe that discrimination
has taken place. Moreover, it is difficult to dis-
cern discrimination in individual cases; pat-
terns of discrimination often emerge only with
the aggregation of multiple incidents across
70 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review
persons, places, or time (Crosby, 1984; Crosby et
al., 2003; O’Leary-Kelly et al., 2000). For all of
these reasons, traditional, reactive, and legalis-
tic approaches to combating blatant discrimina-
tion may not be effective for managing subtle
biases in the form of selective incivility.
Organizational context. To create respectful,
incivility-free work environments, it would be-
hoove organizations to look beyond traditional
methods of achieving and managing diversity;
in particular, the reactive complaint mechanism
may have limited utility. Proactive, preventa-
tive, and educational approaches seem more
promising instead. For example, in the interest
of incivility prevention, senior management can
model appropriate, respectful workplace behav-
ior and clearly state expectations of civility in
mission statements or policy manuals. Refer-
ence checks for prospective employees can in-
clude questions about interpersonal behavior.
All new employees should receive education
about civility expectations, and employees at all
levels could undergo interpersonal skills train-
ing. When incivilities do arise, instigators
should be sanctioned swiftly, justly, and consis-
tently (Pearson et al., 2000; Pearson & Porath,
2004).
Given the links to gender and race theorized
in this article, I would also recommend that ef-
forts to prevent incivility dovetail with those ad-
dressing overt discrimination (e.g., sexual and
racial harassment). For example, organizational
procedures, policies, and practices to set norms
of civility could explicitly discuss equitable re-
spect toward women and men and members of
different ethnic groups (as well as employees
who are young and old, gay and straight, etc.).
Leaders should emphasize that unacceptable
discrimination includes not just overt acts of
misoygyny and bigotry but also subtle devalu-
ation and exclusion of social minorities. The
intended effect should be a broadening of em-
ployees’ construals of what it means to be non-
prejudiced (Brief & Barsky, 2000). Organizational
interventions should also incorporate solutions
from the social psychology literature on stereo-
typing and prejudice, detailed above. This sort
of combined strategy would provide a more ef-
ficient and effective means of combating antiso-
cial work behavior, which has many behavioral
faces (general, gendered, raced, etc.). Related
training programs might then attract broader
audiences, being relevant to all employees (re-
gardless of gender or ethnicity) and avoiding
resistance met by interventions that exclusively
target gender discrimination, racial discrimina-
tion, and so forth (Cortina et al., 2002; Lim &
Cortina, 2005; Podgor, 1996). As Brief et al. note,
“The forces driving discrimination in the work-
place are many, and any legitimate attempt to
combat discrimination must be multifaceted”
(1997: 68).
CONCLUSION
This article integrates the literature on work-
place incivility with that addressing modern
sexism and racism. The resulting concept—
selective incivility—is almost certainly more
pervasive than blatant discrimination and ha-
rassment in the workplace. Incivility is also, by
definition, more insidious, taking hold in such
an ambiguous and stealthy manner that it is
difficult to identify, manage, and prevent. This
speaks to the need for particular vigilance about
issues of “general” incivility, which may not be
so general after all. More research on the nature,
causes, and consequences of this workplace
phenomenon will bring us closer to being able
to combat it effectively. And, to the delight of
many, perhaps this ongoing work will ulti-
mately add a crack to the proverbial glass ceil-
ing.
REFERENCES
Allport, G. W. 1954. The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA:
Addison-Wesley.
Andersson, L. M., & Pearson, C. M. 1999. Tit for tat? The
spiraling effect of incivility in the workplace. Academy
of Management Review, 24: 452–471.
Ashforth, B. 1994. Petty tyranny in organizations. Human
Relations, 47: 755–778.
Banaji, M. R., & Dasgupta, N. 1998. The consciousness of
social beliefs: A program of research on stereotyping
and prejudice. In V. Y. Yzerbt, G. Lories, & B. Dardenne
(Eds.), Metacognition: Cognitive and social dimensions:
157–170. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Barling, J., Dekker, I., Loughlin, C., Kelloway, E., Fullagar, C.,
& Johnson, D. 1996. Prediction and replication of the
organizational and personal consequences of work-
place sexual harassment. Journal of Managerial Psy-
chology, 11: 4 –25.
Barling, J., Rogers, A., & Kelloway, E. 2001. Behind closed
doors: In-home workers’ experience of sexual harass-
ment and workplace violence. Journal of Occupational
Health Psychology, 6: 255–269.
2008 71Cortina
Baron, R. A. 2004. Workplace aggression and violence: In-
sights from basic research. In R. W. Griffin & A. M.
O’Leary-Kelly (Eds.), The dark side of organizational be-
havior: 23– 61. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Baron, S. B., & Banaji, M. R. 2006. The development of implicit
attitudes: Evidence of race evaluations from ages 6 and
10 and adulthood. Psychological Science, 17: 53–58.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. 1995. The need to belong:
Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental
human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117: 497–529.
Benokraitis, N. V. 1997. Sex discrimination in the 21st century.
In N. V. Benokraitis (Ed.), Subtle sexism: Current practice
and prospects for change: 5–33. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Benokraitis, N. V., & Feagin, J. R. 1995. Modern sexism (2nd
ed.). Engelwood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Bjo¨rkqvist, K., O
¨
sterman, K., & Hjelt-Ba¨ ck, M. 1994. Aggres-
sion among university employees. Aggressive Behavior,
20: 173–184.
Brief, A. P., & Barsky, A. 2000. Establishing a climate for
diversity: The inhibition of prejudiced reactions in the
workplace. Research in Personnel and Human Resources
Management, 19: 91–129.
Brief, A. P., Buttram, R. T., Elliott, J. D., Reizenstein, R. M., &
McCline, R. L. 1995. Releasing the beast: A study of
compliance with orders to use race as a selection crite-
rion. Journal of Social Issues, 51: 177–193.
Brief, A. P., Buttram, R. T., Reizenstein, R. M., Pugh, S. D., Cal-
lahan, J. D., McCline, R. L., & Vaslow, J. B. 1997. Beyond
good intentions: The next steps toward racial equality in
the American workplace. Academy of Management Ex-
ecutive, 11(4): 59 –72.
Brief, A. P., Dietz, J., Cohen, R. R., Pugh, S. D., & Vaslow,
J. B. 2000. Just doing business: Modern racism and obe-
dience to authority as explanations for employment dis-
crimination. Organizational Behavior and Human Deci-
sion Processes, 81: 72–97.
Brief, A. P., & Hayes, E. L. 1997. The continuing “American
dilemma”: Studying racism in organizations. Trends in
Organizational Behavior, 4: 89–105.
Buchanan, N. T. 2005. The nexus of race and gender domi-
nation: The racialized sexual harassment of African
American women. In P. Morgan & J. Gruber (Eds.), In the
company of men: Re-discovering the links between sex-
ual harassment and male domination: 294–320. Boston:
Northeastern University Press.
Cleveland, J., & Kerst, M. 1993. Sexual harassment and per-
ceptions of power: An underarticulated relationship.
Journal of Vocational Behavior, 42: 49 – 67.
Cortina, L. M., Lonsway, K. A., & Magley, V. J. 2004. Recon-
ceptualizing workplace incivility through the lenses of
gender and race. Paper presented at the annual meeting
of the Society for Industrial-Organizational Psychology,
Chicago.
Cortina, L. M., Lonsway, K. L., Magley, V. J., Freeman,
L. V., Collinsworth, L. L., Hunter, M., & Fitzgerald,
L. F. 2002. What’s gender got to do with it? Incivility in
the federal courts. Law and Social Inquiry, 27: 235–270.
Cortina, L. M., & Magley, V. J. 2007. Patterns and profiles of
response to incivility in organizations. Working paper,
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.
Cortina, L. M., Magley, V. J., Williams, J. H., & Langhout,
R. D. 2001. Incivility in the workplace: Incidence and
impact. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 6:
64– 80.
Crosby, F. J. 1984. The denial of personal discrimination.
American Behavioral Scientist, 27: 371–386.
Crosby, F. J., Iyer, A., Clayton, S., & Downing, R. A. 2003.
Affirmative action: Psychological data and the policy
debates. American Psychologist, 58: 93–115.
Deitch, E. A., Barsky, A., Butz, R. M., Brief, A. P., Chan, S., &
Bradley, J. C. 2003. Subtle yet significant: The existence
and impact of everyday racial discrimination in the
workplace. Human Relations, 56: 1299–1324.
Devine, P. G. 1989. Stereotypes and prejudice: The automatic
and controlled components. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 56: 5–18.
Devine, P. G., & Monteith, M. J. 1999. Automaticity and control
in stereotyping. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-
process theories in social psychology; 339–360. New
York: Guilford Press.
Dipboye, R. L., & Halverson, S. K. 2004. Subtle (and not so
subtle) discrimination in organizations. In R. W. Griffin
& A. M. O’Leary-Kelly (Eds.), The dark side of organiza-
tional behavior: 131–158. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. 1998. On the nature of con-
temporary prejudice: The causes, consequences, and
challenges of aversive racism. In J. L. Eberhardt &
S. T. Fiske (Eds.), Confronting racism: The problem and
the response: 3–32. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. 2000. Aversive racism and
selection decisions: 1989 and 1999. Psychological Sci-
ence, 11: 315–319.
Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., & Bachman, B. A. 2001. Racial
bias in organizations: The role of group processes and
its causes and cures. In M. E. Turner (Ed.), Groups at
work: Theory and research: 415– 444. Mahwah, NJ: Law-
rence Erlbaum Associates.
Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., Kawakami, K., & Hodson, G.
2002. Why can’t we just get along? Interpersonal biases
and interracial distrust. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic
Minority Psychology, 8(2): 88–102.
Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., Niemann, Y. F., & Snider, K.
2001. Racial, ethnic, and cultural differences in respond-
ing to distinctiveness and discrimination on campus:
Stigma and common group identity. Journal of Social
Issues, 57: 167–188.
Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., & Gaertner, S. L. 2000. Reducing
contemporary prejudice: Combating explicit and im-
plicit bias at the individual and intergroup level. In
S. Oskamp (Ed.), Reducing prejudice and discrimination:
137–163. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Duffy, M. K., Ganster, D. C., & Pagon, M. 2002. Social under-
mining in the workplace. Academy of Management Jour-
nal, 45: 331–351.
72 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review
Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. J. 1984. Gender stereotypes stem
from the distribution of women and men into social
roles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46:
735–754.
Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. 1999. The origins of sex differences
in human behavior: Evolved dispositions versus social
roles. American Psychologist, 54: 408– 423.
Eighth Circuit Gender Fairness Task Force. 1997. Final re-
port and recommendations of the Eighth Circuit Gender
Fairness Task Force. Creighton Law Review, 31(1): 1–181.
Einarsen, S., & Raknes, B. I. 1997. Harassment in the work-
place and the victimization of men. Violence and Vic-
tims, 12: 247–263.
Erber, R., & Fiske, S. T. 1984. Outcome dependency and at-
tention to inconsistent information. Journal of Personal-
ity and Social Psychology, 47: 709 –726.
Fiske, S. T. 1993. Controlling other people: The impact of
power on stereotyping. American Psychologist, 48: 621–
628.
Fiske, S. T. 2000. Stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination
at the seam between the centuries: Evolution, culture,
mind, and brain. European Journal of Social Psychology,
30: 299 –322.
Fiske, S. T. 2001. Effects of power on bias: Power explains
and maintains individual, group, and societal dispari-
ties. In A. Y. Lee-Chai & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Use and abuse
of power: Multiple perspectives on the causes of corrup-
tion: 181–193. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.
Fiske, S. T. 2002. What we know now about bias and inter-
group conflict, the problem of the century. Current Di-
rections in Psychological Science, 11: 123–128.
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. M. C., Glick, P., & Xu, J. 2002. A model
of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and
warmth respectively follow from perceived status and
competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 82: 878 –902.
Fitzgerald, L. F., Drasgow, F., Hulin, C. L., Gelfand, M. J., &
Magley, V. J. 1997. The antecedents and consequences of
sexual harassment in organizations: A test of an inte-
grated model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82: 578–
589.
Forbes, G. B., Adams-Curtis, L. E., & White, K. B. 2004. First-
and second-generation measures of sexism, rape myths
and related beliefs, and hostility toward women. Vio-
lence Against Women, 10: 236–261.
Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. 1986. The aversive form of
racism. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice,
discrimination, and racism: 61– 89. Orlando, FL: Aca-
demic Press.
Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. 2000. Reducing intergroup
bias: The common ingroup identity model. Philadelphia:
Psychology Press.
Gelfand, M. J., Raver, J. L., & Ehrhart, K. H. 2002. Methodolog-
ical issues in cross-cultural organizational research. In
S. G. Rogelberg (Ed.), Handbook of research methods in
industrial and organizational psychology: 216–246. Mal-
den, MA: Blackwell.
Giacalone, R. A., & Greenberg, J. 1997. Antisocial behavior in
organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. 1999. Sexism and other “isms”: Inter-
dependence, status, and the ambivalent content of ste-
reotypes. In W. B. Swann, Jr., J. H. Langlois, & L. A. Gil-
bert (Eds.), Sexism and stereotypes in modern society:
The gender science of Janet Taylor Spence: 192–221.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. 2001. An ambivalent alliance: Hostile
and benevolent sexism as contemporary justifications
for gender inequality. American Psychologist, 56: 109 –
118.
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. 1995. Implicit social cog-
nition: Attitudes, self-esteeem, and stereotypes. Psycho-
logical Review, 102: 4–27.
Griggs v. Duke Power Company, 401, U.S. 424 (1971).
Hebl, M. R., Foster, J. B., Mannix, L. M., & Dovidio, J. F. 2002.
Formal and interpersonal discrimination: A field study
of bias toward homosexual applicants. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 28: 815–825.
Heller, K. 1989. The return to community. American Journal of
Community Psychology, 17: 1–16.
Hilton, J. L., & von Hipple, W. 1996. Stereotypes. Annual
Review of Psychology, 47: 237–271.
Hoel, H., Rayner, C., & Cooper, C. L. 1999. Workplace bully-
ing. International Review of Industrial and Organiza-
tional Psychology, 14: 195–223.
Hulin, C. L., Fitzgerald, L. F., & Drasgow, F. 1996. Organiza-
tional influences on sexual harassment. In M. Stockdale
(Ed.), Sexual harassment in the workplace: Perspectives,
frontiers, and response strategies: 127–150. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Jackman, M. R. 1994. Paternalism and conflict in gender,
class, and race relations. Berkeley: University of Califor-
nia Press.
Jackson, L. M., Esses, V. M., & Burris, C. T. 2001. Contempo-
rary sexism and discrimination: The importance of re-
spect for men and women. Personality and Social Psy-
chology Bulletin, 27: 48–61.
James, E. H., Brief, A. P., Dietz, J., & Cohen, R. R. 2001. Preju-
dice matters: Understanding the reaction of whites to
affirmative action programs targeted to benefit blacks.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 86: 1120 –1128.
James, K., Lovato, C., & Khoo, G. 1994. Social identity corre-
lates of minority workers’ health. Academy of Manage-
ment Journal, 37: 383–396.
James, L. R. 1998. Measurement of personality via condi-
tional reasoning. Organizational Research Methods, 1:
131–163.
James, L. R., & Mazerolle, M. D. 2003. Personality in work
organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Jones, M. 2002. Social psychology of prejudice. Upper Saddle
River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Klein, K. J., & Kozlowski, S. W. J. 2000. Multilevel theory,
research, and methods in organizations: Foundations,
2008 73Cortina
extensions, and new directions. San Francisco: Jossey-
Bass.
Lazarus, R. S. 1999. Stress and emotion: A new synthesis. New
York: Springer.
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. 1984. Stress, appraisal, and
coping. New York: Springer.
Lim, S., & Cortina, L. M. 2004. A multi-level model of incivility
in the workplace. Paper presented at the annual meet-
ing of the Academy of Management, New Orleans.
Lim, S., & Cortina, L. M. 2005. Interpersonal mistreatment in
the workplace: The interface and outcomes of general
incivility and sexual harassment. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 90: 483– 496.
Magley, V. J., Hulin, C. L., Fitzgerald, L. F., & DeNardo, M.
1999. The outcomes of self-labeling sexual harassment.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 84: 390 – 402.
McConahay, J. B. 1986. Modern racism, ambivalence, and the
modern racism scale. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner
(Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination, and racism: 91–125. Or-
lando, FL: Academic Press.
McConahay, J. B., & Hough, J. C. 1976. Symbolic racism.
Journal of Social Issues, 32: 23– 45.
McDonnel Douglas Corp. v. Green, 411 U.S. 792 (1973).
Meyerson, D. E., & Fletcher, J. K. 2000. A modest manifesto for
shattering the glass ceiling. Harvard Business Review,
78(1): 127–136.
Miceli, M. P., & Near, J. P. 1992. Blowing the whistle: The
organizational and legal implications for companies
and employees. New York: Lexington Books.
Miller, D. T., & Turnbull, W. 1986. Expectancies and interper-
sonal processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 37: 233–
256.
Miner-Rubino, K., & Cortina, L. M. 2004. Working in a context
of hostility toward women: Implications for employees’
well-being. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology,
9: 107–122.
Miner-Rubino, K., & Cortina, L. M. 2006. Observed incivility
toward women at work: The role of organizational jus-
tice. Paper presented at the APA/NIOSH Work, Stress
and Health Conference, Miami.
Monin, B., & Miller, D. T. 2001. Moral credentials and the
expression of prejudice. Journal of Personality and So-
cial Psychology, 81: 33–43.
Monteith, M. J. 1993. Self-regulation of prejudiced responses:
Implications for progress in prejudice reduction efforts.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65: 469 –
485.
Neuberg, S. L., & Fiske, S. T. 1987. Motivational influences on
impression formation: Outcome dependency, accuracy-
driven attention, and individuating processes. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 53: 431– 444.
Neuman, J. H. 2004. Injustice, stress, and aggression in orga-
nizations. In R. W. Griffin & A. M. O’Leary-Kelly (Eds.),
The dark side of organizational behavior: 62–102. San
Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
O’Leary-Kelly, A. M., Paetzold, R. L., & Griffin, R. W. 2000.
Sexual harassment as aggressive behavior: An actor-
based perspective. Academy of Management Review,
25: 372–388.
Operario, D., & Fiske, S. T. 1998. Racism equals power plus
prejudice: A social psychological equation for racial
oppression. In J. L. Eberhardt & S. T. Fiske (Eds.), Con-
fronting racism: The problem and the response: 33–53.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Pearson, C. M., Andersson, L. M., & Porath, C. L. 2000. Assess-
ing and attacking workplace incivility. Organizational
Dynamics, 29(2): 123–137.
Pearson, C. M., Andersson, L. M., & Wegner, J. W. 2001. When
workers flout convention: A study of workplace incivil-
ity. Human Relations, 54: 1387–1419.
Pearson, C. M., & Porath, C. L. 2004. On incivility, its impact,
and directions for future research. In R. W. Griffin &
A. M. O’Leary-Kelly (Eds.), The dark side of organization-
al behavior: 403– 425. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Pettigrew, T. F., & Martin, J. 1987. Shaping the organizational
context for black American inclusion. Journal of Social
Issues, 43: 41–78.
Podgor, E. 1996. Lawyer professionalism in a gendered soci-
ety. South Carolina Law Review, 47: 323–348.
Pretty, G. H., & McCarthy, M. 1991. Exploring psychological
sense of community among women and men of the cor-
poration. Journal of Community Psychology, 19: 1351–
1361.
Pryor, J. B., Giedd, J. L., & Williams, K. B. 1995. A social
psychological model for predicting sexual harassment.
Journal of Social Issues, 51: 69 – 84.
Richman, J. A., Rospenda, K. M., Nawyn, S. J., Flaherty,
J. A., Fendrich, M., Drum, M., & Johnson, T. P. 1999. Sexual
harassment and generalized workplace abuse: Preva-
lence and mental health correlates. American Journal of
Public Health, 89: 358–363.
Riger, S. 1991. Gender dilemmas in sexual harassment pol-
icies and procedures. American Psychologist, 46: 497–
505.
Robinson, S. L., & Bennett, R. J. 1995. A typology of deviant
workplace behaviors: A multidimensional scaling
study. Academy of Management Journal, 38: 555–572.
Robinson, S. L., & O’Leary-Kelly, A. M. 1998. Monkey see,
monkey do: The influence of work groups on the antiso-
cial behavior of employees. Academy of Management
Journal, 41: 658 –672.
Rospenda, K. 2002. Workplace harassment, services utiliza-
tion, and drinking outcomes. Journal of Occupational
Health Psychology, 7: 141–155.
Rowe, M. P. 1990. Barriers to equality: The power of subtle
discrimination to maintain unequal opportunity. Em-
ployee Responsibility and Rights Journal, 3: 153–163.
Sanchez, J. I., & Brock, P. 1996. Outcomes of perceived dis-
crimination among Hispanic employees: Is diversity
management a luxury or a necessity? Academy of Man-
agement Journal, 39: 704–719.
Sarason, S. B. 1974. The psychological sense of community:
74 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review
Prospects for a community psychology. Cambridge, MA:
Brookline Books.
Schaffer, B. S., & Riordan, C. M. 2003. A review of cross-
cultural methodologies for organizational research. Or-
ganizational Research Methods, 6: 169–215.
Schneider, K. T., Hitlan, R. T., & Radhakrishnan, P. 2000. An
examination of the nature and correlates of ethnic ha-
rassment experiences in multiple contexts. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 85: 3–12.
Scott, W. R. 1992. Organizations: Rational, natural, and open
systems. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Sears, D. O. 1988. Symbolic racism. In P. A. Katz & D. A. Taylor
(Eds.), Eliminating racism: Profiles in controversy: 53– 84.
New York: Plenum Press.
Sears, D. O. 1988. Racism and politics in the United States. In
J. L. Eberhardt & S. T. Fiske (Eds.), Confronting racism:
The problem and the response: 76–100. Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage.
Skarlicki, D. P., & Folger, R. 1997. Retaliation in the work-
place: The roles of distributive, procedural, and interac-
tional justice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82: 434 –
443.
Spector, P. E., & Fox, S. 2002. An emotion-centered model of
voluntary work behavior: Some parallels between coun-
terproductive work behavior and organizational citizen-
ship behavior. Human Resource Management Review,
12: 269 –292.
Stone, E. F., Stone, D. L., & Dipboye, R. L. 1992. Stigmas in
organizations: Race, handicaps, and physical unattrac-
tiveness. In K. Kelley (Ed.), Issues, theory, and research in
industrial/organizational psychology: 385– 457. Oxford:
North-Holland.
Swim, J. K., Aikin, K. J., Hall, W. S., & Hunter, B. A. 1995.
Sexism and racism: Old-fashioned and modern preju-
dices. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68:
199–214.
Swim, J. K., Scott, E. D., Sechrist, G. B., Campbell, B., &
Stangor, C. 2003. The role of intent and harm in judg-
ments of prejudice and discrimination. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 84: 944 –959.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. 1986. The social identity theory of
intergroup behavior. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.),
Psychology of intergroup relations: 7–24. Chicago: Nel-
son-Hall.
Thacker, R. A., & Ferris, G. R. 1991. Understanding sexual
harassment in the workplace: The influence of power
and politics within the dyadic interaction of harasser
and target. Human Resource Management Review, 1:
23–37.
Tougas, F., Brown, R., Beaton, A. M., & Joly, S. 1995. Neosex-
ism: Plus c¸a change, plus c’est pareil. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 21: 842–849.
U.S. Equal Employment Opportunity Commission. Guide-
lines on harassment based on race, color, religion, gen-
der, national origin, age, or disability. 58 Fed. Reg.
51,266. Washington, DC: U.S. Equal Employment Oppor-
tunity Commission.
U.S. Equal Employment Opportunity Commission. 2003.
Charge statistics FY 1992 through FY 2002. Washington,
DC: U.S. Equal Employment Opportunity Commission.
Valian, V. 1998. Why so slow? The advancement of women.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
VandenBos, G. R., & Bulatao, E. Q. 1996. Violence on the job:
Identifying risks and developing solutions. Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association.
Wheaton, B. 1997. The nature of chronic stress. In B. H. Gott-
lieb (Ed.), Coping with chronic stress: 43–74. New York:
Plenum Press.
Williams, J. H., Fitzgerald, L. F., & Drasgow, F. 1999. The
effects of organizational practices on sexual harass-
ment and individual outcomes in the military. Military
Psychology, 11: 303–328.
Williams, K. D. 1998. Social exclusion. In R. M. Kowalski (Ed.),
Aversive interpersonal behaviors: 133–170. New York:
Plenum Press.
Williams, K. D., & Sommer, K. L. 1997. Social exclusion by
coworkers: Does rejection lead to loafing or compensa-
tion? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23:
693–706.
Lilia M. Cortina (lilia@umich.edu) is an associate professor of psychology and wom-
en’s studies at the University of Michigan. She received her Ph.D. in psychology from
the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign. Her research addresses victimization
and gender in organizations, focusing in particular on employee experiences of
incivility and sexual harassment.
2008 75Cortina