ArticlePDF Available

Astrology Beliefs among Undergraduate Students

Authors:
  • Planetary Science Institute & University of Arizona

Abstract and Figures

A survey of the science knowledge and attitudes toward science of nearly 10000 undergraduates at a large public university over a 20-year period included several questions addressing student beliefs in astrology and other forms of pseudoscience. The results from our data reveal that a large majority of students (78%) considered astrology “very” or “sort of” scientific. Only 52% of science majors said that astrology is “not at all” scientific. We find that students’ science literacy, as defined by the National Science Foundation in its surveys of the general public, does not strongly correlate with an understanding that astrology is pseudoscientific, and therefore belief in astrology is likely not a valid indicator of scientific illiteracy.
Content may be subject to copyright.
A LIVELY ELECTRONIC COMPENDIUM OF RESEARCH, NEWS, RESOURCES, AND OPINION
Astrono my Education Revie w
2011, AER, 10, 010101-1, 10.3847/AER2010040
Astrology Beliefs among Undergraduate Students
Hannah Sugarman
Department of Astronomy, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona 85721
Chris Impey
Department of Astronomy, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona 85721
Sanlyn Buxner
Department of Astronomy, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona 85721
Jessie Antonellis
Department of Astronomy, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona 85721
Received: 11/2/10, Revised: 02/16/11, Published: 04/14/11
V
C
2011 The American Astronomical Society. All rights reserved.
Abstract
A survey of the science knowledge and attitudes toward science of nearly 10000 undergraduates at a large public
university over a 20-year period included several questions addressing student beliefs in astrology and other
forms of pseudoscience. The results from our data reveal that a large majority of students (78%) considered
astrology “very” or “sort of” scientific. Only 52% of science majors said that astrology is “not at all” scientific.
We find that students’ science literacy, as defined by the National Science Foundation in its surveys of the
general public, does not strongly correlate with an understanding that astrology is pseudoscientific, and therefore
belief in astrology is likely not a valid indicator of scientific illiteracy.
1. INTRODUCTION
Astrology is pervasive in Western society. Nearly every major newspaper and many magazines publish daily or
monthly horoscopes; dating websites allow users to search for romantic partners by supposedly compatible “Sun
signs;” and numerous prominent celebrities and politicians consult professional astrologers. Astronomy
educators frequently encounter confusion between astronomy and astrology, and many dedicate course time in
introductory classes for nonscience majors to “debunking” astrology.
However, despite the general awareness that astrology is prevalent and a frequent assumption that
pseudoscientific beliefs must be inversely related to scientific literacy, very little research has been done to
actually examine what characterizes student belief in pseudoscience. Is belief in astrology an accurate barometer
of scientific literacy?
Surveying college nonscience majors is particularly relevant to this debate, as a recent increase in the general
public’s scientific literacy has been ascribed to courses like those that served as the setting for the study
presented in this paper (Hobson 2008). This results described in this article derive from the results of a study
of the science literacy, attitudes, and beliefs about science of nearly 10000 undergraduates. A number of items
on the survey instrument were designed to probe pseudoscience beliefs, two of which relate directly to the
question of astrology. By examining students’ performance on the science literacy items and their beliefs
about astrology, we can determine whether these two constructs are in direct conflict, or if scientific
knowledge can coexist with pseudoscientific misconceptions. A better understanding of patterns in students’
belief in pseudoscience may help educators learn the best ways to combat pseudoscience in the classroom, or
even if it is necessary to combat it at all.
2. BELIEF IN ASTROLOGY
Although astrology has roots in positional astronomy and was historically considered a science (much as
alchemy was), under current constructs of what constitutes science, astrology has no scientific validity. However,
astrological beliefs are still very popular with many educated members of modern society. Despite this,
surprisingly few rigorous studies have been performed to investigate why astrology is so appealing and
persistent. A summary of some of these studies follows, in an attempt to explain the psychological basis for
astrological belief.
One of the most frequently referenced causes for belief in astrology is the so-called “Barnum Effect,” which is
named after the famous American showman P. T. Barnum. Commonly used in psychology, it refers to a
tendency for people to accept vague and general characterizations of themselves as accurate. Forer (1949) found
that vague statements such as “at times you are sociable, while at other times you are reserved” tended to impress
people as accurate personality descriptions. Glick, Gottesman, and Jolton (1989) demonstrated that although
believers in astrology were more likely to accept any type of personality description as accurate, regardless of
whether it was positive or negative, skeptics also were likely to fall prey to the Barnum effect, especially if the
personality descriptions they were given were positive. Moreover, even astrology skeptics were more likely to
call a description accurate if they were told it was based on their sign of the zodiac. Finally, skeptics who were
given a flattering horoscope demonstrated an increase in their favorable attitudes toward astrology.
This result is backed up by research that has shown that people are more likely to pay attention to and remember
feedback that verifies their positive self-conceptions, and interpret negative or contradictory feedback so as to
minimize its impact (Swann 1990; Markus 1977), which may begin to explain how belief in astrology emerges in
the first place. If we assume someone is initially neutral toward astrology, exposure to positively slanted horoscopes
and personality descriptions may influence that person toward belief in astrology. Lillqvist and Lindeman (1998)
explained in more detail how verification of positive self-concept may influence belief in astrology.
In addition to susceptibility to vague but positive personality descriptions, there is evidence that people may be
more inclined to seek astrological explanation for life events when their personal life or the cultural climate is
particularly stressful or uncertain. Lillqvist and Lindeman (1998) surveyed adult students taking an astrology
class and compared them to students taking nonastrology related classes (German language and psychology).
They asked the students about personal crises that they had experienced recently, such as divorce,
unemployment, menopause, or economic difficulties. They found that the students who had enrolled in the
astrology class had experienced more crises recently than the control group of students. Additionally, the control
group was asked about interest in astrology, and those students who demonstrated interest also reported more
personal crises in their recent pasts.
In a similar vein, Padgett and Jorgenson (1982) found that during the most chaotic political and economic years
in Germany between the two world wars interest in astrology, as measured by published astrological articles,
increased with increasing economic and political threat and uncertainty. Similarly, Sales (1973) found an
increased interest in astrology during the Great Depression in the United States. In these situations of personal
and national crises, astrology may give people an (illusory) sense of control, which is otherwise often absent
from stressful situations. Indeed, Lillqvist and Lindeman find that participants reported increased perceptions of
control in their lives after taking a class in astrology.
Finally, people may use astrology for much the same reasons as carrying a good luck charm to a high-stakes job
interview—as a “just in case” strategy. This may especially be true during stressful times in life, when there is much
to gain and very little to lose. Tolbert (1990) framed this as a “cost-benefit” analysis. For most people, there is very
little time, effort, and money involved in belief in astrology, so the “cost” is very low. The benefit, even if it is only
to your self-concept, self-esteem, and sense of control, might be worth the low investment. (Professional astrologers,
of course, put a lot of time and effort into the field, but they also reap a benefit, usually in the form of income.)
Shermer (1997) discussed the same basic concept in the framework of evolutionary psychology. He posited that
it was beneficial to survival to make connections between things and events in the environment (red spiders are
poisonous), and that there was little to be lost by making too many connections or by making false positives
(blue spiders are also poisonous). In contrast, false negatives (red spiders are not poisonous) might be fatal, while
false positives are at worst unnecessary (avoiding all spiders). Therefore, Shermer believes humans might be
naturally inclined toward superstition, and consequently belief in astrology. After all, a belief in astrology is not
likely to have any negative impact on your day-to-day survival.
3. THE SURVEY
The survey instrument used in this study was designed to measure attitudes about science, perceptions of
pseudoscience, and general scientific knowledge using forced choice and open-ended questions. The two parts of
the survey are set of 21 knowledge-based questions, four of which are open-ended with short written answers and
17 of which are true/false or multiple choice, and a set of 24 statements about science, pseudoscience, and
technology where the responses are on a five-point Likert scale. The survey has been administered to students in
General Education astronomy lecture courses for freshmen and sophomores at the University of Arizona since
1988. The survey is given in the first week of class, generally before any discussion of astrology or
pseudoscience has taken place; it is anonymous, voluntary, and does not count for any part of a student’s grade.
Typically 10 to 15 min are allowed for its completion. Although the survey is anonymous, demographic
information is collected: gender, major, class standing, and number of science courses taken at the university.
The instrument is administered on paper, and subsequent data entry and coding are done by hand.
There is a vigorous debate about the definition of scientific literacy and its societal importance, but there is
general consensus in the literature that science literacy is composed of (1) knowledge of basic scientific concepts
and principles, (2) an understanding of how science works and how it differs from other modes of knowing, and
(3) an ability to apply that knowledge and understanding to evaluate science and technology in everyday life
(Bybee 1997; Miller 2002; Hazen and Trefil 2009). Our survey attempts to measure some aspects of science
literacy with 17 “knowledge” questions with true/false or multiple choice answers. These questions cover a
broad range of scientific subjects, from physics (“Which travels faster, light or sound?”) to basic biology (“The
oxygen we breathe comes from plants, true or false?”). To obtain a “science literacy score” for each participant,
we used 15 of the 17 questions, in order to have maximum overlap with the biannual National Science
Foundation (NSF) Science Indicators survey. From the answers to these questions, each student is given a score
on a scale from 0 to 15, with each correct question worth one point. For a more detailed discussion of the entire
survey, plus a description of the data entry and coding process, see Impey et al. (2011).
Of special interest to this discussion are the two Likert-scale items related directly to astrology. Students were
asked to respond to the statement “The positions of the planets have an influence on the events of everyday life.”
While this is not the only principle of astrology, it is the most well-known aspect of astrological predictions
because of its ubiquity in the mass media. Additionally, we asked students the forced-choice question: “Would
you say that astrology is very, sort of, or not at all scientific?” This question is identical to the question asked by
the NSF in its biannual surveys, and allows for direct comparison between our results and the national average.
There are some potential problems with the questions as they appear on the survey. As many authors have noted
(i.e., Stehling 1990; Thorndike 1955; Cohen 1985), the early history of astronomy is as intimately tied with
astrology as alchemy is tied to chemistry. Kepler, Galileo, and Brahe routinely cast horoscopes for prominent
politicians of their time (Cohen 1985). Even today, astronomers still refer to the zodiacal constellations when
mapping the sky. Additionally, a signature of pseudoscience is that it presents itself with the trappings of science
(Shermer 1997), while not subjecting itself to rigorous scientific tests. Knowledge of the historical ties between
astrology and astronomy, and/or the fact that astrology presents itself as science, might even cause some
professional astronomers to declare astrology “sort of” scientific, despite their awareness that astrology does not
adhere to the modern scientific method. The Likert-scale item that specifically talks about planetary positions
affecting everyday life should control for this issue to some degree. It also does not contain the word “astrology,”
in order to avoid any potential confusion that students may have concerning the phonetic and morphological
similarity of the words “astrology” and “astronomy.”
4. RESULTS BY DEMOGRAPHIC
Figure 1 presents the overall results to the two astrology items, broken down by gender. Only 22% of all students
state that astrology is “not at all” scientific. Similarly, only 29% of students “disagree” or “strongly disagree”
with the statement that the planets affect everyday life. There is a slightly greater tendency for females to adhere
to the principles of astrology, a result that has been duplicated by the NSF and others researchers (e.g., DeRoberts
and Delaney 1992, 2000).
Table 1 and Figure 2 present results on the astrology questions, broken down by major and number of science
courses completed. Science majors, not surprisingly yet gratifyingly, perform better than their counterparts in
other fields. Similarly, the fraction of correct responses rises with the number of science classes that students
have completed. If we measure the success of our science curricula by the ability of students to distinguish
pseudoscience from hard science, this could very well indicate an encouraging, although not great, success rate.
However, despite the fact that students who have completed three science classes—the number of classes
required to fulfill the General Education requirement at the University of Arizona—perform twice as well as
students who have not yet taken any science, only 30% of these students answer the astrology question correctly.
Figure 3 reports result by class year. Seniors in nonscience majors perform surprisingly well, matching the
performance of science majors and students who have taken four or more science classes.
5. COMPARISON TO NATIONAL FINDINGS
Every two years as part of the Science and Engineering Indicators series, a document prepared by the National
Science Board, which guides scientific policy and strategy for the U.S. government (the NSF), conducts a survey
to ascertain public understanding and attitudes about science and technology. As previously mentioned, the
survey asks the question: “Would you say that astrology is very, sort of, or not at all scientific?” Since 1979, the
NSF has recorded a gradual decline in belief in astrology, indicating a trend toward fewer Americans believing
Figure 1. Responses to the two astrology items, broken down by gender. There is a modest but statistically significant tendency
for female students to state that astrology is sort of or very scientific, as well as to demonstrate agreement with the idea that the
planets affect everyday life. Note that a very small percentage of students strongly agree with this idea, and the gender discrep-
ancy for that particular response is statistically insignificant (12.5% of males and 11.8% of females chose strongly agree).
that astrology is scientific. The most recent data reports that around 60% of all adults and around 50% of people
in the 18–24 year old or college-age group state that astrology is “not at all scientific.” This result is notably
different from the results obtained using our survey instrument.
It is not entirely clear why our results indicate a much higher degree of confusion about the scientific validity of
astrology, although there are several possibilities. First, the NSF survey is typically given over the phone, while
our survey is paper-based, which could have an unpredictable effect on responses. Second, our survey is given in
the context of an astronomy course, which could unintentionally bias the results or increase confusion due to
vocabulary confusion between astrology and astronomy. This is also one factor that may change in the context of
a phone survey, where subjects hear the word rather than reading it on the page. Finally, our survey population is
largely from the Southwest, which may introduce a regional effect that distinguishes the two sample populations.
Since the NSF does not report results by state, it is not possible at present to test this final hypothesis.
6. SCIENCE LITERACY AND ASTROLOGY
One of the common assumptions about astrology is that students who know or who understand more
science are less likely to subscribe to the principles of astrology, or be susceptible to the other forms of
Table 1. Correct responses by major. Students majoring in the sciences perform notably better than stu-
dents in other colleges, although still only half of science students state that astrology is “not at all“ scien-
tific. Over 80% of our future doctors and nurses, teachers, and business people believe that astrology is
very or sort of scientific.
Major
Percentage responding that astrology
is “not at all“ scientific
(%)
Medicine/Nursing/Pharmacy/Public Health n ¼ 52ðÞ 7.7
Architecture/Management/Business/Law n ¼ 2347ðÞ 16.5
No major selected, undecided, blank n ¼ 1122
ðÞ
18.8
College of Education n ¼ 336ðÞ 19.0
Social and Behavioral Sciences n ¼ 1127ðÞ 24.7
Fine Arts/Humanities n ¼ 851ðÞ 29.1
Engineering and Agriculture/Life Sciences n ¼ 211ðÞ 36.0
College of Science n ¼ 216ðÞ 51.9
Figure 2. Responses by number of college science classes completed. After completion of the General Education science
requirement of three courses, correct responses have nearly doubled. Many students who have completed four or more science
courses are science majors.
pseudoscience, presumably because they have learned critical thinking skills and the scientific method
along with their factual knowledge. The 15 content-knowledge questions contained in our survey allow us
to carefully consider this assumption.
Figure 4 examines the relationship between correct answers to the science knowledge questions and a correct
response to the forced-choice astrology question, where “astrology is not at all scientific” was scored as being
correct and “astrology is ‘very’ or ‘sort’ of scientific” was scored as being incorrect. As can be seen in the graph,
students who answered the astrology question correctly had an average science literacy score of 12.5 (83%) and
those who answered incorrectly had an average score of 11.5 (77%). There is a small but significant correlation
between overall science literacy score and being able to identify astrology as not at all scientific (r
pb
¼ 0:21,
Figure 3. Responses by class year to the forced-choice astrology question. The n size is quite small for seniors, but overall
there is a strong trend of increasing awareness that astrology is not scientific as students pass through the university, regardless
of major. Only 5 students did not report their class standing, and are not included in this graph.
Figure 4. The relationship between overall science literacy scores (out of 15 questions) and responses to the forced-choice as-
trology question. The number of responses has been normalized for clarity.
p < 0:01). Similarly, students who state that they strongly disagree or disagree that the positions of the planets
influence everyday life answer an average of 11.6 questions correctly, while students who “agree,” “strongly
agree,” or have “no opinion” answer 11.0 questions correctly. (Students who agreed and had no opinion were
binned for analysis.) These results very likely are related to the data discussed above, which demonstrated that
science majors, seniors, and students who have taken more science classes were more likely to answer the
astrology question correctly. The difference of one question right or wrong is hardly a dramatic demonstration of
an improvement in scientific literacy. In general, the results of our survey indicate that belief in astrology is not
strongly linked to scientific literacy. Belief in astrology is also relatively impervious to a college education.
Therefore, the frequent use of astrology as an indicator of scientific literacy or as a symptom of general scientific
illiteracy appears to be invalid.
7. PSEUDOSCIENCE BELIEFS AND SCIENCE KNOWLEDGE
Figure 5 and Table 2 presents the results to five additional statements relating to science and pseudoscience
beliefs, as well as the astrology question for comparison. Astrology is the second-most prevalent belief in our
sample among the five pseudoscience examples, followed by the idea that “some people possess psychic
powers.” Students demonstrate the most skepticism toward the idea that “some ancient civilizations were visited
Figure 5. Responses to statements concerning science and pseudoscience beliefs. The astrology question is also displayed for
purposes of comparison.
by extraterrestrials.” These results, taken from our larger instrument to diagnose science knowledge and
attitudes, indicate that astrology is part of a larger landscape of susceptibility to pseudoscience thinking among
college undergraduates.
A similar analysis to that performed for astrology shows that the acceptance of pseudoscience often coexists with
strong performance on science knowledge indicators. We conclude that the prevalence of belief in astrology
among college undergraduates does not imply poor scientific literacy, as measured by an instrument similar to
that used to guide public science education policy. Pseudoscience beliefs are widespread, and are somewhat
mitigated by college science classes, but they can coexist with legitimate scientific knowledge. Particular
instructional strategies designed to combat belief in astrology may not have a larger benefit. In a future paper, we
will present a broader view of levels of science knowledge and their relationship to various pseudoscience
beliefs, with a view to informing the current debate on scientific literacy and suggesting instructional strategies
to more broadly instill scientific ways of thinking in nonscience majors.
REFERENCES
Bybee, R. W. 1997, Achieving Scientific Literacy: From Purposes to Practices, Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Cohen, J. B. 1985, Revolution in Science, Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
De Roberts, M. M., and Delaney, P. A. 1992, “A Survey of the Attitudes of University Students to Astrology
and Astronomy,” J. R. Astron. Soc. Canada, 87, 34.
De Roberts, M. M., and Delaney, P. A. 2000, “A Second Survey of the Attitudes of University Students to
Astrology and Astronomy,” J. R. Astron. Soc. Canada, 94, 112.
Forer, B. R. 1949, “The Fallacy of Personal Validation: A Classroom Demonstration of Gullibility,” J. Abnorm.
Soc. Psychol., 44, 118.
Glick, P., Gottesman, D., and Jolton, J. 1989, “The Fault is Not in the Stars: Susceptibility of Skeptics and
Believers in Astrology to the Barnum Effect,” Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull., 15, 572.
Hazen, R. M., and Trefil, J. 2009, Science Matters: Achieving Scientific Literacy, New York: Random House.
Hobson, A. 2008, “The Surprising Effectiveness of College Science Literacy Courses,Phys. Teach., 46, 404.
Impey, C. D. S., Buxner, S., Antonellis, J., Johnson, E., and King, C. 2011, “A Twenty Year Survey of Science
Literacy among College Undergraduates,” J. Coll. Sci. Teach., 40, 70.
Table 2. Percentage of students responding to the statements about pseudoscience and the paranormal.
Strongly disagree
(%)
Disagree
(%)
No opinion
(%)
Agree
(%)
Strongly agree
(%)
There are phenomena that physical
science and the laws of nature cannot
explain 1.69 6.59 10.50 49.01 32.22
Some people possess psychic powers 9.27 19.31 32.29 31.77 7.36
Some ancient civilizations were visited
by extraterrestrials 10.53 22.34 51.66 12.31 3.17
Faith healing is a valid alternative to
conventional medicine 14.05 25.42 33.99 20.67 5.87
Some numbers are especially lucky for
some people 14.28 23.34 33.84 23.18 5.35
The positions of the planets have an
influence on the events of everyday life 9.90 18.45 29.89 29.62 12.15
Lillqvist, O., and Lindeman, M. 1998, “Belief in Astrology as a Strategy for Self-Verification and Coping with
Negative Life-Events,” Eur. Psychol., 3, 202.
Markus, H. 1977, “Self-Schemata and Processing Information about the Self,” J. Pers. Soc. Psychol., 35, 63.
Miller, J. D. 2002, “Civic Science Literacy: A Necessity in the 21st Century,” FAS Public Interests Report,
55, 3.
Padgett, V. R., and Jorgenson, D. O. 1982, “Superstition and Economical Threat: Germany, 1918–1949,” Pers.
Soc. Psychol. Bull., 8, 736.
Sales, S. M. 1973, “Threat as a Factor in Authoritarianism: An Analysis of Archival Data,” J. Pers. Soc.
Psychol., 36, 988.
Shermer, M. 1997, Why People Believe Weird Things, New York: W.H. Freeman.
Stehling, K. 1990, “Classical Astrology: A Precursor to Astronomy?,” in The Universe and Its Origins , ed. S. F.
Singer, New York: Paragon House, 73.
Swann, W. B. 1990, “To be Adored or to Be Known? The Interplay of Self-Enhancement and Self-
Verification,” in Handbook of Motivation and Cognition. Foundations of Social Behavior, eds. E. T. Higgins
and R. M. Sorrentino, New York: Guilford, vol. 2, 408.
Thorndike, L. 1955, “The True Place of Astrology in the History of Science,” Isis, 46, 273.
Tolbert, C. R. 1990, “Why Does Astrology Still Flourish?,” in The Un iverse and Its Origins: From Ancient
Myth to Present Reality and Fantasy, ed. F. Singer, New York: Paragon House, 80.
ÆR
010101-1–010101-9
... Relevant research has shown that astrology does not experience any challenges for its existence. Studies on various age and occupation groups (Francis, Williams, 2009;Happs, 1991;Kallery, 2001;Kose, Ayten, 2009;Losh, Nzekwe, 2011;Martin, 1994;Morier, Keeports, 1994;Roig et al., 1998;Science and Indicators, 2002, 2004Sugarman, Impey, Buxner, Antonellis, 2011;Williams, Francis, Robbins, 2007;Yates, Chandler, 2000) involve fi ndings supporting that astrology today is a pseudo-scientifi c belief of public interest. Very few studies in Turkey (Aglarci, Kabapinar, 2012;Turgut, 2009;Turgut, Akcay, Irez, 2010) focused on the distinction between science and pseudo-science. ...
... This result is supportive of the fi ndings of this study. Similar outcomes were obtained from pre-service homeroom teachers as the sampling of Happs (1991), college students as the sampling of Roig et al. (1998), pre-service homeroom teachers, and other subjects as the sampling of Yates and Chandler (2000) in Australia, kindergarten teachers as the sampling of Kallery (2001) in Greece, young adolescents as the sampling of Williams et al. (2007) aged between 13 and16, pre-service teachers as the sampling of Losh et al. (2011), college students as the sampling of Sugarman et al. (2011), and pre-service teachers of chemistry as the sampling of Aglarci and Kabapinar (2012). The study did not involve any actions to chance pseudo-scientifi c beliefs of pre-service teachers about astrology; it only aimed to predict their perceptions. ...
Article
The purpose of this study was to determine perceptions embraced by preservice science teachers on astrology, which is defined as a pseudo-science. It also aimed to reflect their abilities to distinguish between science and pseudo-science through the examples of astronomy – astrology. Twenty-nine grade 4 preservice teachers studying Science Education participated in the study. Among the qualitative research patterns, the phenomenological approach was used for the study. Data were collected through an open-ended question form, group discussion records, and research assignments, while individual interviews were conducted where necessary. Open coding was used for the evaluation of the data, and the validity of the study was obtained through verifications of preservice teachers on themes and theories during the interviews. The findings of the study showed that a vast majority of preservice teachers perceived astrology as a field of science or a subfield of astronomy. Quite a few preservice teachers expressed that astrology was a nonscientific field and that knowledge obtained through astrology could not be considered as scientific knowledge. Key words: astrology, astronomy, pre-service science teachers, pseudo-science, science.
... They found that 44% of students could not distinguish between astronomy and astrology [35]. In a 2011 American study, Sugarman et al. found that over 70% of students believed that astrology was, at least in part, a scientific discipline [36]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This article reports the findings of a qualitative study that aimed to explore the ideas of 22 preservice physics teachers regarding astronomy concepts both within and beyond the solar system, as well as their understanding of the Nature of Science in the context of astronomy. The study employed a combination of open-ended vignette tasks and ranking tasks, which were adapted from previous research. The results reveal that while the majority of the preservice teachers held appropriate conceptions of astronomy concepts, some of their ideas lacked depth and were superficial. Moreover, their understanding of the Nature of Science in the context of astronomy was found to be limited. This study highlights the importance of providing further education and training in this area, as well as the need to develop and test effective teaching strategies to enhance preservice teachers' and physics education students' understanding of astronomy concepts and the Nature of Science.
... Some research outcomes on individual differences indicate that people have difficulties distinguishing between science and pseudoscience (Gaze, 2014;Lyddy & Hughes, 2012) 1 . Nevertheless, the trappings of science do not explain these dif ficulties, as the same lack of discernment has also been found in relation to non-science-mimicking paranormal beliefs (Brewer, 2013;Garrett & Cutting, 2017) and astrology, a borderline doctrine between pseudoscientific and paranormal rhetoric (DeRobertis & Delaney, 2000;Sugarman, Impey, Buxner, & Antonellis, 2011). The negative association between well-oriented scientific literacy and pseudoscientific beliefs, as predicted by the confusion-based conception, is similarly not well-supported. ...
Article
Full-text available
This article presents an integrative model for the endorsement of pseudoscience: the explanation-polarisation model. It is based on a combination of perceived explanatory satisfaction and group polarisation, offering a perspective different from the classical confusion-based conception, in which pseudoscientific beliefs would be accepted through a lack of distinction between science and science mimicry. First, I discuss the confusion-based account in the light of current evidence, pointing out some of its explanatory shortcomings. Second, I develop the explanation-polarisation model, showing its explanatory power in connection with recent research outcomes in cognitive and social psychology.
... Bu sorulara yanıt veren akademik çalışmalar ortaya çıktıkça, yukarıdaki iddia ile ilgili olarak daha net değerlendirme yapılabilir. Ancak belirtilmelidir ki, eğitim seviyesi arttıkça astrolojiye olan inançta düşme eğilimi gözlemlenmektedir (Allum, 2011: 345;Sugarman, Impey, Buxner & Antonellis, 2011). Türkiye'de Süleyman Demirel Üniversitesi'nde yapılmış bir çalışma da eğitim seviyesi arttıkça astrolojiye inananların sayısında azalma olduğunu ortaya koymuştur (Topuz, 2012: 30). ...
Book
Full-text available
... In addition, Good (2012) ISSN 1648-3898 /Print/ ISSN 2538-7138 /Online/ demonstrated that a science education program with special emphasis on the nature of science did not guarantee more skepticism towards pseudoscience. It is striking that neither high school students (Preece & Baxter, 2000) nor university students (Sugarman et al., 2011) have been found to correctly delimit science and pseudoscience. It is not surprising, therefore, that Mugaloglu (2014) advocated for the inclusion of elements, resources and strategies in science and science education curricula that allow the recognition of pseudoscience. ...
Article
Full-text available
One of the main objectives of scientific literacy is the development of rational skills and critical thinking in citizens. This is a central goal for teachers. However, beliefs that lack rational foundation and supporting evidence, named “epistemically unwarranted beliefs” (EUB), spread rapidly among the population. If teachers had some of these EUB, their work could be compromised. The aim of this research was to determine the level of acceptance of different EUB in Spanish pre-service primary school teachers and to analyze the influence of their cognitive style, gender, and academic level. Two hundred and fifty undergraduate students of Bachelor’s Degree in Primary Education participated in this study. Two questionnaires were used to collect data. ANOVA, ANCOVA, correlations, and linear regression analysis were used to quantify that influence. Results showed high levels of acceptance of some EUB in future teachers, with significant influences of gender and academic level, and a mediating role of cognitive styles. Experiential and rational cognitive styles, and academic level were significant predictors of EUB, being experiential thinking the most powerful one. Thus, pre-service teacher education should have an epistemological vigilance on future teachers’ scientific literacy and increase the presence of rational style among teachers
Article
Full-text available
La educación secundaria desempeña un importante papel en la alfabetización científica y, en ella, las creencias en pseudociencias tienen una gran relevancia. Por esta razón, los objetivos de este estudio se centraron en la evaluación del nivel de adhesión a las creencias en pseudociencias de los estudiantes de secundaria y en el análisis de la influencia del género, nivel académico y emociones generadas en el aula de ciencias sobre dichas creencias. Participaron 125 estudiantes de tres cursos diferentes de educación secundaria (3º y 4º de ESO, y 1º de Bachillerato, esto es, 9º, 10º y 11º grado). A todos ellos se les administraron dos cuestionarios, uno para evaluar las creencias pseudocientíficas y el otro para determinar las emociones, positivas y negativas, que se experimentan en las clases de ciencias. A partir de los resultados obtenidos mediante un análisis de las correlaciones entre variables y un análisis de regresión, se puede concluir que: a) el nivel de creencias en pseudociencias de los estudiantes es medio-bajo y comparable al de otros países; b) solo las emociones negativas y el nivel académico de los estudiantes influyen de forma significativa en las creencias en pseudociencias, y ambas tienen una contribución similar a la variabilidad de estas creencias.
Article
En este trabajo, se propone una clasificación epistemológica teórica para el discurso astrológico a partir de evidencia empírica a fin de abordar el problema de cómo determinar específicamente qué es lo que lo hace ilegítimo. A partir de esta clasificación se diseñó una intervención didáctica enmarcada en el enfoque de Enseñanza de las Ciencias Naturales en Contexto (ECNC) con el objetivo de fomentar, en estudiantes de nivel secundario, la capacidad de distinguir enunciados cognoscitivamente ilegítimos -por infalsables o por falsos- respecto de aquellos que pueden constituir genuino conocimiento científico. La secuencia didáctica se implementó con un grupo de 51 estudiantes de 14 a 17 años. Se evaluó su impacto por comparación entre un pretest y un postest. Los resultados mostraron que su capacidad de discernimiento entre enunciados falsables y no falsables se incrementó apreciablemente.
Article
Full-text available
Secondary education plays an important role in scientific literacy and, in it, beliefs in pseudoscience have a great relevance. For this reason, the objectives of this study focused on the evaluation of the level of adherence to pseudoscience beliefs of secondary school students, and on the analysis of the influence of gender, grade level and emotions generated in the science classroom on these beliefs. A total of 125 students from three different grades of Spanish secondary education (9th, 10th and 11th grade, between 14 and 17 years old) participated. Two questionnaires, one to evaluate pseudoscientific beliefs, and the other to determine the positive and negative emotions experienced in the science classroom, were administered to participants. From the results obtained through an analysis of the correlations between variables and a regression analysis, it can be concluded that: a) the level of pseudoscience beliefs of students is medium-low and comparable to that of other countries; b) only negative emotions and grade level of students have a significant influence on pseudoscience beliefs, and both have a similar contribution to the variability of these beliefs.
Article
Full-text available
This study explored over 200 journals and a content-focussed research repository to investigate the occurrence of research on the Big Questions in the field of astronomy education research (AER), focussing on Cosmology and Religion. Using both qualitative and quantitative techniques, 151 articles were selected and analysed. Our results reveal that AER in the context of cosmology has increased vastly over the past few decades. Despite this, there is still a lack of research exploring the various constructs especially focussing on teacher education, curriculum, misconceptions, and the common ground between cosmology and religion. Our textual analysis of the articles revealed 16 themes, which could be encompassed into two overarching themes of “Science” and the “Universe”. We used a 13-criteria scoring rubric to analyse the constructs found in the articles. Using our scoring rubric, the top four criteria were Cosmology “Big Questions” (~ 29%), Cosmology Content (~ 22%), the Cosmology Science-Religion Debate (~ 19%), and Cosmology Attitudes (~ 18%). These results hint at a fertile landscape for research into Cosmology Education, and the Cosmology-Religion interface given the ground-breaking advances in modern Cosmology.
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated the role of astrology in self-concept verification and coping with negative life-events. Forty students in elementary courses of astrology, psychology, and German language rated their certainty about 20 self-describing attributes and three assumptions related to the self. The subjects were also asked to mark whether and how many times they had experienced particular crises and traumatic events. Participation in an astrology course, but not in psychology or German courses, verified the participant's self-concept by increasing certainty of the self-descriptive attributes and the assumptions of self-control and perceived luck. In addition, interest in astrology was positively correlated with the number of personal crises, but not with the number of past traumatic events. The motivational mechanisms underlying astrological beliefs and the enormous popularity of astrology are discussed.
Article
Skeptics' and astrology believers acceptance of generalized personality descriptions of the self was explored. Descriptions were varied in two ways: (a) They were attributed to astrology or were said to be a set of statements that might or might not be self-descriptive, and (b) they were socially desirable or undesirable. Overall, believers were more likely than skeptics to accept descriptions as accurate. Favorable descriptions were generally seen as more accurate than unfavorable descriptions, with one exception -believers accepted a negative astrological description as much as a positive one. Skeptics as well as believers rated astrological descriptions as more accurate than identical non-astrological descriptions. Finally, skeptics who received favorable horoscopes, in contrast to all other groups, became significantly more positive in their opinions toward astrology. Implications for the popularity of belief in astrology are discussed.
Article
Economic threat predicted the level of superstition in Germany for the tumultuous years 1918 to 1940. Indexes of superstition were the number of articles on astrology, mysticism, and cults appearing in a German periodical index. Threat was measured by levels of real wages, unemployment, and industrial production. The economic threat variables significantly predicted level of superstition in two of the three superstition indexes. Experimental and naturalistic evidence concerning situational determinants of superstition are reviewed. These results support Fromm's (1941/1965) thesis that threat produces authoritarianism and corroborate Sales's (1973) investigation of U.S. archival data.