ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Playback experiments were conducted in order to test for the existence of species‐specific vocal recognition in Amazonian manatees. The animals were isolated in pools while acoustic stimuli were played from a tape recorder and transmitted underwater through a loudspeaker. Nine animals were monitored for response to playback vocalizations from eighteen different individuals, nine from each species (Trichechus inunguis and T. manatus manatus). No significant differences were detected in the response of manatees exposed to the different stimuli. Only the time spent close to the speaker was greater when the animals were exposed to conspecific vocalizations (ρ=0.375). This result suggests that Amazonian manatees cannot recognize differences between their own and another manatee species’ vocalization. The methodology was also tested and no difference in the response of animals exposed to silence or to tape hiss (blank tape used as control) was found. Testing the response to the presence or absence of vocalizati...
9:50
3aABa9. Planning for a pilot census of marine life in the Gulf of
Maine: The role of acoustics. Kenneth G. Foote Woods Hole
Oceanogr. Inst., Woods Hole, MA 02543, kfoote@whoi.edu
Preparations are being made for a pilot census of marine life in the
Gulf of Maine ecosystem. The role of acoustics as a rapid, remote sensing
tool is elaborated. Potential target organisms for acoustic surveying range
from mesozooplankton and macrozooplankton to fish and cetaceans. A
number of methodological problems must be addressed. These are illus-
trated for the echo integration method as applied to a stock of Atlantic
herring Clupea harengus兲关J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 105, 995 1999兲兴. Particu-
lar problems of determining target strength and compensating for possible
behavioral effects are also general to the method. Work supported by the
Alfred P. Sloan Foundation.
10:05
3aABa10. Localizing marine animals and how marine animals might
localize sound. Gerald L. D’Spain and Paul A. Lepper Marine Physical
Lab., Scripps Inst. of Oceanogr., La Jolla, CA 92093-0704
We can locate vocalizing marine animals, and the animals themselves
might locate sound sources, in one of several ways. Time-of-arrival
phase differences and amplitude differences of a single arrival between
spatially separated ‘‘ears’’ are well-known techniques. However, they be-
come ineffective in multipath environments and as the frequency of the
sound and/or spatial separation between ears decreases. Another approach
is to sense properties of the acoustic field in addition to acoustic pressure.
This approach, based on a simple Taylor series expansion of the field and
‘‘f ma,’’ apparently is exploited by fish, making them biological equiva-
lents of DIFAR sonobuoys. However, do they also measure acoustic strain
rate? An additional method, used by humans, is to take advantage of
time-of-arrival phase differences between multipath arrivals. The infor-
mation on source location contained in the resulting interference patterns
can be understood in terms of waveguide invariants. These speculations
will be illustrated with numerical simulations and actual ocean acousic
data. Work supported by ONR.
10:20
3aABa11. Acoustic identification of female Steller sea lions. Gregory
S. Campbell Cetacean Behavior Lab., San Diego State Univ., San Diego,
CA 92182-4611, Robert Gisner Code 342, Office of Naval Res.,
Arlington, VA 22217, and David A. Helweg Code D351,
SPAWARSYSCEN San Diego, San Diego, CA 92152
Steller sea lions Eumetopias jubatus breed and rear their young in
coastal rookeries dispersed along the northern tier of the North Pacific.
Population densities are high and no allomaternal behavior occurs, provid-
ing strong selection pressure for motherpup recognition processes. Moth-
ers and pups establish and maintain contact with individually distinctive
vocalizations. Our objective is to understand the acoustic features that
serve to identify individual females, and develop a ruggedized computer
system to perform acoustic recognition of females in the field. We have
cataloged almost 2000 contact calls from 46 females in 1998 and 25 in
1999. Each female is visually identified by marking patterns, which pro-
vides the ground truth for acoustic identification. Acoustic properties of
the calls were measured and presented to several statistical classifiers.
Representations of the calls had to be robust with respect to acoustical
variability introduced by motivational changes as the mother and pup re-
gained proximity. The calls, classifiers, and results of generalization tests
will be described, and a concept for the field system will be discussed.
Research supported by ONR Research Opportunities for Program Offic-
ers award N00014-00-1-0114 to R. H. DeFran, Cetacean Behavior Labo-
ratory, SDSU.
WEDNESDAY MORNING, 6 DECEMBER 2000 SCHOONER/SLOOP ROOMS, 10:50 TO 11:55 A.M.
Session 3aABb
Animal Bioacoustics: General Topics in Bioacoustics
Lawrence F. Wolski, Chair
Hubbs-Sea World Research Institute, 2595 Ingraham Street, San Diego, California 92109
Chair’s Introduction—10:50
Contributed Papers
10:55
3aABb1. Infrasonic and low-frequency vocalizations from Siberian
and Bengal tigers. Elizabeth von Muggenthaler Fauna
Communications Res. Inst.
Tigers have many vocalizations including chuffling, growling, prusten,
gurgling, grunting, and roaring. It has been well documented that the
tiger’s high-amplitude, low-frequency roars, which are thought to be ter-
ritorial in nature C. Packer and A. E. Pusey, Sci. Am. 276, 52–59 1997兲兴
transmit for miles. It has been suggested that because some tigers inhabit
dense jungles with limited visiblity, the capacity to hear low frequency
may be beneficial for sensing and locating prey G. T. Huang, J. J.
Rosowski, and W. T. Peake, J. Comp. Physiol. A 2000兲兴. In an effort to
understand more about these low-frequency vocalizations and to provide
data to other researchers testing hearing in anesthetized felids, 22 tigers,
both Siberian and Bengal, are being recorded. A portable system can
record from 3 Hz to 22 kHz. On-site real-time analysis of vocalizations is
performed using a portable computer. Real-time and edited playback of
sonic and infrasonic tiger vocalizations is facilitated by car audio speakers
capable of producing frequencies from 10 Hz22 kHz. Initial findings
have documented fundamental frequencies of some roars at 17.50 Hz.
Other vocalizations, including chuffling, have fundamental frequencies of
35 Hz 5. Playback of both real-time and edited vocalizations appear to
illicit behavioral responses, such as roaring, from male tigers.
11:10
3aABb2. On the sound of snapping shrimp: The collapse of a
cavitation bubble. Michel Versluis, Anna von der Heydt,
a
Detlef Lohse
Dept. of Appl. Phys. and J. M. Burgers Res. Ctr. for Fluid Dynam., Univ.
of Twente, P.O. Box 217, 7500 AE Enschede, The Netherlands,and
Barbara Schmitz TU Munchen, 85747 Garching, Germany
Snapping shrimp produce a snapping sound by an extremely rapid
closure of their snapper claw. They usually occur in large numbers pro-
viding a permanent crackling background noise, thereby severely limiting
the use of underwater acoustics for active and passive sonar, both in sci-
entific and naval applications. Source levels reported for Alpheus hetero-
3a WED. AM
2541 2541J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 108, No. 5, Pt. 2, November 2000 Joint 140th Meeting ASA/NOISE-CON 2000
chaelis are as high as 220 dB peak-to-peak re 1
Pa at 1 m distance.
Recent ultra-high-speed imaging of the snapper claw closure Versluis
et al., Science in press兲兴 revealed that the sound is generated by the
collapse of a cavitation bubble formed in a fast flowing jet of water forced
out from between the claws during claw closure. In this work, we develop
a theoretical model for a bubble under such conditions. The dynamics of
the bubble radius and the emitted sound can be described by the
RayleighPlesset equation. The calculated results are compared with the
experimental data. The model fully reproduces the bubble dynamics and it
quantitatively accounts for the time dependence of the bubble radius and
for the emitted sound.
a
Also at Dept. of Physics, Philipps-Univ. Marburg,
Renthof 6, 35032 Marburg, Germany.
11:25
3aABb3. A unique way of sound production in the snapping shrimp
Alpheus heterochaelis. Barbara Schmitz Dept. of Zoology, TU
Muenchen, Lichtenbergstr. 4, 85747 Garching, Germany, Michel
Versluis, Anna von der Heydt, and Detlef Lohse Appl. Phys., Univ. of
Twente, P.O. Box 217, 7500 AE Enschede, The Netherlands
Sound production is known in more than 50, mostly stridulating, crus-
tacean genera. These acoustic signals occur in agonistic interactions as
well as for mate attraction. The mechanism of sound production in snap-
ping shrimp, which also serves to stun or even kill small prey, is especially
interesting. The current assumption was that the sound is produced by
cocking and then rapidly closing the enlarged modified snapper claw.
Snapping shrimp sounds contribute most to coastal biological noise, may
be heard up to 1 mile away, and resemble the crackling of dry twigs in fire
or the sizzle of frying fat. Recent hydrophone measurements close to
tethered shrimp Alpheus heterochaelis revealed pulse-like signals of
500-ns duration, comprising frequencies beyond 200 kHz, and showing
enormous sound pressure levels of up to 220-dB re 1
Pa peak to
peak at 1-m distance. Such high intensities are very unlikely to be pro-
duced by the mechanical contact of two claw surfaces. Ultra-high-speed
video recordings and simultaneous hydrophone measurements reveal that
claw closure results in a water jet, the high velocity of which 25 m/s
leads to the formation of a cavitation bubble, which emits the extremely
loud sound upon its collapse.
11:40
3aABb4. Lack of species-specific vocal recognition in Amazonian
manatees: Trichechus inunguis. Renata S. Sousa Lima and Vera M. F.
da Silva Laborato
´
rio de Mamı
´
feros Aqua
´
ticos, INPA, C.P. 478, Manaus,
AM, Brasil 69083-000, pboi@inpa.gov.br
Playback experiments were conducted in order to test for the existence
of species-specific vocal recognition in Amazonian manatees. The animals
were isolated in pools while acoustic stimuli were played from a tape
recorder and transmitted underwater through a loudspeaker. Nine animals
were monitored for response to playback vocalizations from eighteen dif-
ferent individuals, nine from each species Trichechus inunguis and T.
manatus manatus. No significant differences were detected in the re-
sponse of manatees exposed to the different stimuli. Only the time spent
close to the speaker was greater when the animals were exposed to con-
specific vocalizations (
0.375). This result suggests that Amazonian
manatees cannot recognize differences between their own and another
manatee species’ vocalization. The methodology was also tested and no
difference in the response of animals exposed to silence or to tape hiss
blank tape used as control was found. Testing the response to the pres-
ence or absence of vocalizations, significant differences were found in
time elapsed between the playback and the response (
0.001) and in the
time spent next to the speaker (
0.032), confirming their great ability to
perceive sounds underwater. Work supported by FBPN, MacArthur
Foundation, CI, FINEP, MCT/PPG7 and CNPq.
WEDNESDAY MORNING, 6 DECEMBER 2000 CALIFORNIA SALON 3, 7:20 A.M. TO 12:05 P.M.
Session 3aAO
Acoustical Oceanography: Special Topics
Thomas K. Berger, Cochair
Scripps Institute of Oceanography, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla, California 92093-0238
Christopher D. Jones, Cochair
Applied Physics Laboratory, University of Washington, 1013 NE 40th Street, Seattle, Washington 98105
Chair’s Introduction—7:20
Contributed Papers
7:25
3aAO1. The F factor: Ocean climatology and internal-wave acoustic
effects. Stanley M. Flatte
´
and Kimberly J. Noble Phys. Dept. and Inst.
of Marine Sci., Univ. of California at Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, CA 95064
The strength of acoustic fluctuations due to internal waves is affected
by the temperature-salinity relation of an oceanic region. We define an
acoustic fluctuation strength parameter F as the ratio of the fractional
potential-sound-speed change to the fractional potential-density change. F
is calculated at three depth levels 275, 550, and 850 m, on a one-degree
grid of latitude and longitude, using NODC/OCL’s World Ocean Atlas
1994. Representative values of F in upper waters range between 5 and 35.
Results for intermediate depths range from 5 to 60. In general, F exhibits
higher values in the Atlantic Basin than in the Indian or Pacific, and has a
maximum at 550 m. The main use of F will be the prediction of travel-
time fluctuations in acoustic propagation experiments, which will be pro-
portional to the value of F, given a universal strength of internal waves.
7:40
3aAO2. Imaging acoustic fluctuations in shallow water using
dislocation theory. D. P. Williams, G. L. D’Spain, W. S. Hodgkiss, and
W. A. Kuperman Marine Physical Lab., Scripps Inst. of Oceanogr.,
Univ. of California at San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093-0238
The distribution of the amplitude of a moderate-frequency sound field
in a shallow ocean is considered in relation to the existence of dislocations
in the phase front Nye and Berry 1974兲兴 where the amplitude is close to
zero. Phase front dislocations strictly occur when the amplitude is zero; in
actual ocean acoustic measurements, only low-field amplitudes at a
minima can be distinguished due to the interference of background noise.
2542 2542J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 108, No. 5, Pt. 2, November 2000 Joint 140th Meeting ASA/NOISE-CON 2000
... Hides were used for making machine belts, pulleys and hoses, and rest of the animal was rendered for oil and sold for meat (Best 1984 (IBAMA 2001) and in the Red List of IUCN (International Union for Conservation of Nature and Natural Resources). As a result of the inclusion of the Amazonian manatee on the Endangered Species List, much effort has gone to understand their ecology in order to aid in their conservation (Best 1981;Rosas 1994;da Silva 1999;Sousa Lima & da Silva 2000). ...
Article
Full-text available
We used mitochondrial DNA control region sequences to examine phylogeography and population differentiation of the endangered Amazonian manatee Trichechus inunguis. We observe lack of molecular differentiation among localities and we find weak association between geographical and genetic distances. However, nested clade analysis supports restricted gene flow and/or dispersal with some long-distance dispersal. Although this species has a history of extensive hunting, genetic diversity and effective population sizes are relatively high when compared to the West Indian manatee Trichechus manatus. Patterns of mtDNA haplotype diversity in T. inunguis suggest a genetic disequilibrium most likely explained by demographic expansion resulting from secession of hunting and enforcement of conservation and protective measures. Phylogenetic analysis of T. manatus and T. inunguis haplotypes suggests that T. inunguis is nested within T. manatus, effectively making T. manatus a paraphyletic entity. Paraphyly of T. manatus and recent divergence times of T. inunguis and the three main T. manatus lineages suggest a possible need for a taxonomic re-evaluation of the western Atlantic Trichechus.
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any references for this publication.