ArticlePDF Available

The Illness Perception Questionnaire: A New Method For Assessing The Cognitive Representation Of Illness

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The Illness Perception Questionnaire (IPQ) is a new method for assessing cognitive representations of illness. The IPQ is a theoretically derived measure comprising five scales that provides information about the five components that have been found to underlie the cognitive representation of illness. The five scales assess identity - the symptoms the patient associates with the illness, cause - personal ideas about aetiology, time-line - the perceived duration of the illness, consequences - expected effects and outcome and cure control - how one controls or recovers from the illness. The IPQ has a specific number of core items but allows the user to add items for particular patient groups or health threats. Data is presented supporting the reliability and validity of the IPQ scales in different chronic illness populations.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Psychology and Health, 1996, Vol. 11, pp. 43lØ5 © 1996 OPA (Overseas Publishers Association)
Reprints available directly from the Publisher Amsterdam B.V. Published in The Netherlands
Photocopying permitted by license only by Harwood Academic Publishers GmbH
Printed in Malaysia
THE ILLNESS PERCEPTION QUESTIONNAIRE:
A NEW METHOD FOR ASSESSING THE
COGNITIVE REPRESENTATION OF ILLNESS
JOHN WEINMAN
1
1
Unit of Psychology, United Medical and Dental Schools of Guy's and
St Thomas's Hospitals, London Bridge, London SEI 9RT, United Kingdom
KEITH J. PETRIE' and RONA MOSS-MORRIS
2
2
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, University of Auckland School
of Medicine, Auckland, New Zealand
ROB HORNE
3
3
The John Harris Clinical Pharmacy Unit, University of Brighton, Brighton,
United Kingdom
(Received 21 December, 1994; in final form 16 October, 1995)
The Illness Perception Questionnaire (IPQ) is a new method for assessing cognitive representations of illness. The IPQ is
a theoretically derived measure comprising five scales that provides information about the five components that have been
found to underlie the cognitive representation of illness. The five scales assess identity - the symptoms the patient
associates with the illness, cause - personal ideas about aetiology, time-line - the perceived duration of the illness,
consequences - expected effects and outcome and cure control - how one controls or recovers from the illness. The IPQ
has a specific number of core items but allows the user to add items for particular patient groups or health threats. Data is
presented supporting the reliability and validity of the IPQ scales in different chronic illness populations.
Keywords: Illness perceptions; questionnaire; reliability; validity; chronic illness; personal models.
INTRODUCTION
The onset of illness gives rise to a range of problems, which can vary greatly from patient to patient, even in those with
the same condition. In recent years health psychologists have shown that, in order to make sense of and respond to these
problems, patients create their own models or representations of their illness. The most influential theoretical framework
adopted in this work is the self-regulation model of Leventhal and colleagues, who have proposed that patients' illness
representations are based around distinct components which, in turn, determine coping (Leventhal, Nerenz and Steele,
1984; Leventhal and Diefenbach. 1991). Thus they maintain that each patient will have their own ideas about the identity,
cause, time-line, and consequences of their illness. Lau and colleagues (1989) have indicated that patients' models also
incorporate beliefs about the cure and controllability of the condition. Recent overviews of research in this area, based on
differing methodologies across a range of different clinical conditions, confirm the consistency and validity of these five
components of patients' illness representations (Skelton and Croyle, 1991).
The identity component is concerned with patients' ideas about the label, the nature of their condition (ie
associated symptoms) and the links between these. The causal component comprises the patient's ideas about the likely
cause or causes of the illness and the time-line component indicates their perceptions of the likely duration of their health
Corresponding Author: Processor Weinman
1
problems and these have been categorised as acute/short-lasting, chronic, or cyclical/episodic. The consequences
component reflects the individual's beliefs about the illness severity and likely impact on physical, social and
psychological functioning. The cure component indicates the extent to which the patient believes their condition is
amenable to cure or control. Although the components of illness representations are distinct in the sense they can have
specific effects on coping and outcomes, they are not necessarily independent. Thus there may be very direct links
between the identity and consequences components or between representations of the cause and control of an illness.
These representations come into play as soon as patients experience their initial symptoms and typically change with
disease progression, emergent symptoms and treatment responses. Leventhal proposes that these representations reflect
the patient's cognitive response to symptoms and illness, and that emotional responses are processed in parallel to illness
representations (Leventhal et al., 1984).
Although there is increasing interest in this area of work, different methodologies have been employed by
researchers (Leventhal and Nerenz, 1985). In their original work. Leventhal and colleagues used in-depth, semi-structured
interviews focusing on patients' concrete illness experiences in order to elicit their representations. While this approach
was clearly productive, it is time-consuming, produces large variations in the quantity and quality of response, and no
psychometric data have been produced in support of this methodology. In one of their studies investigating a community
sample's cognitions about six different illnesses, they have used a questionnaire to assess illness representations
(Prohaska, Leventhal, Leventhal and Keller, 1985) and other researchers have developed questionnaires for this purpose
(Lacroix, 1991), but these have not generally been theoretically derived and have not been evaluated with more than one
type of patient group. Moreover, one of these measures (Turk, Rudy and Salovey, 1986) appeared to provide data which
was inconsistent with the selfregulation model, but this was based on the ratings of contrasting diseases by patient,
student, and nursing samples rather than on patients' representations of their own illness. Also, Lau et al. (1989) were
critical of the way in which these data were analysed and concluded that the findings were quite compatible with illness
representation theory.
In view of the growing interest in patients' representations of illness, both in attempting to understanding the
nature of illness-related coping and for developing interventions to facilitate self management in chronic illness, we feel
that there is scope for developing an assessment questionnaire which is theoretically-based and psychometrically sound,
but with sufficient flexibility for the user to add items which are particularly salient for specific patient groups or in
relation to specific health threats or contexts. In this paper we describe the structure and development of a new instrument,
the Illness Perception Questionnaire (IPQ), which contains five scales that assess the components of illness
representations.
METHOD
Development of the Illness Perception Questionnaire (IPQ)
The test items were theoretically-derived to assess each of the five illness representation components. The symptom list
incorporated twelve common symptoms from other symptom checklists (eg. Bowling, 1991). The rest of the items were
either generated by patients or by us to fit with Leventhal's description of illness representation components (Leventhal
and Nerenz, 1985). The patient-generated items were elicited during preliminary interviews and the items which were
selected were those which reflected specific representations. These items and those initially generated by us were assessed
for comprehensibility in early pilot studies and a few additional changes were made to ensure that each item could be
understood clearly.
The full IPQ with instructions to subjects is shown in Appendix (1). The Identity scale is comprised of 12 core
symptom items that the patient is asked to rate for frequency on a four point scale ranging from “all of the time” to
“never” according to how often each symptom is experienced as part of the patient's illness. This core list of items may be
added to by researchers to tailor the scale to specific illnesses and lists for some illnesses are already in use including an
expanded 28 item Identity scale for CFS which includes such items as “muscle pain”, “light sensitivity” and “mild fever”
(see Moss-Morris, Petrie and Weinman, in press). The Identity scale is scored by summing the number of items endorsed
at “occasionally” or greater, so that the total score ranges from 0 to 12 for the core list. This therefore provides a simple
measure of the number of symptoms perceived to be associated with the illness. Since the alternative, weighted scoring
system is highly correlated with this, we chose the simpler method.
The items from the four other IPQ scales are presented in a mixed order and rated by the patient on a five point
scale ranging from “Strongly Disagree” to “Strongly Agree” (scored 1 to 5). After reverse scoring appropriate items,
scores for Time-line, Consequences and Cure Control scales are obtained by summing all the scales items and dividing by
the number of items. For the Cause scale it is not appropriate to sum all of the items as each item represents a specific
causal belief, although some researchers may find it appropriate to combine items for their needs (e.g. external vs internal
causal factors).
In the most general version of the IPQ, each items refers to “Illness” but it also possible to replace this with the
name of a particular illness (e.g. diabetes, asthma etc.) in studies with selected illness populations. Copies of the general
and specific versions are available from the authors. A significant other/carer version of the IPQ has also been developed
to elicit the partner's/carer's cognitive model of the patient's illness. This version may be appropriate where researchers
want to assess the illness perceptions of significant of others, caretakers or other family members. Further details about
this scale are available from the authors.
Data assessing the reliability and validity of the scales, are shown below. One additional stage of checking the
items comprised a comparison of the questionnaire with a semi-structured interview designed to elicit illness
representations, using the guidelines offered by Leventhal and Nerenz (1985). This study was conducted with a sample of
52 insulin-dependent diabetic patients and it found that all the themes mentioned in the interview were also evident in the
questionnaire data. Moreover, the questionnaire appeared to provide a more complete data set since about a third of the
interview respondents failed to provide information about some or all of the illness representations components. Some
aspects of this interview - questionnaire comparison study are also discussed in the final section of this paper.
PARTICIPANTS
Data from 7 illness groups provide the basis for evaluating the psychometric properties of the IPQ scales. The diabetes
and rheumatoid arthritis populations were recruited from a London teaching hospital out-patient facility. The renal
patients were all undergoing renal dialysis and the asthma patients had all been in-patients at the same London hospital.
Chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS) patients were contacted through a New Zealand national support group on the basis that
they reported a confirmed medical diagnosis of CFS. Chronic pain patients were recruited from a private Auckland pain
clinic where they were seeing an anaesthetist pain specialist. The myocardial infarction (MI) patients were involved in a
large prospective study of recovery from first-time MI in Auckland. The demographic and other characteristics of these
samples are presented in Table (1).
Table
1 Characteristics of illness samples used in the IPQ studies
Hospitalized Discharged Discharged Chronic Rheumatoid Diabetes Pain Renal Asthma
MI Patients Ml Patients Ml Patients Fatigue Arthritis
Syndrome
N 143 104 91 115 22 88 60 32 193
Sex (% Female) 1 %F 13%F 12%F 73% F 81 %F 48%F 40%F 41 %F 66%F
Age (Mean, SD) 53.0 (8.5) 53.8 (8.2) 53.5(8.1) 48.2(1 2.6) 62.2(16.6) 45.6(15.9) 42.2(13.9) 48.3(15.6) 37.5(13.3)
2-5 days 3 months 6 months 11.8 years 14.5 years 15.2 years 3.8 years 9.3 years 16.8 years Average length
of illness
RESULTS
(1) Reliability and scale intercorrelation
Data collected from the myocardial infarction and renal samples show the IPQ scales to have good levels of both internal
consistency and test-retest reliability. These data are represented in Table (2) As would be expected, both the Control
Cure and Consequences scales have higher levels of test-retest reliability than the Identity and Time-line scales as patients'
perceptions of the consequences and cure of their illness are less likely to change over time.
The intercorrelations between the IPQ scales are shown in Table (3) and demonstrate logical relationships. Patients with a
stronger illness identity are more likely to perceive their illness as lasting longer and having more serious consequences.
Patients with higher Time-line score are less likely to see their illness as potentially controllable or curable and to have
more severe personal consequences.
Table 2
Internal consistency (Cronbach Alpha) and re-test reliability scores for IPQ scales
SCALE Alpha
1
One Month
2
Three Month
1
Six Month
1
Identity .82 .84** .34** .06
Timeline .73 .49* .5 I ** .36**
Consequences .82 .68 * * .55 * * .55 *
Control/Cure .73 .68 * * .54 * * .46*
Note : *p < .01, **p < .001
1
MI sample
3
Renal sample
Table 3
Intercorrelations between IPQ scales in the MI sample (N = 143)
SCALES 1 2 3
Identity
Timeline .20*
Consequences .26** .52***
Control/Cure -.07 -.32*** -.09
Note: *P < .05, **P < .0l, ***P < .001
(2) Concurrent Validity
Data from the MI sample at three months post-MI provided the opportunity to test the concurrent validity of the IPQ
scales. The correlations between other measures of perceived health and disability as well as recent doctor visits and
beliefs about recovery are shown in Table (4). As would be expected, the Identity Scale is positively related to current
reported disability, as measured by the Sickness Impact Profile (Bergner, Bobbit, Carter and Gilson, 1981), and recent
doctor visits. It is also inversely related to self-rated health and patients' own ratings of control over their heart problems.
A higher Time-line score, indicating a belief that illness will last a long time, is positively correlated with patients' ratings
of the likelihood of a future heart attack, as well as health distress and with recent visits to the doctor. Time-line scores are
negatively correlated with perceived control over heart disease and self-rated health. The Control Cure scale is
significantly related to scores on the Recovery Self-Efficacy scale (Partridge and Johnston, 1989) and with patients'
perceived control over their heart disease. It is also negatively related to the likelihood of a future MI. Scores on the
Consequences scale are positively related to ratings of health distress, disability, the perceived likelihood of a future heart
attack and recent doctor visits. Moreover they are significantly negatively related to self-rated health and perceived
control over heart disease (see Table 4).
Table 4
Correlations of IPQ scales with other measures in MI sample (N = 104)
Identity Timeline Consequences Control/Cure
SIP total disability score
1
.54*** .25* .34*** -.14
Recent doctor visits
2
.31 ** .17 .21 * .05
Health Distress
3
.32** .23* .53*** -.03
Recovery Self Efficacy
4
.01 -.18* -.14 .38***
Self-rated health
5
-.55*** -.29** -.52** .15
Likelihood of future MI
6
.24* .42**”' .36*** -.28**
Control over heart problems
7
-.30** -.38*** -.39*** .42***
Note : *P < .03, **P < .01, ***P < .001
1
Sickness Impact Profile (Bergner, Bobbitt. Carter and Gilson, 1981)
2
Last 3 months apart from scheduled check-up
3
Health Distress Scale
4
Recovery Locus of Control Scale (Partridge and Johnston, 1989)
5
Rated on a 7 point scale from terrible to excellent
6
In next 12 months, rated on a 7 point scale from very low to very high
7
Rated on a 7 point scale from absolutely no control to extreme amount of control
We have also obtained data from patients with severe asthma, comparing their Control Cure scores on the IPQ with scores
on an asthma specific version of the Multidimensional Health Locus of Control Scale (MHLC, Wallston, Wallston and
DeVellis, 1978). This showed a significant positive correlation with the MHLC `Internal' scale (r = 0.44; p < 0.001), a
significant negative correlation with the MHLC `chance' scale (r = -0.37; p < 0.001) and no correlation with the `Powerful
Others' scale. Thus the pattern of these correlations provides further support for the concurrent validity of the Control
Cure scale.
(3) Discriminant Validity
To assess the extent to which the IPQ could discriminate between different illnesses we compared the IPQ scale scores in .
patients with insulin dependent diabetes, rheumatoid arthritis (RA), chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS) and chronic
idiopathic pain. Each of these illnesses has distinct presentations and effects on patients' lives. Other common features of
these illnesses make them particularly relevant to the concept of illness representations. The precipitating factors and
aetiology of all four chronic illnesses are unclear and most treatments emphasise management of symptoms rather than
cure. Here, patients' beliefs are likely to have an important impact on their adjustment to their illness. We were
specifically interested to see whether, despite uncertain medical explanations for the illnesses, the IPQ scale could help
identify distinct patient illness beliefs.
Due to differences in sample sizes, tests for homogeneity of variance were conducted on the length of illness and
the age of the subjects. The groups were homogenous for age (Cochran's C = .30, p = .42), but not length of illness
(Cochran's C = .41, p <01). As would be expected, length of illness was significantly correlated with the IPQ Timeline
Scale (r = .29, p < .O1) but not with any of the other IPQ Scales.
A series of one-way analyses of variance, followed by post-hoc Scheffe tests were conducted on the IPQ scales
to assess group differences in illness cognitions. The results of these analyses are presented in the figure and in Tables (5)
and (6). Comparing the CFS and pain groups on the IPQ scales, CFS patients had a significantly stronger illness identity,
belief in serious consequences and chronic time-line. CFS patients' illness identity and serious consequences beliefs were
also significantly higher than those of diabetic patients. RA patients' illness beliefs were distinguished from those with
chronic pain by a strong illness identity and chronic time-line, which is consistent with an illness that is characterised by
severe joint pain and swelling for which there is no cure. RA patients also had significantly higher identity scores than
diabetic patients.
Table 5
IPQ scale mean scores (SD) in diabetes, rheumatoid arthritis, CFS and pain patients
COMPONENT Diabetes Rheumatoid CFS Pain F'
Illness identity 6.56(3.11)
a,b
9.05(1.84)
a,c
10.42(2.00)
b,d
7.02(2.9)
c,d
37.53***
Consequences 3.15(1.00)
a
3.62(0.97) 4.10(0.75)
a,b
3.57(0.89)
b
13.34***
Timeline 4.24(0.66)
a,b
3.95(0,71)
c
3.70(0.71)
b,d
3,29((1.81)
a,c,d
18.84***
Control/Cure 3.55(1.51) 3.41(0.04) 3.38(0.59) 3.56(0.58) 1.79
Note
a,b,c,d
denotes pairs of groups different at 0.05 level Scheffe test
*P < .05, **P < .01, ***P < .001
Table 6
Illness attribution mean scores (SD) in diabetes, rheumatoid arthritis, CFS and pain patients
ATTRIBUTION Diabetes Rheumatoid CFS Pain F
Germ or virus 2.45(1.03)
a,b
2.48(1.08)
c,d
3.85(0.84)
a,c,e
1.70(0.6)
b,d,e
76.97***
Diet 2.35(1.13)
a
2.24(0.94) 2.24(0.96) 1.82(0.68)
a
3.61**
Pollution 1.79(0.78)
a
2.05(0.92)
b
3.03(0.99)
a,b,c
1.70(0.5)
c
44.68***
Genetics 2.77(1.13)
a,b
2.57(1.21) 2.19(0.95)
a
2.30(0.75)
b
9.19***
Chance 2.91(1.30) 3.00(1.05) 2.84(1.24) 2.44(1.21) 2.01
Stress 2.88(1.24)
a
2.91(1.06) 2.83(1.2) 2.30(1.03)
a
3.41*
My own behaviour 2.12(1.11)
a
2.09(0.87)
b
1.67(0.90)
c
2.88(1.24)
a,b,c
14.90***
Other peoples
behaviour
2.01(1.11) 2.09(1.02) 2.42(0.99) 1.97(1.34) 2.17
Note :
a,b,c,d,e
denotes pairs of groups different at .05 level Scheffe test
*P < .05, **P < .01, ***P < .001
The IPQ Cause scale showed CFS patients to endorse most strongly “a virus” or “pollution” as causes for their
illness, with “my own behaviour” being the least favoured attribution. These findings are consistent with other studies that
have shown that most CFS patients make external physical attributions for their illness, rather than internal ones (Powell,
Dolan, Wessely, 1990; Ray Weir, Cullen and Phillips, 1992). In contrast, pain patients made significantly fewer
attributions to viruses and pollution than CFS patients, and were most likely to attribute their condition to their behaviour.
This reflects the fact that most pain conditions are triggered by an accident or injury. Pain patients also made significantly
fewer stress attributions when compared to diabetic patients, which is consistent with chronic pain patients' reluctance to
adopt psychological explanations for their condition. Diabetic patients selected a genetic cause for their illness
significantly more than CFS and pain patients. The RA group were less likely than chronic pain patients to attribute their
illness to their own behaviour and more likely to make viral attributions.
These results show that IPQ scale scores distinguished clearly between chronic pain patients and RA patients
illness beliefs, even though
the central symptom of both
these disorders is extreme
pain. CFS was also clearly
differentiated from the other
three illness groups, by having
the most negative illness
beliefs and distinct
attributions.
PREDICTIVE VALIDITY
The predictive validity of the
IPQ scales was examined by
looking at the correlations
between the IPQ scale scores
of the MI sample in hospital
with a subsequent follow-up
assessment at three and six
months. These correlations are
presented in Table (7) and
show the Identity scale to be
significantly negatively
related with self-rated health
at three months follow-up but
not at six months. Time-line
beliefs at baseline were
significantly related to both
three and six month ratings of
the likelihood of a further MI
and the baseline Control Cure
scores were significantly
related to patients' three and
six month ratings of control
over their heart problem and
were negatively related to
perceived likelihood of future
MI at both time points. The
baseline Consequences scale
score was significantly
negatively related to self-rated
health at three months and
positively related to perceived
likelihood of future MI at six
months.
Table 7
Predictive validity of IPQ scales: Correlations of baseline IPQ scale scores with measures at the three
and six month follow-up in the MI sample
Identity Timeline Consequences Control/Cure
Three months follow-up (N = 104)
Self-rated health -.24* -.05 -.28* -.06
Doctor visits in past 3 months .14 .15 .21 * -.07
Likelihood of future MI .06 .30* .19 -.27**
Control over heart problems .10 -.02 .16 .35***
Six months follow-up (N = 115)
Self-rated health -.07 -.12 -.18 .10
Doctor visits in past 3 months .00 -.10 -.11 -.08
Likelihood of future MI .02 .26** .23* .20*
Control over heart problems -.1 I -.07 -.17 .25**
Note : *P < .05, **P < .01, ***P < .001
In another paper we have also examined the relation between baseline illness perceptions in MI patients and subsequent
attendance for cardiac rehabilitation and return to work (Petrie, Weinman, Sharpe and Buckley, submitted). These results
show that rehabilitation attendance was significantly related to a stronger belief during hospitalisation that the illness
could be cured or controlled. Return to work within six weeks was significantly related to lower initial scores on the
time-line and consequences subscales. Additional data is needed to assess a wider range of predictive validity in other
illness groups, not only with subsequent questionnaire scores but also with important behavioural and social outcomes,
such as the resumption of work and other activities, at longer intervals following the onset of illness.
SIGNIFICANT OTHER IPQ VERSION
A significant other version has been tested on a group of 50 MI patients and their spouses. Significant intercorrelations
between the IPQ scales administered to MI patients and those administered to their spouses were found for the
Consequences scale (r = .27, p < .05), the Control Cure scale (r = .30, P < .05) and Time-lime (r = .31, p < .05). The
Identity scale which asks the spouse to identify the symptoms that are part of their partner's illness did not reach
significance. Inevitably there is considerable variance in the level of agreement between patients' and significant others'
illness representations and we feel that this could provide useful insights into the role of careers and others in the recovery
process.
DISCUSSION
In this paper we have outlined the development and evaluation of a new questionnaire for assessing patients'
representations of illness. The questionnaire was specifically constructed to assess the five components of illness
representation described in Leventhal's self-regulation model (Leventhal et al. 1984). Since further work is needed to
establish the psychometric status of the IPQ, particularly to provide normative data and internal consistency indices for
different populations, the present conclusions should be treated with caution. The data obtained so far indicate that the
internal consistency and test-retest reliability of the separate scales are encouraging. The subscales vary in the number of
items and the scoring system was developed to produce comparable score ranges. However, the two scales with less items
have lower internal consistency and an increase in the number of time line and cure/control items might be worthwhile.
However, with the time line items, there are a limited number of ways to express these and they could become rather
repetitive for patients if another 3 or 4 items are added.
The one month test-retest reliability coefficients in the renal sample are generally high, as would be expected in
a group of patients with a relatively stable, chronic condition. In contrast, the three and six month test-retest correlation
coefficients were progressively lower and this is not surprising since self regulation theory proposes that illness
representations may change over time. This is especially likely if the illness is unstable or if the “profile” of the illness
changes. These test-retest results were obtained from a group of patients who had experienced a first myocardial infarction
and it is reasonable to anticipate that their representations may change, especially during the six month period after their
heart attack. In particular one might expect to see changes in the identity and time-line components as the patient's illness
experience changes For example, the initial heart attack might generate an acute time-line with a clear identity derived
from the experience of symptoms such as pain, nausea and breathlessness. As the patient recovers from this acute MI, the
illness experience is more likely to be that of a chronic condition which may be asymptomatic or with intermittent
symptomatology (e.g. chest pain on exertion). Thus these changes in MI patients' representations may explain the lower
retest correlations on the identity and time line subscales obtained at the three and six month assessment.
The data on the concurrent, discriminative and predictive validity of the IPQ are also encouraging. Expected
correlations were obtained between IPQ scales and established measures of disability, coping, self-rated health status and
health distress. Self regulation theory predicts that illness representations would be directly associated with coping and,
via this, with other outcomes such as mood and disability. In this model coping is a mediating factor between illness
representations and outcome. We have certainly found associations IPQ scale scores and coping in patients with CFS
(Moss-Morris et al., in press) and in patients with a recent MI. However, in the study of CFS patients we found that IPQ
scale scores were more strongly predictive of levels of distress and disability than were coping scores. This indicates that
illness representations may well have direct effects on mood and adjustment which are not mediated by coping. Similar
findings have been obtained in recent studies of illness representations in chronic illnesses (Earll, 1994) and careers of
stroke patients (Martin, 1994).
Much of the early data on illness representations was obtained during interviews in which patients were invited
to speak openly about their illness (Leventhal and Nerenz, 1985). Open or semi-structured interviews provide qualitative
information which may facilitate a deeper understanding of the individual's representations and experience of illness.
However, such data is time consuming and expensive to collect and analyse which severely limits the sample size of most
qualitative studies. In contrast, a questionnaire method for eliciting illness representations facilitates investigation of
illness perceptions in larger samples. Additional support for concurrent validation of the IPQ was obtained from a study,
mentioned earlier, in which 52 diabetic patients received a structured interview as well the IPQ. Half the patients were
interviewed prior to the IPQ and the other half were asked to complete the IPQ immediately before being interviewed.
There was a close fit between interview and questionnaire data and no order effects were found for the content of IPQ
responses; the pattern of IPQ responses did not differ between those received the questionnaire followed by interview and
those who were interviewed prior to receiving the questionnaire. However, patients found the IPQ easier to complete
when they had been interviewed first, even though the data from the latter was not always complete. We believe that this
might be due to a “priming effect” since the interview may have served to activate the appropriate illness schemata, hence
facilitating the subsequent completion of the questionnaire. This finding is similar to that of Bishop and colleagues who
have demonstrated how illness prototypes can be primed, thereby speeding up subsequent response times on a related task
(Bishop, 1991). In practical terms, this suggests that if there is sufficient time, then the administration of the IPQ may be
preceded by a brief interview focusing on the patient's ideas about the nature, cause, time line, controllability and
consequences of their illness.
Users may wish to amend the IPQ to include items which are specific to particular illness or patient groups. For
example, in one of our studies of patients with CFS, we included thirteen additional symptom items in the identity scale,
as well as including additional “consequences” and “cause” items which were particularly relevant to this group (Moss
Morris et al. in press). Similarly we have devised equivalent versions with slightly modified wording to assess spouse's or
career’s perceptions of the patient's illness (eg. Martin, 1994).
In conclusion, we believe that the IPQ could have considerable scope in health psychology research. There is
growing interest in patients' representations of their illness for understanding the psychological impact of illness (e.g.
Skelton and Croyle, 1991), for explaining patterns of care seeking and adherence to treatment advice (Cameron, Leventhal
and Leventhal 1993; Leventhal and Cameron, 1987) and examining the responses to psychological interventions,
particularly those with a cognitive-behavioural focus (Pimm, Byron, Curson and Weinman, 1994). The IPQ is a simple,
flexible method for assessing illness representations and we hope that it will prove to be of value in facilitating further
research into illness perceptions.
Acknowledgements
We would like to acknowledge a number of colleagues who played important roles in different stages of the development
of the test. In London, Romilla Bahl and Shereen Rahman collected early interview data and Karla Theil completed the
test-retest study with the renal patient sample. Also, discussions with John Pimm have proved very useful for sharpening
our ideas about the measurement properties of the IPQ. In Auckland, thanks are also due to Deanna Buick, Sarah Bennett
and Jude Buckley.
APPENDIX (1)
Illness Identity
(Core symptom list)
(please indicate how frequently you now experience the following symptoms as part of your (illness)
Rated : all of the time, frequently, occasionally, never
Pain
Nausea
Breathlessness
Weight Loss
Fatigue
Stiff Joints
Sore Eyes
Headaches
Upset Stomach
Sleep Difficulties
Dizziness
Loss of Strength
We are interested in your own personal views of how you now see your (illness).'
Please indicate how much you agree or disagree with the following statements
about your illness.
Rated : Strongly agree, agree, neither agree nor disagree, disagree, strongly disagree
Cause
A germ or virus caused my illness
Diet played a major role in causing my illness Pollution of the environment caused my illness My illness is hereditary - it
runs in my family It was just by chance that I became ill
Stress was a major factor in causing my illness My illness is largely due to my own behaviour Other people played a large
role in causing my illness My illness was caused by poor medical care in the past* My state of mind played a major part
in causing my illness*
*(N.B. The last two cause items have been added since some the earlier studies and hence do not appear in Table 6).
Time-line
My illness will last a short time
My illness is likely to be permanent rather than temporary
My illness will last for a long time
Consequences
My illness is a serious condition
My illness has had a major consequences on my life.
My illness has become easier to live with OO
My illness has not had much effect on my life. O
My illness has strongly affected the way others see me
My illness has serious economic and financial consequences
My illness has strongly affected the way I see myself as a person
Control/Cure
My illness will improve in time
There is a lot which I can do to control my symptoms
There is very little that can be done to improve my illness O
My treatment will be effective in curing my illness
Recovery from my illness is largely dependent on chance or fate O
What I do can determine whether my illness gets better or worse
® = Reversed Scoring
References
Bergner, M., Bobbitt, R.A., W.B. and Gilson, B.S. (1981) The Sickness Impact Profile: Development and final revision of
a health status measure. Medical Care, 19, 787-805.
Bishop, G.D. (1991) Understanding the understanding of illness: lay disease representations. In J.A. Skelton and R.T.
Croyle (Eds), Mental Representation in Health and Illness, p.p. 32-60. New York: SpringerVerlag.
Bowling. A. (1991) Measuring Health. Milton Keynes: Open University Press.
Cameron, L., Leventhal, E.A. and Leventhal, H. (1993) Symptom representations and affect as determinants of care
seeking in a community dwelling adult sample population. Health Psychology, 12, 171-179.
Earll, M.L. (1994) Coping with Chronic Neurological Illness: an Analysis using Self-regulation theory. Unpublished PhD
thesis, University of London.
Lacroix, J.M. (1991) Assessing illness schemata in patient populations in J.A. Skelton and R.T. Croyle (Eds) Mental
Representation in Health and Illness, p.p. 193-219. New York: Springer Verlag.
Lau, R, Bernard J.M. and Hartman, K.A. (1989) Further explorations of common sense representations of common
illness. Health Psychology, 8, 195-219.
Leventhal, H. and Cameron, L. (1987) Behavioural theories and the problems of compliance. Patient Education and
Counselling, 10, 117-138.
Leventhal, H. and Diefenbach, M. (1991) The active side of illness cognition In J. A. Skelton and R. T. Croyle (Eds)
Mental Representation in Health and Illness, pp.247-272. New York: Springer Verlag.
Leventhal. H. and Nerenz D. (1985) The assessment of illness cognition. In P Karoly (Ed) Measurement Strategies in
Health Psychology, pp. 517-555. New York: Wiley.
Leventhal, H., Nerenz, D.R. and Steele, D.J. (1984) Illness representations and coping with health threats. In A. Baum,
and S.E. Taylor and J.E. Singer (Eds) Handbook of Psychology and Health, Volume IV: Social Psychological Aspects
of Health. pp. 219-252. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Martin, E. (1994) Factors associated with distress in carers of stroke patients. Unpublished BSc. thesis, University of
London.
Moss-Morris, R., Petrie, K.J. and Weinman, J. Functioning in Chronic Fatigue Syndrome: do illness perceptions play a
regulatory role? British Journal of Health Psychology, in press.
Partridge, C. and Johnston, M. (1989) Perceived control of recovery from physical disability: measurement and
prediction. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 28, 53-59.
Petrie, K., Weinman, J., Sharpe, N. and Buckley, J. Predicting return to work and function following myocardial
infarction: The role of the patient's view of their illness Paper Submitted for publication.
Pimm, T.J., Byron, M.A., Curson, D. and Weinman, J. (1994) Personal illness models and the self management of
arthritis. Arthritis and Rheumatism, 37, 5.358 (abstract).
Powell, R., Dolan, R. and Wessely, S. (1990) Attribution and self esteem in depression and chronic fatigue syndrome.
Journal of Psychomatic Research, 34, 665-673.
Prohaska, T.R., Leventhal, E.A., Leventhal, H. and Keller, M.L. (1985) Health practices and illness cognition in young,
middle aged and elderly adults Journal of Gerontology, 40, 569-578.
Ray, C., Weir, W.R.C, Cullen, S. and Phillips, S. (1992) Illness perception and symptom components in chronic fatigue
syndrome. Journal of Psychomatic Research, 36, 243-256.
Skelton, J.A. and Croyle, R.T. (1991) (Eds) Mental Representation in Health and Illness. New York: Springer-Verlag
Stewart, A.L. and Ware, J.E. (1992) Measuring function and well-being. Durham: Duke University Press.
Turk, D.C., Rudy, T.E. and Salovey. P. Implicit Models of Illness. Journal of Behavioural Medicine, 9, 453-474 (1986)
Wallston, K.A., Wallston, B.A. and DeVellis, R. (1978) Development of the multidimensional health locus of control
(MHLC) scales. Health Education Monographs, 6, 161-170.
... The Illness Perception Questionnaire (IPQ) was developed by Weinman et al. (1996) and revised by Moss-Morris et al. (2002). The revised questionnaire consists of three dimensions: symptoms (identity), perceptions, and reasons. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction This study aims to address the positive changes due to traumatic experiences, such as being diagnosed with cancer and experiencing this disease for a certain period. Within this purpose, socio-demographic and disease-related variables, coping ways and illness perceptions that affect posttraumatic growth in cancer patients were examined. Secondly, the findings of this study, which is one of the first studies on posttraumatic growth in cancer patients in Turkey, were compared with the findings of current studies on the subject. Method Datums were collected by an interview form and three scales (Posttraumatic Growth Scale, Ways of Coping Inventory and Illness Perception Scale-R) to 78 cancer outpatients in Istanbul University Oncology Institute in 2007. Results Results showed that cancer patients have higher posttraumatic growth levels than the mean. According to analysis, posttraumatic growth total score between confrontive coping ( t = −2.344, p < 0.05), self-controlling ( t = −3.704, p < 0.001), accepting responsibility ( t = −3.032, p < 0.01), escape-avoidance ( t = −2.285, p < 0.05), planful problem solving ( t = −2.502, p < 0.05), positive reappraisal ( t = −5.241, p < 0.001), and seeking social support ( t = −3.527, p < 0.01) has relationship. Also, there is a relation between posttraumatic growth subscales and the Revised form of Illness Perception Questionnaire; Change in relationships with others subscale ( t = 2.887, p < 0.01) and Change in self-perception subscale ( t = 2.660, p < 0.01) between timeline (acute/chronic), Change in self-perception subscale between timeline (cyclical) ( t = −2.788, p < 0.01) and uncontrollable body factors ( t = −1.916, p < 0.05) Change in philosophy of life subscale between external attributions ( t = −2.057, p < 0.05) and Change in relationships with others subscale ( t = −2.920, p < 0.01) between chance factors. It was found that positive reappraisal ( F = 78.290, p < 0.001), self-controlling ( F = 39.814, p < 0.001), and distancing ( F = 46.311, p < 0.001) were significant predictors of posttraumatic growth total score. Results showed that ways of coping and illness perceptions were essential variables in posttraumatic growth. Discussion Studies on posttraumatic growth in Turkey and the world have significantly increased in recent years. This study aimed to examine the findings obtained from cancer patients in 2007 in discussion with the findings in the current literature. In this context, it is seen that the relevant variables affecting posttraumatic growth in cancer patients in different cultures do not change.
... IPQ was developed by Weinmann et al. (1996) to measure the components of illness representations and the revised IPQ (IPQ-R) was presented by Moss-Morris et al. (5,13) IPQ-R was adapted to Turkish population and the reliability and validity of the scale was tested by Kocaman et al. (14) IPQ-R was composed of three subscales, namely illness identity, views about illness and the causes of illness. Scores of the subscales were evaluated separately. ...
Article
Importance In primary chronic back pain (CBP), the belief that pain indicates tissue damage is both inaccurate and unhelpful. Reattributing pain to mind or brain processes may support recovery. Objectives To test whether the reattribution of pain to mind or brain processes was associated with pain relief in pain reprocessing therapy (PRT) and to validate natural language–based tools for measuring patients’ symptom attributions. Design, Setting, and Participants This secondary analysis of clinical trial data analyzed natural language data from patients with primary CBP randomized to PRT, placebo injection control, or usual care control groups and treated in a US university research setting. Eligible participants were adults aged 21 to 70 years with CBP recruited from the community. Enrollment extended from 2017 to 2018, with the current analyses conducted from 2020 to 2022. Interventions PRT included cognitive, behavioral, and somatic techniques to support reattributing pain to nondangerous, reversible mind or brain causes. Subcutaneous placebo injection and usual care were hypothesized not to affect pain attributions. Main Outcomes and Measures At pretreatment and posttreatment, participants listed their top 3 perceived causes of pain in their own words (eg, football injury, bad posture, stress); pain intensity was measured as last-week average pain (0 to 10 rating, with 0 indicating no pain and 10 indicating greatest pain). The number of attributions categorized by masked coders as reflecting mind or brain processes were summed to yield mind-brain attribution scores (range, 0-3). An automated scoring algorithm was developed and benchmarked against human coder–derived scores. A data-driven natural language processing (NLP) algorithm identified the dimensional structure of pain attributions. Results We enrolled 151 adults (81 female [54%], 134 White [89%], mean [SD] age, 41.1 [15.6] years) reporting moderate severity CBP (mean [SD] intensity, 4.10 [1.26]; mean [SD] duration, 10.0 [8.9] years). At pretreatment, 41 attributions (10%) were categorized as mind- or brain-related across intervention conditions. PRT led to significant increases in mind- or brain-related attributions, with 71 posttreatment attributions (51%) in the PRT condition categorized as mind- or brain-related, as compared with 22 (8%) in control conditions (mind-brain attribution scores: PRT vs placebo, g = 1.95 [95% CI, 1.45-2.47]; PRT vs usual care, g = 2.06 [95% CI, 1.57-2.60]). Consistent with hypothesized PRT mechanisms, increases in mind-brain attribution score were associated with reductions in pain intensity at posttreatment (standardized β = −0.25; t 127 = −2.06; P = .04) and mediated the effects of PRT vs control on 1-year follow-up pain intensity (β = −0.35 [95% CI, −0.07 to −0.63]; P = .05). The automated word-counting algorithm and human coder-derived scores achieved moderate and substantial agreement at pretreatment and posttreatment (Cohen κ = 0.42 and 0.68, respectively). The data-driven NLP algorithm identified a principal dimension of mind and brain vs biomechanical attributions, converging with hypothesis-driven analyses. Conclusions and Relevance In this secondary analysis of a randomized trial, PRT increased attribution of primary CBP to mind- or brain-related causes. Increased mind-brain attribution was associated with reductions in pain intensity.
Article
Full-text available
The aim of the current systematic review is to examine relationships among illness identity and illness-specific variables, adherence, and health-related outcomes. Studies were included if they (a) presented quantitative data on illness identity’s relationship with adherence or health-related outcomes, (b) included chronic medical illness samples, (c) were peer-reviewed, and (d) were available in English. PubMed and EBSCOhost were searched. Quality was evaluated using the EPHPP Tool. Twelve papers were included. Moderate evidence supports the relationship between engulfment, enrichment, and illness complexity. Moderate evidence supports relationships between multiple identities and adherence as well as with various health-related outcomes. There is somewhat consistent evidence for associations between engulfment and negative health-related outcomes. It may be important to inform healthcare providers of possible identity challenges that patients face and their associations with adherence and health-related outcomes. Routine illness identity screening may allow for identification of individuals who would benefit from increased support.
Article
Background The Brief Illness Perception Questionnaire (Brief IPQ) is a widely used multifactorial scale that assesses the individuals’ perceptions of illness. Although there are studies investigating the psychometric properties of the Brief IPQ in many languages, the Turkish version of Brief IPQ on periodontal diseases has not been revealed so far. This study aimed to evaluate the Turkish validity and reliability of the Brief IPQ and contribute to the literature. It is also aimed to evaluate the patients’ illness perception with periodontal disease and to reveal the possible effects of the disease on the patients’ daily life. Methods This cross-sectional study was conducted with 209 patients with periodontal diseases (137 gingivitis and 72 periodontitis cases). Sociodemographic characteristics and clinical periodontal measurements of all patients were recorded. The Turkish versions of the Brief IPQ and the HAD Scale were applied to the patients via face-to-face. The construct validity was determined using confirmatory factor analysis. Test–retest reliability and internal consistency were performed using ICC test and Cronbach’s alpha, respectively. The concurrent validity was determined by using Spearman’s correlation coefficient. Results The confirmatory factor analysis showed that the scale has one factor. The Spearman’s correlation coefficient results were found 0.843 and 0.854 for concurrent validity. Cronbach’s alpha value of the scale was 0.944 in the internal consistency analysis. ICC value was found to be 0.987 for test-retest reliability. Floor/ceiling effects were considered not to be present. Conclusions It was found that the Turkish version of The Brief Illness Perception Questionnaire is valid and reliable. Brief IPQ may be used to determine the illness perception in patients with periodontal diseases.
Article
Objective: To investigate the prognostic value of illness perception (IP) on knee pain, quality of life (QoL) and functional level in elderly individuals reporting knee pain. Design: A prospective cohort study of 1552 elderly with knee pain comparing two previously established clusters based on the Brief Illness Perception questionnaire. Cluster 1 ("Concerned optimists" [hypothesized unfavorable profile]; n = 642) perceived their knee pain as a greater threat to them than Cluster 2 ("Unconcerned confident" [hypothesized favorable profile]; n = 910). Primary outcome was the change from baseline to year 2 in the KOOS Pain subscale. Secondary outcomes were changes from baseline in quality of life (EuroQol-5 Domain and EQ VAS) and in the KOOS subscales Symptom, Activities of Daily Living, Knee-related QoL and Sports and recreation. Analyses were done on the original Intention-To-Survey (ITS) population, using repeated measures mixed linear models. Results: Among the ITS population, 841 (54%) responded to the 2-year survey. There was a statistically significant but clinically irrelevant cluster difference in the 2-year change from baseline in KOOS pain (mean difference: 6.0 KOOS points [95% CI: 7.3 to -4.7]) explained by a minor improvement in Cluster 1: (6.2 points) and no changes in Cluster 2: (0.2 points). Comparable results were found across the secondary outcomes. Clinically irrelevant cluster changes in IP were seen. Conclusion: In a cohort of people with knee pain, IP phenotype (i.e., Clusters) were of no prognostic value for the 2-year changes in pain, function, and QoL. Targeting IP may not be relevant in this patient population. Trial registration number and date of registration: The Frederiksberg Cohort study was pre-registered at clinicaltrials.gov (NCT03472300) on March 21, 2018.
Article
Stroke, as a disease, describes a group of disorders characterized by the presence of central nervous system symptoms either as a result of ischemia (ischemic stroke) or bleeding (hemorrhagic stroke). The appearance of a stroke results in a permanent physical or cognitive disability. The stroke incidence is the third cause of death after heart disease and cancer, and is the main cause of long-term disability. The effects of a stroke on a patient’s daily life, and hence on his quality of life, are intense and long-lasting. These include memory problems, speech difficulty, depression, reduced vision loss, and decreased walking ability. This limitation of the patient’s motor activity has a direct negative impact on the quality of his life. To investigate the degree of this impact, a research was carried out at a hospital of Central Greece. The total sample consisted of 90 patients and the responses showed that post-stroke symptoms are significant. Consequences and treatment control of the disease on the life of the sample were the questions with the highest score showing the significant effect that a stroke has on life the patients. More specifically, the sample showed through responses that the disease affects their lives to a great extent. The pre-stroke scores on the domains of the sample are clearly higher than post-stroke. The highest difference was found in the use of the upper extremities in self-care and family roles while the lowest was found in the domain of thinking. The most affected domains were as follows: thinking, vision, and language. Alternatively, the least affected domain was family roles.
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter has three objectives. The first is to provide a few historical details respecting the development of illness cognition research in our laboratory. As these activities extended over three decades, our brief comments may help the interested reader to integrate several lines of research that are frequently viewed as separate. The second, primary objective is to advance a constructivist view of behavioral processes. We believe people are active problem solvers and work in illness cognition must identify the procedures that people use to elaborate and test their illness models. If taken seriously, this theme can generate new directions for conceptual and empirical development. Our third, final objective is to highlight a few of the points made in prior chapters suggesting how they relate to the directions we now envision.
Article
Full-text available
Capsule presentations are given of the 5 major theoretical approaches to compliance research (Biomedical; Behavioral — Operant and Social Learning; Communication; Rational Decision — Health Belief and Reasoned Action; Self-Regulative Systems) and brief summaries made of their respective contributions and deficits. Suggestions are made for integrating the Biomedical, Behavioral and Rational Decision models into the Self-Regulative Systems Model. The advantages for doing so include completeness of understanding and improved interventions; better integration of the natural history of illness with the individual's perception, understanding, and strategies for coping with illness; recognition of the separate contributions of automatic (habitual) and deliberative (reasoned) determinants of compliance; and the possibility of taking into account the uniqueness of individual understanding of illness and individual patterns of coping with illness. It is also suggested that the self-regulative approach helps to organize insights into the differences between compliance to behavioral measures for prevention and compliance to behavioral measures for cure.
Article
Full-text available
The development of the Multidimensional Health Locus of Control scales is described. Scales have been developed to tap beliefs that the source of reinforcements for health-related behaviors is primarily internal, a matter of chance, or under the control of powerful others. These scales are based on earlier work with a general Health Locus of Control Scale, which, in turn, was developed from Rotter's social learning theory. Equivalent forms of the scales are presented along with initial internal consistency and validity data. Possible means of utilizing these scales are provided.
Book
How do individuals conceive illness and symptoms? Do their conceptions conflict with the physician's views of their illness, and what happens if they do? This book thoroughly explores the field of disease representation, describes and discusses lay illness models in a variety of social, histo- rical and cultural contexts.
Thesis
Self-regulation theory was used to examine how people cope with the diagnosis and management of chronic neurological illness. Three studies are reported, all of which examined the three main elements of self-regulation theory as outlined by Leventhal et al (1984), people's representations of their condition, the actions they took to manage and their evaluation of those efforts, and the outcome as assessed by their feelings about themselves, their emotional wellbeing and severity of disability. A longitudinal study examined 20 people at three stages, before coming into hospital for tests and investigations for multiple sclerosis, 6 weeks and 6 months later. This study provided some support for representations as being important in guiding coping. However, both representations and people's own evaluations of their coping efforts were more predictive of outcome. The second, cross-sectional study compared people being investigated for three differing chronic diseases; the previous 20 people being investigated for multiple sclerosis, 11 and 22 people being investigated for motor neurone disease and liver disease respectively. Results showed that how people represented their condition, and not the diagnosis, was related to outcome. The third study was pseudo-longitudinal and examined people at different times since the diagnosis of multiple sclerosis. People at six months (n=20 from first study), 2 years (n=19), and over 7 years (n=25) from diagnosis were included. This study again highlighted the relationship between representations and outcome; perceiving more symptoms and adverse consequences being associated with poorer outcomes of all types. Coping was also associated with outcome, in particular, more social supports and greater satisfaction with that support being associated with higher Self Esteem, better emotional wellbeing and less severe disability. It is concluded that self-regulation theory is a useful framework within which to understand how people cope with chronic neurological illness, in chronic illnesses with no cure or effective palliative treatments people's representation of their condition and evaluation of their coping efforts predicted outcome. The low level of psychological distress found in all studies suggests that while no particular coping actions are associated with better outcomes, it may be that taking some kind of action, rather than taking no action, in a situation where there is nothing the medical profession can do is sufficiently motivating to minimise depression.
Chapter
As health psychologists one of our primary goals is to understand the determinants of health-related behavior. Topics such as preventive health behavior, help seeking and the use of medical services, and compliance with medical recommendations occupy center stage in health psychology (Leventhal, 1983; Matarazzo, Weiss, Herd, Miller, & Weiss 1984; Stone, Cohen, & Adler, 1979). In each of these areas we seek to understand individuals’ responses to actual or perceived health threats. Further, we are concerned with developing interventions to promote desirable health-related behavior. In this chapter these issues are addressed through examination of cognitive representations of physical illness and the implications of these representations for specific health-related behaviors.
Chapter
There is often considerable discrepancy between the symptoms which patients present and the underlying pathology (Mechanic, 1962; Skelton, this volume). Several studies suggest that as many as 60% of visits to general practioners have no medical basis (Cummings, 1986), while substantial proportions of patients with clinically significant symptoms fail to consult a medical doctor (White, Williams, & Greenberg, 1961). As a consequence, a distinction is made with increasing frequency between disease and illness, with the former referring to strictly pathological conditions evident in a particular patient and the latter referring to the entire cluster of symptoms which he or she presents, regardless of origin (Cott & Pavloski, 1985). As the illness, by definition, has broader behavioral consequences than the disease, and as patients determine their degree of disability and their compliance with treatment regimens on the basis of their illness rather than their disease, it behooves us to understand the origin of patients’ symptomatic complaints. It is evident that understanding of the nondisease component of patients’ illnesses is fundamentally a psychological problem.
Article
Two-hundred and eight patients with chronic fatigue syndrome (post-viral fatigue syndrome) completed a questionnaire which dealt both with their illness in general and with the extent to which they experienced specific symptoms. A factor analysis of the symptom data yielded four components: emotional distress; fatigue; somatic symptoms; and cognitive difficulty. Emotional disturbance is a common feature of the disorder and its role has been widely debated. When the symptom components were considered independently, fatigue, somatic symptoms and cognitive difficulty were associated with questionnaire items relating to general illness severity, but emotional distress was not. Thus negative emotions did not contribute directly to patients' perception of illness severity. They were, however, correlated with the other symptom components. It is argued that this correlation reflects a reciprocal influence, with negative emotions exacerbating fatigue and other key symptoms and the debilitating nature of these symptoms enhancing emotional vulnerability.