ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

On many nesting beaches, hatchling marine turtles are exposed to poled street lighting that disrupts their ability to crawl to the sea. Experiments were done to determine how hatchlings responded to street lighting transmitted through 2 filters that excluded the most disruptive wavelengths (those < 530 nm; those < 570 nm). Filtered lighting, however, also attracted the turtles though not as strongly as an unfiltered (high-pressure sodium vapor) lighting. Filtering is therefore of limited utility for light management, especially since other alternatives (such as lowering, shielding, or turning off unnecessary lighting; use of dimmer lights embedded in roadways) are more effective.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Chelonian Conservation and Biology, 2006, 5(2): 000–000
Ó 2006 Chelonian Research Foundation
Filtered Streetlights Attract Hatchling Marine Turtles
K
RISTEN NELSON SELLA
1
,MICHAEL SALMON
2
, AND BLAIR E. WITHERINGTON
3
1
Palm Beach County Department of Environmental Resource Management, 3323 Belvedere Road, Bldg 502, West Palm Beach,
Florida 33406 USA [turtlewomyn@juno.com];
2
Department of Biological Sciences, Florida Atlantic University, Boca Raton, Florida 33431 USA [salmon@fau.edu];
3
Florida Fish and Wildlife Conservation Commission, Florida Marine Research Institute, 9700 South A1A,
Melbourne Beach, Florida 32951 USA [witherington@cfl.rr.com]
ABSTRACT. – On many nesting beaches, hatchling marine turtles are exposed to poled street lighting
that disrupts their ability to crawl to the sea.
?1
Experiments were done to determine how hatchlings
responded to street lighting transmitted through 2 filters that excluded the most disruptive
wavelengths (those , 530 nm; those , 570 nm). Filtered lighting, however, also attracted the
turtles though not as strongly as an unfiltered (high-pressure sodium vapor) lighting. Filtering is
therefore of limited utility for light management, especially since other alternatives (such as
lowering, shielding, or turning off unnecessary lighting; use of dimmer lights embedded in
roadways) are more effective.
K
EY WORDS. Reptilia; Testudines; Cheloniidae; Caretta caretta ; Chelonia mydas; sea turtle;
hatchlings; artificial lighting; light ‘‘ trapping’’ ; orientation; seafinding; Florida; USA
Hatchling sea turtles emerge from their nests at night
(Bustard 1967; Mrosovsky 1968; Witherington et al. 1990)
and crawl toward the sea. This behavior, known as
‘‘ seafinding’’ (Parker 1922; Daniel and Smith 1947; Carr
and Ogren 1960; Ehrenfeld and Carr 1967; Mrosovsky
1972), is based upon 2 orientation cues. Hatchlings crawl
toward the brightest area (typically, the seaward horizon)
using a positive phototaxis (Mrosovsky 1972; Mrosovsky
and Kingsmill 1985). Hatchlings also detect regions
elevated above the horizon (such as a tall dune and its
associated vegetation). Turtles crawl away from the dune
and toward the beach that presents a lower, flatter, horizon
(Salmon et al. 1992; Witherington 1992).
Artificial lighting disrupts seafinding orientation.
Bright luminaires on land attract turtles so that they crawl
toward the lights and away from the ocean (‘‘ misorienta-
tion’’ or ‘‘ light trapping’’ ; Verheijen 1958, 1985). When
sources of artificial light are less attractive, hatchlings may
show ‘‘ disorientation’’, or an inability to maintain a
directional crawl. This response probably occurs when
hatchlings simultaneously respond to natural cues and
artificial lighting, but cannot orient toward either stimulus
(Tuxbury and Salmon 2004).
At night, most marine turtle hatchlings will crawl
toward visible light; the shorter wavelengths (violet, blue)
are especially attractive (Mrosovsky and Carr 1967;
Mrosovsky and Shettleworth 1968). However, this re-
sponse varies with wavelength among the species. With-
erington (1992) and Witherington and Bjorndal (1991a)
used monochromatic lights as stimuli in laboratory
experiments showing that green turtle (Chelonia mydas),
olive ridley (Lepidochelys olivacea), and hawksbill (Eret-
mochelys imbricata) hatchlings were attracted to wave-
lengths between 350 and 600 nm (ultraviolet to yellow).
Loggerheads (Caretta caretta), however, were attracted to
wavelengths between 350 and 500 nm (ultraviolet to green)
and were either indifferent to, or repelled by, wavelengths
between 530 and 700 nm (green-yellow to red).
These results led to the hypothesis that lights containing
only green-yellow to red wavelengths (530–700 nm) would
not attract loggerhead hatchlings or interfere with their
orientation. Assuming this hypothesis was correct, the
Florida Power and Light Company (FPL) installed light
filters (orange acrylic sheets) in hundreds of pole-mounted
streetlights bordering coastal roadways in South Florida.
Without the filters, these lights disrupt seafinding because
they are frequently visible at nest sites, and because their
high-pressure sodium vapor (HPS) luminaires emit wave-
lengths shorter (as well as longer) than 530 nm (With-
erington and Bjorndal 1991b). Orange filters exclude the
shorter but transmit the longer light wavelengths.
Filtered lighting consists of a spectrum of longer
(orange to red) wavelengths. Monochromatic light within
these wavelengths evokes either indifference or aversion
from loggerhead hatchlings tested under laboratory
conditions (Witherington 1992). The present study there-
fore had 2 goals. The first was to determine if loggerheads
showed indifference and/or aversion when presented with
a spectrum of orange or red (filtered HPS) wavelengths.
The second goal was to determine how other species
(represented here by green turtle hatchlings), attracted to
longer monochromatic light wavelengths than logger-
heads, responded to the same stimuli.
METHODS
Hatchlings. Hatchling loggerhead turtles and green
turtles were obtained during the 2000 nesting season from
previously marked nests at Coral Cove Beach in Palm
Beach County, Florida (26857
0
N, 80805
0
W). These
hatchlings were used in arena experiments conducted
nearby at the Marinelife Center, Juno Beach, Florida.
Hatchlings during the 2001 season were obtained from
nests relocated to a hatchery at the Hillsboro Club,
Broward County, Florida (26818
0
N, 80805
0
W). These
turtles were used in T-maze experiments conducted nearby
at Florida Atlantic University in Boca Raton, Florida. All
turtles were transported from the collection site in
polystyrene foam containers that allowed for air exchange
but were covered with a black cloth to exclude light.
Experiments were done in air-conditioned, dark rooms
rendered lightproof by sealing all windows with black
plastic sheeting.
During both years, hatchlings were collected in the
afternoon of the evening when they were scheduled to
emerge. Date of emergence was estimated by adding 55
days to the egg deposition date. Turtles were used only if
they were captured within 15 cm of the sand surface, and if
their plastron was ‘‘ flat’’ (indicating they were ready to
emerge and developmentally competent to migrate off-
shore). To ensure genetic diversity, hatchlings from at least
2 nests were used each evening, and all tests were repeated
over 2 or more evenings.
Before testing, turtles were held in a dark room for
several hours at ambient temperatures (278–308C), until
dusk. They were then briefly (ca. 15 min) exposed to dim
light and slightly cooler temperatures to stimulate
locomotor activity (Bustard 1967; Mrosovsky 1968). Each
turtle was used in a single trial, then released later that
evening on a nearby dark beach, in accordance with state
guidelines (Florida Department of Environmental Protec-
tion 1996).
Light Measurements. Light was measured (in
fractions of watts) with a radiometer (Model 351; United
Detector Technology, Baltimore, MD) that had a uniform
response between 400 and 700 nm. An Optec stellar
photometer (Optec, Inc, Lowell, MI; Model 351 with a 168
angle of acceptance; range of 300-1100 nm; peak
sensitivity at 520 nm) was used to determine relative
street light radiance at coastal roadways. Both the
radiometer and stellar photometer were calibrated against
a 500-nm light of known intensity across a 3 decade range
of light amplitudes. Measurements from both instruments
were then converted to a common scale of absolute
radiance (in photonscm
2
s
1
at 500 nm).
Arena Experiments. The arena was a circular
horizontal platform used to determine how green turtle and
loggerhead hatchlings responded to HPS and to filtered
HPS light (Fig. 1). It was made of rough-textured
plywood, painted light brown (sand) in color. It contained
60 cloth-lined pockets at the periphery sufficiently large to
trap each turtle that had crawled there from the arena
center (where each turtle was released). Crawling vectors
for each turtle were measured by the angle between the
light source, the center of the arena, and the pocket. The
pocket in front of a light was arbitrarily designated as 08.
The light (an attenuated 70-W HPS light enclosed in a
small wooden box) was located 1.87 m from the arena
center and elevated 40 cm above the platform surface (Fig.
1). Light escaped from the box through a small pinhole
opening made in aluminum flashing. This configuration
matched the elevation and radiance of poled streetlights
observed from the location of several nests. Filters were
attached to the box over the pinhole. Two identical light
boxes were placed near the arena about 908 apart, but
during each experiment only 1 light was turned on. The
light beam from each box was aimed down at the arena
center but could be seen from any location within the
arena.
Turtles were exposed to 4 treatments: 1) light turned
off (turtles crawl in complete darkness), 2) HPS light on,
3) HPS light transmitted through a 2422 amber filter
(excludes wavelengths , 530 nm), and 4) HPS light
transmitted through a NLW red filter (excludes wave-
lengths , 570 nm; Fig. 2).
?2
Both filters were made of dyed
plastic sheets manufactured by the General Electric
Lighting Corporation (Lexington, KY).
Equal numbers of green turtles and loggerheads from
several nests were exposed to each treatment until a target
sample size (n ¼ 30 turtles/treatment) was achieved for
Figure 1. The arena: light boxes were 908 apart and designed to
mimic the radiance of a 70-W HPS streetlight. Boxes were
positioned 40 cm (178) above the arena surface and 1.87 m from
the center of the arena.
Figure 2. Transmission characteristics of the General Electric
Lighting Company filters. The orange-colored 2422 filter
excludes wavelengths , 530 nm; the red-colored NLW filter
excludes wavelengths , 570 nm.
CHELONIAN CONSERVATION AND BIOLOGY, Volume 5, Numbe r 2 2006
each species. A trial began with the release in the arena
center of 5 hatchlings (to simulate the typically simulta-
neous emergence of several turtles from one nest)
contained inside a black cloth bag and ended after all of
the turtles had fallen into a pocket. Crawling progress was
monitored using a video camera and monitor (suspended
out of sight of the turtles, above the arena). In the ‘‘ no
light’’ treatment, records were videotaped under infrared
illumination.
Because filtering excluded some wavelengths, filtered
radiance was lower than unfiltered (HPS) radiance. The
intensities of the light stimuli used (as measured at the
arena center) were (in photonscm
2
s
1
): HPS,
12.0 3 10
12
; 2422 filter, 7.0 3 10
12
; and NLW, 7.0 3
10
12
. The 2422 intensity fell within the range of values
measured in the field from several filtered 70-W cut-off
fixtures, mounted 60 m distant from nests on 10-m-tall
street poles.
T-Maze Experiments. This apparatus was used to
study the response of the turtles to lighting in choice
situations (Fig. 3). Turtles crawled down the runway
toward a white plate that reflected light from a single or
paired source. Once it reached the ‘‘ T’’ , the turtle turned
either to the right or left. Twenty-five hatchlings from 2 or
more nests were used in each trial. Because green turtle
hatchlings were less abundant, only loggerheads were used
in these tests.
The same light boxes were used to house each light.
One box was placed on each side of the maze and
positioned so that its illumination was only directly visible
to turtles that had crawled to the end of the runway (Fig.
3). Light intensity (as measured at the position of the ‘‘ T’’ )
of the HPS light was adjusted to match an unfiltered 70-W
HPS streetlight located either 40 or 60 m distant from a
nest.
The turtles were exposed to 4 treatments: 1) a single
HPS light from either the right or left side of the maze; 2) a
single filtered HPS (2422 or NLW) light from either the
right or left side; 3) paired HPS and filtered HPS (2422 or
NLW) lights (as in Fig. 3) presented from opposite sides of
the T-maze; and 4) paired filtered HPS (2422 and NLW)
lights presented from opposite sides of the T-maze. In the
paired light treatments, each treatment was replicated 4
times: with 1 of the 2 luminaires (HPS or the 2422 filtered
HPS light) presented at full intensity, or with their
intensity reduced by 1, 2, or 3 log units using neutral
density filters (made from layered plastic hardware cloth).
In control experiments, hatchlings were exposed to
pairs of lights (HPS or filtered HPS) adjusted to an
identical radiance. These tests were done to confirm that
no variable other than light was responsible for deviations
from an expected 50:50 turning ratio.
Statistics. Crawl vectors for each turtle in a single
arena treatment were used to calculate a second-order
group mean angle and r-vector (measure of dispersion).
Rayleigh tests (Zar 1999) were employed to determine
whether groups were significantly oriented (p 0.05).
The number of hatchlings that turned right or left was
recorded for each T-maze treatment. The null hypothesis
of a 50:50 turning distribution was rejected when that
distribution resulted in a p 0.05 (by a binomial test;
Sokal and Rohlf 1995).
RESULTS
Arena Experiments. Neither the loggerheads (Fig.
4) nor the green turtles (Fig. 5) showed significant
orientation when tested in darkness. Turtles exposed to a
HPS or to a filtered (2422 or NLW) HPS light were
strongly attracted to the stimulus.
Figure 3. The T-maze (overhead view). Hatchlings were released
at *, crawled toward the light reflecting barrier at the end of the
runway, then turned left or right toward one of the lights.
Figure 4. Response of loggerheads to a 70-W HPS light at 08
(top of each circle). In different treatments, the light is either
turned off (A) or on (B–D). When on, it passes through a filter in
C and D. Sample size is 30 turtles per treatment. Line length is
proportional to the number of turtles orienting in each direction.
A, group mean angle; r, dispersion; n.s., no significant group
orientation.
SELLA ET AL. Filtered Streetlights Attract Hatchling Marine Turtles
T-Maze Experiments. None of the control turtles
showed a distribution of turns that deviated statistically
from an expected 50:50 ratio. Hatchlings were significant-
ly attracted to a single HPS light at the 40- and 60-m
radiance levels (Table 1). Both types of filtered lighting
(orange 2422, red NLW) failed to result in a significant
attraction at the higher (40-m) radiance level. However, at
the lower (60-m) radiance, the turtles were attracted to
each light source. Attraction to filtered lighting was
weaker (76% of the turtles to the 2422; 84% to the
NLW) than attraction to HPS lighting (96%; Table 1).
More turtles turned toward the HPS light when it was
paired with either a 2422 (Fig. 6) or a NLW (Fig. 7)
filtered light. When HPS radiance was reduced by 2 log
units, the number of turtles that turned toward each light
did not differ statistically (Figs. 6 and 7). When HPS
radiance was reduced by 3 log units, more turtles turned
toward the 2422 filtered than the HPS light (Fig. 6). This
trend was also evident when the HPS light was paired with
a NLW light, though the probabilities just missed
significance (Fig. 7).
Turning tendencies shown in response to paired
filtered lights were statistically equal (Fig. 8). When the
2422 source was reduced in radiance by 3 log units, more
turtles turned toward the NLW light (Fig. 8).
DISCUSSION
Response to HPS and Filtered Lighting. Our arena
experiments showed that in the absence of lighting, green
turtle and loggerhead hatchlings did not show significant
group orientation. However, in the presence of lighting the
turtles of both species crawled toward the source (Figs. 4
and 5). We conclude that both HPS and filtered HPS
lighting attract sea turtle hatchlings.
Our T-maze experiments (Figs. 6 and 7) indicate that
HPS lighting is more attractive to loggerheads than filtered
HPS lighting. Two variables might account for these
results. The first is light intensity because initially (in the
0:0 tests), the HPS light was slightly brighter than the
filtered HPS light with which it was paired. A second
possibility is that the HPS light was more attractive
because its spectral composition included some shorter
light wavelengths. Intensity was eliminated as a factor by
Figure 5. Arena experiments with green turtles. Format and
sample size, as in Fig. 4.
Figure 6. T-Maze experiments that show the percentage of
hatchlings turning toward a HPS light when it is paired with a
filtered (2422) HPS light. The 2 lights are initially presented at
intensities comparable to a street light at a distance of 40 m (top
graph) or 60 m (lower graph) from the turtle (0:0, left side of each
graph). In 3 additional treatments, the HPS light is reduced in
intensity by 1 (1:0), 2 (2.0), or 3 (3:0) log units while the
filtered light remains unchanged in intensity. n ¼ 25 different
hatchlings in each treatment. Points falling on or above the upper,
or on or below the lower dashed lines are significant statistical
departures (at p 0.05 level) from a 50:50 ratio (by a binomial
test).
Table 1. Percentage of hatchlings turning toward a single high-
pressure sodium vapor (HPS), 2422, or NLW light stimulus
presented from one side of the T-maze. Intensities are comparable
to a 70-W streetlight placed 40 or 60 m from the nest. Sample
size, n ¼ 25 hatchlings for each light stimulus. Probabilities (p)
are based upon the outcome of a binomial test.
40-m light 60-m light
Light stimulus % p % p
HPS 100 , 0.001 96 , 0.001
2422 68 n.s. 76 , 0.02
NLW 68 n.s. 84 , 0.002
CHELONIAN CONSERVATION AND BIOLOGY, Volume 5, Numbe r 2 2006
repeating the tests after HPS radiance was reduced by 1 or
more log units below the radiance of the filtered light. In
response, the turtles either continued to orient preferen-
tially toward the dimmer HPS source, or showed no
significant orientation toward either light (Figs. 6 and 7).
We conclude that in our experiments, the spectral
composition of the HPS light made that stimulus more
attractive to the hatchlings than filtered lighting.
Amber (2422) and red (NLW) filters were designed
for use with pole-mounted HPS streetlights on roadways
adjacent to nesting beaches. In a field experiment, the 2422
filter proved effective with adult nesting female logger-
heads (Pennell 2000). However, field experiments with
loggerhead hatchlings produced equivocal results (Cowan
and Salmon 1998) because of nightly variation in other
sources of artificial lighting (skyglow from nearby
communities). Because this lighting could not be con-
trolled, it was impossible to distinguish between responses
caused by filtered lighting and responses to changes in
background illumination. These problems led us to do
further testing in a laboratory setting where extraneous
sources of illumination could be excluded.
HPS lighting at both a higher (40-m) and lower (60-
m) intensity attracted the turtles, but the hatchlings were
attracted to filtered lighting only at a lower (60-m)
radiance level (Table 1). In addition, the NLW light at
60 m was apparently more attractive to the turtles than the
2422 filtered light at 60 m (Table 1). Yet the NLW light
excluded a larger proportion of the wavelengths around
530 nm that elicit ‘‘ indifference’’ , while leaving present
those wavelengths (. 570 nm) that elicit ‘‘ aversion’’
(Witherington 1992).
The explanation for these responses may center on
how hatchling loggerheads respond to different intensities,
rather than wavelengths, of light. In previous experiments,
Witherington (1992) found that responses such as
‘‘ attraction’’, ‘‘ indifference’’ , and ‘‘ aversion’’ were elicited
at relatively high (perhaps photopically mediated) light
levels. At lower light levels (perhaps mediated scotopi-
cally), all wavelengths of monochromatic light were
attractive to hatchling loggerheads. We hypothesize that
the 40-m light stimulus was sufficiently intense to permit
wavelength discrimination (and indifference or aversion),
whereas the 60-m light stimulus was not (and therefore
attracted the turtles).
These results reveal some of the complexities
associated with using filtered lighting as a management
tool. Filtered lighting may be unattractive to hatchlings
when they emerge from their nests because the light source
is in close proximity, and therefore more intense. But as
Figure 7. T-Maze experiments that show the percentage of
hatchlings turning toward the HPS light when it is paired with a
filtered (NLW) HPS light. Format as in Fig. 6.
Figure 8. T-Maze experiments that show the percentage of
hatchlings turning toward a 2422 filtered HPS light when it is
paired with a NLW filtered HPS light. Format as in Fig. 6 except
that the 2422 filtered light is reduced in intensity using neutral
density filters while the NLW light is left unchanged in intensity.
SELLA ET AL. Filtered Streetlights Attract Hatchling Marine Turtles
the turtles crawl away from the light (and toward the sea),
the light source decreases in perceived intensity and could,
as a consequence, become attractive. To properly assess
the impact of filtered lighting on turtles at any location,
then, hatchlings must be exposed to the entire range of
light intensities they encounter as they crawl from their
nests (and the light) toward the sea.
Management Implicatio ns. Ideally, filtered HPS
lighting should have no effect on the orientation of
hatchling sea turtles. Our results show, however, that
hatchlings can, under some circumstances, be attracted to
filtered lighting. Other problems are also associated with
the use of filtered lighting.
First, responses to filtered lighting probably vary,
depending upon the species. The 2422 and NLW filters
were developed primarily for use near loggerhead
rookeries and are based upon the unique response of
loggerhead hatchlings to light wavelengths (Witherington
1992). The few tests that have been done with leatherbacks
(Dermochelys coriacea) and green turtles (Cowan and
Salmon 1998; Tuxbury 2004) suggest that these species
respond differently even to the longer wavelengths
transmitted by these filters.
?3
Second, the filters currently in use may represent the
best technology that can be used with HPS luminaires,
which for economic reasons are preferred for street
lighting. Excluding any more of the shorter wavelengths
transmitted by HPS luminaires may reduce luminance
levels below levels required for roadway safety (as
mandated by the Florida Department of Transportation
[Scott Stephens, Florida Power and Light Co, pers.
comm.]). These standards were set by engineers to provide
minimum levels of illumination for motorists. However, a
variance from these standards can be obtained if the
roadway custodian accepts liability for accidents and
installs warning signs to notify motorists of poor lighting
conditions (Ecological Associates, Inc 1998). Currently,
roadways in Florida with lighting that affects nesting
beaches are being identified, and new standards are being
determined for lighting roadways. Whether filtered
lighting can meet those standards remains to be deter-
mined.
Third, there are better alternatives for managing
coastal roadway lighting. One promising technology is
the use of light-emitting diodes placed in the pavement
itself (‘‘ embedded’’ roadway lighting). These lights
produce far less illumination than streetlights and confine
that light to the roadway itself (where it is needed). Field
tests were recently done at a coastal roadway where
embedded lighting was installed. Their illumination could
not be detected at the beach either by humans, their
instruments, or by loggerhead hatchlings. Turtles crawled
toward the sea when the embedded lights were on and
when they were turned off. However, when the poled HPS
streetlights were turned on, orientation dispersion (and in
some tests, mean angle) were affected (Bertolotti and
Salmon, in press).
Fourth, filtered lighting is a ‘‘ half-way technology’’
(Frazer 1992) because it fails to eliminate the cause of the
problem (light scatter to the beach); rather, it seeks to alter
the impact of that light by modifying spectral output. The
only proven methods of light management, however, are to
turn off or redirect lighting so that it is no longer visible at
the beach (Witherington and Martin 2000).
On the other hand, there are circumstances where
filtered lighting might be useful. Hatchling loggerheads are
less likely to crawl toward visible lighting if a tall, dark
landward silhouette is present (Witherington et al. 1994;
Tuxbury and Salmon 2004). Because filtered lighting is
less attractive to the turtles, it might be used to illuminate
roadways without affecting seafinding, even if some
lighting escapes to the beach. However, before such a
modification is made permanent, tests must be done at
these sites to confirm that the turtles exposed to both
filtered lighting and tall silhouettes will complete a
seaward crawl.
Filtered lighting may also be beneficial at locations
where the public believes that lights prevent crime and/or
reduce roadway accidents (Witherington and Martin
2000). Filtered lighting at such a site has 3 benefits. It
has a favorable psychological impact on users, and (for the
turtles) reduces light intensity while transmitting less
attractive spectra to the environment.
New technologies must be explored to determine their
potential for reducing the impact of artificial lighting on
wildlife. Initially, light filters were a new technology
promoted by their manufacturer (General Electric Lighting
Corporation) as a simple method for converting harmful,
attractive lights into those that were ‘‘turtle friendly’’ .
These claims; however, were made in the absence of
adequate testing. Having now completed testing, we
conclude that at the present time filtered lighting is
potentially beneficial only under special, and unfortunately
somewhat limited, circumstances.
A
CKNOWLEDGMENTS
This study was completed by K.A.N. as part of the
requirement for a Master’s degree in the biological
sciences at Florida Atlantic University. C. Makowski
assisted in the laboratory experiments. W.P. Irwin, J.A.
Seminoff, A.G.J. Rhodin, and an anonymous referee made
suggestions that improved the manuscript. Financial
support was provided by the Florida Power and Light
Company and the National Save-the-Sea-Turtle Founda-
tion of Fort Lauderdale, Florida.
LITERATURE CITED
BERTOLOTTI,L.AND SALMON, M. Do embedded roadway lights
protect sea turtles? Environmental Management (in press).
?4
BUSTARD, H.R. 1967. Mechanism of nocturnal emergence from
the nest in green turtle hatchlings. Nature 214:317.
C
ARR,A.AND OGREN, L. 1960. The ecology and migrations of sea
CHELONIAN CONSERVATION AND BIOLOGY, Volume 5, Numbe r 2 2006
turtles. IV. The green turtle in the Caribbean Sea. Bulletin of
the American Museum of Natural History 121:6–45.
?5
COWAN,E.AND SALMON, M. 1998. Influence of filtered roadway
lighting on the orientation of hatchling loggerhead turtles,
Caretta caretta L. Unpublished report to the Florida Power and
Light Company, 29 pp.
D
ANIEL, R.S. AND SMITH, K.U. 1947. The sea-approach behavior of
the neonate loggerhead turtle (Caretta caretta). Journal of
Comparative Physiology and Psychology 40:413–420.
?6
ECOLOGICAL ASSOCIATES,INC. 1998. Coastal Roadway Lighting
Manual. Unpublished report to the Florida Power and Light
Company, 71 pp.
E
HRENFELD, D.W. AND CARR, A. 1967. The role of vision in the sea-
finding orientation of the green turtle (Chelonia mydas).
Animal Behavior 15:25–36.
?7
FRAZER, N.B. 1992. Sea turtle conservation and halfway
technology. Conservation Biology 6:179–184.
F
LORIDA DEPARTMENT OF ENVIRONMENTAL PROTECTION. 1996.
Marine turtle conservation guidelines, January, 1996. Florida
Department of Environmental Protection, Tallahassee, 43 pp.
M
ROSOVSKY, N. 1968. Nocturnal emergence of hatchling sea
turtles: control by thermal inhibition of activity. Nature 220:
1338–1339.
M
ROSOVSKY, N. 1972. The water-finding ability of sea turtles.
Brain Behavior Evolution 5:202–225.
M
ROSOVSKY,N.AND CARR, A. 1967. Preference for light of short
wavelengths in hatchling green sea turtles, Chelonia mydas,
tested on their natural nesting beaches. Behaviour 28:217–231
M
ROSOVSKY,N.AND KINGSMILL, S.F. 1985. How turtles find the
sea. Zeitschrift von Tierpsychologie 67:237–256.
?8
MROSOVSKY,N.AND SHETTLEWORTH, S.J. 1968. Wavelength
preferences and brightness cues in the water finding behaviour
of sea turtles. Behaviour 32:211–257.
P
ARKER, G.H. 1922. The crawling of young loggerhead turtles
toward the sea. Journal of Experimental Zoology 36:323–331.
P
ENNELL, J. 2000. The effect of filtered roadway lighting on
nesting of loggerhead sea turtles (Caretta caretta L.) MS
Thesis, Florida Atlantic University, Boca Raton, Florida.
S
ALMON, M., WYNEKEN, J., FRITZ, E., AND LUCAS, M. 1992.
Seafinding by hatchling sea turtles: role of brightness,
silhouette and beach slope as orientation cues. Behaviour
122:56–77.
S
OKAL, R.R. AND ROHLF, F.J. 1995. Biometry. New York: W.H.
Freeman and Company, 776 pp.
T
UXBURY,S.M.AND SALMON, M. 2004. Competitive interactions
between artificial lighting and natural cues during the
orientation of marine turtle hatchlings. Biological Conservation
121:311–316.
V
ERHEIJEN, F.J. 1958. The mechanisms of the trapping effect of
artificial light sources upon animals. Arch. Neerl. Zool. 13:
1–107.
?9
VERHEIJEN, F.J. 1985. Photopollution: artificial light optic spatial
control systems fail to cope with. Incidents, causations,
remedies. Experimental Biology 44:1–18.
?10
WITHERINGTON, B.E. 1992. Sea-finding behavior and the use of
photic orientation cues by hatchling sea turtles. PhD Disser-
tation, University of Florida, Gainesville.
W
ITHERINGTON, B.E. AND BJORNDAL, K. 1991a. Influences of
wavelength and intensity on hatchling sea turtle phototaxis:
implications for sea-finding behavior. Copeia 1991:
1060–1069.
W
ITHERINGTON, B.E. AND BJORNDAL, K. 1991b. Influences of
artificial lighting on the seaward orientation of hatchling
loggerhead turtles (Caretta caretta). Biological Conservation
55:139–149.
W
ITHERINGTON, B.E., BJORNDAL, K.A., AND BOLTEN, A. 1994. An
evaluation of the use of dune structure to reduce effects of
artificial lighting on hatchling sea turtle sea-finding orientation
and offshore orientation. Unpublished report submitted to US
Air Force, Cape Canaveral Air Force Station, and the Florida
Cooperative Fish and Wildlife Research Unit, Xx pp.
?11
WITHERINGTON, B.E., BJORNDAL, K., AND MCCABE, C. 1990.
Temporal pattern of nocturnal emergence of loggerhead turtle
hatchlings from natural nests. Copeia 1990:1165–1168.
W
ITHERINGTON, B.E. AND MARTIN, R.E. 2000. Understanding,
assessing, and resolving light-pollution problems on sea turtle
nesting beaches. Second edition. Florida Marine Research
Institute Technical Report TR-2, 73 pp.
Z
AR, J.H. 1999. Biostatistical Analysis. Fourth edition. NJ:
Prentice Hall, 663 pp.
?12
Received: 29 September 2004
Revised and Accepted: 13 September 2005
SELLA ET AL. Filtered Streetlights Attract Hatchling Marine Turtles
... Field and laboratory studies on sea turtles have found the degree of impact that artificial light has on their dispersal behaviour is dependent on the spectral composition of the light emissions with hatchlings more attracted to short (blue-green) over long (orange-red) light wavelengths (Osovsky and Shettleworth 1968;Bjorndal 1991a, 1991b;Pendoley 2005;Sella et al. 2006;Fritsches 2012). These studies led to the recommendation that lights containing the least amount of short wavelength light (e.g. ...
... orange low and high pressure sodium) should be used near turtle nesting beaches if lighting cannot be avoided (Witherington and Martin 2003). Filters can also be used to eliminate any shorter light wavelengths emitted from these sources (Sella et al. 2006). Brighter lights are also more attractive and for this reason it is also recommended that light intensity is kept as low as possible near turtle nesting beaches Bjorndal 1991a, 1991b;Witherington and Martin 2003;Pendoley and Kamrowski 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
It is well known that light pollution disrupts the early dispersal of marine turtles. But now, light emitting diodes (LEDs) are replacing traditional lights, however, we know little about how they influence hatchling dispersal. Here, we used acoustic telemetry to assess the early in-water dispersal and predation rates of hatchlings in response to different intensities of LEDs ranging from 10 to 120 W. We found no effect of LEDs on hatchling bearing when lights were in the direction they dispersed under ambient conditions. When LEDs were not in their usual direction of travel, variability in mean bearing increased, and a change in bearing occurred with the highest light intensity. We found weak evidence that predation was also higher at this light intensity compared to ambient, and also in two of the lower light intensities (10 and 30 W), but only on one experimental night. We were unable to find a relationship between hatchling speed and time spent in the tracking area with light intensity. However, reduced sample sizes (due to predation) might have affected our ability to detect effects. Although more effort is required to increase the confidence in our findings, LEDs disrupted hatchling dispersal and are therefore likely to negatively affect their survival.
... Pole-mounted streetlights along coastal roadways can be especially problematic. Streetlights fitted with full-spectrum light sources are a well-documented cause of hatchling misorientation and disorientation (Bertolotti and Salmon 2005;Cowan et al. 2002;McFarlane 1963;Nelson Sella, Salmon, and Witherington 2006;Peters and Verhoeven 1994;Salmon, Wyneken, and Foote 2003). In Florida, the most important nesting grounds for loggerheads globally (Ceriani et al. 2019), streetlights contributed to approximately 20% of all lighting-related impacts to sea turtles reported to the Florida Fish and Wildlife Conservation Commission (FWC) between 2011 and 2020 (Florida Fish and Wildlife Conservation Commission 2021a). ...
... In Florida, the most important nesting grounds for loggerheads globally (Ceriani et al. 2019), streetlights contributed to approximately 20% of all lighting-related impacts to sea turtles reported to the Florida Fish and Wildlife Conservation Commission (FWC) between 2011 and 2020 (Florida Fish and Wildlife Conservation Commission 2021a). Unfortunately, previous attempts to mitigate these impacts by fitting existing streetlights with amber and red acrylic filters were ultimately unsuccessful (Cowan et al. 2002;Nelson 2003;Nelson Sella, Salmon, and Witherington 2006;Salmon, Wyneken, and Foote 2003;Tuxbury and Salmon 2005). Although the filters were designed to omit most wavelengths below 530 nm and 570 nm respectively, both filters allowed a partial transmission of light below the intended cutoff wavelengths (Nelson 2003). ...
Article
Full-text available
Coastal roadways with tall, full-spectrum streetlights along sea turtle nesting beaches present a challenge for managers seeking to balance protection of sea turtles with public safety. Many communities extinguish these lights during nesting season to avoid impacting nesting and hatchling sea turtles. Long-wavelength light emitting diodes (LEDs) offer an alternative for managers in these communities, but additional information on sea turtle response to these lights is warranted prior to installation. We conducted arena assays on Florida’s west coast to evaluate hatchling orientation when exposed to a shielded, long-wavelength (624 nm) prototype lamp compared to an adjacent beach with the streetlights turned off. We compared orientation in test and control arenas simultaneously over two consecutive nights, recording crawl direction and timing for individual hatchlings. Hatchlings in test and control arenas oriented correctly toward the ocean in all trials, with no differences in hatchling dispersion or circling. Thus, the fully shielded, long-wavelength LED streetlight fixture tested provides an appropriate option to minimize impacts to sea turtles along coastal roadways throughout the Unites States and elsewhere. As such, this alternative solution to extinguishing necessary streetlights can aid coastal managers in concurrently protecting nesting habitat and providing light for public safety.
... Sea-finding is a complex, visually guided process which relies upon multiple kinds of visual information. Turtle hatchlings are known to integrate the brightness of visual inputs from multiple angles, in order to determine the brightest direction [63][64][65]; to prefer some spectral wavelengths over others, irrespective of light intensity [66]; and to use shape/form perception to orient away from high silhouettes [63,67,68]. (The intricacies of turtle hatchling sea-finding are reviewed excellently by Lohmann et al. [69].) ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: By examining species-specific innate behaviours, neuroethologists have characterised unique neural strategies and specializations from throughout the animal kingdom. Simultaneously, the field of evolutionary developmental biology (informally, “evo-devo”) seeks to make inferences about animals’ evolutionary histories through careful comparison of developmental processes between species, because evolution is the evolution of development. Yet despite the shared focus on cross-species comparisons, there is surprisingly little cross-talk between these two fields. Insights can be gleaned at the intersection of neuroethology and evo-devo. Every animal develops within an environment, wherein ecological pressures advantage some behaviours and disadvantage others. These pressures are reflected in the neurodevelopmental strategies employed by different animals across taxa. Summary: Vision is a system of particular interest for studying the adaptation of animals to their environments. The visual system enables a wide variety of animals across the vertebrate lineage to interact with their environments, presenting a fantastic opportunity to examine how ecological pressures have shaped animals’ behaviours and developmental strategies. Applying a neuroethological lens to the study of visual development, we advance a novel theory that accounts for the evolution of spontaneous retinal waves, an important phenomenon in the development of the visual system, across the vertebrate lineage. Key Messages: We synthesise literature on spontaneous retinal waves from across the vertebrate lineage. We find that ethological considerations explain some cross-species differences in the dynamics of retinal waves. In zebrafish, retinal waves may be more important for the development of the retina itself, rather than the retinofugal projections. We additionally suggest empirical tests to determine whether Xenopus laevis experiences retinal waves.
... Therefore, if lights were not turned off, interventions to the nest were implemented on a case-by-case basis, including shielding the nest from the light source (e.g., with the use of heavy cloth sheeting or tarpaulin), observing the hatch, inducing the hatch and intervening if hatchlings misoriented, or removing hatchlings prior to, or as they emerged for release elsewhere. The frequency of interventions to nests were recorded, as well as the frequency of hatchling misorientation, defined as the misdirection of a hatchling toward an artificial light source (Sella et al., 2006). Misorientation was determined based on observations of hatchlings or the orientation of hatchling tracks away from the sea. ...
Article
Full-text available
Given differing trajectories of sea turtle populations worldwide, there is a need to assess and report long-term population trends and determine which conservation strategies are effective. In this study, we report on sea turtle nest monitoring in the Cayman Islands over a 22-year period. We found that green (Chelonia mydas) and loggerhead (Caretta caretta) nest numbers increased significantly across the three islands since monitoring began in 1998, but that hawksbill nest numbers remained low with a maximum of 13 nests recorded in a season. Comparing the first 5 years of nest numbers to the most recent 5 years, the greatest percentage increase in green turtle nests was in Grand Cayman from 82 to 1,005 nests (1,126%), whereas the greatest percentage increase for loggerhead turtle nests was in Little Cayman from 10 to 290 nests (3,800%). A captive breeding operation contributed to the increase in the Grand Cayman green turtle population, however, loggerhead turtles were never captive-bred, and these populations began to increase after a legal traditional turtle fishery became inactive in 2008. Although both species have shown significant signs of recovery, populations remain at a fragment of their historical level and are vulnerable to threats. Illegal harvesting occurs to this day, with multiple females taken from nesting beaches each year. For nests and hatchlings, threats include artificial lighting on nesting beaches, causing hatchlings to misorient away from the sea, and inundation of nests by seawater reducing hatch success. The impacts of lighting were found to increase over the monitoring period. Spatial data on nest distribution was used to identify critical nesting habitat for green and loggerhead turtles and is used by the Cayman Islands Department of Environment to facilitate remediation of threats related to beachside development and for targeted future management efforts.
... Light at night can have complex effects on wildlife (e.g., Witherington and Bjorndal, 1991;Sella et al., 2006), and use of amber lighting may be beneficial in some contexts (Longcore et al., 2018). However, our findings raise the possibility that filtering streetlights to reduce emission of blue light may not help reduce impacts of light at night on sleep in birds. ...
Article
Full-text available
Artificial light at night could have widespread and detrimental impacts on sleep. To reduce disruptive effects of artificial light on sleep in humans, most smartphones and computers now have software that reduces blue light emissions at night. Little is known about whether reducing blue light emissions from city lights could also benefit urban wildlife. We investigated the effects of blue-rich (white) and blue-reduced (amber) LED streetlights on accelerometry-defined rest, electrophysiologically-identified sleep, and plasma melatonin in a diurnal bird, the black swan (Cygnus atratus). Urban swans were exposed to 20 full nights of each lighting type in an outdoor, naturalistic environment. Contrary to our predictions, we found that night-time rest was similar during exposure to amber and white lights but decreased under amber lights compared with dark conditions. By recording brain activity in a subset of swans, we also demonstrated that resting birds were almost always asleep, so amber light also reduced sleep at night. We found no effect of light treatment on total (24 h) daily rest or plasma melatonin. Our study provides the first electrophysiologically-verified evidence for effects of streetlights on sleep in an urban animal, and furthermore suggests that reducing blue wavelengths of light might not mitigate these effects.
... The presence of lights on or adjacent to nesting beaches alters the behavior of nesting adults (Witherington 1992) and is often fatal to emerging hatchlings as they are attracted to light sources and drawn away from the water (Witherington and Bjorndal, 1991;Nelson Sella et al. 2006). Sea turtle nesting in the CNMI is already restricted to a handful of beaches and associated strand that are currently little-influenced by artificial lighting. ...
Technical Report
Full-text available
The CNMI Division of Fish and Wildlife led a participatory planning process through which professionals and the public identified conservation priorities for the Commonwealth of the Northern Mariana Islands. About 60 terrestrial and marine "Species of Greatest Conservation Need" were identified. The Plan describes current status, measurable objectives, and priority actions for each.
... municipal and county lighting ordinances) and efforts (e.g., retrofitting houses with turtle-friendly lights) in place to reduce light pollution at marine turtle nesting grounds in the United States (Witherington and Bjorndal 1991;Witherington and Martin 2000). Some of the earliest lighting ordinances implemented in Florida were in the 1980s and most of the work to investigate new technologies in lighting to minimize impacts have been conducted in the United States (Bertolotti and Salmon 2005;Nelson Sella, Salmon, and Witherington 2006;Witherington and Martin 2000). Overall, other shoreline stabilization (groins and jetties) was perceived by experts as the third greatest threat, which interferes directly with marine turtle nesting as it occurs at the nesting habitat (e.g., nearshore, sandy beach, dunes) (Bouchard et al. 1998). ...
Article
Marine turtles utilize sandy beaches as nesting grounds, which can be impacted by a variety of coastal modifications. In the context of limited resources, managers need to prioritize which impacts from coastal modifications to mitigate. However, data on the relative impacts of coastal modification activities are not often available. To address this, we determined the perceived relative impact of twelve coastal modification activities on marine turtle nesting grounds by eliciting information from researchers and managers who are experts on the impacts of coastal modifications on marine turtles and their nesting grounds. Experts were asked to answer a series of pair-wise comparison matrices that compared the impacts of each coastal modification activity. Beach armoring, light pollution, and other shoreline stabilization structures (such as groins and jetties) were weighted by our experts as having the greatest impact to marine turtle nesting grounds and non permanent coastal modifications (e.g., special events and beach cleaning) were weighted by experts as having the lowest impact to marine turtle nesting grounds. Managers can use this information to prioritize their efforts and resources to manage marine turtle nesting grounds if funds are available and policy allows.
... Instruments such as wide-field charge-coupled device (CCD) camera systems can be used to accurately measure brightness of a night sky by stitching together images taken from a robotic mount to form a mosaic of an entire sky (Duriscoe et al., 2007;Pendoley et al., 2012). Researchers have also used handheld or tripod mounted stellar photometers and light meters to study how nighttime light affects sea turtle behavior (Salmon et al., 1992Salmon and Witherington, 1995;Bertolotti and Salmon, 2005;Sella et al., 2006;Kamrowski et al., 2015). Researchers have mounted light meters on small tripods aimed at the horizon to collect relative light radiance in various directions (Salmon and Witherington, 1995). ...
Article
Full-text available
Nesting sea turtles appear to avoid brightly lit beaches and often turn back to sea prematurely when exposed to artificial light. Observations and experiments have noted that nesting turtles prefer darker areas where buildings and high dunes act as light barriers. As a result, sea turtles often nest on darker beaches, creating spatial concentrations of nests. Artificial nighttime light, or light pollution, has been quantified using a variety of methods. However, it has proven challenging to make accurate measurements of ambient light at fine scales and on smaller nesting beaches. Additionally, light has traditionally been measured from stationary tripods perpendicular to beach vegetation, disregarding the point of view of a nesting sea turtle. In the present study, nighttime ambient light conditions were assessed on three beaches in central North Carolina: a developed coastline of a barrier island, a nearby State Park on the same barrier island comprised of protected and undeveloped land, and a completely uninhabited wilderness on an adjacent barrier island in the Cape Lookout National Seashore. Using an autonomous terrestrial rover, high resolution light measurements (mag/arcsec²) were collected every minute with two ambient light sensors along transects on each beach. Spatial comparisons between ambient light and nesting density at and between these locations reveal that highest densities of nests occur in regions with lowest light levels, supporting the hypothesis that light pollution from coastal development may influence turtle nesting distribution. These results can be used to support ongoing management strategies to mitigate this pressing conservation issue.
... This misdirection of a hatchling towards an artificial point source of light, such as a street light, is termed misorientation. In urban areas hatchlings are also disoriented by skyglow, causing them to wander on the beach without direction; this is termed disorientation (Sella et al., 2006). Misorientation or disorientation can cause hatchlings to travel inland where they face a host of hazards such as roadway automobile traffic and predators (McFarlane, 1963;Tomillo et al., 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
Following emergence from the nest, sea turtle hatchling dispersal can be disrupted by artificial lights or skyglow from urban areas. Mis- or disorientation may increase exposure to predation, thermal stress, and dehydration, and consume valuable energy, thus decreasing the likelihood of survival. In this study hatchlings were run on a treadmill for 200m or 500m to investigate the physiological impacts of disorientation crawling on loggerhead (Caretta caretta) and green (Chelonia mydas) sea turtle hatchlings. Oxygen consumption, lactate production, and blood glucose levels were determined and swim performance measured over 2h following crawls. Crawl distances were also determined for hatchlings which disoriented on the Boca Raton, Fl, beach, with plasma lactate and blood glucose sampled for both properly oriented and disoriented hatchlings. Green and loggerhead hatchlings rested for 8-12% and 22-25% of crawl time, respectively, both in the laboratory and when disoriented on the beach, which was significantly longer than the time spent resting in non-disoriented turtles. As a result of these rest periods, the extended crawl distances had little effect on oxygen consumption, blood glucose, or plasma lactate levels. Swim performance over 2h following the crawls also changed little compared to controls. Plasma lactate concentrations were significantly higher in hatchling sampled in the field, but did not correlate with crawl distance. The greatest immediate impact of extended crawling due to disorientation events, then, is likely to be the significantly greater period of time spent on the beach and thus exposure to predation.
Book
Full-text available
At present, some 11,440 extant reptile species have been described on Earth and several hundred new species have been described each year since 2008 (Uetz & Hosek 2018). As grazers, seed dispersers, predators, prey and commensal species, reptiles perform crucial functions in ecosystems (Böhm et al. 2013). Reptiles are a hugely diverse group of animals (Pincheira-Donoso et al. 2013) and are adapted to live in a wide range of tropical, temperate and desert terrestrial habitats, as well as freshwater and marine environments (Böhm et al. 2013). That said, reptile species usually have narrower geographic distributions than other vertebrate taxonomic groups (e.g. birds or mammals), and this coupled with particular life history traits makes some reptile species particularly vulnerable to anthropogenic threats (Böhm et al. 2013, Fitzgerald et al. 2018). For example, some turtle species are 16 typically very long lived, take years to reach full maturity, produce small clutches and have variable reproductive success, which means that they are vulnerable to loss of adults and take many years to recover from declines (Congdon et al. 1994). Multiple threats to reptile populations have been identified and are implicated in species declines (Gibbons et al. 2000, Todd et al. 2010). These threats include habitat modification, loss and fragmentation (Neilly et al. 2018, Todd et al. 2017), environmental contamination (Sparling et al. 2010), potentially unsustainable harvesting and/or collection (van Cao et al. 2014), invasive species (Fordham et al. 2006), climate change (Bickford et al. 2010, Sinervo et al. 2010) and disease and parasitism (Seigel et al. 2003). Also, due to their physical characteristics, reputation (warranted or otherwise) and in some cases venomous bites, some reptile species are viewed with distaste, which leads to apathy around their conservation (Gibbons et al. 1988). According to the IUCN Red List, of 10,148 reptile species that have been assessed, some 21% are considered to be threatened (IUCN 2021). Extinction risks are particularly high in tropical regions, on oceanic islands and in freshwater environments (Böhm et al. 2013), with some 59% of turtle species assessed at risk of extinction (van Dijk et al. 2014). Reptiles with specialist habitat requirements and limited ranges that are in areas accessible to humans are likely to face greater extinction risks (Böhm et al. 2016). Many island reptile species are endemic and are therefore even more vulnerable to extinction as a result of human disturbance (Fitzgerald et al. 2018). For a comprehensive summary of threats to different families of reptiles see Fitzgerald et al. (2018). Evidence-based knowledge is key for planning successful conservation strategies and for the cost-effective allocation of scarce conservation resources. To date, reptile conservation efforts have involved a broad range of actions, including protection of eggs, nests and nesting sites; protection from predation; translocations; captive breeding, rearing and releasing; habitat protection, restoration and management; and addressing the threats of accidental and intentional harvesting. However, most of the evidence for the effectiveness of these interventions has not yet been synthesised within a formal review and those that have could benefit from periodic updates in light of new research. Targeted reviews are labour-intensive and expensive. Furthermore, they are ill-suited for subject areas where the data are scarce and patchy. Here, we use a subject-wide evidence synthesis approach (Sutherland et al. 2019) to simultaneously summarize the evidence for the wide range of interventions dedicated to the conservation of all reptiles. By simultaneously targeting all interventions, we are able to review the evidence for each intervention cost-effectively, and the resulting synopsis can be updated periodically and efficiently. The synopsis is freely available at www.conservationevidence.com and, alongside the Conservation Evidence online 17 database, is a valuable asset to the toolkit of practitioners and policy makers seeking sound information to support reptile conservation. We aim to periodically update the synopsis to incorporate new research. The methods used to produce the Reptile Conservation Synopsis are outlined below. This synthesis focuses on global evidence for the effectiveness of interventions for the conservation of reptiles. This subject has not yet been covered using subject-wide evidence synthesis. This is defined as a systematic method of reviewing and synthesising evidence that covers broad subjects (in this case conservation of multiple taxa) at once, including all closed review topics within that subject at a fine scale, and analysing results through study summary and expert assessment, or through meta-analysis. The term can also refer to any product arising from this process (Sutherland et al. 2019). This global synthesis collates evidence for the effects of conservation interventions on terrestrial, aquatic and semi-aquatic reptiles, including all reptile orders, i.e. Crocodilia (alligators, crocodiles and gharials), Testudines (turtles and tortoises), Squamata (snakes, lizards and amphisbaenians) and Rhynchocephalia (tuatara). This synthesis covers evidence for the effects of conservation interventions for wild reptiles (i.e. not in captivity). We have not included evidence from the substantial literature on husbandry of marine and freshwater reptiles kept in zoos or aquariums. However, where these interventions are relevant to the conservation of wild declining or threatened species, they have been included, e.g. captive breeding for the purpose of increasing population sizes (potentially for reintroductions) or gene banking (for future release).
Article
Full-text available
Upon emerging from underground nests, sea turtle hatchlings immediately crawl toward the ocean. The primary cues used in orientation are visual but the nature of the visual cues was a matter of speculation. Hatchlings might also respond to secondary cues, such as beach slope. Experiments were carried out in an arena where specific visual and slope cues, simulating those present at nest sites, could be precisely controlled and manipulated. Subjects were green turtle (Chelonia mydas L.) and loggerhead (Caretta caretta L.) hatchlings. Both species oriented toward the more intensely illuminated sections of the arena. They also oriented away from dark silhouettes which simulated an elevated horizon, typical of the view toward land. Turtles responded primarily to stimuli (both silhouettes and photic differences) at or near eye level. When presented simultaneously with a silhouette and a photic gradient located in different directions, hatchlings oriented away from the silhouette and ignored photic stimuli. Under infrared light, both species oriented down slopes. However in the presence of nocturnal levels of visible light loggerheads ignored slope cues and responses of green turtles to slope were weakened. The data suggest that loggerhead and green turtle hatchlings usually find the sea by orienting away from elevated silhouettes. This is a prominent and reliable cue for species which typically nest on continental beaches.
Article
Full-text available
Artificial lighting disrupts the nocturnal orientation of sea turtle hatchlings as they crawl from their nest to the ocean. Laboratory experiments in an arena were used to simultaneously present artificial light (that attracted the turtles toward “land”) and natural cues (a dark silhouette of the dune behind the beach) that promoted “seaward” orientation. Artificial lighting disrupted seaward crawling in the presence of low silhouettes, but not high silhouettes. Low silhouettes provided adequate cues for seaward crawling when the apparent brightness of artificial light was reduced. Based upon these results, we postulate that artificial light disrupts orientation by competing with natural cues. Current restoration practices at nesting beaches emphasize light reduction. However at many sites some lights cannot be modified. Our results suggest that pairing dune restoration (to enhance natural cues) with light reduction (to the extent possible) should significantly improve hatchling orientation, even at nesting beaches where lighting cannot be entirely eliminated.
Article
Visual cues are important to sea turtle hatchlings in determining seaward direction upon emerging from the nest. In this study, we examined the roles that color and intensity play in the sea-finding mechanisms employed by loggerhead (Caretta caretta) and green turtle (Chelonia mydas) hatchlings. We tested hatchling preference for a standard source of constant intensity and color (1.26× 1015 photons s-1 m-2 at 520 nm), versus an adjustable light source (one of five monochromatic colors at each of seven photon intensities), using a two-choice apparatus. Both species oriented toward near-ultraviolet (360 nm), violet (400 nm), and blue-green (500 nm) light but chose the standard light source over yellow-orange (600 nm) and red (700 nm) light. There was a positive relationship between intensity and preference with 360, 400, and 500 nm light. We also examined hatchling choice of either a darkened window or a window lighted by one of eight monochromatic colors at each of two intensities. In these experiments, loggerheads oriented toward 360, 400, and 500 nm light but away from light in the green-yellow to yellow-orange range (560, 580, and 600 nm). Loggerheads oriented toward 700 nm light only at high intensity. Green turtles responded insignificantly to 600 or 700 nm light at either intensity. The contrast of green turtle behavioral responses with published electrophysiological data and the aversion to yellow light observed in loggerheads suggest some level of spectral quality assessment in sea finding for both species.
Article
Attempts were made to find out why insects and fishes can be captured with the help of lamps, why birds fly against lighthouse lanterns, and why in the laboratory phototaxis is preponderantly positive phototaxis. An extensive review of the literature revealed that none of the numerous old and new theories on photic orientation can account for either of these phenomena. Analysis of the abundance of data on the trapping effect of an artificial light source upon insects, fishes and birds has led to the working hypothesis according to which the low illumination intensity of the environment around such a light source interferes with normal photic orientation resulting in a drift of the animal towards the light source. The observed concentration of animals in the vicinity of a lamp is thought to be the statistical result of this drift. Experiments with insects (bees) demonstrated that an adequate screening of the light scattered from the sky, together with the elimination of the reflection of light by the environment really result in a disorientated drift towards the light source, even when this is the natural light source (the sun). Fishes and birds were forced to move towards a lamp under similar illumination conditions. Photic orientation is assumed to be accomplished by the goal-directed functioning of a number of hierarchically coordinated centres. The animal's movements are controlled by optic feedback based upon the normal differences in the intensities of the light stimuli acting upon the respective photosensitive surfaces. During more detailed orientation, fixation mechanisms are put in circuit by higher coordinating centres in response to sign stimuli. The normal values of these stimuli are determined by the normal angular light distribution in the animal's habitat, which is caused by: i. the nature of the light sources (sun, moon, stars); 2. the scattering and absorbing capacities of the media (the atmosphere and the water) ; and 3. the reflecting capacity of the environment. The abnormal feedback resulting from the abnormal angular light distribution around a lamp-brought about by the elimination of the factors 2 and 3-makes the animal deviate from the intended position or direction of locomotion. Moreover, the servomechanisms of lower coordination levels controlling the fixation movements of the eyes become a play-thing of the stimuli from the lamp that are quantitatively supernormal as compared with the adequate sign stimuli which normally activate the higher coordination centres of the fixation mechanisms. In this way these higher centres are more or less eliminated from the orientation process. Under extreme laboratory illumination conditions this results in a forced drift of the animal towards the lamp irrespective of factors which are incompatible with survival. Similar phenomena in human beings suffering from disturbance of the centres mediating eye movements, and in patients with far advanced cerebral degenerations (apallic syndrome) are thought to favour this concept. The implications of the present concept of photic orientation and disorientation are discussed with regard to the current concepts of pho- totaxis and photokinesis, the light trap technique, some optical illusions, and glaring lights in traffic. The tendency among cyberneticians to overrate the performances of life-imitating-e.g. "phototropic"- machines, which trifle with the complexity of living organisms, is criticized.
Article
IT has been known for many years1 that hatchling green turtles (Chelonia mydas (Linn.)) almost all emerge from the nest after dark. Moorhouse referred to hatchlings which emerged during the day usually burying themselves again, and noted that the few which attempted to reach the water were invariably taken by gulls and herons. Hendrickson2 has pointed out that nocturnal emergence has marked survival value, because during the heat of the day surface sand temperatures would be rapidly lethal to hatchlings and the danger of predation is much greater during the day. He wrote: “It is believed that, upon encountering temperatures much above about 33° C, the hatchlings cease activity in their escape chamber, resuming active movements only when lower temperatures return with the fall of night”.
Article
Field experiments with hatchling green sea turtles (Chelonia mydas) show that a positive phototropotaxis will account for many aspects of their ability to find the sea after emerging from the nest. The tropotactic reaction is discussed with respect to retino-tectal projections in reptiles and it is suggested that visual stimulation of one eye may initiate both ipsilateral and contralateral turning tendencies, depending on which part of the retina is stimulated. The influence of the sun's position and the effects of varying the spectral composition of light stimuli are outlined. Some complexities in experiments on the responses to light of frogs (Rana temporaria) and freshwater turtles (Pseudemys scripta elegans) are reported, and it is concluded that for correlating behaviour with physiological mechanisms a more dependable behaviour such as sea-finding in marine turtles offers some advantages.
Article
How we define a problem often determines what we are willing to consider as a solution. When we define the impending extinction of a sea turtle species solely in terms of there being too few turtles, we are tempted to think of solutions solely in terms of increasing the numbers of turtles. Hence, some of our attempts to conserve sea turtles involve “halfway technology,” which does not address the causes of or provide amelioration for the actual threats turtles face. Programs such as headstarting, captive breeding, and hatcheries may serve only to release more turtle into a degraded environment in which their parents have already demonstrated that they cannot flourish. Furthermore, captive programs may keep turtles from serving important ecological functions in the natural environment, or place them at some disadvantage relative to their natural counterparts once released. Such programs can be contrasted with more appropriate technologies that directly address and correct particular problems encountered by sea turtles without removing them from their natural habitat. For example, installing turtle excluder devices in shrimp trawl nets will reduce mortality of adults and larger juvenile sea turtles, and using low pressure sodium lighting on beaches may prevent hatchlings and nesting females from becoming disoriented. In the final analysis, we need clean and productive marine and coastal environments. Without a commitment to such long term goals, efforts to protect sea turtles will be futile.
Article
The seaward orientation behavior of hatchling loggerhead turtles Caretta caretta when exposed to five different artificial light sources (high-pressure) and low-pressure sodium vapor, and yellow, red, and white incandescent lamps) was examined. Each light source affected hatchling sea-finding performance either in direction of orientation or width of dispersion. Hatchlings were attracted to light sources emitting short-wavelength visible light and long-wavelength sources that excluded intermediate wavelengths. A negative response was observed toward sources emitting predominately yellow light. For this reason, low-pressure sodium vapor (LPS) luminaires, which emit only yellow light, are expected to affect loggerhead hatchling sea-finding minimally, if positioned behind the primary dune. LPS luminaires positioned between emerging hatchlings and the ocean, however, will disrupt hatchling orientation.