Content uploaded by Reesa Sorin
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Reesa Sorin on Jul 09, 2014
Content may be subject to copyright.
Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood, Volume 4, Number 1, 2003
80
Validating Young Children’s
Feelings and Experiences of Fear
REESA SORIN
James Cook University, Cairns, Australia
ABSTRACT Children experience a wide range of emotions, from happiness and
excitement to anger and disgust. When children are happy, their caregivers
encourage their expression of happiness and often join in to share this emotion
with them. Yet when they are angry, afraid or disgusted, often children are
encouraged to suppress or change their emotions. This is particularly true of the
emotion of fear. While parents and caregivers currently employ a variety of
strategies to respond to fear in young children, some of these methods may be
positioning children in ways that increase their fears or invalidate their feelings
of fear altogether. Well-intentioned parents and caregivers may be unaware of
the effect that messages such as ‘there’s nothing to be afraid of’ or ‘don’t worry,
you’re safe with me’, could have on children. This article examines a range of
adult responses to children’s fears and the effectiveness of these responses.
Introduction
Fear is considered by most theorists to be one of a number of emotions that
are innate or basic to the individual (Darwin, 1872; Watson, 1970; Ekman &
Friesen, 1975; Izard, 1977; Plutchik, 1980; McDougall in Strongman, 1987;
Sroufe, 1995). While serving a role in motivating the individual to act in the
face of danger (Darwin, 1872; Izard, 1991; Ledoux, 1998) and to bond with
others for protection (Izard, 1977; Izard & Kobak in Garber & Dodge, 1991),
fear can also have a devastating effect on individual functioning. Described as
‘the most toxic of all emotions’ (Izard, 1977, p. 355), fear can reduce the brain’s
capacity to store and process information (Darke in Hamilton & Mackie,
1993), thus limiting problem-solving ability and learning in general (Gur et al
in Bodenhasen, 1993). Social interactions and interpreting others’ behaviour
can be less positive due to fear (Garber & Dodge, 1991).
Young children may experience a variety of fears (Ollendick, 1983).
These include fear of being separated from a primary caregiver (Bowlby, 1973;
Gardner, 1978), fear of the unfamiliar (Harlow & Mears, 1983), fear of animals
(Bowlby, 1973; Izard, 1977; Sarafino, 1986; Kindt et al, 1996) and fear of harm
(Kindt et al, 1996; Eibl-Eibesfeldt in Ledoux, 1998).
YOUNG CHILDREN AND FEAR
81
Parents and caregivers (university-trained teachers, technical and further
education [TAFE] trained teachers and untrained employees of the venues) are
advised to accept children’s fears (Izard, 1991; Izard & Kobak in Garber &
Dodge, 1991) and to strive for secure attachments as children gradually work
through their fears (Bowlby, 1973; Harlow & Mears, 1983; Herzog, 1996).
They are advised to praise children, to model appropriate emotional responses
(Harris & Saarni, 1989; Rotenberg & Eisenberg, 1997) and to avoid
exaggerating the fear (Berger & Thompson, 1994). Caregivers are encouraged
to provide opportunities for young children to learn about emotions through
play and through teaching them to recognise and verbalise emotions (Catron
& Allen, 1999). The arts are suggested as a tool for teaching emotions (Dunlop,
1984; Goleman, 1995).
This article is based on a study of children’s fears and adults’ responses to
these fears (Sorin, 2001). It involved 45 children aged three to five years and
their parents and caregivers from a range of early childhood educational
services in the state of New South Wales, Australia. These services included a
long day care centre, a private pre-school, a pre-school attached to a public
school and a multifunctional Aboriginal children’s service. The study began
with surveys of parents, caregivers and children, and continued through
weekly observations and ongoing informal interviews with caregivers and
children.
In this research, parents were asked to describe their responses to
children’s fears, and all participants were asked to describe how caregivers
responded to children’s fears. All participants were then asked to evaluate the
effectiveness of caregivers’ current practices in responding to fears.
Responses to Children’s Fears
Both parents and caregivers described a range of techniques for responding to
children’s fears. These included verbal responses, such as reassurance and
explanation, physical responses, such as cuddling and staying in close
proximity to the child, offering the child safety, modelling non-fearful
behaviour, taking the child to the feared object, and redirecting the fearful
child. While well-intentioned parents and caregivers used these techniques to
try to promote children’s understanding of the emotion, not all of them
achieved this goal. Some tended to invalidate or escalate the fear or to increase
the instances in which the child was afraid. This article discusses these
responses in two distinct categories: those that tended to invalidate children’s
fears and those that validated them.
Responses that Invalidate Fear
A number of adults’ responses to children’s fears seemed to invalidate the fear,
by minimising or not accepting it. These included both explicit and implicit
Reesa Sorin
82
verbal messages, redirecting the child, the use of metaphors, controlling
through fear and accepting some emotions but rejecting others.
What Messages are We Sending to Children?
Young children’s ideas about emotions are still formative (Harris & Saarni,
1989), so messages can be misconstrued and fears can be escalated to
unmanageable levels (Arthur et al, 1999). Through explicit or implicit
messages, some verbal responses seemed to increase the child’s fear or
invalidate his/her feelings. For example, Sally, mother of Wade, aged five,
reported her response to Wade’s fears as: ‘I try to tell him not to be afraid, to
make him feel secure about it. I always tell him that he’s safe when I’m with
him’. While Sally’s intention is to address Wade’s fear, she is also giving him
two other messages: firstly, that his fear should not be experienced and
secondly, that while he is safe with her, he may not be safe when she is not
there. Wade’s behaviours, which included refusing to leave Sally when she
was in the pre-school and staying close to her at home, seemed to indicate that
he had received her unintended message and felt safe only in her company.
Adults who tell children ‘there’s nothing to be afraid of’ may actually be
invalidating their feelings; making it more difficult for them to work through
the issues that lead to emotion understanding (Stein & Trabasso, 1989). For
example, on a number of occasions, Matthew, aged five, expressed a fear of
the family house being robbed and someone stealing the baby. In her
frustration and anger with his ongoing fear, Matthew’s mother responded
with, ‘Look, no one’s gonna rob our house. It’s not going to happen; you’ll be
all right’. Matthew’s fear continued, possibly through being invalidated by his
mother. Ray, father of Sophie, aged four, said he told his daughter that while
it’s okay to be afraid, ‘there’s no real need to ... it can’t get you or it won’t hurt
you or Daddy’s got hold of you’. Here Ray made the decision that Sophie
should not be afraid because he was offering her safety. Instead, he could be
encouraging her to construct her own understanding of the situation and of
how she might face similar situations when separated from her father.
Other, more explicit messages, while intended to alert children to
potential danger, may serve to instil unnecessary fear in young children. This
is demonstrated in the following transcript:
Laura [mother of Kevin, aged four]: Hopefully he’s afraid of cars on the road. [To
Kevin]: You can’t go running out to the road, can you?
Kevin: NO, they’ll get me.
Laura: Yeah, that’s right.
While Laura was intending to protect her child from potential harm, she may
also be escalating his fear of traffic unnecessarily (Izard, 1991), as is implied in
Kevin’s comment about the cars: ‘they’ll get me.’ It may have been better to
talk with Kevin and encourage him to understand and assess the situation,
rather than to increase his fear of traffic (Sorin, 2001).
YOUNG CHILDREN AND FEAR
83
Redirecting
Redirecting children who express fear may serve as a ‘stop-gap’ measure, but
does not actively address the fear or help children to understand it. The
following scenario between caregiver Melissa and four year-old Kyle
demonstrates this.
Kyle (crying): I want mummy and daddy.
Melissa: Finish morning tea and we’ll do some woodwork. (She got a tissue and
wiped Kyle’s nose. Kyle continued crying and saying that he wanted his
parents)
Melissa: Don’t cry. That’s enough. You’re going to choke on your food. (She
talked to him about the woodwork the children are doing).
Kyle: I want daddy and mummy.
Melissa (patted Kyle’s head): You’ll see them at 3:00. (She stroked Kyle’s
back, wiped his eyes and kept reassuring him).
Melissa: I can see some chooks over there. If you’ll eat your banana, we can go
look at them.
Kyle stopped crying and looked toward the chickens. They went to see the
chickens together. (Sorin, 2001)
Melissa persevered and was eventually able to distract Kyle and redirect him to
other activities. However, his separation fear persisted during the time I
remained at the pre-school. Redirecting the child served as a temporary
measure only, but is another form of invalidating the child’s fear. Kyle was
inadvertently being told to repress and internalise his fear, which as an
unresolved emotion in early childhood holds potential to become a source of
anxiety and conflict later in life (Santrock, 1994).
Metaphors
While metaphors were both reported as a response by some adults and
encouraged in the literature (Dunlop, 1984), their impact on young children is
questionable. Christa, mother of Keiran (aged five) and Toby (aged three),
explained to her children that thunder is ‘God playing ten-pin bowling’. Rather
than assisting children to process their fear of thunder, the use of metaphor
with very young children whose understanding does not go beyond the literal
may actually increase their fear of thunder, while also encouraging a fear of
bowling and possibly of God. May’s more gentle metaphor of thunder being
‘clouds clapping together’ may have been easier for her four year-old son,
Aidan, to understand, but could still have confused him and left him fearful.
Control through Fear
While fear should not be used to control a child (Izard, 1991; Berger &
Thompson, 1994), it is often used in this way by adults. Karen, a caregiver and
Reesa Sorin
84
mother, explained that Aboriginal children are taught in the home ‘not to be
afraid of their fear ... It’s just in their culture’. She described her situation at
home with her children, noting, ‘I say to my kids, Gunje [an Aboriginal “hairy
man” spirit] is gonna get you. He’s comin’ there’. While Karen justified this
usage of fear as part of a cultural tradition that accepted fear, the element of
controlling children through fear is unmistakable. In other cultures and
families, ‘Gunje’ takes the form of the Bogey Man, or other characters that
parents claim will ‘get’ the child for misbehaviour. This can serve not only as a
deterrent to behaviour, but also as a great source of fear. Adults are advised
against using these ‘fantasy untruths’ as ways of scaring young children into
good behaviour (Berger & Thompson, 1994, p. 271).
Valildating Some Emotions While Invalidating Others
While parents and caregivers celebrate the expression of some emotions, other
emotions are ignored, stifled or discouraged in young children. For example,
happiness, considered by most people as a positive emotion, is an emotion
encouraged in children. Caregiver Ida said of happiness:
If a child’s happy, I think everybody’s happy. If a child’s happy, we always
maintain to keep that child happy at all times ... We might copy that child in
looking at a sad child to try and bring that child around to make that child as
happy. (Sorin, 2001)
Ida was encouraging happiness, not only as a passing emotion but as a
permanent state. At the same time she was invalidating the sadness another
child felt by attempting to have the sad child observe and copy the happy
child’s emotion (Bandura in Plutchik & Kellerman, 1990). While her intentions
are altruistic, she is nonetheless invalidating an emotion that she considered
unacceptable for the child to experience. Other caregivers said that they would
try to change sadness to happiness or reassure children that everything is okay
(Sorin, 2001). Everything is not okay, or the child would not be experiencing
the sadness, so this is another way of invalidating children’s feelings.
High cognitive functioning in older children and adults is influenced by
the understanding and regulating of emotions in the pre-school years
(Eisenberg et al, 1997). By invalidating fear and other emotions, children are
not given the opportunity to understand and deal with fear and its associated
issues. They may be distracted or reassured for a brief period of time, but the
feelings persist and remain unresolved and this could lead to problems later in
life (Ledoux, 1998).
Responses that Validate Fear
By validating fear and other emotional experiences, adults can encourage
children to explore a range of emotions and to develop an understanding of
them. Parents and caregivers reported validating children’s fears in a number
YOUNG CHILDREN AND FEAR
85
of ways, including empathy and acknowledgement, explanation and
discussion, exploration and expression, and taking action against the fear.
Empathy and Acknowledgment
Empathy, putting yourself in the child’s position, is another way of validating
children’s fear as well as helping adults to see a situation from the child’s
perspective. Leona, mother of four year-old Cindy, said she empathised with
Cindy’s fear and told her ‘It’s okay to experience her emotion’. By
acknowledging children’s fears, adults are not only validating children’s
feelings, but are also positioning themselves to utilise other techniques to help
them to deal with their fears. Caregiver Simone said that her response to
children’s fears is to ‘Find out first what it is that they’re afraid of, then
acknowledge their fears, and try and talk them through it’. Adults who teach
children to accept fear as an inevitable part of life and to not be overwhelmed
by it are those who also acknowledge and accept fear that they also experience
from time to time (Izard, 1991).
Explanation and Discussion
Adults who used explanation and discussion to respond to children’s fears
reported more success and less anger toward their children. Ann, mother of
Avral (aged five), described her response to Avral’s fears: ‘I ask what her fears
are and we sit down and talk about why she’s afraid of it and I explain, well I
tell her that her fears are justified or I explain why she should not be afraid of
it’. However, Ann is still making the judgement for Avral as to whether her
fear is justified, rather than helping to develop Avral’s agency in making her
own decisions. Other adults reported that they gave children the opportunity
to judge fears themselves. Maggie said, ‘We try to encourage the children to
consider, for example, spiders as part of the natural world and that we all have
our place ... while things are dangerous, so long as we’re careful and take a
responsible attitude’.
Through explanation and discussion, children are included in the
assessment and evaluation of their fears. This helps them to take responsibility
for themselves, and shows acceptance of the fear and support for the child’s
growing emotional understanding (Arthur et al, 1999).
Exploration and Expression
Besides explanations from adults, children can be encouraged to explore for
themselves to develop a better understanding of the situation and to find
suitable ways to express their fear. Renee, a parent of a pre-school child, noted
that ‘When a child is feeling sad because he or she has no one to play with, it
may be difficult for the child to express or communicate the feeling. This is
where caregivers can help by accepting the feeling and giving the child ways to
Reesa Sorin
86
express it’. This process can be facilitated by helping children to understand
more about the feared object and by offering children words and ways to
express their emotions. Children can be encouraged to seek more information
about the feared object or situation. Caregiver Simone stated, ‘If it’s a fear that
was a really strong fear, like every time a thunderstorm came, I would
probably work with the child and encourage them to find out more about
thunderstorms ... like how does it happen – and look in books, introduce songs
and all that, relevant to that fear’.
Adults can also offer the child ways of responding to the fearful situation.
Kuebli (1994) urges adults to explain to children emotions that may occur in
various situations as well as suggesting ways that the child can express each
emotion. These could include talking about the fear, visually representing the
fear through drawing, painting or sculpture, or expressing the fear through
movement, music or drama.
Taking Action against the Fear
At times, taking some form of physical action is an appropriate way of
addressing children’s fears (Berger & Thompson, 1994). This can involve
facing the feared object, or acting out the situation of confronting the feared
object. Mother Hayley reported that her five year-old son, Lenny, was afraid of
spiders.
She asked him, ‘Can we look at the spider together?’ And if he says, ‘Yep that’s
fine’, then we go and have a look at the spider and talk about spiders and also try
and say, ‘We’re very big in comparison to that spider and he’s going to be really,
really scared of us as well. And he wants to just get away from you rather than
hurt you.’
YOUNG CHILDREN AND FEAR
87
Besides giving Lenny the choice of facing his fear, Hayley has also offered
him some information that will help him to understand and address his
fear.
Other adults took action by turning on the lights in a dark room, or taking the
child outside during a storm. Renee, mother of Adam (aged four), said that
when Adam demonstrated a fear of the dark, she had ‘gone back into the room
with him to turn the light on and show him there’s nothing there’. Rather than
Renee showing Adam that nothing is there, she could look around with him
and allow him to discover that there is nothing there, as a way of allowing him
to take responsibility for his understanding of the situation.
Conclusion
While both parents and caregivers report a range of approaches in responding
to children’s fears, not all of these approaches may be helpful in assisting
children to understand and address their fears. Some may even invalidate or
increase the child’s fear. This is an issue of particular concern to early
childhood educators as it is during the early childhood years that emotions are
being shaped, and children’s understanding of emotions is of critical
importance to overall learning and development (Harris & Saarni, 1989). How
we as educators respond to children’s fear and other emotions can make a
difference to how the child comes to know and understand her/himself.
Correspondence
Dr Reesa Sorin, School of Education, James Cook University, PO Box 6811,
Cairns, Queensland 4870, Australia (reesa.sorin@jcu.edu.au).
References
Arthur, L., Beecher, B., Dockett, S., Farmer, S. & Death, E. (1999) Programming and
Planning in Early Childhood Settings, 2nd edn. Sydney: Harcourt.
Berger, E.H. & Thompson, R.A. (1994) The Developing Person through the Lifespan, 3rd
edn. New York: Worth.
Bodenhasen, G.V. (1993) Emotions, Arousal, and Stereotypic Judgments: a heuristic
model of affect and stereotyping, in D. Mackie & D. Hamilton (Eds) Affect,
Cognition, and Stereotyping: interactive processes in group perception, pp. 13-37. San
Diego: Academic Press.
Bowlby, J. (1973) Attachment and Loss. Vol. 2: Separation – anxiety and anger. London:
Penguin.
Catron, C. & Allen, J. (1999) Early Childhood Curriculum, 2nd edn. Englewood Cliffs:
Prentice Hall.
Darwin, C. (1872) The Expression of Emotions in Man and Animals. New York:
Philosophical Library.
Reesa Sorin
88
Dunlop, F. (1984) The Education and Feeling of Emotion. London: Allen & Unwin.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R., Shepard, S., Murphy, B., Guthrie, I., Jones, S., Friedman, J.,
Poulin, R. & Maszk, P. (1997) Contemporaneous and Longitudinal Prediction of
Children’s Social Functioning from Regulation and Emotionality, Child
Development, 68, pp. 642-664.
Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. (1975) Unmasking the Face: a guide to recognising emotions from
facial clues. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.
Garber, J. & Dodge, K.A. (1991) The Development of Emotion Regulation and
Dysregulation. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Gardner, H. (1978) Developmental Psychology. Boston: Little, Brown.
Goleman, D. (1995) Emotional Intelligence. New York: Bantam.
Hamilton, D.L. & Mackie, D.M. (1993) Cognitive and Affective Processes in
Intergroup Perception: the developing interface, in D. Mackie & D. Hamilton
(Eds) Affect, Cognition, and Stereotyping – interactive processes in group perception,
pp. 1-11. San Diego: Academic Press.
Harlow, H.F. & Mears, C.E. (1983) Emotional Sequences and Consequences, in
R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds) Emotion – theory, research and experience. Vol. 2.
Emotions in Early Development, pp. 171-197. New York: Academic Press.
Harris, P.L. & Saarni, C. (1989) Children’s Understanding of Emotion: an introduction,
in C. Saarni & P.L. Harris (Eds) Children’s Understanding of Emotion, pp. 3-24. New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Herzog, J. (1996) Fear, Sense of Humor, Stress and Separation: birth to two, Sesame
Street Parents. Available at: http://ctw.org/3496/349603tl.htm [1997, February 2].
Izard, C.E. (1977) Human Emotions. New York: Plenum.
Izard, C.E. (1991) The Psychology of Emotions. New York: Plenum.
Kindt, M., Brosschot, J. & Muris, P. (1996) Spider Phobia Questionnaire for Children
(SPQ-C): a psychometric study and normative data, Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 34, pp. 277-282.
Kuebli, J. (1994) Young Children’s Understanding of Everyday Emotions, Young
Children, March, pp. 36-47.
Ledoux, J. (1998) The Emotional Brain. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Ollendick, T. (1983) Reliability and Validity of the Revised Fear Survey Schedule for
Children (FSSC–R), Behaviour Research and Therapy, 21, pp. 685-692.
Plutchik, R. (1980) Emotion: a psychoevolutionary synthesis. New York: Harper & Row.
Plutchik, R. & Kellerman, H. (Eds) (1990) Emotion: theory, research, and experience. Vol.
5. Emotion, Psychopathology and Psychotherapy. San Diego: Academic Press.
Rotenberg, K. & Eisenberg, N. (1997) Developmental Differences in the
Understanding of and Reaction to Others’ Inhibition of Emotional Expression,
Developmental Psychology, 33, pp. 526-537.
Santrock, J. (1994) Child Development (6th Edn). Madison, WI: Brown & Benchmark
Publishers.
Sarafino, E. (1986) The Fears of Childhood: a guide to recognising and reducing fearful states
in children. New York: Human Sciences Press.
YOUNG CHILDREN AND FEAR
89
Sorin, R. (2001) Responding to the Fears of Early Childhood – an investigation of
perception and practice in emotion education, unpublished doctoral thesis,
University of Wollongong.
Sroufe, L. (1995) Emotional Development: the organisation of emotional life in the early years.
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Stein, N.L. & Trabassa, T. (1989) Young Children’s Acquisition of Emotion Concepts,
in C. Saarni & P.L. Harris (Eds) Children’s Understanding of Emotion, pp. 50-77. New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Strongman, K. (1987) The Psychology of Emotion. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
Watson, J. (1970) Behaviorism. New York: W.W. Norton.