ArticlePDF Available

Do Interrupted Users Work Faster or Slower? The Micro-analysis of Computerized Text Editing Task

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Previous research on the effects of interruptions on the speed of performing the computerized tasks gave rather non-homogeneous results: many authors insist that interrupted users always complete tasks slower than when performing the same tasks without interruption, but others showed that interrupting a user during some categories of tasks caused that user to complete the tasks faster. The micro-analysis of concrete text editing operations conducted in our experimental study revealed the difference between the effects of interruptions on cognitively simple and cognitively complex tasks. While the performance of simple tasks was not influenced by interruptions, interruptions slowered complex task performance. The task re-orientation after the interruption was found to be responsible for performance degradation.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Do
Interrupted Users Work Faster
or
Slower?
The
Micro-analysis
of
Computerized Text Editing Task
Ivan
Burmistrov
Anna
Leonova
Moscow
State University
Department
of
Psychology
8-5
Mokhovaya
Ul.
Moscow
103009
Russia
ivan@psychology.ru, aleon@chair.cogsci.msu.su
Abstract
Previous
research
on the
effects
of
interruptions
on the
speed
of
performing
the
computerized
tasks
gave rather non-homogeneous results: many authors insist that interrupted users always
complete
tasks slower than when performing
the
same tasks without interruption,
but
others
showed
that interrupting
a
user during some categories
of
tasks caused that user
to
complete
the
tasks
faster.
The
micro-analysis
of
concrete text editing operations conducted
in our
experimental
study
revealed
the
difference between
the
effects
of
interruptions
on
cognitively simple
and
cognitively
complex
tasks.
While
the
performance
of
simple
tasks
was not
influenced
by
interruptions,
interruptions slowered complex task performance.
The
task re-orientation
after
the
interruption
was
found
to be
responsible
for
performance degradation.
1
Introduction
Research
on the
effects
of
interruptions
on the
computerized work
has
extensively proliferated
in
the
last
five
years (see McFarlane
&
Latorella (2002)
for
detailed review
of the
research
findings
and the
existing user
interface
design literature relative
to
coordinating human interruption).
In
many cases,
this
research gave rather non-homogeneous results.
For
example, Bailey,
Konstan
&
Carlis (2000,
2001)
and
Cutrell, Czerwinski
&
Horvitz (2000, 2001) insist that interrupted users
always
complete tasks slower than when performing
the
same tasks without interruption.
However,
experiments
of
Speier, Valacich
&
Vessey (1997, 1999)
and
Zijlstra, Roe, Leonova
&
Krediet
(1999) showed that interrupting
a
user during some types
of
tasks
(e.g.,
the
document-
editing
tasks
and
simple decision-making tasks) caused that user
to
complete
the
tasks
faster.
Clearly,
the
conclusions derived
from
these independent studies
are
inconsistent
and
further
investigation
into
the
effects
of
interruptions
on a
user's
task performance
is
required.
It
must
be
noted, that
in
abovementioned experiments, researchers used
"macro"
measures
of
task
performance
such
as
"time
on
task"
(TOT)
and did not
conduct
a
micro-analysis
of
concrete
operations,
of
which
the the
whole task consists.
The
main idea
of our
experiment
was to
conduct
this
micro-analysis
of
concrete operations.
621
2
Experiment
2.1
Design
In
our
experiment,
30
subjects performed
a
computer-assisted text editing tasks.
The
experimental
task
was to
make corrections
in a
computer
file,
based
on a
hard-copy version
of a
text containing
hand-written corrections.
No a
priori time limits
for
completing
the
tasks were given,
and
subjects
could work
at
their
own
pace. Experimental sessions took approximately
40
minutes
each,
depending
on the
individual
speed.
During
the
experimental sessions, subject's work activity
was
disturbed
by a
number
of
interruptions
-
phone calls
-
when
the
subject
was
told
to
perform another task, referred
to as
secondary task. Interruptions affected three types
of
concrete editing operations:
(a)
typing
in new
text
-
new,
(b)
regular editing (making simple corrections)
-
regular,
(c)
moving
a
block
of
text
to
a
new
location
-
move.
The
independent variable
was the
presence/absence
of
interruption.
The
dependent variable
was
editing latency
for
concrete operations.
2.2
Apparatus
and
Materials
The
experiment took place
in a
simulated
office
environment.
The 40
m2
laboratory
was
divided
into
two
rooms
by a
wall.
One
room
has
been equipped
as an
office
workplace
(with
furniture,
personal computer, intercom telephone), while
the
other
was
used
as a
control room.
At the
office
location
a
tripod video camera
was
placed
to
monitor
the
subject.
The
video signals
from
the
camera
and
from
computer screen were routed
to a
video mixer
in the
adjacent control room. From
this
room
the
experimenter controlled
the
experiment
and
watched
the
mixed video signal
(view
of
the
subject plus contents
of the
subject's computer screen)
via the
video monitor.
The
mixed video
signal supplied with 0.01 second precision timecode
was
also recorded
on a VCR for
further
analysis.
2.3
Moments
Measured
and
Time
Intervals
Calculated
The
general scheme
of
moments measured
in the
experiment
and
time intervals calculated
for
data
analysis
is
presented
in
Figure
1.
Moments
measured:
Tstart
and
Tend:
start
and end of
operation;
tring:
moment
of
ringing
of the
phone;
picking
up the
phone
has
been considered
to be the
start
of the
secondary task
(t^);
the
last visible operation
of the
secondary
task
has
been considered
to be the end of the
secondary task
(tend);
Tstop:
stop
of
performing
the
main
task,
i. e.
full
switch
to the
secondary
task;
resumption
of the
main task
(Tresumption):
return
of
subject's attention
to the
main task
after
finishing the
secondary
task;
point
of
continuation
(Tcontinuation):
first
action
in
continuation
of the
main task
after
interruption.
622
Figure
1:
Moments
measured
in the
experiment
and
time intervals calculated
for
data analysis
Time
intervals calculated:
operation time
(Toperation
=
Tend
-
Tstart):
time
spent
to
perform
the
operation
including
the
secondary task;
duration
of
interruption
(tjnterruption
=
tend
-
tstart):
time between picking
up the
phone
and
the
last visible operation
of the
secondary task;
returns
to
main task
(Treturns):
sum
time
of
returns
to the
main task while working
on the
secondary
task;
break-between
(Tbreak-between
=
Tresumption
-
tend):
time interval between
the
last visible
operation
of the
secondary task
and
starting
the
resumption
of the
main task;
orientation
(Torientation
=
Tcontinuation
-
Tresump,ion):
time between starting
the
resumption
of
the
main task
and the first
action
in
continuation
of the
main task;
changeover
(Tchangeover
=
Tbreak.between
+
Torientation):
a sum of
break-between
and
orientation;
623
net
operation
time
(Tnet
=
Toperation
-
tinterruptjon
+
Treturns):
operation
time
minus
duration
of
interruption
plus
returns
to the
main
task;
net
operation
time
minus
time
of
orientation
(Qnet
=
Tnct
-
Torientation)-
3
Results
3.1
Effects
of
Presence
of
Interruptions
on
Concrete Operation
Performance
The
ANOVA revealed significant main
effect
of the
presence
of
interruption
on the net
operation
time
(Tnet)
for
operation
move,
F(l,93)
=
9.91,
p -
0.0022,
but
showed
no
significant
effect
of the
presence
of
interruption
for
operations
new
and
regular.
After
obtaining this result,
we
made
an
attempt
to
answer
the
question
why net
operation time
increases when operation
is
interrupted.
Our
working hypothesis
was
that this increase could
be
explained
by the
time
of
re-orientation
in the
main task
after
completing
the
secondary task
(Torientation)-
In
order
to
confirm this hypothesis,
we
conducted
the
same analysis
for the net
operation time minus time
of
orientation
(Qnet).
Difference between experimental conditions
Yes
Interruption
and No
Interruption became nonsignificant. This
finding
suggests that namely
the
orientation interval
(Torien,atjon)
is
mainly responsible
for
increase
in the net
operation time
(Tne,)
for
interrupted operations.
3.2
Factors That Influence
the
Orientation
In
order
to
explore factors that
may
influence orientation interval
(Tonenlalion),
we
have analysed
its
dependence
on the
type
of
interrupted operation. Mean values
for
Torientation
for
operations
new,
regular,
and
move
were 5.4, 8.7,
and
12.8 seconds, respectively.
The
difference
was
significant
between operations
new and
regular,
/(38)
=
-2.15,
p =
0.038,
and
between operations
new and
move,
t(55)
=
-3.04,/>
=
0.004.
4
Discussion
Statistical analyses revealed
the
significant
effects
of
presence/absence
of
interruptions
on the
editing latencies
for
cognitively
complex editing operations (such
as
moving
a
paragraph
to a new
location), while
the
performance
for
cognitively simple editing actions
(e. g.
typing
in a new
paragraph) were
not
affected
by
interruptions.
A
probable explanation
to
this
fact
may be
that
operation
new is the
simplest operation
in
text editing.
It
involve neither search
and
location
of
some point
in the
text
(as for
operation regular)
nor
include complex sequences
of
actions
and
additional mental load caused
by the
necessity
to
mentally track
the
contents
of the
clipboard
(as
for
operation move). Operation move
is an
example
of a
"functional
thread",
i. e. a
series
of
commands
or
actions,
and
effects
of
interruptions
on
this type
of
operations were more disruptive.
These
results
are
consistent with findings
of
Speier, Valacich
&
Vessey
(1997,
1999), where
interruptions were found
to
facilitate performance
on
simple tasks,
while
inhibiting
performance
on
more complex
tasks,
and
also results
of
Bailey,
Konstan
&
Carlis (2000, 2001), where
interruptions slowered
all
categories
of
tasks
except
registration
task,
because
the
latter required
the
lowest memory load
at the
point
of
interruption
and
less
effort
to
resume
the
main task
after
interruption.
624
Our
results also suggest that
the
task re-orientation
after
the
interruption
is
mainly responsible
for
increase
in net
operation time
(Tnet)
for
interrupted operations.
5
Acknowledgements
This
work
has
been supported
by the
Russian Foundation
for
Basic Research grant
#
02-06-80189.
References
Bailey,
B. P.,
Konstan,
J.
A.,
&
Carlis,
J. V.
(2000). Measuring
the
effects
of
interruptions
on
task
performance
in the
user interface.
In
IEEE
Conference
on
Systems, Man,
and
Cybernetics
2000
(SMC
2000), IEEE, 757-762.
Bailey,
B. P.,
Konstan,
J.
A.,
&
Carlis,
J. V.
(2001).
The
effects
of
interruptions
on
task
performance,
annoyance,
and
anxiety
in the
user interface.
In
Human-Computer Interaction
-
INTERACT2001
Conference
Proceedings.
IOS
Press,
IFIP,
593-601.
Cutrell,
E.
B.,
Czerwinski,
M,
&
Horvitz,
E.
(2000).
Effects
of
instant messaging interruptions
on
computing
tasks.
In
Proceedings
of
the CHI
2000
conference
on
Human factors
in
computing
systems, Extended
Abstracts.
New
York:
ACM
Press, 99-100.
Cutrell,
E.
B.,
Czerwinski,
M.,
&
Horvitz,
E.
(2001). Notification, disruption,
and
memory:
Effects
of
messaging interruptions
on
memory
and
performance.
In
Human-Computer
Interaction-INTERACT
2001
Conference
Proceedings.
IOS
Press,
IFIP,
263-269.
McFarlane,
D. C., &
Latorella,
K. A.
(2002).
The
scope
and
importance
of
human interruption
in
human-computer
interaction design. Human-Computer
Interaction,
17
(1),
1-61.
Speier,
C.,
Valacich,
J.
S.,
&
Vessey,
I.
(1997).
The
effects
of
task
interruption
and
information
presentation
on
individual decision making.
In
Proceedings
of the
XVIII
International
Conference
on
Information
Systems. Atlanta: Association
for
Information Systems,
21-36.
Speier,
C.,
Valacich,
J.
S.,
&
Vessey,
I.
(1999).
The
influence
of
task interruption
on
individual
decision
making:
An
information overload perspective. Decision
Sciences,
30
(2), 337-360.
Zijlstra,
F. R. H.,
Roe,
R.
A.,
Leonova,
A. B. &
Krediet,
I.
(1999). Temporal factors
in
mental
work:
Effects
of
interrupted ativities. Journal
of
Occupational
and
Organizational
Psychology,
12,
163-185.
625
... Thus, these guidelines may not apply to interruptions relevant to the complex task of debugging. Regarding complex tasks (such as tasks that require the user to hold many things in their short-term memory), interrupting the user during the task can harm their performance because they must re-orient themselves when returning to the primary task [1, 2]. Although choosing appropriate times to interrupt them [7, 10] can reduce the reorientation penalty, overall this body of research suggests that immediate-style interruptions will slow down users' debugging. ...
... Most previous research has found that immediate-style interruptions have a negative impact on performance [1, 2, 7, 14, 16, 18], although generally, relevant interruptions do less harm than irrelevant ones. But prior work has not addressed how relevant interruptions affect performance when attempting to support cognitively complex tasks such as debugging. ...
... As previously mentioned, other researchers have established that there is a reorientation period after an immediate-style interruption [1, 2], and this should hold true in the case of debugging as well. But, is that the only reason for the productivity difference? ...
Article
Full-text available
Although researchers have begun to explicitly support end- user programmers' debugging by providing information to help them find bugs, there is little research addressing the proper mechanism to alert the user to this information. The choice of alerting mechanism can be important, because as previous research has shown, different interruption styles have different potential advantages and disadvantages. To explore impacts of interruptions in the end-user debugging domain, this paper describes an empirical comparison of two interruption styles that have been used to alert end-user programmers to debugging information. Our results show that negotiated-style interruptions were superior to immediate-style interruptions in several issues of importance to end-user debugging, and further suggest that a reason for this superiority may be that immediate-style interruptions
... Other studies describe a more complex picture and suggest that the effects of interruptions are not necessarily negative (O'Connail andFrohlich 1995, Zijlstra et al. 1999) and others even report on positive effects on work (O'Connail and Frohlich 1995, Speier et al. 1997, Bailey et al. 2000. Burmistrov and Leonova (2003) suggest that interruptions facilitate individual performance in the case of simple tasks, but inhibit performance in the case of complex tasks. Cutrell and colleagues (2001) found that the disruptive effect of an interruption was dependent on timing but also the relevance of an interruption for the current task. ...
... There is not much insight in existing research on how to use specific strategies in real-work con-38 texts as most prior studies of interruptions are based on laboratory experiments (e.g. Cutrell et al. 2000, Bailey et al. 2001, Burmistrov and Leonova 2003, Altman and Trafton 2004, with a specific focus on individual attitudes and behavior towards interruptions (e.g. Horwitz and Apaible 2003, Czerwinski et al. 2004, Dabbish andKraut 2004). ...
... A script captured participant responses to a database unobtrusively in the background. For the exit survey, we adapted measures of decision quality from prior research, such as comprehension and response time (Bailey, Konstan, and Carlis 2001;Burmistrov and Leonova 2003;McFarlane 2002), and decision accuracy (Eyrolle and Cellier 2000). As shown in Table 4, questions required the respondent to rank the conditions from easiest to hardest at different stages of the decision. ...
Article
Full-text available
Anti-malware software must be frequently updated in order to protect the system and the user from attack. Makers of this software must choose between interrupting the user to update immediately or allowing them to update later. In either case, assessing the content of the interruption may still require cognitive investment. However, by allowing the user to negotiate a delayed response to these interruptions, users can instead focus on their work. This paper experimentally examines the effect of immediate and negotiated interruptions on user decision time and decision accuracy in multiple stage tasks. For complex tasks, decision performance is higher when the user can negotiate the onset of and response to interruptions. The option to defer response also results in greater subjective perceptions of control, improved task resumption and reduced feelings of interruption and distraction on the part of the user, even within a short period of time. These findings have practical implications for endpoint security and where there is a need to mitigate the effects of user interruptions from computer-mediated communication in complex task situations.
... With interactive technologies becoming increasingly pervasive the interruptions caused by the technologies are becoming pervasive, as well. Understanding how interruptions happen, their effect on people and their activities, as well as possible ways to designing technological support for handling interruptions are key research questions in HCI and CSCW [4,9,11]. ...
Article
Full-text available
In recent decades technology-induced interruptions emerged as a key object of study in HCI and CSCW research, but until recently the social dimension of interruptions has been relatively neglected. The focus of existing research on interruptions has been mostly on their direct effects on the persons whose activities are interrupted. Arguably, however, it is also necessary to take into account the "ripple effect" of interruptions, that is, indirect consequences of interruptions within the social context of an activity, to properly understand interrupting behavior and provide advanced technological support for handling interruptions. This paper reports an empirical study, in which we examine a set of facets of the social context of interruptions, which we identified in a previous conceptual analysis. The results suggest that people do take into account various facets of the social context when making decisions about whether or not it is appropriate to interrupt another person.
... The only way is to call and interrupt him and thereby invade his solitude [23] [62]. Once being interrupted, the receiver of a call needs up to 15 minutes to recover and get back to his former work performance [26] [111]. As having a prior knowledge of the communication availability of remote colleagues is critical factor for the success of communication, it is not surprising that Bellotti and Bly [11] found phone conversations between remote team members to be infrequent compared to the local interactions. ...
Article
Full-text available
This paper illustrates the importance of awareness and informal communication in work environments and outlines the resulting implications for the design of future information and communication technologies. The paper starts by providing an introduction into intellectual teamwork, showing its benefits and explaining, why informal communication and awareness are decisive factors for successful teamwork. After a brief characterization of virtual teams, it is shown, how the lack of physical proximity effects distributed teamwork. The paper closes with an assessment of state-of-the-art communication media and illustrating why existing communication technologies are not adequate for supporting awareness and informal communication in future work environments.
... Research results across decades have supported both kinds of theories, with some findings of negative interruption effects (Schiffman & Greist-Bousquet, 1992;Schuh, 1978), some findings of negligible effects (O'Conaill & Frohlich, 1995), and some findings of positive and negative effects depending on factors such as task complexity (Burmistrov & Leonova, 2003;Speier, Valacich, & Vessey, 1999;Tétard, Learning and Individual Differences 30 (2014) Trafton and Monk's (2008) review concluded that interruption complexity, similarity of the primary and secondary tasks, control over interruption engagement, and availability of retrieval cues were the most consistent predictors of disruptiveness. Specifically, simple, brief interruptions were less disruptive than complex, long ones. ...
Article
Full-text available
It has become common practice for people to multitask with electronic devices in everyday situations. We examined the effects of interrupting participants with instant messages while they watched a video presentation in a situation that resembled commonplace events such as a business meeting, a training presentation, or a classroom lecture. We compared them to participants who were not interrupted. We also investigated how interest in the topics presented affected learning. Results showed that interruptions reduced learning, by a small but statistically significant margin, which is consistent with the findings of similar studies. Importantly, interest level was as strong a predictor of learning as being interrupted, although interest did not moderate the effect of interruptions. Results showed that interruptions are disruptive but perhaps not as much as is commonly believed. The results also highlight the importance of studying individual difference factors, such as interest levels, in conjunction with experimental manipulations, when assessing the effects of multitasking.
... Interruptions in the workplace have been studied in many specific domains including decision-making (Speier, Valacich, & Vessey, 1997), map-reading (Nivala & Sarjakoski, 2004), code-debugging (Robertson et al., 2004), text editing (Burmistrov & Leonova, 2003), skill acquisition (Langan-Fox, Armstrong et al., 2002), and within organizational life (Jett & George, 2003). Despite the attention given to workplace interruptions, there has been very little focus of this issue in the context of information search and retrieval. ...
Article
Full-text available
In this paper, we explore the characteristics of workplace situation and their impact on the search process, with an emphasis on disruptions and interruptions in the workplace.
Article
Full-text available
Computer-mediated communication (CMC) interruptions are a common feature of people’s work activity. In studying interruptions, researchers can understand how people manage and co-ordinate their work when faced with multiple, often competing, demands. However, CMC interruptions are characteristically different from each other and impact people’s work performance in different ways. In this theoretical paper, we define and differentiate between computer-mediated communication (CMC) interruptions, according to the level of control people are able to exert over an interruption at different points in the delivery timeline. Informed by the extant interruptions literature and Action Regulation Theory, a classification framework is provided, to help researchers and work designers distinguish which types of real-world CMC interruption are more or less disruptive, based on levels of control. Using the developed framework, two key research propositions are made, which we encourage future research to attend to. Unique contributions and implications of this paper are discussed.
Conference Paper
This paper presents our efforts in redesigning the conventional on/off interruption management tactic (a.k.a. "Do Not Disturb Mode") for situations where interruptions are inevitable. We introduce an implicit dialogue injection system, in which the computer implicitly observes the user's state of busyness from passive measurement of the prefrontal cortex to determine how to interrupt the user. We use functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS), a noninvasive brain-sensing technique. In this paper, we describe our system architecture and report results of our proof-of-concept study, in which we compared two contrasting interruption strategies; the computer either forcibly interrupts the user with a secondary task or requests the user's participation before presenting it. The latter yielded improved user experience (e.g. lower reported annoyance), in addition to showing a potential improvement in task performance (i.e. retaining context information) when the user was busier. We conclude that tailoring the presentation of interruptions based on real-time user state provides a step toward making computers more considerate of their users.
Article
Full-text available
The present study examined human-computer interactions in which the operator has to detect errors made by software designed to automatically recognize technical documents. The goal was to assess the effect of user-initiated interruptions of the recognition process to correct these errors. Participants were asked to check the interpretations, either with or without the possibility of interrupting the process. Results showed that interruptions can improve efficiency by decreasing task duration, especially in the post-recognition verification phase. Interruptions provide an opportunity to correct errors during rather than after the recognition process, which is easier because it requires fewer cognitive resources.
Article
Full-text available
At first glance it seems absurd that busy people doing important jobs should want their computers to interrupt them. Interruptions are disruptive and people need to concentrate to make good decisions. However, successful job perfor- mance also frequently depends on people's abilities to (a) constantly monitor their dynamically changing information environments, (b) collaborate and communi- cate with other people in the system, and (c) supervise background autonomous services. These critical abilities can require people to simultaneously query a large set of information sources, continuously monitor for important events, and re- spond to and communicate with other human operators. Automated monitoring
Article
Full-text available
This paper describes a study that probes the cost of interrupting users with instant messages during different "phases" of a computing task. We found that interrupting users during the "evaluation phase" of the task resulted in significantly longer completion times than interruptions in other phases. We also found that interruptions that were irrelevant to the task resulted in longer times to process the message and longer task resumption times than relevant messages. These initial results have implications for the principled design of intelligent interrupters and instant messages.
Article
Full-text available
Although interruptions are daily occurring events for most working people, little research has been done on the impacts of interruptions on workers and their performance. This study examines the effects of interruptions on task performance and its regulation, as well as on workers' psychological and psychophysiological state. Two parallel experiments were carried out in the Netherlands and in Russia, using a common conceptual framework and overlapping designs. Employees with relevant work experience carried out realistic text editing tasks in a simulated office environment, while the frequency and complexity of interruptions were experimentally manipulated. It was hypothesized that interruptions: (i) would cause a deterioration of performance; (ii) evoke strategies to partially compensate for this deterioration; (iii) affect subjects' emotions and well‐being negatively; and (iv) raise the level of effort and activation. It was also hypothesized that greater frequency and complexity of interruptions would enhance the expected effects. The hypotheses are only partially confirmed. The results show that, contrary to what was expected, interruptions cause people to perform the main task faster while maintaining the level of quality. Participants develop strategies enabling them to deal effectively with the interruptions, while actually over‐compensating the potential performance decline. Interruptions do have a negative impact on emotion and well‐being, and lead to an increase of effort expenditure, although not to an increase in activation. Thus the improved performance is achieved at the expense of higher psychological costs. Greater complexity evoked more favourable responses among the Dutch participants and more unfavourable ones among the Russian participants. These differences are interpreted in terms of the participants' professional background. The research demonstrates that the effects of interruptions reach beyond the execution of additional tasks and the change of work strategies. Interruptions appear to have an after‐effect, influencing the workers' subsequent readiness to perform. Detailed analysis of the activity in the interruption interval, focusing on cognitive processes during episodes of ‘change‐over’ and ‘resumption’ support this interpretation.
Article
When an automating application needs a user's input or has feedback or other information for that user, it typically engages the user immediately, interrupting the user's current task. To empirically validate why unnecessarily interrupting a user's task should be avoided, we designed an experiment measuring the effects of an interruption on a user's task performance, annoyance, and anxiety. Fifty subjects participated in the experiment. The results demonstrate that an interruption has a disruptive effect on both a user's task performance and emotional state, and that the degree of disruption depends on the user's mental load at the point of interruption. We discuss the implications of these results in terms of building a system to better coordinate interactions between the user and applications competing for that user's attention.
Article
Interruptions are a common aspect of the work environment of most organizations. Yet little is known about how intemptions and their characteristics, such as frequency of occurrence, influence decision-making performance of individuals. Consequently, this paper reports the results of two experiments investigating the influence of interruptions on individual decision making. Interruptions were found to improve decision-making performance on simple tasks and to lower performance on complex tasks. For complex tasks, the frequency of interruptions and the dissimilarity of content between the pri-mary and interruption tasks was found to exacerbate this effect. The implications of these results for future research and practice are discussed.
Conference Paper
As users continue offloading more control and responsibility to the computer, coordinating the asynchronous interactions between the user and computer is becoming increasingly important. Without proper coordination, an application attempting to gain the user's attention risks interrupting the user in the midst of performing another task. To justify why an application should avoid interrupting the user whenever possible, we designed an experiment measuring the disruptive effect of an interruption on a user's task performance. The experiment utilized six Web-based task categories and two categories of interruption tasks. The results of the experiment demonstrate that: (i) a user performs slower on an interrupted task than a non-interrupted task, (ii) the disruptive effect of an interruption differs as a function of the task category, and (iii) different interruption tasks cause similar disruptive effects on task performance. These results empirically validate the need to better coordinate user interactions among applications that are competing for the user's attention
Article
: We describe a study on the influence of instant messaging (IM) on ongoing computing tasks. The study both replicates and extends earlier work on the cost of sending notifications at different times and the sensitivity of different tasks to interruption. We investigate alternative hypotheses about the nature of disruption for a list evaluation task, an activity identified as being particularly costly to interrupt. Our findings once again show the generally disruptive effects of IM, especially during fast, stimulus-driven search tasks. In addition, we show that interruptions coming early during a search task are more likely to result in the user forgetting the primary task goal than interruptions that arrive later on. These findings have implications for the design of user interfaces and notification policies that minimize the disruptiveness of notifications. Keywords: Notifications, user study, interruptions, information overload, divided attention 1 Introduction With the adv...