ArticlePDF Available

Enhancing Emotional Vocabulary in Young Children

Authors:

Figures

Content may be subject to copyright.
Handout 2.4: Social Emotional Teaching Strategies
Module 2
The Center on the Social and Emotional Foundations for Early Learning University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign csefel.uiuc.edu H 2.4
(p. 1/5)
Rev. 11/03
Gail E. Joseph, Ph.D. & Phillip S.
Strain, Ph.D.
Center on Evidence Based Practices
for Early Learning
University of Colorado at Denver
F
our-year-old Shantay is an avid
builder with blocks. At free play
he has busied himself with an
elaborate tower construction. To
complete his masterpiece he needs an
elusive triangle piece. As he searches
the room in vain for the last, crucial
piece his initial calm hunt becomes
more hurried and disorganized. He
begins to whimper and disrupt other
children’s play. His teacher approaches
and asks what the matter is. Shantay
swiftly turns away to resume his now
frantic search. This behavior persists
for several minutes until the signal for
cleanup is given, whereupon Shantay
launches into a major, 15-minute
tantrum.
Four-year-old Kelly is relatively
new to preschool. She wants to play
with her new classmates, but is too shy
and frightened to approach and join in
with the group. This day at free play
she intently watches, as three other
girls are absorbed in an elaborate tea
party, complete with pandas and
wolves. With a forlorn look, Kelly
passively observes the ongoing play.
Her teacher approaches and says,
“Honey, is something wrong?” Kelly
shrugs her shoulders. Her teacher
persists, “Kelly are you frustrated?”
Kelly says, “Yes.” Her teacher then
reminds her of the class rule; if you
feel frustrated, ask a friend or teacher
for help. Kelly and her teacher quickly
discuss how she might get another
animal and ask her classmates if the
zebra can come to the party.
In each of these cases, children
experience some of the common, often-
repeated challenges of life in preschool.
Shantay, in the end, was overwhelmed
by his feelings of frustration. Unable to
label his legitimate feeling he acted-
out— a sure recipe for not getting his
needs met. Kelly, equally upset and, in
this example, paralyzed temporarily by
her social anxiety was able to achieve
an outcome she deeply desired. She
was able to do this by the good
teaching that had previously occurred.
She was able to communicate her need
and access strategic help to get that
need met. In contrast with Shantay,
Kelly’s experience demonstrates one of
the ways that emotional literacy
enables children to be socially
competent. Consider two other case
examples of emotional literacy at work.
Tony is a master of rough and
tumble play. As a game of superheroes
commences, Tony runs headlong into
other children. Two of his playmates
happily reciprocate; smiling and
giggling they continue their preschool
version of “slam dancing.” Tony,
however, seeks out other partners as
well. In particular, Eddie and Darrin
want no part of this. They frown as he
approaches and yell, “No.” Tony
seems to interpret their behavior as an
invitation for more. Both Eddie and
Darrin start to cry and quickly seek out
their teacher who has Tony sit quietly
for 2 minutes while play continues.
This time-out angers Tony and he pouts
alone for the remainder of free play.
Tamika loves to play dress-up. This
day at free play she asks Seth to join
her, but he says, “Later,” and goes
about his computer play. Tamika then
gets a big hat and takes it to April.
April just frowns and goes about
tending to the hamster cage. Tamika
next takes the hat to Bo. “Bo,” she
says, “let’s go play.” Again she is
rebuffed. Finally Tamika finds a play
partner in Darrin; who is walking from
one activity area to the next.
In these two scenarios great
variation can be noted in children’s
ability to read social cues. Tony’s
choice of rough and tumble partners is
ubiquitous. His inability to read social
cues ultimately resulted in a poor
outcome. Tamika, on the other hand,
was readily able to read social cues
and, as a result of good teaching, she
had a strategy (try again with another
friend) to achieve her desired outcome.
Figure 1 below provides an overall
schematic of children’s emotional
literacy (Crick & Dodge, 1994;
Lemerise & Arsenio, 2000). Note first
that the foundational element, the
necessary context, for emotional
literacy development is a supportive,
caring relationship (see Joseph &
Strain, 2002). In order to act upon the
social environment in ways that are
collectively supportive and rewarding
it is first necessary for children to read
the affective cues of others and of
themselves. Discriminating among
affective states such as anger, sadness,
frustration, and happiness requires a
vocabulary of feeling words. Like
other forms of literacy the richer the
vocabulary, the more rewarding the
experiences. In this article we will
concentrate on how to build a
meaningful lexicon of feeling words.
This instructional emphasis bears, not
coincidentally, a close resemblance to
cognitive behavior modification
(Meichelbaum, 1976).
Enhancing Emotional Vocabulary in
Young Children
Once children are reading and
correctly labeling affective cues from
words, internal stimuli, and body
language they then proceed to make
crucial judgments about both the cause
and the intent of other’s affect (e.g.,
Tamika has, appropriately, a neutral
judgment abut peers’ lack of interest in
her play and she simply proceeds to
look until she finds a willing partner).
Many children, however, make crucial
errors at this point. Partly because of an
absence of feeling words they often
interpret the behavior of others as
intentionally hurtful and eventually act
out in ways that invariably lead to
social isolation and stigmatization
(Kazdin, 1989).
Once children make a judgment
about cause and intent they proceed, in
this model, to clarify their interpersonal
goals. In earlier examples, Tony
wanted to play rough and tumble,
Tamika wanted to play dress-up, Kelly
wanted to join in the tea party, and
Shantay just wanted that final block.
The clarification of goals then
allows children to generate solutions to
achieve their goals. Solutions might
include a self-regulation notion such as,
“I need to calm down.” Solutions
might be trying again, finding someone
to help, trying a different way, and so
on. Solution generation, however, must
be followed by a contingent decision-
making paradigm. For example,
children might be taught to consider if
the solution is fair, if it has worked
before, if it is a safe, if it would result
in positive feelings, and so on. Finally,
children act in accordance with their
decision. While we will focus only on
establishing a vocabulary of feeling
words that permit accurate reading of
affective cues and accurate
interpretation of cause and intent,
teachers needs to be aware that many
children will require careful step-by-
step instruction from reading affective
cues to acting on decisions.
Emotional literacy is the ability to
recognize, label, and understand
feelings in one’s self and others. It is a
prerequisite skill to emotional
regulation and successful interpersonal
interactions and problem solving and is
one of the most important skills a child
is taught in the early years
(Denham,1986; Webster-Stratton,
1999). Limited emotional literacy, on
the other hand, can result in
misperceptions of feeling in one’s self
and others.
Building emotional vocabulary
In order to correctly perceive
feelings in yourself and others, you
first have to have words for those
feelings, a feeling lexicon. Many
children are either “happy” or “mad”
and miss all the subtle gradations of
feelings in-between because they do
not have labels and definitions for
those emotions. Alarge and more
complex feeling vocabulary allows
children to make finer discriminations
between feelings; to better
communicate with others about their
internal affective states; and to engage
in discussions about their personal
experiences with the world. Children
with disabilities (Feldman, McGee,
Mann & Strain, 1993; Walker, 1981)
and children from low income families
(Eisneberg, 1999; Hart & Risley, 1995;
Lewis & Michalson, 1993) have more
limited feeling vocabularies than their
typically developing and middle
income peers. Parents and teachers can
foster emotional vocabulary by
teaching feeling words and their
emotional definitions. Adults can
increase children’s feelings words by
teaching different feeling words and
definitions directly; incidentally in the
context of conversation and play; and
through special activities.
Adults can teach feeling words
directly by pairing a picture or photo of
a feeling face with the appropriate
affective label. Preschoolers are better
at recognizing feelings with drawn
Handout 2.4: Social Emotional Teaching Strategies
Module 2
The Center on the Social and Emotional Foundations for Early Learning University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign csefel.uiuc.edu H 2.4
(p. 2/5)
Figure 1. Emotional Literacy Schematic
Emotional Literacy Schematic
Rev. 11/03
pictures at first then progressing to
photographs. Children’s books are an
excellent way to label feeling faces
with children. Many books are written
explicitly about feelings and contain
numerous feeling words. See Box 1 for
some of our favorites.
Adults can also teach children new
feeling words by explicitly providing
emotion labels as children experience
various affective states. For example,
an infant smiles brightly and the parent
says, “Oh, you are happy.” Similarly,
Kelly’s teacher noticed her aroused
state and labeled it “frustrated.”.
Labeling a child’s affective state allows
them to begin to identify their own
internal states. This is an important step
in learning to regulate
emotions(Joseph, 2001; Lochman &
Dunn, 1993; Webster-Stratton,1999).
For example, one needs to recognize
(this happens most effectively when
there is a label) their affective state,
say, “angry” before they can proceed
with steps to regulate or calm down. A
first step would be to vocalize this
negative feeling (“I’m mad”) versus
acting out. Using varied and complex
feeling words will develop powerful
feeling vocabularies for children. Box 2
provides a list of more complex feeling
words that 3-5 year olds who are
developing language normally know
(Joseph, 2001; Ridgeway, Waters &
Kuczaj, 1985).
Adults can also plan special
activities to teach and reinforce the
acquisition of feeling words. Children
can “check in” each morning by
picking a feeling face picture that best
depicts their affective state and sticking
it next to their name. Children can be
encouraged to change their feeling face
throughout the day as their feelings
change. Teachers can make feeling
dice by covering small milk cartons
with paper and drawing a different
feeling face on each side. Children can
toss the dice; label the feeling face and
describe a time they felt that way. Box
3 lists some other fun feeling activities.
INSERT BOX 3 ABOUT HERE
Handout 2.4: Social Emotional Teaching Strategies
Module 2
The Center on the Social and Emotional Foundations for Early Learning University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign csefel.uiuc.edu H 2.4
(p. 3/5)
Children’s Books featuring
feeling faces and words
On Monday when it rained by
Cherryl Kachenmeister,
Glad Monster, Sad Monster: A
Book About Feelings by Anne
Miranda & Ed Emberley
(Illustrator)
My Many Colored Days by Seuss,
Steve Johnson (Illustrator), Lou
Fancher (Illustrator)
When Sophie Gets Angry- Really,
Really Angry... by Molly Garrett
Bang
Feelings (Reading Rainbow
Book) by Aliki
I’m Mad (Dealing With Feelings)
by Elizabeth Crary, Jean Whitney
(Illustrator)
I’m Frustrated (Dealing With
Feelings) by Elizabeth Crary, Jean
Whitney (Illustrator)
When I Feel Angry by Cornelia
Maude Spelman, Nancy Cote
(Illustrator)
Feeling Words
Affectionate
Agreeable
Annoyed
Awful
Bored
Brave
Calm
Capable
Caring
Cheerful
Clumsy
Confused
Comfortable
Cooperative
Creative
Cruel
Curious
Depressed
Disappointed
Disgusted
Ecstatic
Embarrassed
Enjoying
Excited
Fantastic Fearful
Fed-up
Free
Friendly
Frustrated
Gentle
Generous
Box 1
Feeling Activities
Pass the hat: The teacher cuts
out pictures that represent various
feeling faces and places them in a
hat (or large envelope) that is
passed around the circle as music
plays. When the music stops, the
child holding the hat picks out a
picture designating an emotion and
is asked to identify it, express how
they look when they feel that way,
or describe a time when he or she
felt that way.
Feeling hunt: The teacher puts
“feeling face” pictures up all around
the room (and around the building
if possible). Children can be given
child-size magnifying glasses, and
they walk around looking for
different feeling faces. When they
find one, they label it and tell a time
they felt that way. An expansion of
this activity is to provide each child
with a “Feeling Face BINGO
Board” and they can cross out faces
on their boards as they find them
around the room.
Mirrors: Children are given
small hand held mirrors at circle
time or small group. As the teacher
reads a story with many feeling
words in it – the children make the
face to the corresponding affective
expression while looking at
themselves in their mirrors. Then,
the children put their mirrors down
and show their peer their “feeling
face.”
Changing faces: During small
group time, children make paper
plate faces. The teacher attaches the
“mouth” and “eyebrows” to the
paper plate with brads. This allows
Box 3
Box 2
Gloomy
Guilty
Ignored
Impatient
Important
Interested
Jealous
Joyful
Lonely
Lost
Loving
Overwhelmed
Peaceful
Pleasant
Proud
Relaxed
Relieved
Safe Satisfied
Sensitive
Serious
Shy Stressed
Strong
Stubborn
Tense
Thoughtful
Thrilled
Troubled
Unafraid
Uncomfortable
Weary
Worried
(continued)
Rev. 11/03
Teaching children to recognize
feelings in others
Children can be taught explicitly
how to identify feelings in other
people. Identifying feelings in others
involves noticing facial expressions
and body language, listening to the
tone of voice and, considering the
situational context.
Young children can be taught how
to detect the cues of how someone is
feeling by having their attention drawn
to the salient physical features of
someone’s affective state. Teachers can
model detecting how someone is
feeling by looking at their face
(noticing their eyebrows, their eyes,
and their mouth). This can be
accomplished directly and more
incidentally throughout the day.
Children can then be provided with
practice activities and opportunities to
notice facial expressions and body
language to determine how someone is
feeling.
Teachers can model for children
how they can tell how someone is
feeling by listening to the tone of the
person’s voice. Teachers can close their
eyes and a puppet or another adult can
make a statement such as, “UGGGHH,
I can’t get my shoes tied!” and then
guess that the person is feeling
frustrated. The children can practice by
closing their eyes and listening to the
teacher make statements using varying
tones, then guess how the teacher is
feeling.
Teachers can also teach children to
think about how someone might feel in
certain situations. Children’s literature
is a very effective for teaching and
practicing this skill. Read a story aloud,
pick a situation in the story and ask the
children to consider the character’s
reactions and feelings. This question
invites further conversation. Continue
discussing situations for as long as you
have the children’s interest. The
children’s books in Box 1 can be used
very effectively in this matter.
What do you do with a feeling?
Adults can model emotional
regulation skills for children by
verbalizing the course of action they
will take in order to calm down or cope
with certain feelings. For example, a
teacher doesn’t notice a loose lid on the
glitter bottle and consequently spills the
contents all over the table and floor. In
front of the children she says, “Oh no!
Boy, do I feel frustrated. I better take
some deep breaths to calm down.”
Kelly’s teacher developed a classroom
rule that when you feel frustrated you
ask a teacher or peer for help. In this
case, when the teacher labels a child’s
affective state as “frustrated” the child
is primed to ask for help. Eventually
the child will be able to label the
feeling themselves and seek out an
appropriate solution. Adults can
proactively teach young children
coping strategies for many emotions
(taking a deep breath when mad;
requesting a break when annoyed;
talking to someone when sad, etc.)
through modeling and role plays.
Positive emotions sometimes need to be
regulated as well.
Conclusion
In classrooms that devote planned
attention to helping children acquire a
rich and varied feeling vocabulary we
may expect fewer challenging
behaviors and more developmentally
sophisticated and enjoyable peer social
relations (Denham, 1986). Emotional
vocabulary is, however, only part of
this picture. For emotional vocabulary
teaching to be effective adults must first
spend the time necessary to build
positive relationships with children
(Joseph & Strain, 2002). Within this
foundational context of a warm and
responsive relationship with children,
teachers can maximize their influence
to enhance emotional vocabulary.
As the emotional literacy schematic
(Figure 1) suggests, having feeling
words and being able to recognize
emotions in others and in oneself is a
necessary but insufficient step toward
helping children achieve social and
emotional competence. Adults also
need to assist children in developing
and becoming fluent with the skills of
emotional regulation (e.g., calming
down; controlling anger and impulse)
and problem-solving (e.g., generating
solutions to interpersonal problems that
are safe, equitable, and result in
positive feelings).
In the Box 4 we provide teachers
with a brief checklist of classroom
the child to change facial expressions
on their plate by changing the mouth
from a smile to a frown, and the
eyebrows from facing in (angry,
frustrated, etc.) to out (worried,
scared, surprised, etc.). Children can
color the rest of the faces. The
teacher can then read a story and
pause after key incidents and ask the
children to show how they would feel
by changing their paper plate face
appropriately.
Singing, “If you’re happy and
you know it…”: Teachers can add
new verses to “If you are happy and
you know it” as they introduce new
feeling words to the class.
If you’re happy and you know it,
hug a friend
If you’re sad and you know it, cry
a tear – “boo-hoo”
If you’re mad and you know it, use
your words “I’m mad”
If you’re scared and you know it,
get some help, “HEEELLLLPPP!”
If you’re silly and you know it,
make a face,
“BBBBLLLUUUUHHHH!”
For more feeling activities see
Dinosaur School (Joseph, Webster-
Stratton & Reid, 2002; Webster-
Stratton, 1990), PATHS (Kusche &
Greenberg, 1994), or Second Step
(Committee for Children, 2002)
Box 3
(continued)
Feeling Activities
Handout 2.4: Social Emotional Teaching Strategies
Module 2
The Center on the Social and Emotional Foundations for Early Learning University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign csefel.uiuc.edu H 2.4
(p. 4/5)
Rev. 11/03
characteristics known to promote
emotional literacy.
Handout 2.4: Social Emotional Teaching Strategies
Module 2
The Center on the Social and Emotional Foundations for Early Learning University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign csefel.uiuc.edu H 2.4
(p. 5/5)
Characteristics of
Classrooms that Foster
Emotional Vocabulary
Photos of people with various
emotional expressions are
displayed around the room
Books about feelings are
available in the book corner
Teachers label their own
feelings
Teachers notice and label
children’s feelings
Teachers draw attention to
how a child’s peer is feeling
Activities are planned to teach
and reinforce emotional
literacy
Children are reinforced for
using feeling words
Efforts to promote emotional
vocabulary occur daily and
across all times of the day
Box 4
References
Committee for children (2002). Second step for preschoolers. Third Edition.
Seattle, WA: Committee for Children
Crick, N. R. & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social
information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment.
Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74-101.
Denham, S. A. (1986). Social cognition, prosocial behavior and emotion in
preschoolers: Contextual validation. Child Development, 57, 194-201.
Eisneberg, A. R. (1999). Emotion talk among Mexican-American and Anglo
American mothers and children from two social classes. Merrill-Palmer
Quarterly, 45(2), 267-284.
Feldman, R. S., McGee, G., Mann, L. & Strain, P. S. (1993). Nonverbal affective
decoding ability in children with autism and in typical preschoolers. Journal of
Early Intervention, 17(4), 341-350.
Hart, B. & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of
young American children. Baltimore: Paul H. Brooks.
Joseph, G. E. (in preparation). If you’re happy and you know it: examining the
emotional and social information processing scripts of young children.
Joseph, G. E. & Strain, P.S. (2002). Building positive relationships with young
children. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Joseph, G. E., Webster-Stratton, C. & Reid, M. J. (2002). Promoting young
children’s social and emotional competence. Manuscript submitted for
publication.
Kazdin, A. (1993). Treatment of conduct disorder. Development of
Psychopathology, 5, 277-310.
Kusche, C. A. & Greenberg, M. T. (1994) The PATHS Curriculum. Seattle:
Developmental Research and Programs.
Lemerise, A.A. & Arsenio, W. F. (2000). An integrated model of emotional
processes and cognition in social information processing. Child Development,
71, 107-118.
Lewis, M. & Michalson, L. (1993). Children’s emotions and moods:
developmental theory and measurement. New York: Plenum Press.
Lochman, J.E. & Dunn, S. E. (1993). An intervention and consultation model
from a social cognitive perspective: a description of the anger coping program,
School Psychology Review, 22, 458-71.
Meichenbaum, D. & Tirk, D. (1976). The cognitive-behavioral management of
anxiety, anger, and pain. In P. Davidson (Ed.), Behavioral management of
anxiety, depression, and pain. New York: Brunner/Mazel
Ridgeway, D., Waters, E., & Kuczaj, S. A. (1985). Acquisition of emotion-
descriptive language: Receptive and productive vocabulary norms for ages 18
months to 6 years. Developmental Psychology, 21(5), 901-908.
Walker, E. (1981). Emotion recognition in disturbed and normal children: A
research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 22(3), 263-268.
Webster-Stratton, C. (1990). The teachers and children videotape series: Dina
dinosaur school. Seattle, WA: The Incredible Years.
Webster-Stratton, C. (1999). How to promote children’s social and emotional
competence. London: Paul Chapman Publishing.
Rev. 11/03
... The learning of proper emotional vocabulary is required so that a person's feelings or emotions, such as sadness, anger, frustration and happiness, can be appropriately distinguished and the meaning delivered by the person is not misinterpreted. Most children are unable to express their emotions because they do not know how to label and interpret those emotions [38]. A larger and more complex vocabulary containing words that involve emotions enables children to identify their feelings unambiguously [38]. ...
... Most children are unable to express their emotions because they do not know how to label and interpret those emotions [38]. A larger and more complex vocabulary containing words that involve emotions enables children to identify their feelings unambiguously [38]. This can facilitate children to not only better communicate with others about their circumstances or feelings but also engage in conversations with people around them [38]. ...
... A larger and more complex vocabulary containing words that involve emotions enables children to identify their feelings unambiguously [38]. This can facilitate children to not only better communicate with others about their circumstances or feelings but also engage in conversations with people around them [38]. Previously, a traditional method that involved matching an image to an effective and appropriate vocabulary was used to express one's emotions [38]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The power of mobile devices has been harnessed as a platform for augmented reality (AR) that has embarked on opportunities to combine more inputs called multimodal inputs. There has been little research into the multimodal inputs that combine emotion, speech and markers in a mobile AR learning environment. This study aims to propose a framework for a mobile AR learning system that incorporates the combination of multimodal inputs, namely emotion, image-based marker and speech in order to determine how such a combination can enhance the learning experience. The proposed framework integrates the multimodal inputs based on the decision tree and develops into a four-phase learning system based on Kolb’s experiential learning model. To evaluate this learning system, 38 students were selected and divided into two groups for an experiment on vocabulary at a primary school. Quantitative findings showed better results concerning learning effectiveness, mental load, engagement, competency and challenge when the three multimodal inputs, speech, marker and emotion, were combined. Therefore, the proposed multimodal framework can be used as a guideline to develop a multimodal based AR application for learning environment by integrating multimodal inputs and Kolb’s experiential learning model.
... instead of becoming agitated or overwhelmed. Ultimately, students who can articulate their emotions clearly have a greater chance of building meaningful relationships and developing a deeper level of trust and intimacy with others (Joseph & Strain, 2003;Webster-Stratton, 1999). Two simple activities adapted from Dodigovic (2018) are: ...
Article
Full-text available
In recent years, there has been a growing recognition of the importance of emotional intelligence (EI) in children's development. For those with a learning disability (LD), developing EI is particularly important, as it can have a positive impact not only on their academic performance but also on their social-emotional development, mental well-being, and physical health. By fostering EI, educators can create a supportive and inclusive learning environment that addresses the unique needs of these students. However, many teachers may face challenges in finding effective ways to foster EI in children with LD. A rapid review was undertaken to offer practical and actionable recommendations for classroom use. These suggestions encompass adapted language tasks and informed communication strategies that teachers can readily apply. By accessing this research, educators can gain valuable tools and guidance to better support the overall growth of their students.
... Conforme a lo que proponen Chaplin y Aldao (2013), para mejorar la conciencia de las propias emociones y las ajenas, se requiere un vocabulario emocional amplio y elaborado que permita describir de manera precisa qué emoción se está sintiendo, y de esta manera favorecer una eficiente adaptación al entorno. Por ello, los niños que disponen del vocabulario para expresar sus sentimientos con precisión pueden fortalecer su alfabetización emocional, que es un componente clave de la competencia social (Joseph & Strain 2003); asimismo, el reconocimiento y comprensión de las emociones ajenas les permitirá desarrollar el sentimiento de empatía (Soldevila et al., 2007). ...
Article
Full-text available
El dominio del vocabulario emocional contribuye a construir nuestro saber acerca del mundo y favorece la construcción de la realidad emocional que se percibe a nivel individual. Este estudio se centró en identificar las diferencias en el vocabulario emocional en función de la edad y el género de alumnos de educación primaria. La muestra total fue de 551 participantes, estudiantes de educación primaria de cuatro centros de la región de Lleida (España), cuya edad oscilaba entre 6 y 12 años. Se realizó un estudio de diseño transversal y de enfoque descriptivo-interpretativo, y se aplicó un instrumento desarrollado ad hoc con tiempo limitado para su ejecución. Los resultados demostraron el incremento del vocabulario emocional a medida que aumenta la edad, llegando a duplicar su volumen inicial, y el aumento del repertorio de palabras que aluden a emociones negativas y ambiguas a medida que aumenta la edad. Además, se detectaron diferencias en el tipo y número de palabras del vocabulario emocional según el género de los participantes, de manera que las niñas presentaron mayor número de palabras asociadas a emociones positivas y negativas, y los niños a negativas.
... Emotions can be expressed verbally and non-verbally; verbally, associating a word with an emotion using different intonation of voice, 14,15 , and non-verbally with physiological reactions (i.e heart pounding, cold hands, sweating or turning red, which are the result of a subcortical amygdala-based system), facial expressions, body language, gestures and feelings 2,5,15,16 . Emotions with meaning and cognitive content are the result of reciprocal connections between the subcortical emotions system and the medial prefrontal cortex, and feedback and control of the prefrontal cortex in the amygdala 17,18 . ...
Article
Full-text available
In this study we explore how different levels of emotional intensity (Arousal) and pleasantness (Valence) are reflected in Electroencephalographic (EEG) signals. We performed the experiments on EEG data of 32 subjects from the DEAP public dataset, where the subjects were stimulated using 60-second videos to elicitate different levels of Arousal/Valence and then self-reported the rating from 1-9 using the Self-Assessment Manikin (SAM). The EEG data was pre-processed and used as input to a Convolutional Neural Network (CNN). First, the 32 EEG channels were used to compute the maximum accuracy level obtainable for each subject as well as for creating a single model using data from all the subjects. The experiment was repeated using one channel at a time, to see if specific channels contain more information to discriminate between Low vs High Arousal/Valence. The results indicate than using one channel the accuracy is lower compared to using all the 32 channels. An optimization process for EEG channel selection is then designed with the Non-dominated Sorting Genetic Algorithm II (NSGA-II) with the objective to obtain optimal channel combinations with high accuracy recognition. The genetic algorithm evaluates all possible combinations using a chromosome representation for all the 32 channels, and the EEG data from each chromosome in the different populations are tested iteratively solving two unconstrained objectives; to maximize classification accuracy and to reduce the number of required EEG channels for the classification process. Best combinations obtained from a Pareto-front suggests that as few as 8-10 channels can fulfill this condition and provide the basis for a lighter design of EEG systems for emotion recognition. In the best case, the results show accuracies of up to 1.00 for Low vs High Arousal using 8 EEG channels, and 1.00 for Low vs High Valence using only 2 EEG channels. These results are encouraging for research and healthcare applications that will require automatic emotion recognition with wearable EEG.
Article
This article uses my personal journey to demonstrate how therapeutic writing can be an adjunct to therapy. I am Chinese, I was born in New Zealand, where I trained as a counsellor and, more recently, as a facilitator of therapeutic writing groups. Therefore, a particular focus of this article is on the possible benefits of therapeutic writing for the Chinese community. Research has shown that Chinese people underutilise mental health services and that cultural issues may be a factor in this. In this article, I suggest that therapeutic writing could benefit this population, predominantly as a support for talking therapy and, in some instances, as an alternative. I conclude by describing some of the parallels between talking therapy and therapeutic writing. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR] Copyright of New Zealand Journal of Counselling is the property of New Zealand Association of Counsellors and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. This abstract may be abridged. No warranty is given about the accuracy of the copy. Users should refer to the original published version of the material for the full abstract. (Copyright applies to all Abstracts.)
Conference Paper
Full-text available
PHILOSOPHICAL GAMES IN PRIMARY EDUCATION: AN INTERDISCIPLINARY APPROACH Philosophical games provide an innovative transformative structure in the learning process for all levels of formal education. The motivation is to provide elementary school teachers with an innovative methodology for Game-based-Learning of Philosophy/in Philosophy teaching. A combination and attentive collaboration of Philosophy, Art and games/ Game-based Learning provides new tools in approaching and solving the problems that education faces today. Since Game-based Learning constitutes a strong trend in technologically enhanced learning, is the, where/with the employment of gaming elements both in learning content and learning pathways, the proposed methodology leads to a series of novel applications about teaching philosophy that enable young agents to cultivate hypothetic-deductive and critical thinking with a positive attitude towards others and developing feelings of constructive antagonism. The teaching scenario proposed aims at cultivating hypothetic – deductive and critical thought/moreover, enhances the linguistic ability in the vocabulary of ancient Greek philosophy as well. The scenario is part of a game suite entitled “Entering the Socratic school” and targets 10–12-year-old children. It is easy to implement on any digital platform with open-source tools used by almost every teacher. The game elements rely on the structure of the learning content rather than on the digital tools themselves. The methodology consists in designing a concept map and defining the game narrative, the game levels and transitions between levels, the mechanics to be used, such as polls, badges, and leaderboards. Online activities include digital games such as quizzes and crossword puzzles, student generated comic stories, and a digital guide. They are complemented by physical activities involving movement and dialogue using fishbowl techniques and Socratic circles. The proposed teaching scenario will be implemented in the classroom in the following academic year and our work team applies interdisciplinary approaches inspired by at least three different fields of expertise. Keywords: Socratic games, gamified learning, philosophy, art, Moodle. p-ISSN: 2184-044X e-ISSN: 2184-1489 ISBN: 978-989-53614-3-4 © 2022
Article
The purpose of this study is to examine which teaching methodology – context provision or imagination elicitation – is more efficient to learn emotional vocabulary in the English as a foreign language (EFL) classroom. A pre-test / post-test design was used to test the benefit of the two types of instruction, compared to a traditional teaching method. A total of 60 EFL students at a B2 level (Council of Europe, 2001, 2018) participated in the research. A set of 40 emotional words was selected according to the following criteria: to be emotional and mid-frequency vocabulary in English, and rather unknown words by B2-level students in order to allow room for learning. Statistical analyses showed that the instruction phase was successful, since the groups significantly differed in the post-test. Although the two experimental treatments were effective, the imagination elicitation approach turned out to be better than the context provision method. The conclusion has relevant pedagogical implications for the EFL classroom.
Conference Paper
"The ability to master emotional vocabulary is evidenced to enhance emotional expression, as well as broaden knowledge of the world and facilitate interpersonal relationships. Also, it helps to develop critical thinking, promotes abstraction, deepens self-knowledge, emotional regulation, forge solid social relationships and improves emotional competencies: emotional awareness, emotional regulation, emotional autonomy, social competence and competences for life and well-being. Some of them are mediated by language, such as emotional awareness which is the first step towards becoming aware of one’s own emotions and the emotions of others as well as capturing the emotional climate of a particular context. This study aims to be a needs analysis to detect the level of emotional vocabulary that primary students know differentiating between positive, negative and ambiguous emotions. Consequently, the differences between gender and grades had been explored. The sample of this study was constituted of 551 primary students (335 boys and 216 girls). An instrument developed ad-hoc was implemented to measure the emotional vocabulary. A quantitative analysis was done with the program IBM SPSS Statistics 24.0 software. The results showed that as participants' ages increased, more positive emotions were detected. Apart from that, a greater number of negative and ambiguous emotions were noticed in the older participants. On the other hand, the younger participants reported an increased number of positive words. As compared to the boys, the girls reported more positive words. To explain these results, a variety of explanations and arguments could be considered. According to previous research, studies have demonstrated how relevant is to carry out emotional competence’s programmes based in emotional education in primary schools at early ages, since then exists major flexibility and capacity to acquire emotional vocabulary. These interventions also prevent bullying and aid in conflict resolution, in addition to improving emotional vocabulary."
Article
Full-text available
Twelve male children with autism and twelve male typical children viewed a series of videotaped vignettes in which two interacting children displayed differing facial expressions of happiness, sadness, or anger. To test their skills in decoding nonverbal facial expressions, participants were asked which of the two children was displaying a particular expression. Results showed that the typical children were more accurate in their decoding of expressions than were the children with autism for each of the three emotions studied. However, the children with autism did show certain decoding ability: they were able to identify happiness vignettes at above-chance levels. The results have implications for assessing and ultimately remediating nonverbal skill deficits in children with autism.
Article
Full-text available
Research on the relation between social information processing and social adjustment in childhood is reviewed and interpreted within the framework of a reformulated model of human performance and social exchange. This reformulation proves to assimilate almost all previous studies and is a useful heuristic device for organizing the field. The review suggests that overwhelming evidence supports the empirical relation between characteristic processing styles and children's social adjustment, with some aspects of processing (e.g., hostile attributional biases, intention cue detection accuracy, response access patterns, and evaluation of response outcomes) likely to be causal of behaviors that lead to social status and other aspects (e.g., perceived self-competence) likely to be responsive to peer status. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Collected normative data on children's ability to understand emotion-descriptive adjectives when used by adults and their own use of these words in productive vocabulary in 9 age ranges from 18 mo to 71 mo. Within each age range, 30 parents reported on comprehension and use of 125 adjectives that adults had rated most basic (vs derived) among the complete set of 518 adjectives that describe emotion in the English language. Split-half reliabilities for norms on the percentage of Ss' understanding and percentage of Ss using the 124 adjectives ranged from .91 to .96 and from .92 to .96, respectively. In addition to their obvious applications in the study of semantic development and use of emotion-related verbal labels, these norms enable researchers to take vocabulary development into account when selecting stimulus materials or preparing verbal instructions for research on mood and emotion in very young children. (19 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Ethnic, social class, and gender differences in mothers' and children's references to emotions were examined. Twenty middle-class and 20 working-class Anglo American and 20 middle-class and 20 working-class Mexican American mothers audiotaped conversation with their 4-year-old children during car rides. Analyses of 1,226 references to emotions confirmed the existence of many patterns of emotion talk identified in prior studies of mother-child conversations about emotions, including gender differences in the frequency of emotion mentions and the use of a more varied emotion vocabulary. The study also established that social class is associated with the context in which feeling states are discussed, whereas ethnicity is related to the content of such talk. Implications for development are discussed.
Article
This article provides information for early childhood educators so that they will understand what goes into the task of building positive relationships with children. Having a warm and open relationship with his or her teacher or child care provider is identified as an important protective factor for young children. Empirical evidence for the importance of building positive relationships with children is provided in this article. Two practical strategies are presented: (1) "Getting to Know You Strategies"; and (2) "Making Deposit Strategies." These include examples of descriptive commenting during play. Finally, effective strategies for building relationships with difficult children are described. A majority of this article focuses on what children gain through positive relationships with adults. However, the article concludes by noting that adults benefit from building such relationships too in that teachers experience children who are easier to teach and get to know and are less likely to engage in challenging behaviors. Another benefit is that teachers feel more positive about their skills and efforts and enjoy their jobs more. (Contains 2 figures and 2 tables.)
Article
Provides a rationale and description of a school-based anger coping program for aggressive children. The need for school-based, preventive interventions with this population is discussed. The conceptual model for this intervention is outlined, and relevant findings on social cognitive processes are briefly overviewed. Based on this conceptual model, the cognitive-behavioral intervention framework is described, and relevant outcome research findings are reviewed. Universal interventions and teacher consultation can be used to augment secondary prevention programs such as the Anger Coping Program. The use of comprehensive social-cognitive interventions which also provide programming for parents is advocated. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)