ArticlePDF Available

Using Point-of-View Video Modeling to Teach Play to Preschoolers With Autism

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

This study evaluated the effectiveness of point-of-view video modeling in teaching selected toy-play skills to two preschoolers with autism. This type of modeling involved the experimenters carrying or holding the video camera at eye level (from the child's perspective) and without recording models (persons]) to show the environment as a child would see it when he or she was performing the targeted skills. The researchers used a multiple-probe design across two children and two behaviors to evaluate the effect of the point-of-view modeling on the children's acquisition and maintenance of play actions. They used generalization probes to assess the degree to which the participants used the new skills across novel toys and during classroom activities. The results indicated that point-of-view modeling was an effective tool for teaching toy-play actions to preschoolers with autism. The authors discuss the extension of current video modeling research and implications for home and school interventions.
Content may be subject to copyright.
TECSE 26:2 83–93 (2006) 83
Using Point-of-View Video Modeling to Teach
Play to Preschoolers With Autism
T
his study evaluated the effectiveness of point-of-view video modeling in teaching
selected toy-play skills to two preschoolers with autism. This type of modeling
involved the experimenters carrying or holding the video camera at eye level
(from the child’s perspective) and without recording models (persons]) to show the en-
vironment as a child would see it when he or she was performing the targeted skills.
The researchers used a multiple-probe design across two children and two behaviors to
evaluate the effect of the point-of-view modeling on the children’s acquisition and
maintenance of play actions. They used generalization probes to assess the degree to
which the participants used the new skills across novel toys and during classroom ac-
tivities. The results indicated that point-of-view modeling was an effective tool for
teaching toy-play actions to preschoolers with autism. The authors discuss the extension
of current video modeling research and implications for home and school interventions.
Jeffrey F. Hine
and
Mark Wolery
Peabody College of
Vanderbilt University
Address: Jeffrey F. Hine, 27730 Lake Jem Rd., Mount Dora, FL 32757; e-mail: jeffhine@hotmail.com
Play involves a number of diverse and complex behav-
iors and is viewed as central to children’s development
(Jordan & Libby, 1997). Young children with autism of-
ten are described as having impairments in play (Jarrold,
2003; Jarrold, Boucher, & Smith, 1993; Jordan & Libby,
1997; Ungerer & Sigman, 1981; Van Berckelaer-Onnes,
2003), and they often do not develop spontaneous play
(Van Berckelaer-Onnes, 2003). Libby, Powell, Messer,
and Jordan (1988) showed that children with autism had
similar levels of functional play as children with Down
syndrome and young children with typical development,
but they had impairments in symbolic (pretend) play.
Toy play is an essential forerunner for symbolic play
(Van Berckelaer-Onnes, 2003) and perhaps for social
play (Pierce-Jordan & Lifter, 2005).
Learning through observation and imitation of others
can account for acquisition of some behaviors, and sim-
ply watching another child receive reinforcement (vicar-
ious reinforcement) for a behavior often increases rates
of that behavior (Bandura, 1965). In vivo modeling (using
live models) is effective in teaching various developmental
skills to children with autism (Charlop, Schreibman, &
Tryon, 1983), including appropriate play (Stahmer, In-
gersoll, & Carter, 2003). For modeling strategies to be
effective, the child with autism must attend to another
person, must attend to that person’s actions, and must be
imitative. The developmental course of imitation in chil-
dren with autism is unclear. Dawson and Osterling (1997)
suggested that imitation is a fundamental mechanism for
learning and, in particular, a precursor for learning so-
cial skills. Young children with autism seem to imitate
actions on toys more readily if sensory feedback occurs
than if it is absent (Ingersoll, Shreibman, & Tran, 2003),
which suggests a lack of motivation for acting on toys;
thus, studies of interventions featuring supplementary
reinforcement contingencies are needed.
Technology, particularly video modeling, provides
another way to promote observational learning. Video
modeling interventions can include any number of com-
ponents, but they generally involve the following:
1. edited images of appropriate or new be-
havior shown on a monitor to a child;
2. repeated clips of the same behavior or mul-
tiple exemplars of the behavior shown to
the participant;
3. discrete practice sessions or role-playing of
the new skills;
4. assessment of skill generalization (e.g.,
probes across settings, people, or materi-
als); and
5. periodic review of tapes, if needed.
84 Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 26:2
Stahmer et al. (2003) suggested that video modeling
may succeed when other methods have failed, especially
for children who do not learn when social interactions
are involved. Researchers have used video modeling to
teach individuals with autism such skills as purchasing
(Alcantara, 1994; Haring, Kennedy, Adams, & Pitts-
Conway, 1987), conversation (Charlop & Milstein, 1989;
Sherer et al., 2001), perspective taking (Charlop-Christy
& Daneshvar, 2003; LeBlanc et al., 2003), spelling (Kin-
ney, Vedora, & Stromer, 2003), and daily living (Shipley-
Benamou, Lutzker, & Taubman, 2002). Video modeling
also can be used to promote play skills in children with
autism. Taylor, Levin, and Jasper (1999) used video mod-
eling to teach two children with autism to engage in
play-related statements with their siblings. D’Ateno,
Mangiapanello, and Taylor (2003) used video modeling
to increase children’s use of complex play sequences.
Video modeling has several advantages over in vivo
modeling, including (a) being more time and cost effi-
cient; (b) depicting a variety of naturalistic settings that
would be difficult to create in vivo in a clinic or class-
room; (c) having more control over the model, because
the tape can be recreated until the desired scene is ob-
tained; (d) presenting the model repeatedly without the
person modeling the desired behavior being present; and
(e) reusing videotapes with other individuals, which means
that more children can be treated (Charlop-Christy, Le,
& Freeman, 2000). Zihni and Zihni (2005) pointed out
that video modeling takes advantage of the visual skills
of children with autism, eliminates the social context
that occurs during in vivo modeling, increases the pre-
dictability and controllability of the model, and allows
extraneous features to be filtered out, which makes learn-
ing easier. Video modeling also can promote generaliza-
tion by exposing participants to a sufficient number of
exemplars and response variations. Haring et al. (1987)
demonstrated generalization of purchasing skills to three
community settings using video modeling with adults with
autism and noted that video modeling was an effective
way to promote generalization. Charlop and Milstein
(1989) demonstrated generalization of conversational
speech involving untrained topics (new toys, unfamiliar
persons, siblings, peers, and other settings).
Video modeling interventions have used other-as-
model (adults or peers doing target behaviors) and self-
as-model (participants act as their own models) formats.
In the latter instance, the video is edited to show the
child performing only the desired behaviors. Self-as-
model videos have been effective in increasing respond-
ing behaviors (Buggey, Toombs, Gardener, & Cervetti,
1999), requesting (Hepting & Goldstein, 1996; Wert &
Neisworth, 2003), and conversation skills (Sherer et al.,
2001) in children with autism. Sherer et al. (2001) com-
pared the efficacy of self versus other video modeling and
found no overall differences in the rate of acquisition.
An alternative to other- and self-models is observer point-
of-view modeling, which involves recording tapes using
the perspective of the person who is the target of the in-
tervention and showing him or her the target behavior
from the viewer’s vantage point but without showing the
entire person who is modeling the behavior. Schreibman,
Whalen, and Stahmer (2000) used point-of-view model-
ing to reduce problematic behaviors in difficult transi-
tions by taping what the transition would look like as
the child did it (e.g., taking different routes through a
mall). Shipley-Benamou et al. (2002) taught daily living
skills to children with autism via this procedure. They
suggested that point-of-view modeling eliminates the
need for staging performances and editing tapes, which
can be time-consuming in self-as-model videotapes. In
addition, using the child’s point of view avoids having to
identify which model characteristics (e.g., peer or adult,
familiar or unfamiliar) are most effective when teaching
new skills.
To date, researchers have had success in teaching
new skills to participants using other persons or the child
as models in video modeling studies, and positive ef-
fects have been most evident for participants older than
preschool age. In this study, we attempted to extend the
literature by teaching symbolic (pretend) play to preschool-
age children with autism and evaluated the use of point-
of-view video modeling. Only two studies (Schreibman
et al., 2000; Shipley-Benamou et al., 2002) used point-
of-view modeling, and only five studies included children
with autism age 5 years of age or younger. Although
some studies (e.g., Nikopoulos & Keenan, 2004) focused
on play skills, only one study addressed symbolic (pre-
tend) play (D’Ateno et al., 2003). In this study, our goal
was to evaluate the effectiveness of point-of-view video
modeling on the play of preschoolers with autism using
materials during sensory bin activities. We posted the fol-
lowing research questions:
1. Will preschoolers with autism readily imi-
tate actions seen through point-of-view
video modeling?
2. Will any acquired skills generalize to chil-
dren’s classroom sensory activities and
across untrained materials?
METHOD
Participants
Our participants were two girls. Christine was 30 months
old at the beginning of the study, and Kaci was 43 months
old. Both girls were verbal, but their communication, so-
cial, and play skills had been targeted as intervention
goals. Both participants met the criteria for autism of the
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders–
Point-of-View Video Modeling 85
Fourth Edition (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Asso-
ciation, 1994). According to teacher reports, both chil-
dren participated regularly during sensory bin activities
and consistently contacted materials. Christine’s sensory
bin play often involved repetitive movements, usually
with one object. Kaci’s play involved repetitive manipu-
lation of toys and stereotypic behavior. Both girls needed
instruction on how to use the toys. From teacher and
parent reports, both children enjoyed watching videos at
home and school. Both girls were accustomed to leaving
the classroom with another adult. We administered the
Motor Imitation Scale (Stone, Ousley, & Littleford, 1997).
Christine scored 29 and Kaci scored 30 (32 was the ceil-
ing), indicating that they readily imitated simple adult
actions with materials.
Settings and Materials
The study occurred at an inclusive, full-day, university-
based preschool program accredited by the National
Association for the Education of Young Children. The
participants attended for the full day and were in sepa-
rate classrooms. Each classroom contained 10 to 14 chil-
dren (5 to 7 with disabilities) and 2 to 4 adults. Video
modeling sessions occurred in the preschool therapy
room. The therapy room (3 m × 4.5 m) contained an of-
fice desk (2 m × 1 m) with the experimental materials on
it. During all sessions, the first author, the participant,
and a graduate student observer were present. Materials
generalization probes occurred in the same therapy
room. Settings generalization measurement occurred in
the child’s classroom and during sensory table activities
at a large table with the sensory bin on it. Other class-
mates and adults were present during the settings gener-
alization probes.
We chose sensory activities as the focus of this study
for several reasons. First, sensory bin activities occurred
daily in the children’s classrooms, as well as in many other
early childhood classrooms, making generalization of
play skills relevant. Second, the sensory bin materials al-
lowed the children to do a variety of appropriate actions
that were not dictated by the materials themselves. This
allowed us to evaluate precisely the effects of the video
modeling on the target children’s behavior. Third, use of
sensory bin materials in a nonrepetitive (stereotypic)
manner allowed the children to appear more like their
typically developing classmates. Fourth, these types of
skills also provided new opportunities for object explo-
ration, play expansions, social initiations and responses,
and promotion of engagement (e.g., participating appro-
priately in an activity for more than 10 min). The same
sensory material (e.g., potting soil) was used for the base-
line, treatment, and maintenance phases for Christine;
the only stimuli we changed were the toys used to ma-
nipulate the sensory material. In Kaci’s case, the sensory
material was changed to colored rice in Session 48.
During material generalization probes, the toys used to
manipulate the materials were unfamiliar to both partic-
ipants. A list of toys used for all conditions is provided
in Table 1. We chose toys based on their availability in
most early childhood classrooms (e.g., shovels and pots)
and because they could be categorized as a pretend play
set (i.e., gardening toys or cooking toys). At the start of
each session, the sensory bin was placed on the floor or
on another storage bin so the materials were at the
child’s height. A laptop computer used for video viewing
was placed on a storage bin, and the child could watch it
while sitting or kneeling on the floor.
Video Production
We presented the modeling video clips, which we created
using a digital video camera, on a laptop computer.
These videos showed a pair of adult hands performing
the actions with the toys to manipulate the sensory ma-
terial. A screen shot of the video clips viewed by the par-
ticipants is shown in Figure 1. The hands on the video
performed the specific actions with the toys, manipulat-
ing the sensory materials discretely. The video clip de-
picted exactly what the child’s viewpoint should look
like: two hands, a few toys (e.g., shovels and buckets),
and a tub containing the sensory materials (e.g., potting
TABLE 1. Materials Used in Both Tasks and During Materials Generalization
Probes
Gardening set materials Cooking set materials
3 toy shovels (1 shovel) 3 cooking spoons (1 cooking spoon)
3 small planter pots (1 plastic bucket) 3 plastic toy plates (1 toy plate)
3 synthetic aquarium plants (2 red plants) 3 measuring scoops (1 scoop)
10 large flower bulbs (5 flower bulbs) 3 plastic toy bowls (1 toy bowl)
2 small tin pots (1 tin pot)
Note. Materials used during materials generalization probes are shown in parentheses.
86 Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 26:2
soil). We chose this viewpoint so the camera could focus
on manipulations being made to the materials, thus con-
trolling the model characteristics and other irrelevant
cues to which the children might have attended. We iden-
tified the actions shown in the video modeling through
observing the play actions of typical children during sen-
sory time.
During intervention, each participant viewed two
separate videos containing one behavior set: gardening
play and cooking play. Each video clip containing the de-
sired actions was less than 2 min in length and contained
three examples of each action to expose the participant
to a variety of exemplars of the desired actions with a
variety of objects belonging to the same stimulus class.
Dependent Measures
The number of different types of modeled actions per-
formed during each probe was the dependent measure. A
research assistant used a digital video recorder to video-
tape each daily probe and practice session, which the
first author then coded. We trained graduate student ob-
servers via the use of a training video and actual clips of
the participants during practice sessions. The measure-
ment system used a coding sheet listing the modeled ac-
tions the participant could imitate from the video clip
(six for the gardening task and five for the cooking task).
When the child exhibited one of the modeled actions at
any time during the probe, the action was marked as
completed. A list of each measured action type is shown
in Table 2.
We selected increasing the use of modeled actions as
a dependent measure for four reasons:
1. Studies of pretend (symbolic) play often
measure actions on materials from which
a theme, routine, or narrative can be in-
ferred—in this case, gardening and cooking.
2. Increasing children’s appropriate actions
may reduce repetitive actions, which in
turn may promote opportunities for social
exchanges with peers at the sensory bin.
3. Increasing children’s actions on materials
would increase the number of behaviors on
which adults could comment (addressing
communicative goals) and expand (pro-
moting play goals).
4. Increasing actions on sensory bin materials
would reduce the apparent differences be-
tween the participants and their classmates.
Design
We used a multiple-probe design (Horner & Baer, 1978)
across two behaviors (gardening task and cooking task)
and across two participants to evaluate the effectiveness
of the point-of-view modeling. The intermittent probes
provided an alternative to continuous baseline measure-
FIGURE 1. Screen shot of a video clip viewed by participants.
Point-of-View Video Modeling 87
ment to reduce (a) time involved in measurement and
(b) reactivity (Horner & Baer, 1978). Each target child
participated in three phases for each of the two behaviors:
a baseline phase, a treatment phase, and a posttreatment
maintenance phase. For each child, we collected data
during baseline probes, daily treatment probes, daily
treatment practice sessions, and posttreatment mainte-
nance probes. Daily treatment probes occurred prior to
each daily video viewing to assess acquisition in the ab-
sence of immediate video viewing. Setting and material
generalization probes were taken at least once in both
the baseline and maintenance conditions.
Baseline Procedures. We conducted baseline mea-
surement for each of the two sequences of behavior. For
the baseline probes, each participant knelt in front of the
bin in the therapy room. The investigator placed the first
toy set (e.g., gardening toys) in the bin and verbally cued
the child to play with the toys. No further instructions
on how to use the toys or what to do with the materials
were given. After an interval of 2 min, the investigator
removed the first toy set, and the child watched a 2-min
cartoon. The investigator then gave the child the second
toy set (e.g., cooking toys). Only the toys to which the
child was to be exposed during the video modeling were
used for the probes. The order in which the toys sets were
given was randomized for each session.
Video Modeling Treatment Procedures. The treat-
ment sessions consisted of three segments: a daily probe of
the behavior being modeled, a video modeling/viewing
segment, and a daily practice segment. Each instructional
session lasted a maximum of 15 min (2-min for the daily
probe, 3 to 5 min for viewing the video, 3 min for prac-
tice, and 3 min were reserved for transitioning between
these segments). Video modeling sessions occurred at the
same time of day for each participant. We conducted
probes each day prior to viewing the videotape to assess
acquisition in the absence of immediate video viewing.
Treatment probes were identical to baseline probes ex-
cept only the materials for the toy set were available. The
investigator gave the child the set of materials to be
viewed in the video and then cued her to play with the
materials for 2 min. At the end of each probe, the inves-
tigator provided verbal praise and tangible rewards (e.g.,
edibles) for on-task behavior (i.e., staying at the sensory
bin). Reinforcement was contingent on contacting the
toys and staying at the bin; it was not delivered for per-
forming specific actions (including modeled actions)
with the toys.
After the probe, the investigator turned on the lap-
top, and a clip of the child’s favorite cartoon prefaced
the video modeling clip. We chose the cartoons through
informal interviews with the participants’ parents. The
cartoon introduction helped the child attend to the com-
puter screen, but it did not overly upset the child when
turned off. The cartoon played for 2 min, after which the
video modeling clip began. The clip showed the first set
of materials (e.g., a bin of potting soil, three toy shovels,
three pots). A recorded female voice unfamiliar to the
participants said, “Play with your toys!” A pair of hands
entered the screen and modeled the appropriate actions.
The video showed the hands picking up each object, per-
forming a specific action, putting the object down, pick-
ing up the next object, performing the same action, and
so forth, until the action had been performed once with
each exemplar in the set. After the hands modeled the ac-
tion with each object, the same female voice said, “Great
job playing with your toys!” and the cartoon played for
another 60 s. The investigator provided the child with
the materials used on the video and told her to play with
the toys. No further instructions on how to use the toys
or what to do with the materials were given during these
practice sessions. The practice sessions lasted 3 min, and
the child only received prompts for standing at the bin
and playing with the toys, not for performing the mod-
eled actions. The investigator did not deliver reinforce-
ment for imitating the modeled actions at any time
during the baseline, treatment, or maintenance condi-
tions.
During the maintenance phase, we withdrew the
video treatment and the practice sessions. The mainte-
nance probe procedure was identical to the baseline
probe procedure in that the child was given 2 min. to
play with each set of materials separately. No instruc-
tions on how to use the toys were given, and reinforce-
ment was limited to staying at the sensory bin and
making contact with the materials.
Adapted Procedure. We adapted the procedure for
Kaci beginning in the eighth session of the treatment
phase during the cooking behavior set. We decided that
TABLE 2. Play Actions Modeled During the Video
Modeling Sessions
Gardening Cooking
behavior set behavior set
1. Uses shovel to dig hole 1. Uses scoop to scoop soil
2. Puts soil into empty pot 2. Scoops soil into pot using
only the scoop
3. Puts flower in pot 3. Uses spoon to mix in pot
4. Puts seed in the ground 4. Pours soil from pot to
plate only
5. Covers seed with soil 5. Uses spoon to dish soil
from pot to bowl
6. Covers plant with soil
88 Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 26:2
adaptation was necessary due to Kaci’s apparent satia-
tion with the materials, which had resulted in decreased
responding during both the treatment probes and the
practice sessions. First, we changed the sensory material
(potting soil) to colored rice to introduce novelty to the
task. The toys used to manipulate the materials were the
same. We then provided a specific prompt (e.g., “Do
what you saw on the video”) to Kaci prior to each treat-
ment probe and practice session. Third, during only the
practice sessions, we reinforced the modeled actions on a
fixed-ratio-1 schedule. Reinforcement consisted of spe-
cific praise (e.g., “Good job! You did what you saw on
the video!”) and a small edible reward.
Settings Generalization Probes. We conducted set-
tings generalization probes once during each baseline
and maintenance phase, using the same stimuli from the
treatment sessions. The child was in the classroom and
standing at the sensory table. We gave the child each set
of materials separately for 3 min and told her to play
with the toys. Measurement was identical for both the
baseline and treatment phases. No rewards were pro-
vided for imitating the modeled actions.
Materials Generalization Probes. Generalization
probes occurred at least once during the baseline and
maintenance conditions. Probes occurred in the therapy
room and were procedurally identical to baseline probes,
with the only difference being the availability of un-
trained toys. Stimulus generalization materials were not
shown in the videos. Probes involved objects and toys
similar enough to the trained stimuli so that the child
could discriminate which action to perform with them.
The directions provided to the child were the same as in
the baseline condition, and we delivered reinforcement
only for attending to the materials.
Social Validity
Upon completion of the study, 20 graduate students in
special education rated videotapes of the pretraining and
posttraining sessions. We randomly assigned these grad-
uate students to view four video clips. Ten of the gradu-
ate students viewed two pretraining tapes for Christine,
and two posttraining tapes for Kaci. Ten of the graduate
students viewed two pretraining tapes for Christine, and
two posttraining tapes for Kaci. They completed a five-
item questionnaire for each segment in which they rated
the child’s engagement, manipulation of materials, ap-
propriate use of materials, enjoyment of the activity, and
need for help using the materials on a 5-point Likert-type
scale (1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = neither agree
nor disagree, 4 = agree, 5 = strongly agree). Taped ses-
sions were selected based on the degree to which the child
exhibited the modeled actions (i.e., no modeled actions
for preintervention tapes and five to six modeled actions
for postintervention tapes). The rater was not informed
as to whether the video clip was from before or after im-
plementation of the video modeling intervention.
Interobserver Agreement
We collected interobserver agreement (IOA) measures
for at least 25% of the baseline, treatment, and mainte-
nance sessions in each condition for each child. We cal-
culated IOA by dividing the number of agreements by
the number of agreements plus disagreements and multi-
plying by 100. We rated a response as an agreement if
both observers marked the same action as being per-
formed by the participant within the session. The IOA
for this study was 97.7% for the gardening task and
100% for the cooking task. For Kaci’s measured behav-
iors, IOA was 100% agreement; for Christine, it was
98.0% agreement.
Procedural Fidelity
To assess the degree to which all intervention sessions
were executed according to protocol, all observers and
the experimenter completed a procedural fidelity check-
list outlining the procedure for each session. Procedural
fidelity was assessed for at least 25% of all sessions in
each condition for each child. Observers completed a
task list containing all of the behaviors that were to oc-
cur in each phase and condition of the study, including
placement of toys, placement of sensory bin, verbal cues
for the child to play with the toys, reinforcement for con-
tacting materials, absence of experimenter-implemented
reinforcement for correct responding, and order of inter-
vention steps (e.g., probe, video viewing, practice session).
Procedural fidelity results showed that the baseline,
treatment, maintenance, and generalization session pro-
tocol was followed with 95% accuracy (range = 83.3%–
100%) throughout the study.
RESULTS
Probe and Practice Session Data
The number of action types performed on the gardening
and cooking tasks for Christine and Kaci are shown in
Figure 2. The highest number of action types possible
was six for the gardening task and five for the cooking
task. The open circles represent the number of actions
from the probe sessions across the three conditions: base-
line, video modeling, and maintenance (no video).
During the video modeling condition, these probe ses-
sions occurred each day before the children watched the
video. Figure 2 also contains data from the video model-
ing condition on the number of actions performed dur-
Point-of-View Video Modeling 89
FIGURE 2. Multiple probe data across two participants and two behaviors, with number of types of play actions per-
formed during baseline, treatment, and posttreatment maintenance sessions. Probes are marked as open circles
connected by a solid line. Practice sessions are marked as an “x” connected with a dotted line.
90 Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 26:2
ing practice sessions (depicted by an ×). These sessions
followed the daily viewing of the video; thus, they oc-
curred only during the video modeling condition.
The top panel of Figure 2 represents the number of
action types used by Christine in the gardening task.
During the baseline condition in the probe sessions for
the gardening task, Christine had between none and two
action types per session, with one action in 3 of 5 ses-
sions. With the introduction of the video modeling con-
dition, Christine displayed four action types in the first
session, dropped to one action in the next 2 sessions, and
then increased to three or four action types for the re-
mainder of the condition (with one exception on the
10th datum point of the condition, which had two ac-
tion types). In 11 of 14 probe sessions during the video
modeling condition, she performed three or four action
types per session, exceeding the highest level (i.e., two)
obtained during the baseline condition. During the 12
maintenance assessments with no video modeling,
Christine performed at least four action types in all ses-
sions except for 1. In 4 of 12 maintenance assessments,
she performed five action types per session, which was a
greater number than occurred during video modeling.
The practice sessions (depicted by an “×” in Figure 2) oc-
curred only during the video modeling condition, imme-
diately after she watched the video. Christine performed
between two and five action types each session, with 3 of
the last 4 sessions resulting in five action types each.
The second panel of Figure 2 shows Kaci’s data on
the gardening task. During baseline, Kaci performed one
or two action types per probe session, with four of six
probe sessions being one action type. With the introduc-
tion of the video modeling, she performed one action in
the first session and then four action types during each
of the next four sessions. This dropped to one action per
session for the next two sessions, increased to four ac-
tion types for two sessions, and then dropped to one ac-
tion type for the last session of the condition. During the
maintenance condition, Kaci performed between one ac-
tion type and six types per session, with four of seven
maintenance probe sessions containing four or more ac-
tion types per session. On the practice sessions in the
video modeling condition, Kaci performed from one ac-
tion type to six action types per session. She tended to do
more action types per practice session early in the condi-
tion as compared to later in the condition.
The third panel of Figure 2 shows the number of ac-
tion types performed by Christine on the cooking task.
During 10 baseline probe sessions, Christine exhibited
none of the action types. After introduction of the video
modeling, Christine’s performance of modeled actions
during treatment probes increased to between one action
type and five action types per probe session. In 3 of the
last 4 sessions of the video modeling condition, she per-
formed five action types per session. In 5 maintenance
probes, she performed between three and five action
types. Christine performed four action types in the last 2
maintenance probe sessions. During the practice sessions
of the video modeling condition, she performed one to
five action types per session. In the last six sessions, she
performed five action types per session.
The fourth panel of Figure 2 represents Kaci’s data
on the cooking task. In 10 baseline probe sessions, Kaci
ranged between no actions and two action types per ses-
sion. In the first baseline session, she performed two ac-
tion types; in the 9th probe session, she performed one
action, but in all other baseline probe sessions no actions
occurred. Introduction of the video modeling did not
change Kaci’s behavior. In 6 probe sessions, she had no
actions in 5 sessions and one action in 1 session. This re-
sulted in the adaptation described previously: the investi-
gator changed the sensory materials to colored rice, told
Kaci before each probe and practice session to play with
the toys like she saw on the video, and reinforced actions
on the toys on a fixed-ratio-1 schedule during the prac-
tice sessions. The investigator did not deliver any rein-
forcement during the probe sessions. In the probe
sessions, the adaptation resulted in an increase in the
number of actions performed, but then a drop occurred
to two actions or one action per session. For the practice
sessions before the adaptation, most sessions resulted in
no actions being performed. After the adaptation, the
number of actions increased sharply, with the last 6 prac-
tice sessions containing five action types per session.
Generalization
Generalization Across Materials. Generalization data
across materials for the gardening and cooking tasks are
shown in Table 3. As shown, Christine and Kaci dis-
played few actions during the baseline materials general-
ization probes for the gardening task and increased the
number of actions performed in the maintenance condi-
tion probes. This pattern was replicated for Christine in
the materials generalization probes for the cooking task.
Kaci performed no actions in the baseline probe sessions
on the cooking task, and she performed only one action
in the maintenance condition probe session.
Generalization Across Settings. The generalization
across settings occurred in the girls’ classrooms. As
shown in Table 3, for the gardening task, Christine and
Kaci displayed fewer actions in the baseline generaliza-
tion probes than in the maintenance probe sessions. For
the cooking task, however, neither girl displayed an in-
crease in the number of actions performed in the setting
generalization probes.
Social Validity Results
The results for the social validity assessment are shown
in Table 4. For the engagement, use of multiple actions,
Point-of-View Video Modeling 91
appropriate use of materials, and enjoyment of the activ-
ity items, posttreatment means were higher than pre-
treatment means. For the needing help with materials
item, the mean decreased. Children in the posttreatment
clip were rated higher on their engagement in the activ-
ity, their use of multiple actions while manipulating ma-
terials, using materials appropriately, and enjoying the
activity than children viewed in the pretreatment clip.
Children viewed in the posttreatment videos were also
rated as needing help in using materials less often. These
results suggest that the raters viewed the treatment as so-
cially valid.
DISCUSSION
This study evaluated the effectiveness of point-of-view
video modeling on preschoolers’ performance of play ac-
tions to determine if these skills generalized to novel ma-
terials and to the classroom. The results indicated that
the video modeling was effective in teaching specific ac-
tions on toys and sensory materials to two girls with
autism. In three of four behavior sets (gardening for both
girls, and cooking for Christine), the children acquired
new play behaviors in the absence of experimenter-im-
plemented reinforcement for performing the video-mod-
eled actions and without instructional cues on what to
do with the materials. The addition of reinforcement
during practice sessions and instructions to imitate the
actions from the video allowed Kaci to acquire the be-
haviors of the cooking set. These findings extend those
of D’Ateno et al. (2003) and Taylor et al. (1999), sug-
gesting that viewing videotapes may change not only ver-
bal behavior but also motor movements on toys in the
absence of programmed reinforcers. Whether the in-
crease in new actions constitutes symbolic or pretend
play deals in part with the level of evidence required to
infer pretense; however, this increase clearly provided the
children with additional behaviors from which pretend
routines and narratives could be displayed.
The children also generalized the skills to untrained
materials for both tasks and generalized skills into the
classroom for the gardening task but not for the cooking
task. This finding indicates that generalization can be
promoted through point-of-view video modeling involv-
ing multiple exemplars of materials. It confirms the find-
ings of Haring et al. (1987) and Charlop and Milstein
(1989) showing that multiple exemplars presented in
videos result in generalized responding to novel stimuli.
In this study, children generalized their actions across
settings for the gardening task but not for the cooking
task. The videotape did not contain multiple exemplars
of location, and this reasonamong other reasons—may
account for the lack of generalization to the classroom
for the cooking task. Other reasons may be (a) the lim-
ited number of classroom probes, (b) activities in the
classroom competing for the target children’s attention,
and (c) satiation with the materials.
TABLE 3. Performance of Participants in Probes of Generalization Across Materials and Across Settings for Baseline
and Maintenance Conditions
Baseline Maintenance
Participant # of assessments M Lowest–highest # of assessments M Lowest–highest
Generalization across materials—Gardening
Christine 1 1 4 4 (2–5)
Kaci 2 1.5 (1–2) 3 4 (4–4)
Generalization across materials—Cooking
Christine 2 1 (1–2) 3 3 (0–5)
Kaci 3 0 1 1
Generalization across settings—Gardening
Christine 1 2 2 2.5 (1–4)
Kaci 2 0.5 (0–1) 2 3.5 (2–5)
Generalization across settings—Cooking
Christine 2 0 1 0
Kaci 3 0.67 0–2 1 0
92 Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 26:2
These findings suggest that point-of-view video mod-
eling may be useful in helping young children with autism
learn to perform actions on toys from which pretense can
be inferred. We should note, however, that both partici-
pants readily imitated in vivo actions of adults with ma-
terials, contacted materials without adult prompting,
and were verbal. Future research should explore the ex-
tent to which point-of-view modeling is successful with
children who do not display these skills or who have
variations of them.
This study has a number of notable limitations. First,
acquisition of skills did not occur in the cooking task for
Kaci until a verbal instruction to imitate the video and
contingent reinforcement for doing so were used. Thus,
watching the video by itself may not be sufficient to
promote skill acquisition in some instances; other inter-
ventions may be needed. In some cases, a cue telling par-
ticipants to do what they saw in the video was necessary
for them to apply those skills in the training situation as
well as across settings and materials. Previous studies
have used reinforcement of correct imitative responding
during the video modeling phase (Charlop & Milstein,
1989, Shipley-Benamou et al., 2002). Video modeling
plus specific cues and reinforcement of correct respond-
ing may increase the rate at which skills are acquired,
and in conjunction with the use of multiple exemplars
may be more effective in promoting skill generalization.
This assertion could be a focus of future studies.
Second, performance of modeled actions may have
been more positively affected with a reduced number of
probes. In the current study, we exposed the participants
to the same materials once before the video (probe), dur-
ing the video, and after the video (practice sessions) dur-
ing each treatment session. Limiting sessions to just the
video and practice, while probing once every few ses-
sions, would decrease the amount of treatment time and
may inhibit satiation. Also, alternating materials across
sessions and thereby decreasing exposure may increase
novelty, interest, engagement, and subsequent perfor-
mance of the modeled actions. Procedures also could be
altered so the child views the video clip in the classroom
before the day’s sensory bin activity. This may promote
faster acquisition and more generalized responding due
to more functional practice. Future studies could mea-
sure the diversity of play and the effects of using toys
functionally on children’s play sequences, repetitive ac-
tions, and social exchanges with peers.
Third, the study had relatively limited measures of
generalization, especially for generalization to the class-
room. Classroom measurement was made difficult by
competing activities, which in conjunction with apparent
satiation on trained materials made other activities and
materials more interesting to the participants. In addi-
tion, the same adult was involved in implementation,
observation, and reinforcer delivery, suggesting some
stimulus control was possible over participants’ perfor-
mance. Evaluating children’s behaviors with different
adults as well as teachers’ use of the procedures need to
be studied.
Video modeling interventions capitalize on the vi-
sual strengths of children with autism, and teachers can
use them in the classroom. This study indicates that pos-
itive effects occurred on play skills after introduction of
point-of-view video modeling, which was efficient in terms
of intervention preparation. Video modeling interven-
tions can be included as resourceful methods to foster
play in preschoolers with autism.
AUTHORS’ NOTES
1. This study was conducted as a thesis study by the first author in
partial fulfillment of the master’s of education degree from the
Department of Special Education, Peabody College, Vanderbilt Uni-
versity.
2. The study was supported by a U.S. Department of Education,
Personnel Preparation, PR award (H325A030093). The opinions
expressed, however, do not necessarily reflect those of the U.S.
Department of Education.
3. The authors are grateful to the parents who allowed their children to
participate. We also are grateful to teachers Bri Klibbe and Kyle Alm-
gren of the Susan Gray School, Peabody College, and to Dr. Ruth
Wolery, director of the Susan Gray School.
TABLE 4. Mean Responses and Standard Deviations for the Social Validation of Pre- and Posttreatment Video Clips
Pretreatment clip Posttreatment clip
Questionnaire item MSD MSD
1. The child is engaged in the activity 3.73 1.04 4.78 0.42
2. The child uses multiple actions while manipulating materials 2.53 1.15 4.48 0.68
3. The child uses the materials appropriately 3.15 1.25 4.73 0.51
4. The child is enjoying the activity 3.43 0.68 4.18 0.75
5. The child needs help using the materials 3.45 1.24 1.50 0.60
Note. Scale: 1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree.
Point-of-View Video Modeling 93
REFERENCES
Alcantara, P. R. (1994). Effects of videotape instructional package on
purchasing skills of children with autism. Exceptional Children, 61,
40–55.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Bandura, A. (1965). Influence of models’ reinforcement contingencies
on the acquisition of imitative responses. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 6, 589–595.
Buggey, T., Toombs, K., Gardener, P., & Cervetti, M. (1999). Training
responding behaviors in students with autism: Using videotaped self-
modeling. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 4, 205– 214.
Charlop, M. H., & Milstein, J. P. (1989). Teaching autistic children
conversational speech using video modeling. Journal of Applied Be-
havior Analysis, 22, 275–285.
Charlop, M. H., Schreibman, L., & Tryon, A. S. (1983). Learning
through observation: The effects of peer modeling on acquisition
and generalization in autistic children. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology, 11, 355–366.
Charlop-Christy, M. H., & Daneshvar, S. (2003). Using video model-
ing to teach perspective taking in children with autism. Journal of
Positive Behavior Interventions, 5, 12–21.
Charlop-Christy, M. H., Le, L., & Freeman, K. A. (2000). A comparison
of video modeling with in vivo modeling for teaching children with
autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 537–
552.
D’Ateno, P., Mangiapanello, K., & Taylor, B. A. (2003). Using video
modeling to teach complex play sequences to a preschooler with
autism. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 5, 5–11.
Dawson, G., & Osterling, J. (1997). Early intervention in autism. In
M. J. Guralnick (Ed.), The effectiveness of early intervention (pp. 307–
326). Baltimore: Brookes.
Haring, T. G., Kennedy, C. H., Adams, M. J., & Pitts-Conway, V.
(1987). Teaching generalization of purchasing skills across commu-
nity settings to autistic youth using videotape modeling. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 89–96.
Hepting, N. H., & Goldstein, H. (1996). Requesting by preschoolers
with developmental disabilities: Videotaped self-modeling and
learning of new linguistic structures. Topics in Early Childhood
Special Education, 16, 407–427.
Horner, D. R., & Baer, D. M. (1978). Multiple probe technique: A vari-
ation of the multiple baseline. Journal of Applied Behavior Analy-
sis, 11, 189–196.
Ingersoll, B., Schreibman, L., & Tran, Q. H. (2003). Effect of sensory
feedback on immediate object imitation in children with autism.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 6, 673–682.
Jarrold, C. (2003). A review of research into pretend play in autism.
Autism, 7, 379–390.
Jarrold, C., Boucher, J., & Smith, P. (1993). Symbolic play in autism: A
review. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23, 281–
307.
Jordan, R., & Libby, S. (1997). Developing and using play in the cur-
riculum. In S. Powell & R. Jordan (Eds.), Autism and learning: A
guide to good practice. London: David Fulton.
Kinney, E. M., Vedora, J., & Stromer, R. (2003). Computer-presented
video models to teach generative spelling to a child with an autism
spectrum disorder. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 5,
22–29.
LeBlanc, L. A., Coates, A. M., Daneshvar, S., Charlop-Christy, M. H.,
Morris, C., & Lancaster, B. M. (2003). Using video modeling and
reinforcement to teach perspective-taking skills to children with
autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 253–257.
Libby, S., Powell, S., Messer, D., & Jordan, R. (1998). Spontaneous
play in children with autism: A reappraisal. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 28, 487–497.
Nikopoulos, C. K., & Keenan, M. (2004). Effects of video modeling
on social initiations by children with autism. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 37, 93–96.
Pierce-Jordan, S., & Lifter, K. (2005). Interaction of social and play be-
haviors in preschoolers with and without pervasive developmental
disorder. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 25, 34–47.
Schreibman, L., Whalen, C., & Stahmer, A. C. (2000). The use of video
priming to reduce disruptive transition behavior in children with
autism. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 2, 3–11.
Sherer, M., Pierce, K. L., Paredes, S., Kisacky, K. L., Ingersoll, B., &
Schreibman, L. (2001). Enhancing conversation skills in children
with autism. Behavior Modification, 25, 140–158.
Shipley-Benamou, R. S., Lutzker, J. R., & Taubman, M. (2002).
Teaching daily living skills to children with autism through instruc-
tional video modeling. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 4,
165–175, 188.
Stahmer, A. C., Ingersoll, B., & Carter, C. (2003). Behavioral ap-
proaches to promoting play. Autism, 7, 401–413.
Stone, W. S., Ousley, O. Y., & Littleford, C. D. (1997). Motor imita-
tion in young children with autism: What’s the object? Journal of
Abnormal Child Psychology, 25, 475–485.
Taylor, B. A., Levin, L., & Jasper, S. (1999). Increasing play-related
statements in children with autism toward their siblings: Effects of
video modeling. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabili-
ties, 11, 253–264.
Ungerer, J. A., & Sigman, M. (1981). Symbolic play and language com-
prehension in autistic children. Journal of the American Academy
of Child Psychiatry, 20, 318–337.
Van Berckelaer-Onnes, I. A. (2003). Promoting early play. Autism, 7,
415–423.
Wert, B. Y., & Neisworth, J. T. (2003). Effects of video self-modeling
on spontaneous requesting in children with autism. Journal of Posi-
tive Behavior Interventions, 5, 30–34.
Zihni, F., & Zihni, F. (2005).
AZ method: The use of video techniques
to develop language skills in autistic children. Retrieved March 10,
2005, from http://www.autismuk.com/index13.htm
... Another common intervention to target play skills is the use of video modeling. Hine and Wolery (2006) conducted a study using point-of-view video modeling to teach gardening and cooking play sequences to two children with autism. The angle of the camera filmed toys from the child's point-of-view. ...
Article
Full-text available
Play is an essential skill for children in early intervention settings and contributes to social and emotional development. Children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) often show significant delays in action-on-object play skills, vocalizations during play, and novel responding. By assessing and targeting play, children with autism can acquire play skills which may increase the likelihood of inclusion in a classroom setting and provide increased opportunities for peer and adult interaction. The current study evaluated the effects of apps as a video model to increase the duration of play, independent and novel action-on-object play, and independent and novel vocalizations during play. Current assessment and intervention strategies, as well as the need for further research making use of current technology and apps to increase play skills for school-age children with autism are discussed.
... VM allows educators to utilize students' visual strengths and removes extraneous stimuli to focus on salient and relevant information. There are four different types: (a) VM (Gilmour, 2015), where students watch videos of others; (b) video self-modeling (Bellini & McConnell, 2010), where students watch videos of themselves; (c) point-of-view modeling (Hine & Wolery, 2006), where students watch videos (recorded by themselves or others) from their perspective; and (d) video prompting (Cihak et al., 2006), which breaks down a task into clips of each step to teach students to perform specific sections of each selected skill. When using VM, the person demonstrating the target skill may be other students, adults, or animated depictions. ...
Article
General and special education teachers report feeling inadequate in providing social skill instruction to students. Yet, researchers and government bodies (ASELA, 2015) report students receiving social skill instruction display marked improvements in: (a) motivation to learn, (b) commitment to school, (c) time devoted to schoolwork, (d) mastery of subject matter, (e) school attendance, (f) graduation rates, (g) grades, and (h) test scores. Numerous technology tools are available to assist educators in providing social skill instruction to students with disabilities. However, determining a student's social skill need and finding tools that utilize effective research-based interventions can be a time-consuming and daunting task for educators. In this article, the authors provide a streamlined process for determining an individual student’s social skill instructional need and present technology tools available that utilize high-leverage and evidence-based practices (EBPs) to provide instruction for these targeted areas.
... Benefits of using this type of modelling include reducing irrelevant stimuli and thus optimising the ability of the person to focus on the specific task (Tetreault & Lerman, 2010). PVM can assist in social and play skills (Hine & Wolery, 2006), self-help skills (Norman et al., 2001), functional living (Shipley-Benamou et al., 2002) and ...
Article
Full-text available
This is the protocol for a Campbell review. The primary objective for this review is summarising the effectiveness of VBI in promoting prosocial behaviours in a population of young people with ASD. The research questions employed to fulfil this objective include: (1) Do VBI improve prosocial behaviours in children with ASD? (2) Which social skills and interactive behaviours are most successful? (3) Do VBI generally have successful rates of skill generalisation and response maintenance? (4) Do demographic characteristics (age, gender) of participants influence the effectiveness of VBI's?
... Third, play education can reduce the visible differences between children with autism and typical children. This reduction in apparent differences will increase the chances of autistic and Asperger's children participating in social or even educational skills (10). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Autism is one of the prevalent disorders in childhood. Children with autism have difficulties in different life skills and their daily activities. Pretend plays are essential in improving these skills, but the patterns of playing are impaired in this group. Objectives: The present study was designed to compare pretend plays between high-functioning autistic children and typical peers. Methods: The research is the result of a cross-sectional study. Two groups of children with high-functioning autism and typical peers in the age range of 5 - 7 years were studied. Each group consisted of 87 participants who were selected by convenience sampling. Each participant was assessed by the Child-Initiated Pretend Play assessment (ChIPPA) test. The results were analyzed using descriptive statistics and an independent t-test. Results: Data analysis and comparison of scores between the two groups of children showed a significant difference in the mean scores. The average percentage score of the number of object substitutions (NOS) and the elaborate pretend play actions (PEPA) in children with autism were lower than typical peers (P < 0.001). In other words, children with autism were less able to organize play and pretend. But the number of imitated actions (NIA) scores in children with autism was higher than their counterparts (P < 0.001). That is, children with autism relied more on examiner-playing patterns. Conclusions: The significant difference in scores between children with autism and typical peers showed significantly lower pretend skills in children with autism. According to pretend play basics on academic skills, language, and having a flexible mind, and with the attention to the same problems in children with autism, pretend play can be in greater attention to be improved among these groups.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: The purpose of this study was to compare the effectiveness and efficiency of social stories with and without video modeling in teaching how to respond to lures of strangers to children with autism. Method: The study was conducted with four children aged 10-13 years diagnosed with autism. Target skills for the students were how to avoid abduction by strangers and how to respond if a stranger knocked at the door. An adapted alternating treatments design was used in the study. Findings: The findings of the study showed that both procedures were equally effective on promoting acquisition for three of the four students with autism. The findings showed that social stories alone was more effective than social stories with video modeling for one student. There was no considerable difference between the two procedures in terms of efficiency. Social validity findings showed that opinions of the participants and their mothers were positive overall. Discussion: It is likely appropriate for teachers, researchers, practitioners, and families to use only one of the teaching practices compared in this study, taking into account the characteristics and preferences of their students in teaching the skills of protection from strangers. To generalize the findings from this study, it can be suggested that similar studies be conducted with different practitioners and participants in various age groups.
Article
BACKGROUND: Autistic adults have difficulty in learning vocational and social skills, which often translates into low employment rates. Video self-modelling (VSM) is an effective educational technique for high-support-needs autistic adults, with the ability to teach challenging vocational skills as well as basic social skills. OBJECTIVE: The present study examined the use of video self-modelling to teach vocational and social skills to a 22-year-old autistic adult. Target behaviours categories included (1) reading order forms, (2) transporting goods, and (3) engaging with customers. METHODS: A multiple baseline design was used to evaluate the effectiveness of the videos in teaching these target behaviours. The dependent variables were the percentage of tasks completed correctly, and quantitative prompt dependency, using a least-to-most prompting strategy. RESULTS: Results showed that VSM modestly improved reading order forms and transporting goods, and moderately improved engagement with customers. CONCLUSION: This intervention resulted in the successful employment of an autistic adult in a job that he desired, whilst teaching him skills he specifically struggled with. As such, VSM should be considered for others wanting to learn combined social and vocational skills.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Autism is a neurodevelopment disorder that usually appears in early years and its main characteristics are repeated problems in social interaction, abnormal verbal and nonverbal communication, and stereotyped patterns of behavior and interests. Two of the therapies that are currently being discussed are music therapy and play therapy. Music with play is one of the skills necessary for communication in children with autism who do not use language to communicate. As a result the experience of music and play and the interaction between them ends in improving communication which is one of the main problems in these children. Through the process of integrating music and plays, patients will move to reach some predetermined goals by the help of some specialists who are trained in such courses. These goals may include improving motor, social and interpersonal development, reduced stereotypical behaviors, achieving self-awareness, but they are not limited to these. Conclusion: In this article, the importance of music education and play and the effect of training and playing music in children with autism is discussed. It is concluded that music therapy should be useful in conjunction with play therapy
Article
Aim: To identify which interventions are supported by evidence and the quality of that evidence in very young children with or at high likelihood for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) to improve child outcomes. Method: We conducted an overview of reviews to synthesize early intervention literature for very young children with or at high likelihood for ASD. Cochrane guidance on how to perform overviews of reviews was followed. Comprehensive searches of databases were conducted for systematic reviews and meta-analyses between January 2009 and December 2020. Review data were extracted and summarized and methodological quality was assessed. Primary randomized controlled trial evidence was summarized and risk of bias assessed. This overview of reviews was not registered. Results: From 762 records, 78 full texts were reviewed and seven systematic reviews and meta-analyses with 63 unique studies were identified. Several interventional approaches (naturalistic developmental behavioral intervention, and developmental and behavioral interventions) improved child developmental outcomes. Heterogeneity in design, intervention and control group, dose, delivery agent, and measurement approach was noted. Inconsistent methodological quality and potential biases were identified. Interpretation: While many early interventional approaches have an impact on child outcomes, study heterogeneity and quality had an impact on our ability to draw firm conclusions regarding which treatments are most effective. Advances in trial methodology and design, and increasing attention to mitigating measurement bias, will advance the quality of the ASD early intervention evidence base.
Chapter
Peer intervention to promote social interaction, play, and conversation—the three building blocks of effective peer relations—is of great need for young preschoolers with ASD, to increase their wellbeing and reduce their social isolation throughout development. However, evidence-based peer-intervention protocols focusing explicitly on preschoolers’ naturalistic peer-to-peer engagement are not very common despite this necessity to facilitate child-child interactions, social forms of peer play, and peers’ social talk. In this chapter, we provide a review of the previous intervention studies that have endeavored to promote peer engagement. We present the available research on each of the three building blocks of adaptive peer engagement separately, although keeping in mind that peer interaction, play, and conversation are all interconnected components within the holistic model of effective peer relations.
Chapter
Teaching in today’s mainstream and special education classrooms seems to be a daunting task for teachers, considering the complexity of academic and behavioural challenges experienced by students with diverse learning needs. Research within the behavioural literature indicates the growing popularity of video-based technology to improve the learning experience for students with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Video instruction refers to the creation of videos that a teacher makes specifically to teach a concept or a specific content area. This method of instruction provides the teacher with control over the exact content to be presented in the videos, and different videos can be made at various levels, catering to diverse student needs, especially visual learners such as students with ASD. This chapter describes (a) the challenges experienced by children with moderate to severe ASD while providing a brief overview of the growing prevalence of ASD in Singapore, (b) a brief description of video instruction and its various types, (c) the benefits of video instructions for students with moderate to severe ASD, and (d) using video instructions via various handheld/mobile platforms to enhance independence and self-reliance with performing tasks across various domains among children with moderate to severe ASD.
Article
Full-text available
We examined the use of computer video models and video rewards to teach generative spelling to a child with an autism spectrum disorder. In Phase 1, Ana viewed video models of her teacher writing target words. After Ana wrote the words correctly, she watched videos of play routines unique to each training word. Ana rapidly learned to spell three five-word sets based on pictures and dictation. In Phase 2, Ana learned to spell four novel words (e.g., lore and tock) that were based on the elements of five words learned in Phase 1 (e.g., tore and lock) and arranged into a teaching matrix of three beginning consonants and three word endings. In Phases 3 and 4, Ana learned to spell subsets of four three-by-three matrices, then immediately proved capable of spelling the remaining words in each matrix. Ana also succeeded on generalization and maintenance tests at home and school throughout the study. Generative spelling thus may be derived from a teaching package that (a) involves video models and rewards and (b) arranges opportunities for learning to recombine initial consonants and word endings. Ana's proficiency in spelling helped her acquire literacy skills commensurate with her general school placement.
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated the use of video priming, or exposure, as a means for reducing or eliminating the disruptive behavior of children with autism in transition situations. Specifically, it was hypothesized that such disruptive behavior would be reduced if the children received prior priming to upcoming transitions. Three children with autism who demonstrated severely disruptive behavior during transitions viewed short videos of specific transition situations in which parents reported behavioral difficulties. A multiple-baseline design across participants showed that the implementation of the video priming procedure led to a reduction or elimination of the disruptive behavior. Further, behavior reductions generalized to new transition situations. The results are discussed in terms of the possible mechanisms responsible for treatment effects and the potential advantages of using video interventions with this population.
Article
Full-text available
Videotaped self-modeling (VSM) has been developed as a means to allow participants to view themselves in situations where they are performing at a more advanced level than they typically function. VSM has been effectively used to train positive behaviors and to reduce unwanted behaviors across a range of ages and behaviors; however, studies of VSM have not been conducted with students with autism Our study was designed to analyze the effects of VSM on the acquisition and maintenance of appropriate verbal responses to questions by children with autism. A multiple baseline design across students was used to evaluate performance. The results indicated that the three participants almost doubled their rates of appropriate responding to questions during play situations. The findings suggest that VSM may constitute a positive behavior change intervention worthy of consideration in a treatment regimen.
Article
This study evaluated the effects of a videotape instructional package (i.e., viewing videotapes, on-site prompting, and reinforcement) on the acquisition and generalization of children's grocery-purchasing skills in community settings. A multiple-baseline design across settings and within three children with autism was implemented to evaluate the training program. Acquisition of the skills taught was rapid and transferred to other settings. After training in two stores, all students increased their level of performance in the third store. Results show that the use of a videotape instructional package is an effective training tool to teach community survival skills.
Article
This study investigated the relationship between the social and play behaviors of young children with pervasive developmental disorder (PDD) and without PDD. Videotaped observations of 21 preschool children (12 with PDD and 9 without PDD) were examined independently for (a) social complexity and (b) play complexity that was assessed on an individualized basis for each child. The researchers used log-linear analyses to analyze the data. The results supported an inverse relationship between play and social interaction: The children's play within a social interaction tended to be less complex than their play outside of the interaction. All children, regardless of diagnosis, demonstrated similar behavioral patterns, although the children without PDD engaged in social interaction to a greater extent. These results suggest that social interventions should be implemented in the context of play activities that the child has mastered, as opposed to those activities the child is in the process of learning.
Article
Multiple-baseline and probe procedures are combined into a “multiple-probe” technique. The technique is designed to provide a thorough analysis of the relationship between an independent variable and the acquisition of a successive-approximation or chain sequence. It provides answers to the following questions: (1) What is the initial level of performance on each step in the training sequence? (2) What happens if sequential opportunities to perform each next step in the sequence are provided before training on that step? (3) What happens when training is applied? (4) What happens to the performance of remaining steps in the sequence as criterion is reached in the course of training each prior step? The technique features: (1) one initial probe of each step in the training sequence, (2) an additional probe of every step after criterion is reached on any training step, and (3) a series of “true” baseline sessions conducted just before the introduction of the independent variable to each training step. Intermittent probes also provide an alternative to continuous baseline measurement, when such measurement during extended multiple baselines (1) may prove reactive, (2) is impractical, and/or (3) a strong a priori assumption of stability can be made.
Article
Research on video modeling has typically utilized either competent peer models or self-models engaging in criterion performances. Although both methods have demonstrated utility in achieving skill acquisition, each has potential disadvantages. The current research utilized a multiple probe design across tasks and replicated across participants in order to demonstrate the efficacy of an instructional video modeling technique to teach functional living skills to three children with autism. Five tasks were selected. Prior to the development of each training video, task analyses were created. Videotapes were developed from the participant's viewing perspective, that is, as the participant would be viewing the task. Instructional video modeling was effective in promoting skill acquisition across all three children and maintained during the postvideo phase and a 1-month follow up.
Article
The identification of efficient teaching procedures to address deficits in imaginative play skills, which are commonly seen in children with autism, is a challenge for professionals who are designing treatment programs. In the present study, video modeling was used to teach play skills to a preschool child with autism. Videotaped play sequences included both verbal and motor responses. A multiple-baseline procedure across three response categories (having a tea party, shopping, and baking) was implemented to demonstrate experimental control. No experimenter-implemented reinforcement or correction procedures were used during the intervention. Results indicated that the video modeling intervention led to the rapid acquisition of both verbal and motor responses for all play sequences. This procedure was shown to be an efficient technique for teaching relatively long sequences of responses in relatively few teaching sessions in the absence of chaining procedures. In addition, the complex sequences of verbal and motor responses were acquired without the use of error-correction procedures or explicit experimenter-implemented reinforcement contingencies.
Article
Perspective taking refers to the ability to determine mental states of others in order to explain or predict behavior. In typically developing children, this skill appears around age 4 years (Baron-Cohen, Leslie, & Frith, 1985), but it is delayed or absent in children with autism. In the present study, video modeling was used to teach perspective taking to three children with autism. A multiple-baseline design across children and within child across tasks was used to assess learning. Generalization across untrained similar stimuli was also assessed. Video modeling was a fast and effective tool for teaching perspective-taking tasks to children with autism, resulting in both stimulus and response generalization. These results concurred with previous research that perspective taking can be taught. Unlike other studies, however, wider ranges of generalization were found.