ArticlePDF Available

Resilience in Children: A Review of Literature With Implications for Education

Authors:

Abstract

In spite of the most adverse circumstances, some children manage to survive and even thrive, academically and socially, into adulthood. A complex array of individual, family, and community factors has been identified that best explains resilience and lays the foundation for programs and interventions targeted at fostering the development and maintenance of resilience in at-risk youth. The literature is reviewed to identify and explain those factors, discuss their mutual interaction, and explain their implications for the creation of programs designed to support resilience in school-aged children.
http://uex.sagepub.com/
Urban Education
http://uex.sagepub.com/content/41/3/211
The online version of this article can be found at:
DOI: 10.1177/0042085906287902
2006 41: 211Urban Education
Steven J. Condly
Resilience in Children : A Review of Literature With Implications for Education
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
can be found at:Urban EducationAdditional services and information for
http://uex.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:
http://uex.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:
http://uex.sagepub.com/content/41/3/211.refs.htmlCitations:
What is This?
- Apr 18, 2006Version of Record >>
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
RESILIENCE IN CHILDREN
A Review of Literature With
Implications for Education
STEVEN J. CONDLY
University of Central Florida
In spite of the most adverse circumstances, some children manage to survive and
even thrive, academically and socially, into adulthood. A complex array of indi-
vidual, family, and community factors has been identified that best explains
resilience and lays the foundation for programs and interventions targeted at
fostering the development and maintenance of resilience in at-risk youth. The
literature is reviewed to identify and explain those factors, discuss their mutual
interaction, and explain their implications for the creation of programs designed
to support resilience in school-aged children.
Keywords: at-risk children; thriving; overcoming; beating the odds
In her book Shattered Assumptions,Janoff-Bulman (1992) posits
the existence of three fundamental assumptions that all people hold
and by which all people operate. They are as follows: The world is
essentially a good place; life and events have meaning and purpose;
and one’s own person is valuable or worthy. Because these assump-
tions are so fundamental, they are not usually consciously consid-
ered (Bargh & Chartrand, 1999). Rather, sensory and mental events
are evaluated through these principles. For most of what occurs in
life, with all its ups and downs, both the routine and the novel, these
assumptions remain intact, unquestioned, and unmoved. Yet occa-
sionally events transpire that are so hurtful to persons that their fun-
damental worldview is questioned and altered. Traumatic events
effect great damage not so much because of the immediate harm
they cause but also because of the lingering need to re-evaluate
one’s view of oneself and the world.
211
URBAN EDUCATION, Vol. 41 No. 3, May 2006 211-236
DOI: 10.1177/0042085906287902
© 2006 Corwin Press, Inc.
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Of course, trauma does not affect all people in the same way and
with the same force. Some people, after initial pain and suffering,
manage to lead essentially normal, and perhaps even highly pro-
ductive, lives. They do not seem embittered, angry, depressed, or
otherwise incapacitated. Although exact percentages are unavail-
able, it is probably fair to state that, given greater severity of trauma
and/or frequency of traumatic events, an individual’s likelihood of
being able to cope and progress lessens. Nevertheless, there are
examples of people surviving severe and prolonged exposure to
trauma who manage both to survive and prosper (e.g., Elie Wiesel,
survivor of Auschwitz, Buna, Buchenwald, and Gleiwitz, and win-
ner of the Nobel Peace Prize). How many others are there who sur-
vive trauma of the nonheadline variety and manage to succeed
in family, work, and in their communities? It is these so-called
resilient people who have become the focus of intense recent psy-
chological research.
Although not demonstrated as empirical fact, most people would
assume that, although an earthquake or brutal robbery would be
devastating to adults and children alike, it is children who suffer
more and therefore warrant more concern and care, as they lack the
cognitive, physical, and financial means to care for themselves,
make sense of happenings, affect changes, and take preventive mea-
sures to help ensure against a repeat of the traumatic event (assum-
ing such a prevention is possible). This, however is a belief, not a
fact determined by research. Nevertheless, this belief has fueled
much professional research interest and private and governmental
investment of time and money in strategies, policies, and programs
that can help protect children against environmental harm, and
when harmed, mitigate the consequences.
Clearly, poverty and its associated problems (crime, lack of
opportunity, violence, etc.) are hard on children (Cicchetti &
Garmezy, 1993, Sigman, 1995). When exposed to the multiple
stressors of poverty, children typically do not do well in school, are
more likely to be delinquent in later years, and have more and greater
marital and occupational problems (Cowen, Wyman, Work, &
Parker, 1990; Garbarino, 1995; Garbarino, Dubrow, Kostelny, &
Pardo, 1992; Katz, 1997; Stouthamer-Loeber et al., 1993; Werner,
1989, 1993; Zimrin, 1986). But there is a class of children who defy
212 URBAN EDUCATION / MAY 2006
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
the conventional wisdom and not only survive hostile environments
but also actually thrive; these are the resilient (Garmezy, 1996;
Masten & Coatsworth, 1998; Masten et al., 1995; Wolff, 1995). A
cautionary note is in order, however. Resilience should not be con-
sidered a single dichotomous variable (you either are or are not
resilient in any and all situations); rather, resilience is better per-
ceived as a label that defines the interaction of a child with trauma
or a toxic environment in which success, as judged by societal
norms, is achieved by virtue of the child’s abilities, motivations,
and support systems. Additionally, it is more accurate to describe
resilience in continuous rather than dichotomous terms.
The study of resilience is rooted in earlier research in psy-
chopathology, poverty, and traumatic stress (both individual and
historical; Cicchetti & Garmezy, 1993). Garmezy (1971, 1991) him-
self serves as the best starting point for modern research in resilience
as he and others began to examine not pathology under adverse
circumstances but rather resistance and growth. While research-
ers were busy discovering principles that would explain the psy-
chopathology associated with conditions of poverty, environmental
stress, and low socioeconomic status (SES) and IQ levels, it became
clear that a significant minority of people living in such conditions
were not suffering obvious or overt ill effects. The medical com-
munity examined how persons exposed to the stressors of disease
and illness led and continue to lead productive lives (Hoekelman,
1991; Leonard, 1991; Patterson, 1991; Sinnema, 1991). Persons
interested in understanding and effectively dealing with delinquency
examined the relationship between IQ and delinquency as well as
factors such as SES and ethnicity (Cederblad, Dahlin, Hagnell, &
Hansson, 1995; Kandel et al., 1988; Lynam, Moffit, & Stouthamer-
Loeber, 1993; Moffitt & Silva, 1988; White, Moffitt, & Silva, 1989).
Educators refined the concept and sought to understand the factors
that explained academic, as opposed to social, resilience (Finn &
Rock, 1997; Gordon, 1996; Masten, et al., 1988; Waxman, Huang, &
Padrón, 1997; Winfield, 1994). Some researchers focused on charac-
teristics of the individual, whereas others examined the family
(Carro, Grant, Gotlib, & Compas, 1993; Conrad & Hammen, 1993;
Gribble et al., 1993; Janoff-Bulman, 1992; Radke-Yarrow & Brown,
1993; Rende & Plomin, 1993; Smith, 1995; Smith & Prior, 1995;
Condly / RESILIENCE IN CHILDREN 213
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Wyman, Cowen, Work, & Parker, 1991; Wyman et al., 1992). Of
particular importance to the discussion of relevant individual and
social (i.e., familial and environmental) characteristics is the theory
of Scarr and McCartney (1983), which seeks to explain how indi-
vidual characteristics interact with the environment to produce new
environments (however, see Baumrind, 1993, for a critique of the
theory). Suffice it to say that there are many perspectives on the
causes of resiliency and how it plays out in the lives of children and
adults.
This review will introduce the discussion of resilience by nesting
it within a brief acknowledgement of the situation in which children
find themselves today. Next, presentation of competing theoretical
perspectives leads to an in-depth analysis of the three main factors
associated with resilience. This analysis segues into a discussion of
present perspectives on the exact nature of the resilience construct
and ends with a description of how the reviewed research informs (or
should inform) the fostering of resilience in an educational setting.
DANGER AND SURVIVAL
Although perhaps society, civilization, and life have never been
ideal and technological and legal advances have improved the lot of
most people, research makes clear that children in modern American
society are being exposed to an ever increasingly hostile or toxic
environment (Garbarino, 1995; Garbarino et al., 1992; Katz, 1997;
Masten & Coatsworth, 1998). There have been increases in child
poverty, drug use, violence, and abuse; declines in academic perfor-
mance; and fundamental changes (for the worse) in discipline and
social behavior. Garbarino (1995) goes so far as to say that the envi-
ronments in which children live today have become “poisonous to
their development” (p. 4). What seems most problematic about the
aforementioned list of social and personal pathologies is their simul-
taneous and concurrent existence. Specifically, it is hypothesized
that no one single factor is, in and of itself, sufficient to pose a
significant risk to the child’s development and behavior. Rather,
there is a cumulative effect, one that is multiplicative (not merely
additive) in nature (Garbarino et al., 1992). For example, Sameroff,
214 URBAN EDUCATION / MAY 2006
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Seifer, Barocas, Zax, and Greenspan (1987) compared verbal IQ
with number of risk factors experienced. There was not much of a
decline in mean IQ scores in the presence of none, one, or two risk
factors, but there was a precipitous drop when there were three or
more risk factors. In addition to this, it was also discovered that the
effect size of multiple risk factors was much greater (three times as
large) than that of any single risk factor. According to Garbarino
et al. (1992), although the danger for urban children is already at
critical levels, children in general are at risk because of the power-
fully negative effect of multiple risk factors such as single parent-
hood, parental rigidity, lack of community support for the child and
family, and economic pressures.
The seminal Kauai Longitudinal Study (Werner, 1989, 1993,
1995), which followed all children born on that Hawaiian island in
1955, indicates that of 698 children born, one third (201) were con-
sidered high risk; and of the high-risk children, one third (72) were
considered resilient. One third of a birth cohort is a large number,
and it must be remembered that this percentage is based on children
born nearly a half a century ago and away from mainland United
States and its metropolitan areas. The situation today is considered
to be even more grim (Garbarino, 1995; Garbarino et al., 1992).
In spite of the condition of today’s children’s environment, many
children manage to survive and even do quite well. Much research
has already examined the correlates and causes of psychological and
social pathology; now research is targeting those individuals who
thrive in spite of risk. It is hoped that, by studying those children and
adults who have succeeded in spite of extremely negative circum-
stances, interventions can be developed and delivered to those in
need (Cicchetti & Garmezy, 1993; Consortium on the School-Based
Promotion of Social Competence, 1996; Cowen, Wyman, Work, &
Iker, 1995; Cowen et al., 1990; Garbarino, 1995; Work, Cowen,
Parker, & Wyman, 1990).
THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES
A prerequisite for the success of any theory is the accurate defin-
ition and operationalization of terms. Resilience can be thought of as
Condly / RESILIENCE IN CHILDREN 215
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
an enduring characteristic of the person, a situational or temporal
interaction between the person and the context, or a unitary or
multifaceted construct, and it can be applied to social, academic,
or other settings (Cicchetti & Garmezy, 1993). Some definitions
include the following: success in meeting tasks and expectations
(Luthar, Doernberger, & Zigler, 1993), the maintenance and orien-
tation of homeostasis and functionally optimal adaptation (Block &
Block, 1980), and the ability to thrive in the face of obstacles or
adverse circumstances (Gordon, 1996).
Garmezy (1991) provides the most widely accepted framework
for understanding resilience. Based on past and current research,
this pioneer in the field posits three factors that seem to be found
universally in all definitions and research on children and adults
and how they deal with and overcome obstacles and hostile envi-
ronments. The first factor deals with the individual and includes
such elements as native intelligence and temperament. The second
concerns the family and the degree of support it can give to the
child. The third is the external support from persons and institu-
tions outside the individual and the family that can assist both the
child and the family. Werner’s (1989) three common factors are
similar to those of Garmezy (1991). She identifies personality
characteristics of the child, emotional integration within the family,
and the degree of outside support that the child and family can
obtain. Each of these three general factors will be explored indi-
vidually. It should be emphasized at the outset that, although each
factor has identifiable characteristics, there is nothing inherently
immutable about them. That is to say, although a child and his or
her family may behave in a certain way, both genetic inheritance
and the social environment interact to produce the observed behav-
ior (Rutter, 1987, 1996). Disagreements abound on which has pri-
macy in a particular situation, but there is little disagreement over
the fact that children’s resilience can be explained as an interaction
between their genetic makeup and the kind of support they receive.
INDIVIDUAL CHARACTERISTICS
The two most frequently mentioned individual characteristics of
resilient children are high intelligence or cognitive ability and an
216 URBAN EDUCATION / MAY 2006
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
easy temperament (Cicchetti, Rogosch, Lynch, & Holt, 1993;
Eysenck, 1998; Luthar, 1993; Quinton, Pickles, Maughan, &
Rutter, 1993; Rutter, 1987, 1996; Wolff, 1995). An above-average
intelligence contributes to children’s resilience by allowing children
to understand what is happening to them, to distinguish between
what is controllable and what is not, to choose effective means of
coping, and to select and modify more supportive environments
(Block & Kremen, 1996; Cederblad et al., 1995; Sameroff et al.,
1987; Scarr & McCartney, 1983). Kandel et al. (1988) found high
IQ (a useful proxy for intelligence; Jensen, 1998) to offer protec-
tion against delinquency for high-risk men. Sameroff et al. (1987)
found that high-risk children were 24 times as likely to have an IQ
below 85 as were low-risk children. It has been argued that class
or race is at least as strongly related to delinquency as IQ is; how-
ever, Hirschi and Hindelang (1977) found that IQ has an indepen-
dent effect on men at high risk of delinquency and that this effect
was mediated by school-related variables and attitudes. The theory
is that high intelligence leads to more rewards in school (teacher
praise, recognition, high grades), and these rewards then serve to
increase the student’s attachment to the school community. Positive
regard for the school’s social community, subsequently, decreases
instances of antisocial behavior and delinquency (Kandel et al.,
1988; White et al., 1989).
High intelligence is not a perfectly protective factor, however,
nor does it come without its own peculiar costs. Resilient children
of high intelligence, when compared to their nonresilient at-risk
peers, seem to suffer more emotional distress and depression
(Luthar, 1991; Luthar et al., 1993). It is hypothesized that highly
intelligent children are more sensitive to environmental stimula-
tion and therefore suffer greater symptoms of internal stress. Thus,
although high intelligence may help a child find a satisfactory
solution to a traumatic event, this same intelligence may also leave
that child more vulnerable to emotional scarring and the internal-
ization of stress. It is normal for children to fight against negative
stressors. Most take the fight outside themselves with aggressive
and disruptive behavior, usually to no avail; more resilient children
shift the fight away from the outside world to their own internal
Condly / RESILIENCE IN CHILDREN 217
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
mental world. The fight is no less energy depleting, but the results
on the child’s psyche may be more harmful.
Parenthetically, this unusual finding raises an issue crucial to the
identity and identification of resilience. When a child makes it out of
a high-crime, low-income neighborhood and enrolls in college, we
label that child resilient. Yet if Luthar (1991; Luthar et al., 1993) is to
be believed, that same child likely is more depressed than a similar
but less bright child from the same neighborhood. Depression clearly
has debilitating effects (see Ekman & Davidson, 1994, and LeDoux,
1996, for a discussion of the powerful beneficial and harmful effects
that both positive and negative emotions can have on individuals)
so the high-IQ child should be in worse shape; yet the research indi-
cates that, except for the greater levels of depression, they often fare
better than do their lower IQ age mates. No long-term studies have
been performed to track the lifespan of the depression, but it is pos-
sible that the depression fades with time as the resilient individual
adjusts, strategizes, and ultimately takes situation-improving actions.
The other individual characteristic frequently mentioned in the
literature is an easygoing temperament. Werner (1989, 1993) states
that such a temperament is obvious from infancy, allows the child
to engage the world on easier terms, and elicits more positive
responses from caregivers. Scarr and McCartney (1983) advance
the notion that, although environments obviously affect people’s
behavior, the reverse is also true. Individuals, through the pheno-
typic expression of their genotype, modify and even actively select
environments that they find to be more conducive to their prefer-
ences and talents. Additionally, through their behavior, people can
evoke responses that work either for their benefit or their harm
(Wolff, 1995). For example, in one study (Smith & Prior, 1995),
teacher ratings of students’ positive temperament better predicted
resilience on academic and social indicators than did ratings of
self-esteem, gender, and mother-child warmth (r2=.38); however,
IQ still was the strongest predictor with more than half the vari-
ance explained (r2=.55).
Research has shown that resilient adults typically attribute their
status to a dogged determinism, held throughout childhood, that
they would conquer their circumstances, that they were people of
218 URBAN EDUCATION / MAY 2006
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
worth and value, and that they had the inner resources to succeed
(Staudinger, Marsiske, & Baltes, 1993; Watt, David, Ladd, &
Shamos, 1995; Werner, 1989, 1993, 1995). The research on locus of
control is surprisingly equivocal: Some studies show an internal locus
of control contributing to resilience (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1997;
Luthar, 1991; Werner, 1989) whereas others do not (Watt et al., 1995).
This is perhaps best understood as a conflation of locus and control-
lability that much research ignores; that is, just because a perceived
cause is deemed to be internal does not necessarily mean that the
cause is subject to the person’s own influence (Pintrich & Schunk,
2002). The importance of individual characteristics pertinent to
resilience cannot be overestimated. Cicchetti and Rogosch (1997)
found that for nonmaltreated at-risk children, relationship variables
were more important for predicting resiliency, whereas for maltreated
at-risk children, individual characteristics were more important.
To summarize, resilient children tend to possess an above-
average intelligence and have a temperament that endears them to
others and that also does not allow them to succumb to self-pity.
High intelligence alone is insufficient for protection against stress;
the increased awareness of the miserable circumstances in which
resilient children find themselves can serve to exacerbate their mis-
ery. What is also needed is a temperament that renders them akin to
velvet-covered steel: soft to the touch outside but strong inside to
provide necessary support in times of stress. Their intelligence and
temperament interact so as to allow them to understand a situation
well, seek out coping mechanisms, not feel sorry for themselves
(and thus incur a paralyzing emotional effect), and persist in their
attempts to survive and survive well.
FAMILY CHARACTERISTICS
When a child manages to thrive in spite of adverse environmen-
tal circumstances such as violence in the neighborhood or extreme
poverty, it is reasonable to examine the family for clues in explain-
ing the child’s resilience. Gribble et al. (1993) focused on this sec-
ond of the three major resilience factors. They found that parents of
stress-resilient children had more positive parental attitudes, were
more involved in their children’s lives, and provided more and better
Condly / RESILIENCE IN CHILDREN 219
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
guidance. The result was that the stress-resilient children were more
securely attached than were stress-affected children. A discriminant
function analysis performed by Wyman et al. (1991) showed that
seven aggregated variables accounted for 57% of group status. Of
the seven, five were direct family effects on the child: caregiver’s
positive expectations for the child’s future, reduced infant separa-
tions from caregiver, inductive discipline, father’s involvement, and
stable family discipline. Other research has confirmed these findings
(Wyman et al., 1992).
Even when the parents themselves are a source of stress for the
child, the parent-child relationship can still somewhat paradoxically
offer protection against stress. Kauffman, Grunebaum, Cohler, and
Gamer (1979) studied resilient children of psychotic mothers.
Maternal depression was found to be more of a risk factor than
was schizophrenia, but children managed to obtain a high level of
social competence if they had a warm relationship with their
mothers. Apparently, the resilient child was able to distinguish
between the mother’s problem behavior and her feelings toward
her child. Thus, although the psychosis offered risk, relational
warmth was demonstrated to be compensatory.
Work by Conrad and Hammen (1993) further dissects the psy-
chosis issue and examines the relationship between children’s
resilience status and mothers who were categorized as normal,
medically ill, suffering from unipolar depression, or suffering
from bipolar depression. The research made it clear that resiliency
is a very complicated issue. For example, strong friendships with
peers served as protective factors for children with medically ill
mothers; however, an adult friend was a risk factor for children of
unipolar depressed mothers. Maternal competence was a protec-
tive factor for children of unipolar depressed mothers but was a
risk factor for children of bipolar depressed mothers. The same
was true for children whose fathers were healthy and in the home.
Thus, it seems clear that it is not enough to point to variables such
as maternal depression or paternal involvement and categorize
them as risk and protective factors, respectively. Apparently, other
factors interact with these that then work together to leave a child
resilient or vulnerable to stress.
220 URBAN EDUCATION / MAY 2006
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
No serious researcher denies the importance of the microenvi-
ronments and macroenvironments as factors in resiliency status;
where there is disagreement, however, is in how much individual
characteristics based on one’s genotype contribute and how much
the environment contributes. If a child grows up in a poor, violence-
ridden neighborhood and lives in a broken, dysfunctional family
and yet manages to thrive, that child is labeled resilient. Clues for
an explanation of the phenomenon of the child’s resilience are
sought in the child and in the child’s environment. An easy tem-
perament and high intelligence are often cited as factors that serve
to protect against or compensate for adverse circumstances. Family
warmth and stable significant others (usually adult) are also fre-
quently cited. Yet it is possible that what appears to be resilience is,
in fact, not.
Plomin (1989) and Rende and Plomin (1993) distinguish
between distal and proximal environmental risks. An abusive parent
would be a proximal risk because the abuse directly affects the
child, whereas parental lack of education would be a distal risk
because it affects family income, kinds and amounts of reading
material, and the like that then affect the child.
Additionally, Plomin (1989) and Rende and Plomin (1993)
make the claim that there are children who do quite well in spite
of being in at-risk families, not because they are resilient but
rather because they are not genetically at risk. That is, the pheno-
typic behavior of high academic and social competence is directly
related to a genotype that, given any chance at all, will be
expressed. Scarr and McCartney (1983) argue that there is a wide
range of environments in which people can thrive and that things
are not so delicate that the slightest stressor will condemn a child
to a less-than-fulfilling life. There are many environments that are
“functionally equivalent” (p. 429), and humans are able to reach
their potential, or at least marginal competency, in environments
that are less than ideal. Therefore, from the point of view of behav-
ior genetics, a child would be considered resilient if he or she did
not exhibit any behavioral abnormalities in spite of a genetic predis-
position toward such abnormalities. Perhaps science is a long way
from devising tests that can help determine genetic predispositions
Condly / RESILIENCE IN CHILDREN 221
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
one way or the other, but the theory is an intriguing one and should
encourage much research.
Against the common wisdom, some researchers question the
importance of the family regarding child development. Rowe
(1994) cites behavior genetic research in proposing the primacy
of individual genotype and nonshared environmental experiences
in producing a particular phenotype. According to Rowe, there
are three main problems with family socialization theories of
development. One, such theories do not account for the presence or
effect of genes. Two, borrowing from Scarr and McCartney (1983),
people are able both to select and modify their environments. And
three, also reminiscent of Scarr and McCartney’s theory, whereas
the child’s development is shaped by how the parents treat and react
to the child, it is also true that the child, through his or her own dis-
positions, evokes certain kinds of treatment and responses from
parents, thus reinforcing certain propensities and depressing others.
Baumrind (1993) offers a major critique of the environmental
theory of Scarr and McCartney (1983), and by extension, behavior
genetics. She argues that because the average expectable environment
is a concept poorly defined, it cannot contribute to an understanding
of the development of resilience or psychopathology. Additionally,
she claims that because percentages of shared as compared with non-
shared environment increase as one moves from attitude and obser-
vation measures to more traditional IQ and personality measures, it is
impossible to be certain just how much each kind of environment is
contributing. Finally, she questions the value of heritability estimates
that cannot explain the developmental process.
In summary, at the very least, it can be claimed that a good and
supportive family can do no harm in helping a child cope with
adverse circumstances and that it very probably helps. There is
presently much debate about the relative contribution of shared as
opposed to unshared environment in the development of person-
ality and how much the environment contributes compared to the
contribution of one’s genotype. Without making an unsubstanti-
ated statement, it is perhaps best, in light of recent evidence, to
claim that the role of the family in the development of resilience
in children is most important early in life and that the role of the
family declines in importance as the child ages.
222 URBAN EDUCATION / MAY 2006
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
EXTERNAL SUPPORT
Enough correlational research has been conducted to substantiate
the claim that parental involvement in a child’s life can be either
a source of risk or resilience depending on the child’s own character-
istics and how the larger environment interacts with the child and
family (Carro et al., 1993; Radke-Yarrow & Brown, 1993). Although
the debate continues on the relative importance of the family envi-
ronment for child development (see Baumrind, 1993; Plomin, 1989;
Rende & Plomin, 1993; Rowe, 1994; Scarr & McCartney, 1983), a
great deal of research has supported the contention of both Garmezy
(1991) and Werner (1989) that external support for the child and the
family is indispensable for the development of childhood resilience.
Wolff (1995), for instance, has found that employment opportunities
for high-risk men greatly reduces risk for the children of these men.
External support does not simply manifest itself magically; there are
causal explanations. Milgram and Palti (1993), while pointing to the
oft-cited characteristics of high intelligence and easy temperament to
explain a child’s resilience, also discuss how such children are supe-
rior in locating and maintaining support from their age mates and
from adults. It seems as though the individual characteristics of supe-
rior cognitive functioning and easy temperament serve as the foun-
dation for the superior social skills that resilient children possess.
Their individual qualities make them more attractive to peers and
adults, and this attraction allows them greater ease in maintaining
close relationships.
Support for the family has been cited as a major discriminating
factor in identifying resilient urban children who have experienced
major life stresses (Wyman et al., 1991); however, the support must
be specific in nature. Carro et al. (1993), when examining parental
depression, did not find evidence for any protective effects that
social support might have for children. Conrad and Hammen (1993)
obtained mixed results for the effects that social support for the child
of depressed, ill, or normal mothers would have. In some instances,
friendships were a protective factor, and in others, it was a risk
factor. It seems that, to be useful for children, social support must
include the at-risk children in a central way but also involve the
larger family. In other words, it is best when the family as a whole
is being supported.
Condly / RESILIENCE IN CHILDREN 223
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
THE NATURE OF RESILIENCE
If one concentrates on examining the characteristics of the
resilient child, one might be tempted to view resilience as an innate
quality that would allow the child to survive a great many adverse
circumstances. If one looks at the child’s family and social envi-
ronment, then an argument can be made that resilience is not so
much a quality of the child as it is the interactional processes
between the child and the environment. The two positions are not
necessarily in conflict, as there is research that supports both
points of view. Wolff (1995) summarizes the situation expertly:
Resilience is an enduring aspect of the person. Genetic and other
constitutionally based qualities both determine and are in turn
modified by life experiences. Good intelligence plays a major part,
as does an easy, adaptable, sociable temperament that, together
with an appealing appearance, attract positive responses from
others which in turn contribute to that inner sense of self-worth,
competence and self-efficacy that has repeatedly been identified
as a vital component of resilience. The sources of such positive
responses are threefold: primary relationships within the family;
the network of relationships with adults and children outside the
family; and competence and achievement. (p. 568)
Although the three factors of individual characteristics, positive
family relationships, and outside support continue to be examined
in the literature, attention is shifting toward an attempt to under-
stand other aspects of the nature of resilience. It is becoming clearer
to researchers that resilience and risk are multifaceted; that is, they
are not unitary in their origin, expression, being, or maintenance.
Masten et al. (1995), in researching both young children and
adolescents, found that competence in childhood had three
dimensions: school or academic, social, and conduct or behavior.
In adolescence, two additional dimensions were found: romantic
and occupational. Risks from trauma and adverse circumstances
could affect a young child’s or adolescent’s competence along any
one or several of the aforementioned dimensions, with the other
dimensions remaining relatively unaffected. The work of Luthar
et al. (1993) echoes this concept. They found that children who
224 URBAN EDUCATION / MAY 2006
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
exhibited resilient behavior in one domain (teacher-rated school
behavior, for example) were not necessarily resilient in another
domain (such as school grades or level of depression).
Risk itself is multidimensional (Work et al., 1990). It can be
viewed as life events, minor stressors that accumulate with time,
specific high-risk factors, low SES, or any combination thereof
(Luthar & Zigler, 1991). Masten and Coatsworth (1998) refer to
the cumulative nature of risk and the necessary effect this has on
the design of interventions. These observations are not unimpor-
tant. An accurate description of the nature of risk is crucial to
understanding how it affects people, how resilience operates, and
how to develop interventions that work in the real world. Because
risks are multifaceted in nature, it necessarily follows that
resilience too is multi-faceted. Thus, the concept of an overall
resilience may have to give way to one that is situational in its
operation (Luthar, 1993).
Many researchers are now defining resilience as a process with
a dynamic character (Rutter, 1987, 1996). Quinton et al. (1993)
describe a resilience that operates both passively, through an
increase in a person’s ability to withstand traumatic situations, and
actively, by shaping the environment so as to minimize the person’s
interaction with trauma and possibly to leave the negative environ-
ment for a more positive one (cf. Scarr & McCartney, 1983). If
resilience is not conceived of as a given character trait but rather a
capacity that develops as a result of how the person interacts with his
or her environment, there should be little evidence of competence
with time; that is, protective factors should only diminish the impact
of negative consequences. This idea has some research support
(Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1997; Egeland, Carlson, & Sroufe, 1993).
Egeland et al. found that the effects of extreme risk are cumulative
and that they increased with the passage of time. Resilience, or the
positive response to extreme risk, would necessarily be under
increasingly greater strain as the various risk factors accumulate. In
examining middle-aged adults who had overcome extreme stress
early in their lives, Watt et al. (1995) noted that in their discussion
with the participants there was unanimity on only one issue. All
adults interviewed held the belief that their triumphing over adverse
Condly / RESILIENCE IN CHILDREN 225
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
childhood circumstances was not a one-step process; instead, it was
an ongoing process that must be chosen by the person constantly,
in a variety of circumstances, and with some assistance to nurture the
decision. Waxman et al. (1997), in examining motivational and learn-
ing environmental differences between resilient and nonresilient
Hispanic middle school students, found that the factors such as
problem-solving skills, use of time, and social competence, all of
which combine to influence overall academic resilience, are them-
selves subject to intervention and modification. Gordon (1996)
contributed the finding that personal and environmental factors asso-
ciated with resiliency do not change across ethnic groups, although
this finding may depend on how resilience and risk were defined and
measured. What is considered risk or resilience in one culture may
be the norm or rebellion in another.
In seeking to understand how human personality is structured,
Block and Block (1980) distinguish between ego control and ego
resiliency. Ego control is concerned with the degree of control one
has over one’s emotions and how such control is modulated. A key
aspect of ego control is permeability, or how much one’s personality
systems’ boundaries allow or disallow the passage of forces or ten-
sions on those systems. This factor is measured on a continuous scale
with overcontrol and undercontrol at the extremes. Ego resiliency
refers to the structures and processes that allow a person to maintain
homeostasis and optimize functional adaptation to adverse circum-
stances. The key concept here is elasticity, the degree to which the
boundaries between personality systems change their permeability
capability and allow the person to adapt to environmental demands.
The scale here is bounded by resiliency or brittleness.
Strong ego development has been found to be a major com-
pensatory factor that remains robust in the face of various stres-
sors and through time (Luthar, 1991). Cicchetti et al. (1993)
showed that ego overcontrol was predictive of resiliency in mal-
treated children. Overcontrol, characterized by suppressed emo-
tionality, empathy, and compliance, permits the stressed child to
limit the number of additional problems that need be faced,
diminishes their effect and the chances of their growing into
larger problems, and perhaps protects them from being targeted
226 URBAN EDUCATION / MAY 2006
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
for further incidents of maltreatment and stress (Block & Block,
1980; Cicchetti et al., 1993). Such overcontrol may assist the
child in determining what is needed for coping successfully with
a difficult environment (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1997).
Another curious phenomenon associated with resilience is the
mediating role of gender. Werner (1989), in her longitudinal study
of resiliency among a cohort of people born in 1955 on the
Hawaiian island of Kauai, found that boys were more vulnerable
than girls to biological insult and caregiver deficits. Women were
more at risk during adolescence, and by age 30, the advantage had
shifted again with men being at greater risk. Boys have been shown
to be at greater risk of developing behavioral and emotional prob-
lems when they are exposed to stressful family arrangements
(Rutter, 1987, 1996). Boys often do not have a positive same-sex
role model in the home as girls do, as many troubled, lower SES
families tend to be headed by single mothers. Additionally, boys
tend to be treated more harshly than girls in both school and in the
home. It is this combination of increased environmental pressure
coupled with a more immature neurological and other biological
development (vis-à-vis girls’ development) that probably best
explains the rather robust finding of sex-related differences in risk
and resilience.
FOSTERING RESILIENCE IN AN
EDUCATIONAL SETTING
Tolan and Dodge (2005) assert that there is a tendency in psy-
chology for “passive reliance on the client to initiate contact”
(p. 612). Perhaps this is more of a problem when offering services
to adult alcoholics as opposed to troubled children; after all, such
adults are not under obligation to obtain these services, whereas
children are required to attend school. It would seem that children
by and large would be enjoying development and support services
because most of them attend public schools for 13 years. However,
the mere presence of the child in the school does not guarantee that
the child will step forward to apply for these services, and that is
Condly / RESILIENCE IN CHILDREN 227
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
assuming that such services even exist at the school. Essential for
attempts at fostering resilience in at-risk students, then, is a proac-
tive (even aggressive?) stance on the part of school personnel to
identify such students and involve them in school-based support
programs.
According to Katz (1997), the key to developing resiliency in
children is opportunities, both plentiful and meaningful. Opportu-
nities to rest from resisting a hostile environment, opportunities
to explore in safety and security, opportunities to believe and to
dream; all these need to be given to at-risk children if they are to
have any chance at all of making it out of their dire circumstances
successfully.
One way of providing the opportunity for developing resilience
is through religious involvement (Katz, 1997; Werner, 1989, 1993).
Many adults who have successfully made their way out of poor
environments have cited religious faith as being extremely impor-
tant in their coming to grips with themselves, their circumstances,
and the world, and cited that it has provided a sense of hope for
them. Most of the adults who were interviewed by Katz (1997) and
Werner (1989, 1993) said that their faith in God was instrumental
in their overcoming bad circumstances and maintained that they
would not have made it otherwise. Thus, children who are at risk of
problems of abuse, violence, drugs, crime, and the like, can poten-
tially be affected positively by the involvement of formal and infor-
mal religious organizations in their lives.
This, naturally, presents a problem for the nonsectarian public
schools; faculty and staff are in no position to recommend reli-
gions, engage in religious acts, or serve as mediators of religious
dogma. However, even the public schools can acknowledge that
religion exists, that there is evidence of its efficacy (Katz, 1997;
Werner, 1989, 1993), and that there are (in all likelihood) a wide
variety of local religious organizations of which students (and
their families) can avail themselves.
Schools’ recommendations to students to avail themselves of
outside services are not limited to religious organizations; nonsec-
tarian organizations and programs abound. Tolan, Gorman-Smith,
and Henry (2004) report that in one such program, SAFEChildren,
228 URBAN EDUCATION / MAY 2006
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
420 inner-city families were assigned either to a control condition
or an experimental one in which family-focused preventive inter-
ventions and academic tutoring were offered. Although quite weak
(the largest effect size was .17), benefits included improved acade-
mic performance and better parental involvement. Thus, it would
appear that even large-scale, multifaceted interventions might not
realize all the benefits for which one hopes and works on.
For good or ill, children spend a great deal of time in school. If the
schools are resource poor, short on qualified staff, and/or exist in dan-
gerous neighborhoods, then the development of resilience is likely to
be hampered. On the other hand, because schools are places in which
children spend so much time, they are ideal locations for the imple-
mentation of programs designed to support children and assist them
in overcoming environmental stressors (Ross, Smith, Casey, &
Slavin, 1995). The Consortium on the School-Based Promotion of
Social Competence (1996) argues that schools are unique in that they
have students for a great many years, deal with academic, moral, and
health issues, and usually enjoy strong community support to suc-
ceed in their endeavors. There are concerns, however, about the abil-
ity of schools to carry out their charges, because the list of failures of
schools to deal with these academic, moral, and health issues seems
to grow endlessly (Price, Cowen, Lorion, & Ramos-McKay, 1988;
Steinberg, 1996). What apparently works best is when an interven-
tion takes into consideration the personal developmental level of the
child and includes all aspects of the school in the intervention. The
curriculum should include development of the target skills; the train-
ing should be intensive and ongoing (i.e., it should last beyond a
simple lesson or even a semester); and the school’s adult staff should
be convinced of the efficacy of the intervention and be devoted both
to the students and to the proper implementation of the intervention
program (Consortium on the School-Based Promotion of Social
Competence, 1996). Starting early seems to increase the likelihood of
developing resilience (Werner & Smith, 1992).
Another point about the development of resilience in children is
that it is important to distinguish between programs that seek to pro-
tect and programs that aim to reduce risk (Consortium on the
School-Based Promotion of Social Competence, 1996). In fostering
Condly / RESILIENCE IN CHILDREN 229
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
protection and reducing risk, researchers and program personnel
need to consider attributes of the individual, the family, and the larger
community (Garmezy, 1971). Researchers need to analyze both
the children at risk and their immediate and larger environments to
determine the effectiveness of the intervention, the suitability of
the program, and provide information useful for troubleshooting
and maintenance of the program (Pless & Stein, 1996; Richters &
Martinez, 1993). Wyman, Cowen, Work, and Kerley (1993) were
able to demonstrate the medium to nearly strong protective effects
of early positive future expectations on outcomes as diverse as
depression (Cohen’s d=.5795), competence (.7949), locus of con-
trol (.4344), reading achievement (.7029), school engagement
(.5557), and socioemotional adjustment (.5083). (nb. Wyman et al.
reported Fstatistics; the effect size calculations were performed by
this author using the statistics in their article.)
Finally, it should be realized that, ultimately, resilience is not a
guaranteed outcome even with the most carefully designed and
thoroughly researched programs. At best, research can inform the
design of interventions; interventions can make attempts at
changing human behavior and thinking. But surprises are still
likely to be found. Occasionally, children will still sink in the
despair of the hostile environment, whereas other children, who
perhaps have even fewer resources, might survive and ultimately
thrive. The imperative remains nonetheless, but it is always good
to be cognizant of the vagaries and difficulties involved with try-
ing to develop rich and full lives in the midst of harsh and toxic
environmental stressors (Garbarino, 1995; Janoff-Bulman, 1992).
CONCLUSIONS
A proper understanding of risk and resilience is essential to the
design and implementation of policies and programs that attempt to
redress some of the effects that community violence, family discord
and abuse, and poverty and minority status can have on children.
Present research in the origin and development of resilience should
inform existing knowledge on risk and psychopathology so that one
is not favored or emphasized over the other and therefore lead to
230 URBAN EDUCATION / MAY 2006
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
ill-designed programs. In short, personality characteristics of the
child interact with small and large environments such as family,
friends, school personnel, and the many people in the community
to create the conditions that will either foster or inhibit positive
growth and development. Powerful and promising new statistical
techniques (such as Structural Equation Modeling) allow for the
analysis and study of independent variables in both an interactional
and causal fashion. This means that future studies can proceed
beyond mere correlation (with implied or imagined causation) and
actually consider how one variable relates to another, determine the
direction of that relationship, and demonstrate how other variables
mediate their relationship. Such future studies can greatly aid in
assistive program design in that they can clearly demonstrate the
primacy of certain variables and can explain the nature of their
direct and indirect effects on outcomes of choice.
Another useful study (though one that cannot be performed fre-
quently) is a meta-analysis. The advantage of meta-analysis over a
traditional review of literature such as the present article is that it
takes the statistics generated in the many studies and equates them;
that is, it translates them into a common statistic called an effect
size (such as Cohen’s d). This allows the researcher to cut across
studies and arrive at a summary numerical evaluation. However, the
meta-analysis technique is constrained by the literature itself; arti-
cles not published (because of the bias against publishing non-
significant findings) cannot be meta-analyzed. Thus, meta-analysis,
oddly enough, often provides a crystal clear view of what is perhaps
inherently unrepresentative.
In that vein, all researchers and the research-consuming commu-
nity would best be served by a more liberal publishing policy, one
that permits (or even encourages) publication of studies with non-
significant statistical results, provided that proper adherence to
research design protocols has been maintained. The advantage here
is that what does not work would be as heralded as what does work;
and that seems particularly important, given the costs associated
with the implementation of interventions in schools and districts
with tight budgets.
Efforts to build resilience in school children, although admirable,
are certainly faced with large difficulties. Regardless of the program
Condly / RESILIENCE IN CHILDREN 231
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
selected, money is and always will be an issue. Assuming that deci-
sion makers select the very best program and have the funds neces-
sary for implementation, there needs to be buy-in from both school
personnel and students; without it, the program will neither be
implemented fully nor received fully. Finally, the very nature of a
program is somewhat at odds with what is known about the nature
of resilience. One of the identified three main characteristics of
resilience is high intelligence and an even temperament. All argu-
ment aside on whether such constructs can be influenced positively
by external forces, it is safe to say that it is very difficult to do so.
Because these individual characteristics seem to be essential, if it
turns out that they are mostly genetic in origin and resistant to inter-
vention, then even the best efforts will succeed only with a minority
of students. Such an acknowledgement is hardly defeatist; after all,
AIDS research progresses apace, even though the disease remains
strongly resistant to attempted cures. It seems certain that, although
no one single program or policy is likely to fit every situation or
person, enough is known to allow for some degree of tailoring that
can allow for the maximum probability of success.
REFERENCES
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. American
Psychologist, 54(7), 462-479.
Baumrind, D. (1993). The average expectable environment is not good enough: A response
to Scarr. Child Development, 64(5), 1299-1317.
Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1980). The role of ego-control and ego-resiliency in the organi-
zation of behavior. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), Minnesota symposia on child psychology:
Development of cognition, affect, and social relations (Vol. 13, pp. 39-101). Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Block, J., & Kremen, A. M. (1996). IQ and ego-resiliency: Conceptual and empirical con-
nections and separateness. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 70(2), 349-361.
Carro, M. G., Grant, K. E., Gotlib, I. H., & Compas, B. E. (1993). Postpartum depression
and child development: An investigation of mothers and fathers as sources of risk and
resilience. Development and Psychopathology, 5(4), 567-579.
Cederblad, M., Dahlin, L., Hagnell, O., & Hansson, K. (1995). Intelligence and tempera-
ment as protective factors for mental: A cross-sectional and prospective epidemiolog-
ical study. European Archives of Psychiatry & Clinical Neuroscience, 245(1), 11-19.
Cicchetti, D., & Garmezy, N. (1993). Prospects and promises in the study of resilience.
Development and Psychopathology, 5(4), 497-502.
232 URBAN EDUCATION / MAY 2006
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1997). The role of self-organization in the promotion of
resilience in maltreated children. Development & Psychopathology, 9(4), 797-815.
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., Lynch, M., & Holt, K. D. (1993). Resilience in maltreated
children: Processes leading to adaptive outcome. Development and Psychopathology,
5(4), 629-647.
Conrad, M., & Hammen, C. (1993). Protective and resource factors in high- and low-risk
children: A comparison of children with unipolar, bipolar, medically ill, and normal
mothers. Development and Psychopathology, 5(4), 593-607.
Consortium on the School-Based Promotion of Social Competence. (1996). The school-based
promotion of social competence: Theory, research, practice, and policy. In R. J. Haggerty,
L. R. Sherrod, N. Garmezy, & M. Rutter (Eds.), Stress, risk, and resilience in children and
adolescents: Processes, mechanisms, and interventions (pp. 268-316). Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Cowen, E. L., Wyman, P. A., Work, W. C., & Iker, M. R. (1995). A preventive intervention
for enhancing resilience among highly stressed urban children. Journal of Primary
Prevention, 15(3), 247-260.
Cowen, E. L., Wyman, P. A., Work, W. C., & Parker, G. R. (1990). The Rochester child
resilience project: Overview and summary of first year findings. Development and
Psychopathology, 2(2), 193-212.
Egeland, B., Carlson, E., & Sroufe, L. A. (1993). Resilience as process. Development and
Psychopathology, 5(4), 517-528.
Ekman, P., & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.). (1994). The nature of emotion: Fundamental ques-
tions. New York: Oxford University Press.
Eysenck, H. (1998). Dimensions of personality. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Books.
Finn, J. D., & Rock, D. A. (1997). Academic success among students at risk for school fail-
ure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82(2), 221-234.
Garbarino, J. (1995). Raising children in a socially toxic environment. San Francisco:
Jossey-Bass.
Garbarino, J., Dubrow, N., Kostelny, K., & Pardo, C. (1992). Children in danger: Coping
with the consequences of community violence. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Garmezy, N. (1971). Vulnerability research and the issue of primary prevention. American
Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 41(1), 101-116.
Garmezy, N. (1991). Resilience in children’s adaptation to negative life events and stressed
environments. Pediatric Annals, 20(9), 459-466.
Garmezy, N. (1996). Reflections and commentary on risk, resilience, and development.
In R. J. Haggerty, L. R. Sherrod, N. Garmezy, & M. Rutter (Eds.), Stress, risk, and
resilience in children and adolescents: Processes, mechanisms, and interventions
(pp. 1-18). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Gordon, K. A. (1996). Resilient Hispanic youths’ self-concept and motivational patterns.
Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 18(1), 63-73.
Gribble, P. A., Cowen, E. L., Wyman, P. A., Wyman, P. A., Work, W. C., Wannon, M.,
et al. (1993). Parent and child views of parent-child relationship qualities and resilient
outcomes among urban children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34(4),
507-519.
Hirschi, T., & Hindelang, M. J. (1977). Intelligence and delinquency: A revisionist review.
American Sociological Review, 42(4), 571-587.
Hoekelman, R. A. (1991). A pediatrician’s view. Pediatric Annals, 20(9), 455-456.
Condly / RESILIENCE IN CHILDREN 233
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Janoff-Bulman, R. (1992). Shattered assumptions: Towards a new psychology of trauma.
New York: Free Press.
Jensen, A. R. (1998). The gfactor: The science of mental ability. New York: Praeger.
Kandel, E., Mednick, S. A., Kirkegaard-Sorensen, L., Hutchings, B., Knop, J., Rosenberg, R.,
et al. (1988). IQ as a protective factor for subjects at high risk for antisocial behavior.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(2), 224-226.
Katz, M. (1997). On playing a poor hand well: Insights from the lives of those who have
overcome childhood risks and adversities. New York: Norton.
Kauffman, C., Grunebaum, H., Cohler, B., & Gamer, E. (1979). Superkids: Competent
children of psychotic mothers. American Journal of Psychiatry, 136(1), 1398-1402.
LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional
life. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Leonard, B. J. (1991). Siblings of chronically ill children: A question of vulnerability versus
resilience. Pediatric Annals, 20(9), 501-506.
Luthar, S. S. (1991). Vulnerability and resilience: A study of high risk adolescents. Child
Development, 62(3), 600-616.
Luthar, S. S. (1993). Annotation: Methodological and conceptual issues in research on
childhood resilience. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34(4), 441-453.
Luthar, S. S., Doernberger, C. H., & Zigler, E. (1993). Resilience is not a unidimensional
construct: Insights from a prospective study of inner-city adolescents. Development
and Psychopathology, 5(4), 703-717.
Luthar, S. S., & Zigler, E. (1991). Vulnerability and competence: A review of research on
resilience in childhood. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 61(1), 6-22.
Lynam, D., Moffitt, T. E., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1993). Explaining the relation
between IQ and delinquency: Class, race, test motivation, school failure, or self-
control? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102(4), 187-196.
Masten, A. S., & Coatsworth, J. D. (1998). The development of competence in favorable
and unfavorable environments. American Psychologist, 53(2), 205-220.
Masten, A. S., Coatsworth, J. D., Neemann, J., Gest, S.D., Tellegen, A., & Garmezy, N.
(1995). The structure and coherence of competence from childhood through adoles-
cence. Child Development, 66(6), 1635-1659.
Masten, A. S., Garmezy, N., Tellegen, A., Pellegrini, D. S., Larkin, K., & Larsen, A.
(1988). Competence and stress in school children: The moderating effects of individ-
ual and family qualities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 29(6), 745-764.
Milgram, N. A., & Palti, G. (1993). Psychosocial characteristics of resilient children.
Journal of Research in Personality, 27(3), 207-221.
Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1988). IQ and delinquency: A direct test of the differential
detection hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97(3), 330-333.
Patterson, J. M. (1991). Family resilience to the challenge of a child’s disability. Pediatric
Annals, 20(9), 491-499.
Pintrich, P. R., & Schunk, D. H. (2002). Motivation in education: Theory, research, and
applications (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson.
Pless, I. B., & Stein, R. E. K. (1996). Intervention research: Lessons from research on children
with chronic disorders. In R. J. Haggerty, L. R. Sherrod, N. Garmezy, & M. Rutter (Eds.),
Stress, risk, and resilience in children and adolescents: Processes, mechanisms, and inter-
ventions (pp. 317-353). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Plomin, R. (1989). Environment and genes: Determinants of behavior. American Psychol-
ogist, 44(2), 105-111.
234 URBAN EDUCATION / MAY 2006
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Price, R. H., Cowen, E. L., Lorion, R. P., & Ramos-McKay, J. (Eds.). (1988). 14 ounces
of prevention: A casebook for practitioners. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Quinton, D., Pickles, A., Maughan, B., & Rutter, M. (1993). Partners, peers, and pathways:
Assortative pairing and continuities in conduct disorder. Development and Psycho-
pathology, 5(4), 763-783.
Radke-Yarrow, M., & Brown, E. (1993). Resilience and vulnerability in children of multiple-
risk families. Development and Psychopathology, 5(4), 581-592.
Rende, R., & Plomin, R. (1993). Families at risk for psychopathology: Who becomes
affected and why? Development and Psychopathology, 5(4), 529-540.
Richters, J. E., & Martinez, P. E. (1993). Violent communities, family choices, and children’s
chances: An algorithm for improving the odds. Development and Psychopathology, 5(4),
609-627.
Ross, S. M., Smith, L. J., Casey, J., & Slavin, R. E. (1995). Increasing the academic success
of disadvantaged children: An examination of alternative early intervention programs.
American Educational Research Journal, 32(4), 773-800.
Rowe, D. C. (1994). The limits of family influence: Genes, experience, and behavior.
New York: Guilford.
Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanism. American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry, 57(3), 316-331.
Rutter, M. (1996). Stress research: Accomplishments and tasks ahead. In R. J. Haggerty,
L. R. Sherrod, N. Garmezy, & M. Rutter (Eds.), Stress, risk, and resilience in children
and adolescents: Processes, mechanisms, and interventions (pp. 354-386). Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge University Press.
Sameroff, A. J., Seifer, R., Barocas, R., Zax, M., & Greenspan, S. (1987). IQ scores of
four-year-old children: Social environmental risk factors. Pediatrics, 79(3), 343-350.
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments:A theory of
genotype environment effects. Child Development, 54(2), 424-435.
Sigman, M. (1995). Nutrition and child development: More food for thought. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 4(2), 52-55.
Sinnema, G. (1991). Resilience among children with special health-care needs and among
their families. Pediatric Annals, 20(9), 483-486.
Smith, J. C. (1995). “More on stress-resilient children”: Reply. Journal of the American
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 34(11), 1403-1404.
Smith, J., & Prior, M. (1995). Temperament and stress resilience in school-age children:
A within-families study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry, 34(2), 168-179.
Staudinger, U. M., Marsiske, M., & Baltes, P. B. (1993). Resilience and levels of reserve
capacity in later adulthood: Perspectives from life-span theory. Development and
Psychopathology, 5(4), 541-566.
Steinberg, L. (1996). Beyond the classroom: Why school reform has failed and what
parents need to do. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Stouthamer-Loeber, M., Loeber, R., Farrington, D. P., Zhang, Q., van Kammen, W., & Maguin, E.
(1993). The double edge of protective and risk factors for delinquency: Interrelations and
developmental patterns. Development and Psychopathology, 5(4), 683-701.
Tolan, P. H., & Dodge, K. A. (2005). Children’s mental health as a primary care and con-
cern: A system for comprehensive support and service. American Psychologist, 60(6),
601-614.
Condly / RESILIENCE IN CHILDREN 235
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Tolan, P. H., Gorman-Smith, D., & Henry, D. (2004). Supporting families in a high-risk
setting: Proximal effects of the SAFE Children preventive intervention. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72(5), 855-869.
Watt, N. F., David, J. P., Ladd, K. L., & Shamos, S. (1995). The life course of psycholog-
ical resilience: A phenomenological perspective on deflecting life’s slings and arrows.
Journal of Primary Prevention, 15(3), 209-246.
Waxman, H. C., Huang, S. L., & Padron, Y. N. (1997). Motivation and learning environ-
ment differences between resilient and nonresilient Latino middle school students.
Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 19(2), 137-155.
Werner, E. E. (1989). High-risk children in young adulthood: A longitudinal study from
birth to 32 years. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 59(1), 72-81.
Werner, E. E. (1993). Risk, resilience, and recovery: Perspectives from the Kauai longitu-
dinal study. Development and Psychopathology, 5(4), 502-515.
Werner, E. E. (1995). Resilience in development. Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 4(3), 81-85.
Werner, E. E., & Smith, R. S. (1992). Overcoming the odds: High risk children from birth
to adulthood. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
White, J. L., Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1989). A prospective replication of the protec-
tive effects of IQ in subjects at high risk for juvenile delinquency. Journal of Clinical
and Consulting Psychology, 57(6), 719-724.
Winfield, L. F. (1994). Developing resilience in urban youth (Urban Monograph Series).
Oakbrook, IL: North Central Regional Educational Laboratory.
Wolff, S. (1995). The concept of resilience. Australian & New Zealand Journal of
Psychiatry, 29(4), 565-574.
Work, W. C., Cowen, E. L., Parker, G. R., & Wyman, P. A. (1990). Stress resilient children
in an urban setting. Journal of Primary Prevention, 11(1), 3-17.
Wyman, P. A., Cowen, E. L., Work, W. C., & Kerley, J. H. (1993). The role of children’s
future expectations in self-system functioning and adjustment to life stress: A prospec-
tive study of urban at-risk children. Development and Psychopathology, 5(4), 649-661.
Wyman, P. A., Cowen, E. L., Work, W. C., & Parker, G. R. (1991). Developmental and
family milieu correlates of resilience in urban children who have experienced major
life-stress. American Journal of Community Psychology, 19(3), 405-426.
Wyman, P. A., Cowen, E. L., Work, W. C., Raoof, B. A., Gribble, P. A., Parker, G. R., et al.
(1992). Interviews with children who experienced major life stress: Family and child
attributes that predict resilient outcomes. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 31(5), 904-910.
Zimrin, H. (1986). A profile of survival. Child Abuse and Neglect, 10(3), 339-349.
Steven J. Condly, PhD, is an assistant professor of educational psychology at the
University of Central Florida in Orlando. His research interest is in the application
of cognitive psychology to developing and enhancing human motivation and learn-
ing in classrooms and workplace settings.
236 URBAN EDUCATION / MAY 2006
by guest on February 19, 2013uex.sagepub.comDownloaded from
... Drawing on Bronfenbrenner's (1979) ecological systems theory, the program targets student needs across levels by considering individual, community, family, and systemic factors. Adopting a multilevel approach has been argued to be critical in moving beyond pathologizing "at-risk" youth in urban contexts and truly supporting their thriving (e.g., Condly, 2006;G. M. Johnson, 1994;Ko et al., 2024). ...
... Last, while some of the evidence is mixed, restorative justice in schools has the potential to address inequities and the school-to-prison pipeline for students with marginalized identities (Morgan, 2021). Many scholars have noted how urban youth, most often from minoritized communities, are framed as problematic individuals with psychological issues, hiding the historical, social, and political dynamics played out in school systems that deepen trauma and pathologize these students (Condly, 2006;Ko et al., 2024). Negative outcomes for these youth can be reframed as inadequately addressing their humanity with trauma based on broader contextual issues (Blitz et al., 2020). ...
... The SC considers the developmental, cultural, and learning differences for students and strives to cultivate resilience so that students can successfully manage the challenges when transitioning back to a traditional educational environment (Knight & Wadhwa, 2014). Importantly, this focus fosters psychosocial coping resources within the individual, including reframing narratives about them to strengths-based perspectives and addressing broader school and relational dynamics (Condly, 2006;Proffitt, 2022). Given the high prevalence of such experiences among MPS students (Schmid, 2017) and the general dearth of these types of support for youth from minoritized backgrounds, including those with This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. ...
Article
Full-text available
Growing evidence over the last few decades has highlighted that punitive approaches in schools perpetuate violence, furthering inequities, deepening issues of safety, and negatively impacting young people’s development. These impacts are particularly felt in underresourced urban schools situated in contexts of community violence. Partially, in response, an increasing number of schools and school districts are engaging in restorative practices. This trauma-informed, strengths-based framework focuses on relationships, healing, and accountability. This article contributes to the growing literature on the potential of restorative justice to promote equity in urban schools and educational systems by outlining the development and model of a restorative alternative to the behavioral reassignment schools’ model that has been used in Milwaukee Public Schools. We describe how this initiative, the Milwaukee Public Schools Success Center (SC) in Milwaukee, Wisconsin, employs a restorative framework to intervene with high-risk students with holistic, supportive, and healing-centered strategies. The model focuses on addressing student needs to move beyond pathologizing “at-risk” youth in urban contexts and truly support their thriving. In the article, we detail the background and conceptualization of the SC, examine how the tenets of restorative philosophy are infused throughout the SC, and illustrate key elements with examples. We situate the SC within the broader literature on ecological systems, development, and peacebuilding. This foundation is operationalized concretely through three main goals across socioecological levels: student empowerment and psychosocial well-being (individual-level), relationship building (interpersonal- and communal-level), and addressing racial gaps and inequities in education (systemic-level). Each of these levels draws on research and theory about building cultures of peace through schools as part of an ecological systems approach. The goals are intentionally not centered on behavior change; instead, they situate behaviors and attitudes that are often seen as problematic within social–emotional dynamics across levels, applying an ecological perspective on risk and resilience for urban students. The article ends with lessons learned that may be applicable in other settings, as well as our next steps in the process of evaluating the efficacy of this initiative. We present this model not as one to be copied directly to other settings but rather to be reimagined within the particular social, cultural, and local ecological contexts to effectively address trauma, distrust, and broken relationships that sustain inequities and cultural and structural violence in urban educational systems. Furthermore, the SC integrates repair and restoration with trauma-informed work to address the psychosocial needs of the urban students and families served in Milwaukee. To this end, it offers an extension for thinking about the role of trauma-informed practices at the intersection of development, peacebuilding, schools, and community violence. The SC and its restorative approach can offer a general framework for policymakers and educators who are interested in a more holistic and therapeutic approach to addressing individuals who are often termed “troubled” or “high-risk” students.
... What was once blind adherence for the greater good, became calculated personal decisions weighing the greater good against social isolation, mental health, and agency [12,13]. The reality for primary caregivers was that coping with the significant shifts and the ongoing uncertainty of the pandemic required tremendous resilience, a vital cognitive and contextual resource that can be used to respond to crisis situations [14][15][16][17][18]. Resilience is a dynamic process in which both psychosocial and environmental factors interact to enable individuals and family units to survive and even thrive when exposed to adversity [16,19]. ...
... The reality for primary caregivers was that coping with the significant shifts and the ongoing uncertainty of the pandemic required tremendous resilience, a vital cognitive and contextual resource that can be used to respond to crisis situations [14][15][16][17][18]. Resilience is a dynamic process in which both psychosocial and environmental factors interact to enable individuals and family units to survive and even thrive when exposed to adversity [16,19]. Through this lens, resilience is positioned as a contextual factor and a personal resource that has transformative potential and can be called forth in times of adversity, depending on both internal and external factors [14][15][16][17][18]. High levels of resilience have been linked to higher psychological well-being, greater life satisfaction, and better quality of life [20,21]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The COVID-19 pandemic has ushered in profound educational, occupational, and familial challenges for school-aged children and their primary caregivers. During this transitional period, resilience was often featured in pandemic discourse as a solution to managing these unprecedented shifts. However, very little concrete information was provided as to how caregivers should or could mobilize this oft-cited resource. Our qualitative study was designed to explore how primary caregivers survived or thrived, using resilience as an experiential window through which to better understand how they coped with the complex, unprecedented challenges ushered in by the COVID-19 pandemic. One-hour interviews were conducted with caregivers (n = 22, all but two identified as female). Analysis revealed that primary caregivers understood resilience to be surviving the pandemic and making it through with the fewest scars for both them and their children. Caregivers cultivated resilience through finding joy during the hardships, supporting their children’s adaptations to the changing context, and resisting rules. Surviving the pandemic required resilience, and while this emerged in caregivers’ definitions, their lived experience highlights how joy, supporting adaptations, and resistance were employed to cultivate resilience.
... The sequelae associated with adverse childhood experiences may contribute to a negative developmental trajectory that lasts throughout life (Aldwin and Igarashi, 2012;Kilian et al., 2017;Masten et al., 2004;Meng et al., 2018). Despite the increased risk of psychopathology, research indicates that some individuals with a history of childhood adversities have better outcomes compared to others with similar histories (Condly, 2006;Khanlou and Wray, 2014;Ungar, 2006Ungar, , 2013Ungar et al., 2013). One factor contributing to better outcomes could be resilience, the attainment of positive adaptation within the context of significant adversity. ...
... External supports were also found to be transient buffers, especially during times of death or loss of a loved one. At the ontogenic level, we note that any type of adversity alters the child's view of the world, their ability to form relationships, and their ability to modulate arousal (Cicchetti and Lynch, 1993;Condly, 2006). Our findings showed that an individual's ability to recognize the various supports available to them, as well as their coping styles, facilitated adaptation in the face of adversity . ...
... As highlighted by Luthar and Brown (2007) resilience is never an "across the board" phenomenon. There are a range of outcomes, from more negative to more positive (Condly, 2006). Thirdly, process-based perspectives focus on the mechanisms involved in navigating adversity and facilitating varying outcomes, emphasizing that these mechanisms manifest dynamically depending on timing and context throughout one's life course (Beeris et al., 2022;Glas et al., 2023;Voie et al., 2024). ...
Article
Full-text available
Older people often face drastic life events, such as spousal loss, that profoundly affect their daily lives. Consequently, resilience—how one navigates life’s changes to avoid further adverse outcomes—is increasingly relevant in ageing studies. Although understanding older adults’ resilience is key to preventing adverse outcomes, the complexity of loss-related events and everyday resilience in later life is underexplored from a process-based perspective. This study employs an Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis (IPA) case study of one individual to investigate resilience in response to spousal loss from a process-based perspective. Four interviews were conducted with this one participant and data was analysed following IPA guidelines. Findings indicate how resilience, in this case, resembles a process of continuous adaptation and renewal or “bouncing forward”, in the face of diverse adversities, as written by Bourbeau. This study enriches our understanding of the process-based perspective on resilience, which is essential for concretely defining resilience and its practical application.
... The stay-at-home orders over a two-and-a-half-year period in Ontario, Canada, destabilized various aspects of people's lives, including their connections with extended family, access to employment, availability of childcare and schools, and leisure activities (Bender et al., 2022;Carli, 2020;Collins et al., 2021;Costa et al., 2022;Lateef et al., 2021;Lyttelton et al., 2022;Petts et al., 2021). Individuals and families across Canada felt compelled and were called to cultivate resilience in response to these significant and ongoing shifts and challenges (Condly, 2006;Garmezy, 1991;Katz, 1997;Prime et al., 2020;Werner, 2014). In Ontario, the government underscored the importance of resilience through its pandemic policies and discussions, much of which revolved around the concept of Building Back Better. ...
Article
Full-text available
When COVID-19 became a global pandemic in March 2020, governments around the world implored citizens to "be resilient" for the greater good. However, very little practical information was provided about how people could mobilize resilience, which is typically presented as an individual-level resource. Our qualitative interpretive description study was designed to explore the structural factors that influenced resilience among primary caregivers of school-aged children during the COVID-19 pandemic in Ontario, Canada. Interviews with 22 caregivers revealed three key structural factors that impacted how they employed resilience during the pandemic: employment, community, and the broader resilience discourse. Together, these results suggest that the structures embedded within society were profoundly impacted by the pandemic, and while this adversity afforded primary care-givers an opportunity to build resilience, ultimately, they often were expected to do so without the supports they previously relied on, including employment and income stability, community supports, and supportive government messaging.
... : employment opportunities and support systems for the whole family (Condly, 2006). ...
Book
Full-text available
This manual is aimed at helping mental health professionals to provide trauma-informed care to children and adolescents who have survived disasters. The interventions described in this manual can be used by counsellors and other mental health professionals (e.g., social workers, psychologists, psychiatrists etc) who have training in mental health. These brief interventions can be applied in any modality ( in-person or remote) It can be accessed at https://ndma.gov.in/sites/default/files/PDF/Technical%20Documents/Child_Trauma_Manual_RAHBAR_TISS.pdf
... Resilience is another moderating variable considered in this study, which refers to a dynamic developmental process of responding more positively than expected after facing risk (Glennie, 2010). It is measured by how well one reacts to a threat using his own abilities and available support systems (Condly, 2006). Pitsoane (2014), in his study discovered that when fathers are emotionally present, the level of resiliency in adolescent increases. ...
Article
Full-text available
Psychological well-being is highly important in the lives of in-school adolescents to function well generally and academically.
Article
Resilience, or the ability to bounce back from hardship, has been studied extensively and identified as a protective factor against negative long-term physical and mental health outcomes. Most research has addressed the child’s resilience in the face of adversity; however, the parent’s capacity for resilience has received limited attention. Parents of gifted children, in particular, face unique stressors that differ from those observed among families of typically developing children; an unanticipated degree of parental resilience is often required in response to the gifted child’s cognitive, social, and emotional needs. In this article, factors that contribute to resilience among gifted children and their parents are reviewed. It is proposed that parental resilience directly affects the child’s well-being and that increased self-awareness enhances the parent’s capacity for greater emotional availability, patience, tolerance, and resilience. In essence, parental resilience is a necessary component for fostering resilience in the child.
Article
Full-text available
There has been a surge in employing artificial intelligence (AI) in all areas of language pedagogy, not the least among them language testing and assessment. This study investigated the effects of AI-powered tools on English as a Foreign Language (EFL) learners’ speaking skills, psychological well-being, autonomy, and academic buoyancy. Using a concurrent mixed-methods design, the study included 28 upper-intermediate EFL students from an Ethiopian university. We gave the Michigan Language Proficiency Test to evaluate degrees of proficiency before the TOEFL iBT speaking section, which used ChatGPT for scoring and feedback. Speaking abilities were assessed using pretests, immediate posttests, and delayed posttests. Furthermore, we evaluated the impacts on psychological well-being, autonomy, and academic buoyancy using narrative frames. We used one-way repeated measurements to examine the quantitative data and thematically evaluated the qualitative data. According to the results, speaking abilities, psychological well-being, learner autonomy, and academic buoyancy showed notable increases. The results suggest that by improving skill development, offering individualized feedback, and meeting students' emotional and psychological needs, AI systems like ChatGPT have the capacity to transform language assessment and pedagogy. Encouraging the incorporation of AI technologies to enhance educational outcomes and provide a more flexible and adaptable learning environment, the study presents important implications for various stakeholders.
Chapter
Students with learning disabilities (LD) often appear to have low academic performance, experience difficulties in performing academic tasks and avoid engaging in them, consider their own academic and self-regulation skills as inferior to other students with typical performance, have low self-perception and self-esteem, fewer expectations of future success, and experience more emotional and social difficulties than their typically developing peers. However, there is no shortage of studies that characterize the developmental course of many individuals with LD as something that exceeds all expectations. Though recent theories of psychological resilience provide a framework for understanding the complex factors that influence adaptation and general wellbeing within this framework, it is important to look into the individual protective factors in building psychological resilience and psychological well-being in children with learning difficulties. The family and school environment are important factors that contribute to young people's psychosocial development.
Article
Full-text available
An inverse relation between IQ and delinquency has been well established, but the direction of effect remains to be specified. Differing explanatory accounts of the relation were empirically examined in the present study using data on 13-year-old boys involved in a high-risk longitudinal study. Accounts that interpreted the relation as spurious or that posited that delinquency-related factors lead to low IQ scores received no support; findings were most consistent with the hypothesis that the direction of effect runs from low IQ to delinquency. The IQ–delinquency relation was robust after race, class, and observed test motivation were controlled statistically. Additionally, the effect of IQ was mediated by school performance for Black youth but not for White youth.
Article
Full-text available
Recent behavioral genetic research has demonstrated that genetic influence on individual differences in behavioral development is usually significant and often substantial and, paradoxically, also supports the important role of the environment. This article reviews research on the heritability of intellectual factors, personality factors, and psychopathology. It discusses the importance of investigating within-family environmental differences in order to understand the environmental origins of individual differences in development.
Article
Full-text available
A sample of 1,803 minority students from low-income homes was classified into 3 groups on the basis of grades, test scores, and persistence from Grade 8 through Grade 12; the classifications were academically successful school completers (''resilient'' students), school completers with poorer academic performance (nonresilient completers), and noncompleters (dropouts). Groups were compared in terms of psychological characteristics and measures of ''school engagement.'' Large, significant differences were found among groups on engagement behaviors, even after background and psychological characteristics were controlled statistically The findings support the hypothesis that student engagement is an important component of academic resilience. Furthermore, they provide information for designing interventions to improve the educational prognoses of students at risk.
Article
Full-text available
The constructs of intelligence and ego-resiliency are discussed. The personality implications of “pure intelligence” and “pure ego-resilience” were identified. Intelligence (IQ) was indexed by the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised and ego-resiliency by an inventory scale. Residual scores measuring “pure intelligence” and “pure ego-resilience” were correlated with the items of the observer-based California Q-sort, used to describe participants. Persons relatively high on ego-resilience tend to be more competent and comfortable in the “fuzzier” interpersonal world; persons defined primarily by raw IQ tend to be effective in the “clearer” world of structured work but tend also to be uneasy with affect and less able to realize satisfying human connections. Gender differences exist in the relations of ego-resilience and intelligence and in their adaptive relevance.
Article
In recent years, two programs have gained national attention for producing significant and sustained effects on the reading performance of at-risk children. One is Reading Recovery which uses highly trained teachers and a systematic set of procedures to tutor first graders who are having difficulty learning to read. The other is Success for All which uses research-based beginning and intermediate reading programs, one-to-one tutoring, family support, and other elements to provide support for all students in the targeted elementary school. The present research examined the processes and outcomes associated with the implementation of these approaches in comparable first grade classes during one school year. Results showed that Reading Recovery strongly benefited tutored students, particularly on passage comprehension. Success for All was more beneficial for special education students and for students who were not tutored, especially on word attack measures. School climate and teacher attitude results also showed advantages of Success for All’s comprehensive approach in integrating the reading curriculum with family support and schoolwide restructuring.
Article
What does it mean to be a child today? Are we doing enough to protect our children - to grant them the right to life, liberty, and the pursuit of happiness?
Chapter
There are questions remaining concerning the topics previously tackled and this has been expounded by the Dalai Lama, translated into a comprehensive form by renowned translators. The issues are: the reasons why biobehvioral sciences neglected compassion, how neutral science is perceived to be, whether compassion is innate in human nature, whether we are naturally competitive or cooperative, where this compassion comes from and whether this compassion is fragile, and what the impact of compassion to happiness is and vice versa. It is generally viewed that compassion and science are in a way very contrasting forms, but the Dalai Lama believes that science will continue to evolve and will meet with compassion. He believes that human nature will take the upper hand at any time.
Article
Remedies to Enhance Resilience