ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

The voices and visions experienced by Emanuel Swedenborg remain a topic of much debate. The present article offers a reconsideration of these experiences in relation to changes in psychiatric practice. First, the phenomenology of Swedenborg's experiences is reviewed through an examination of his writings. The varying conceptualizations of these experiences by Swedenborg and his contemporaries, and by psychiatrists of later generations, are examined. We show how attempts by 19th- and 20th-century psychiatrists to explain Swedenborg's condition as the result of either schizophrenia or epilepsy are unable to account for his experiences. We then demonstrate that the re-emergence of the 19th-century concept of `hallucinations in the sane' offers an alternative way to understand Swedenborg's experiences outside typical discourses of mental illness. Finally we argue that Swedenborg's experiences should be understood as exemplifying phenomena which we term `hallucinations without mental disorder', and investigate how conceiving of Swedenborg in this way can inform future research into the experience and clinical significance of hallucinations.
http://hhs.sagepub.com
History of the Human Sciences
DOI: 10.1177/0952695107086138
2008; 21; 1 History of the Human Sciences
Simon R. Jones and Charles Fernyhough
Swedenborg
Talking back to the spirits: the voices and visions of Emanuel
http://hhs.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/21/1/1
The online version of this article can be found at:
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
can be found at:History of the Human Sciences Additional services and information for
http://hhs.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Email Alerts:
http://hhs.sagepub.com/subscriptions Subscriptions:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:
http://hhs.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/21/1/1
SAGE Journals Online and HighWire Press platforms):
(this article cites 44 articles hosted on the Citations
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Talking back to the spirits: the
voices and visions of Emanuel
Swedenborg
SIMON R. JONES and CHARLES FERNYHOUGH
ABSTRACT
The voices and visions experienced by Emanuel Swedenborg remain a
topic of much debate. The present article offers a reconsideration of
these experiences in relation to changes in psychiatric practice. First,
the phenomenology of Swedenborg’s experiences is reviewed through
an examination of his writings. The varying conceptualizations of these
experiences by Swedenborg and his contemporaries, and by psychiatrists
of later generations, are examined. We show how attempts by 19th- and
20th-century psychiatrists to explain Swedenborg’s condition as the
result of either schizophrenia or epilepsy are unable to account for his
experiences. We then demonstrate that the re-emergence of the 19th-
century concept of ‘hallucinations in the sane’ offers an alternative way
to understand Swedenborg’s experiences outside typical discourses of
mental illness. Finally we argue that Swedenborg’s experiences should
be understood as exemplifying phenomena which we term ‘halluci-
nations without mental disorder’, and investigate how conceiving of
Swedenborg in this way can inform future research into the experience
and clinical significance of hallucinations.
Key words epilepsy, hallucinations, history, schizophrenia,
Emanuel Swedenborg
HISTORY OF THE HUMAN SCIENCES Vol. 21 No. 1
© 2008 SAGE Publications (Los Angeles, London, New Delhi and Singapore) pp. 1–31
[21:1; 1–31; DOI: 10.1177/0952695107086138]
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
INTRODUCTION
Throughout history, individuals as diverse as Pythagoras, Socrates, Galileo,
Joan of Arc, Luther and Pascal have had the experience of hearing voices or
seeing visions imperceptible to others (Leudar and Thomas, 2000). There is
a long history of attempting to account for such experiences reported by such
prominent historical figures using concepts from contemporary psychiatry
such as hallucinations (T. James, 1995), today defined as percept-like experi-
ences occurring in the absence of appropriate stimuli with the full force of
actual perceptions (Slade and Bentall, 1988).
One danger inherent in such attempts at retrospective analysis is that it
assumes it is possible to detach our concept of a hallucination from the
contemporary conditions under which it has been formulated, and to apply
it to cases that antedate its conceptualization which have not previously been
identified in these terms (Leudar and Sharrock, 2003). Many would be skep-
tical about such an approach (e.g. Hacking, 2003; Young, 1995). One such
reason for such skepticism flows from the observation that, while diagnosis
usually requires the engagement of, and has performative consequences for,
the individual, this is not the case in the analysis of historical figures (Leudar
and Sharrock, 2003). Such an approach is also open to the accusation that it
treats the concept of the hallucination as having an objective ontological
existence, being ‘out there, independently of the psychiatric discourses and
practices that attempt to define and to treat it’ (Borch-Jacobsen, 2001: 20).
Borch-Jacobsen has proposed that instead the ‘history of psychiatry and/or
madness should ideally be the history of those complex interactions that give
rise, through feedback, amplification and crystallization, to new psychiatric
concepts’ (ibid.: 28).
The present article aims to demonstrate how the changing conception of
the hallucination has been, and may be, applied to the experiences of Emanuel
Swedenborg. We attempt to show the process through which ‘a spiritual
visionary becomes a madman’ (Leudar and Sharrock, 2002: 249), and con-
versely, how a madman may become a spiritual visionary again. After a
review of the phenomenology of Swedenborg’s experiences, we examine how
these experiences were understood by Swedenborg himself as well as by his
close contemporaries. Next, we show how the advent of medical psychiatry
in the 19th century, and the concepts it generated, led to a reinterpretation
of Swedenborg’s experiences as pathological and symptoms of diseases such
as schizophrenia and epilepsy. We review these accounts and demonstrate
that medico-psychiatric accounts have not yet been able to offer a satisfac-
tory explanation for Swedenborg’s experiences. We then discuss how the re-
emergence of the discourse of ‘hallucinations of the sane’, according to which
such experiences are not necessarily symptoms of an illness, offers a further
shift in the way Swedenborg can be understood. Specifically, we show that
HISTORY OF THE HUMAN SCIENCES 21(1)2
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Swedenborg’s experiences can be understood as exemplifying phenomena
which we term ‘hallucinations without mental disorder’. Finally, we draw
implications for future research into hallucinations.
WHO WAS EMANUEL SWEDENBORG?
The experiences of the Swedish scientist, inventor, mystic and theologian
Emanuel Swedenborg (1688–1772) have been a topic of much debate.
Swedenborg’s copious and lively writings on his anomalous experiences,
coupled with the fact that they emerged relatively abruptly in the middle of
his life, mean that his work presents a particularly attractive example for
historical analysis.
Swedenborg was born into a nation culturally divided. Exploitation of
Sweden’s vast mineral wealth had encouraged the development of a hard-
headed industrial and scientific quarter. However, in the Swedish country-
side, where the ‘supernatural brooded over the landscape and in the hearts
of men’ (Williams-Hogan, 1988: 4), an older and more spiritual outlook
remained. Swedenborg’s life can be seen to encompass both of these conflict-
ing perspectives. Born to the future Lutheran bishop of Skara, his maternal
grandfather was an assessor in the Royal College of Mines. Swedenborg’s
religious inclinations were present from an early age. As a child Swedenborg
claimed his meditations on God ‘revealed things at which my father and
mother wondered; saying that angels must be speaking through me’
(Swedenborg, cited in Swift, 1883: 3). In 1696, at the age of 8, Swedenborg
lost both his mother, Sarah, and his older and first-born brother, Albrecht,
to an epidemic. His father remarried a year later, and relations between
Swedenborg and his new stepmother appear to have been positive (Toksvig,
1948). From adolescence, through to his late 50s, Swedenborg’s life was pre-
dominantly that of a man of science. His interests and works involved him
in physics and chemistry, biology and geology, as well as physiology and
mathematics. His work in what is today recognized as neuroscience has been
acclaimed as being far ahead of its time (Gross, 1997). Swedenborg eventu-
ally settled into a role as an assessor for the Royal College of Mines and in
1747 was recommended for the position of Councillor of Mines, the highest
position a non-noble could hold (Toksvig, 1948).
However, by the mid-1740s Swedenborg had already started moving
away from his work in the natural sciences, towards a more spiritual focus,
occasioned by a number of anomalous experiences. In 1743 he started to
become aware of ‘angelic voices’ (Toksvig, 1948: 153) and by the following
year claimed to have produced writings on the commands of angels (ibid.).
Swedenborg’s desire to devote himself entirely to these spiritual investi-
gations led him to decline the offered position of Councillor of Mines and
TALKING BACK TO THE SPIRITS
3
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
resign his current position (Stanley, 1988). Swedenborg’s subsequent spiri-
tual investigations were not based on a purely philosophical approach, but
on the experiences that he interpreted as sent from God. These took the
form of a range of predominantly visual and auditory experiences, involving
conversations with ‘angels’ and ‘spirits’. As Swedenborg put it ‘I have seen,
I have heard’ (1872[1749–56]: Arcana Coelestia (n6191). Following the onset
of these experiences in the mid-1740s Swedenborg remained functioning at a
sufficiently high level not only to be able to learn Hebrew, but also to publish
a voluminous theological and philosophical biblical literature over a period
of decades. Furthermore, not only was Swedenborg able to continue to make
presentations to the Swedish Diet (the highest authority in the land below the
monarch) on the problems and dangers posed by issues such as inflation, poor
foreign exchange rates, and alcohol, he was also able to do so in a clear and
simple manner (Toksvig, 1948).
WHAT DID SWEDENBORG EXPERIENCE?
Form and content of Swedenborg’s experiences
Swedenborg’s experiences were, in the words of William James (1960[1902]:
460), ‘the palmary one[s] of audita et visa, serving as a basis of religious reve-
lation’. What Swedenborg experienced, how he experienced it, and his expla-
nations of it, may be found in the copious volumes of spiritual writings he
published following the onset of his anomalous experiences in the mid-1740s.
These works include his Spiritual Diary (hereafter cited as SpD; 1746–65),
Arcana Coelestia (hereafter cited as AC; 1749–56), Life on Other Planets
(hereafter cited as LP; 1758), Heaven and Hell (hereafter cited as HH; 1758),
and True Christian Religion (hereafter cited as TCR; 1771). Throughout these
works Swedenborg reiterated how it had been granted him ‘for some years
almost continually to hold discourse with spirits and angels, and to be in their
company as one of themselves’ (AC, n1634). Such experiences continued to
his death in 1772.
As well as spirits and angels, Swedenborg claimed to have been able to
‘speak with those who are in hell’ (HH, n312) as well as almost all of those
‘with whom I was acquainted during their life’ (AC, n1636). Other voices and
visions he experienced were those of historical figures; one was ‘given me to
believe that he was Aristotle’ (AC, n4658). Yet further voices and visions
Swedenborg experienced were understood by him to be from inhabitants of
other planets in the solar system: ‘Some spirits approached me, and I was told
from heaven that they were from the world nearest to the sun, the planet
named in our world Mercury’ (LP, n11). The beings Swedenborg encountered
were able to go into great depth about the specifics of life in a spiritual realm.
HISTORY OF THE HUMAN SCIENCES 21(1)4
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
What Swedenborg experienced is substantially easier to determine than how
he experienced it. Swedenborg’s writings show him to have had anomalous
experiences in visual, auditory, olfactory and tactile modalities. A chrono-
logical examination of his experiences gives an indication of the changing
nature of his experiences. The seeds of Swedenborg’s anomalous spiritual
experiences began in 1736 when he was 48 years old (Benz, 2002), occasion-
ing him to start recording them in his Dream Diary (henceforth cited as DD),
which was published posthumously in incomplete form. Up until 1744, his
experiences included dreams, ‘visions when my eyes were closed’, ‘fiery
lights’ and hearing ‘speech in morning time’ (SpD, n2951). The nature of
these experiences changed radically post-1744. On two separate occasions in
April 1744 and April 1745 Swedenborg experienced seeing Jesus and God
respectively, who spoke to him during night-time visions, when it is not clear
whether he was awake, asleep, or somewhere in between. The vision of God
was to be a turning point in Swedenborg’s life, as it was at this moment that
he experienced what he later described as the opening of his ‘interior sight’
(AC, n1619), allowing him to see and hear ‘things of another life, which
cannot possibly be made visible to the bodily sight’ (ibid.).
Following this experience, Swedenborg started to write his first series of
biblical commentaries on the books of the Old Testament, The Word of the
Old Testament Explained (henceforth cited as WE). Much of this, Sweden-
borg claimed, was done in the presence of spirits. For example, he explains
that ‘These words are written in the presence of many Jews who are around
me; nor do I doubt that Abraham is also present’ (WE, n5292). His anom-
alous experiences at this time appear to have been predominantly auditory,
with visual experiences being the exception rather than the rule (e.g. WE,
n1511). Much of what Swedenborg wrote in this work was copied down as
the voices dictated to him ‘viva voce’ (WE, n1150, n1511). Swedenborg vigor-
ously denied that that these voices were not real. ‘[I]t is not in the least degree
phantasy’, he wrote, ‘but a continuous speech, as one of man with another
. . . and this now for fifteen months’ (WE, n5292). As well as being dictated
to, he also noted that ‘I have written entire pages, and the spirits did not
dictate the words but absolutely guided my hand, so that it was they who
were doing the writing’ (WE, n1150). In these passages in which Swedenborg
claims his hand is being guided, his handwriting differs from his normal hand,
having an angular, slashing style (Toksvig, 1948). At this period of life some
of his other movements also appear to have been experienced as controlled
by spirits, ‘They so ruled my actions that I went whithersoever they desired
. . . they ruled my steps’ (WE, n1149).
Swedenborg’s frequency of visual experiences increased gradually over
time. As Toksvig (1948: 217) puts it, ‘He could “see” more and more as well
as “hear”’. Swedenborg’s next set of biblical commentaries, published as
Arcana Coelestia, were not written in the form of automatic writing but were
TALKING BACK TO THE SPIRITS
5
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
reflections on what he had seen and heard. By 1758 Swedenborg had discerned
at least four distinct ways in which he experienced his voices and visions,
although he also claims ‘there are a great many other kinds of vision which
can never be described’ (WE, n7387). The first clearly identified state when
visions and voices would occur corresponds to states of consciousness associ-
ated with the transition from wakefulness to sleep, and vice versa, today
referred to as hypnagogic and hypnopompic states respectively (Vaitl et al.,
2005). Swedenborg referred to this state as being ‘withdrawn from the body,
and not knowing whether one is in the body or out of it’ (HH, n440), but
noted that he had been in this state ‘only three or four times’ (ibid.). The
second class of experiences occurred when Swedenborg was walking through
the streets, yet subjectively perceived himself to be walking through groves,
rivers and palaces, conversing with spirits. Again, Swedenborg reports that
he only found himself in this state two or three times. Both these first two
states were ‘extraordinary’ (HH, n442) and, Swedenborg believed, were
shown to him ‘only that I might know what they are’ (ibid.). The third
category occurred in ‘full wakefulness, with the eyes closed’ in which ‘things
are seen as though in clear day’ (WE, n7387). The final class of experiences,
‘to speak with spirits, and to be with them as one of them’ was experienced
by Swedenborg ‘in full wakefulness of body’ and was experienced over a
period of many years. This appears to have been the most frequent form of
Swedenborg’s experiences, occurring on a day-to-day basis. He referred to
this as being ‘in the spirit’ (e.g. TCR, n76).
When the spirits spoke to Swedenborg he typically heard them ‘as sonor-
ously as the speech of man’ (AC, n4652), although in other circumstances the
voices were less distinct and would ‘mutter’ (AC, n4657). The muttering
voices appeared to be associated with a negative content, typically being ‘of
such a nature, that they observe the faults and failings of others . . . they see
and interpret all things unfavourably, and prefer themselves to others’ (ibid.).
Yet other voices spoke ‘by a gurgling of words as if from the belly’ (AC,
n1763). Although Swedenborg typically appears to have heard only one voice
speaking at a time, he does also report sometimes experiencing ‘a simultane-
ous speech of many spirits together’ (AC, n1763).
Swedenborg did not only experience his voices and visions when he was
alone; he noted that he had
. . . discoursed with them whilst I was also in company with men [and]
I also observed, that as I heard the sound of a man’s voice in discourse,
so I heard also that of spirits, each alike sonorous. (AC, n1635)
Swedenborg also, intriguingly, notes that his voices themselves also thought
other people could hear them:
. . . [a]t times, when spirits have spoken with me in the midst of the
company of men, some of them have supposed, because their speech
HISTORY OF THE HUMAN SCIENCES 21(1)6
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
was heard so sonorously, that they would be heard also by those that
were there present. (AC, n4652)
As well as speaking in complete formal language, Swedenborg also notes that
the spirits were able to speak in the language of thought. For example, he
states that ‘speech consisting of ideas of thought is the speech proper to
spirits’ (AC, n1639). Swedenborg claims to have often spoken with spirits ‘in
their proper tongue, that is, by ideas of thought’ (ibid.).
From the 1760s through to his death in 1772, Swedenborg’s visions typi-
cally took one of two forms (Benz, 2002). In the first he experienced a vision
‘in a state of complete absorption with closed eyes’ (ibid.: 280). In the second
he was still aware of his surroundings but ‘his spirit was directed towards
persons and phenomena of the spiritual world, which became partially visible
in the context of earthly reality and seemed to intrude into his sensory world
of experience’ (ibid: 279). Benz (2002) identifies the following typical example
of this second type of vision: Swedenborg is meditating on a problem, an
‘angel’ appears in a specific relation to him (e.g. above and to the left, or to
his right). Swedenborg then heads into the spirit world, has a long and
didactic conversation, then returns home. For example, Swedenborg reported
the experience of being ‘engaged in meditation upon the creation of the
universe’ when an angel ‘above me on the right’ noticed this, ‘descended,
and invited me up to them’ (TCR, n76). Swedenborg claims he was then
conducted to the palace of a prince where there ensued a long discussion with
its occupants, after which ‘the angel, who had introduced me, accompanied
me to the house where he found me, and from thence re-ascended to his own
society’ (TCR, n76). In contrast to his earlier experiences in the 1740s when
he wrote down what the spirits said to him as they spoke, or recorded their
message via automatic writing, in the 1770s Swedenborg generally returned
from his spiritual state into his physical state and only then wrote down the
things he had heard and seen (Benz, 2002).
In these later visions there still remains some phenomenological hetero-
geneity. Some visions involve the spiritual figure appearing in the real world,
although these are rare (Benz, 2002). For example, Swedenborg reports an
experience where
A single satan was once permitted to ascend out of hell, together with
a woman, and come to the house where I was. As soon as I perceived
them I shut the window, but entered into conversation with them
through it. (TCR, n80)
Finally, it is worth noting a specific physical state that Swedenborg observed
to be associated with his anomalous experiences. Swedenborg noted that ever
since his childhood he had been able to breathe in a ‘scarcely perceptible’ way
(Benz, 2002: 158). This breathing technique appears to have consisted of
suppression of his breathing in order to focus thought (ibid.). After his
TALKING BACK TO THE SPIRITS
7
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
anomalous experiences had been occurring for some years Swedenborg made
the direct link between his controlled breathing and his spiritual experiences:
claiming that ‘my respiration was so formed by the Lord that I could respire
inwardly for a considerable time, without the aid of external air . . . in order
that I may be with spirits and speak with them’ (SpD, n3317). He states that
‘when heaven was opened to me, and I was enabled to converse with spirits,
I sometimes scarcely breathed’ (SpD, n3464). Benz (2002: 159) concludes
that ‘one cannot dismiss the possibility that Swedenborg’s varied visionary
experiences . . . are related to this phenomenon’.
Veracity of Swedenborg’s reports
Before examining in depth how Swedenborg’s experiences have been under-
stood it must be established that Swedenborg did actually experience what
he claimed (Talbot, 1998). In this respect we are concerned to establish, not
that Swedenborg heard and saw spiritual beings with independent ontologi-
cal statuses, but that he had experiences of hearing and seeing things that others
could not. It is not our intention to address theological issues surrounding
Swedenborg’s experiences. Swedenborg himself was acutely aware of the
skepticism that would meet his claims, writing that he was ‘well aware that
many will say that no one can possibly speak with spirits and angels . . . and
many will say that it is all fancy, others that I relate such things in order to
gain credence, and others will make other objections. But by all this I am not
deterred, for I have seen, I have heard, I have felt’ (AC, n68).
In support of Swedenborg’s accounts there is first the testimony of his peers.
Talbot (1998) cites a Mr Shearsmith who shared a house with Swedenborg.
Shearsmith states that:
. . . what he [Swedenborg] saw was in a wakeful state, as he generally
stood between the bed and front room when conversing in the day with
spirits or those who were invisible to others; which conversations
would often also be held in the night, or towards 2 and 3 o’clock in the
morning, and would last for an hour or more, he often appearing to be
in a kind of conflict, and saying, Nay! nay! nay! often, and sometimes
loud; but when it met his approbation, Yea! yea! was pronounced, and
more often.
Swift (1883) also put forward a number of reasons why it is unlikely that
Swedenborg was simply fabricating his experiences. First, Swift noted that it
is implausible that an eminent mathematician and philosopher known for
his truth and integrity should ‘voluntarily submit for nearly thirty years to
a life of self denial and obscurity, merely for the purpose of deluding
mankind’ (ibid.: 69). Second, Swift noted that Swedenborg’s claims were
‘never associated with anything of self-ostentation or self-commendation’
HISTORY OF THE HUMAN SCIENCES 21(1)8
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
(ibid.). Indeed, as noted above, Swedenborg had rejected a prestigious
employment offer in order to pursue his spiritual investigations.
Finally, as Benz (2002) notes, only rarely did Swedenborg experience a
traditional Christian vision where he saw events before his spiritual eye, and
then received an explanation of them. In this way Swedenborg distinguishes
himself from all earlier Christian visionaries in the ‘preponderance of didac-
ticism over imagery’ (ibid.: 315) in his visions. If Swedenborg had fabricated
such experiences it would have been more convincing to his peers if they had
conformed to the traditional form of Christian visions. If, as seems probable,
Swedenborg was reporting bona fide experiences, this still leaves the question
of whether he embellished his reports. A review of his writings shows his
voices and visions increasing in complexity, richness and colour over the
course of his life. However, the content and relation of Swedenborg to the
spirits remained broadly consistent. It hence appears likely that the visions
and voices experienced did genuinely evolve over Swedenborg’s lifetime.
Another potential issue is that later in life Swedenborg generally wrote down
his experiences after they had occurred, leaving open the possibility of
memory errors and distortions. Where Swedenborg appears unreliable is not
so much in what he reports seeing or hearing, as in his explanations for his
experiences. For example, Swedenborg variously claims that pains he experi-
ences in his head (AC, n5180) or stomach (AC, n5723) were caused by evil
spirits. Such explanations could be understood as delusional elaborations
(though the pains themselves were probably very real). Ultimately, the veracity
and accuracy of Swedenborg’s reports can never be conclusively determined.
However, we conclude that it is fair to assume that his experiences were not
fabricated or overly embellished.
HOW HAVE SWEDENBORG’S EXPERIENCES BEEN
UNDERSTOOD?
In western Europe, between around
AD 500 and AD 1500, individuals who
reported hearing voices or seeing visions were not typically considered mad,
but regarded by their peers as having actual perceptual experiences (Kroll and
Bachrach, 1982). Such experiences could be understood as communications
from spiritual beings (God, the Devil, etc.), as being merely imaginations, or
as resulting from illness (Watkins, 1998). As late as the 17th century, the
explanation of hallucinations as religious madness was still common (Porter,
2002). However, by the time of Swedenborg’s experiences in the mid- to late
18th century, opinion was shifting towards a conception of hallucinations as
illness. Works such as William Battie’s Treatise on Madness (1758) were
formative in identifying lunacy with false perceptions such as reported
conversations with angels (Schmidt, 2002). As Schmidt (2002: 191) puts it,
TALKING BACK TO THE SPIRITS
9
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
‘Enlightenment epistemologies . . . demanded the disciplining of religious
enthusiasm, a confinement of those “unguarded fancies of a man’s own
brain” within a secure domain from which reason and the state might avoid
contamination’.
Swedenborg’s own understanding
Swedenborg himself made sense of his experiences through the still access-
ible religious framework. He understood the ‘opening of his inner sight’ as
directly equivalent to the experiences of the Old Testament prophets (AC,
n1532). Before his vision of Christ in 1744, Swedenborg’s scientific back-
ground led him to struggle with the question as to whether figures in the
Bible like Moses did actually experience God directly communicating with
them. He ironically notes that his doubting ‘is the reason that the angels and
God showed themselves to the shepherds and not to philosophers who let
their understanding enter into these matters’ (Swedenborg cited in Benz,
2002: 166). Swedenborg initially struggled with himself to assess whether his
experiences were mere fantasy (i.e. hallucinatory) or genuine experiences. For
example, he writes: ‘What can this be? Is it Christ, God’s son, that I have
seen?’ (DD, n55). However, he came to believe his experiences were genuine
and to ‘obtain faith without reasoning’ (DD, n149). Swedenborg was then
clear to differentiate his experiences from phantasy, where images created in
the imagination appear to come from the external world (Benz, 2002). In
Swedenborg’s words, his visions were not the ‘vision of phantasy which is of
the interior sight; for then, that meets the eye as though from without which
yet is within the natural mind’ (WE, n7386).
In addition to trying to understand the source of his experiences, Sweden-
borg also attempted to address the reasons behind them. Swedenborg supposed
they occurred ‘in order that the man of the Church may not continue any
longer in his erroneous faith as to a resurrection at the day of judgment, and
the state of the soul in the meantime, and also as to angels and the devil’
(HH, n312). This ‘erroneous faith’, Swedenborg claimed, did not allow indi-
viduals to have satisfactory answers to fundamental questions such as ‘How
can bodies eaten up by worms, consumed by corruption and scattered to all
the winds, be gathered together again to their soul?’ (ibid.). These gaps in
humanity’s understanding, he thought, would lead to doubts and denials
about the existence of the soul, heaven and hell, and faith itself.
However, perhaps reflecting his scientific background, Swedenborg had
another discourse available through which to explain the experience. He
writes:
That the speech of an angel or spirit flows down from within even into
the ear, was proved to me from this, that it also flows into the tongue
causing a slight vibration; but not with any motion, such as takes place
HISTORY OF THE HUMAN SCIENCES 21(1)10
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
when the sound of speech is thereby articulated into words by the man
himself. (HH, n248)
In this account Swedenborg was able to attempt to knit together potential
religious and natural science accounts of his experiences. Strikingly, Sweden-
borg concurred with modern researchers in seeing the origin of his voices in
the realm of thought and speech (e.g. Jones and Fernyhough, 2007). Indeed,
his understanding here seems to be as far ahead of its time as his work in
neuroscience has been claimed to be (Gross, 1997). His belief that, what
today are termed auditory verbal hallucinations, may be accompanied by
‘slight vibrations’ in the speech musculature was, two hundred years later,
shown to be the case (e.g. Gould, 1948; Inouye and Shimizu, 1970). Thus,
overall, Swedenborg cannot be adjudged guilty of Francis Bacon’s claim that
we love better to believe than examine.
Although Swedenborg was able to understand his experiences through a
dual religious-natural science framework, he still had to justify this stance
against alternative discourses, particularly that of insanity. Swedenborg
himself defined insanity as involving ‘one who acts contrary to accepted
propriety and the customs of society, or, still more, who obstinately defends
his own opinion against acknowledged truths and the judgement of sound
mind, and . . . who, deranged and empty of mind, exposes himself to public
sport’ (Swedenborg, cited in Toksvig, 1948: 133). Swedenborg did not believe
himself to meet these criteria (ibid.). However, he did appear to have worried
that, on occasions such as when he responded to a spiritual command to wash
his feet, he may have been perceived as insane (ibid.). One source of proof
to himself that he was not insane came from a voice that told him ‘one can
become insane as to the body and yet not as to the mind or thinking’ (SpD,
n2421). The association of madness with irrationality that had developed out
of the work of the 17th-century philosophers such as Locke and Hobbes also
appears to have helped convince Swedenborg that he was not insane. Sweden-
borg’s emphasis that he could ‘enjoy my rational mind, just as if they [the
spirits] were not present’ (SpD, n2659) appears to have been a major factor
in convincing him of his sanity. Others, however, did not concur.
The understanding of Swedenborg’s contemporaries
It appears that Swedenborg himself, as well as a large number of those who
met him, had a more charitable view of his experiences than those of the
contemporary medical and philosophical establishment. However, as noted,
Swedenborg was acutely aware that he may have been thought of as mad,
writing of individuals who ‘persuaded others that I was insane’ (SpD, n2772).
Perceptions of Swedenborg by his contemporaries may be categorized
according to the extent to which the observer was personally familiar with
him. As noted in the introduction, terms such as hallucination and madness
TALKING BACK TO THE SPIRITS
11
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
have a performative aspect. Thus, we should not be surprised that those who
encountered Swedenborg face-to-face should have responded differently to
those who did not.
Swift (1883) reports the testimony of an innkeeper, Mr Bergstrom, who
encountered Swedenborg in the midst of an anomalous experience while
staying at his inn. He found Swedenborg talking to invisible entities with
his hands raised in an agitated state, and could not understand what he was
saying. As Toksvig (1948) notes, this may be explained by the fact that
Swedenborg was speaking in Latin, a language Bergstrom did not know.
Even given these experiences, Bergstrom noted that Swedenborg was ‘very
reasonable’ (Swift, 1883: 174), ‘dressed neatly in velvet, and made a good
appearance’ (ibid.), and that he was a ‘reasonable, sensible, and good man’
(ibid.). Others who met Swedenborg in person, such as the Swedish states-
man, Count Hopken, also appear to have agreed that he was of sound
judgment (Toksvig, 1948).
Many of those who leveled charges of insanity against Swedenborg before
meeting him seemed subsequently to have had second thoughts (ibid.). In
addition to his demonstrated rationality in everyday life, Swedenborg’s high
social standing, combined with his failure overtly to cross the line from
religious into political prophecy (Ingram, 1998), may also have played a role
in his generally sympathetic treatment. Others who did cross into political
prophecy, such as the English preacher Richard Brothers who in 1795
prophesied the fall of the monarchy, were rapidly institutionalized (ibid.).
Those who encountered Swedenborg solely through his writings appear
to have made less charitable judgments. For example, Swedenborg’s contem-
porary, the Swedish poet Johan Henrik Kellgren, wrote in a 1787 poem
entitled ‘You Are Not a Genius even if You Are Mad’ that ‘Swedenborg will
never be anything else than an idiot’ (our translation). Furthermore, in 1770
the early Methodist John Wesley wrote that Swedenborg ‘is one of the most
ingenious, lively, entertaining madmen that ever set pen to paper’ (Wesley,
1986[1770]: 216).
Perhaps the most influential commentator, in terms of cementing a particu-
lar view of Swedenborg, was his contemporary Immanuel Kant. In his 1766
book, Dreams of a Spirit-Seer, Kant (2002[1766]) addressed the experiences of
Swedenborg at the insistence of ‘inquisitive and idle friends’ (ibid.: 56). Kant
distinguished between the raw experiences Swedenborg had, which Kant
termed ‘deludedness of the senses’ (ibid.: 50), and the rationalizations, elab-
orations and interpretations Swedenborg made from these, which he termed
‘deludedness of the reason’ (ibid.). Kant did not believe that Swedenborg had
simply invented his experiences, and found the ‘coherent delusion of the
senses’ to be a ‘remarkable phenomenon’ (ibid.). Despite this measured tone,
certain passages of Kant’s work are more critical of Swedenborg. For example,
Kant states he would not blame the reader if he dismissed characters such as
HISTORY OF THE HUMAN SCIENCES 21(1)12
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Swedenborg as ‘candidates for the hospital’ (ibid.: 35), and at one point also
states that Swedenborg’s works are ‘nonsense’ (ibid.: 49). The recent comment
by Ward (2003: 515) that Swedenborg was ‘a character whom everybody
knew had been rubbished in his lifetime by critics as various as Kant and
John Wesley’ appears to bear out this conclusion. The weight which subse-
quent scholars have attached to certain of Kant’s words may have contributed
to succeeding generations being handed down an influential view of Sweden-
borg as mentally ill.
19th-century understandings of Swedenborg: hallucinations in the sane?
In 1817 a number of the experiences undergone by Swedenborg (hearing
voices, seeing visions, etc.) were subsumed into the first medical definition
of ‘hallucination’ by Esquirol. It was during this period of history that, for
the first time, medicine claimed as part of its domain experiences that had
traditionally fallen under the authority of the Church (T. James, 1995). Indeed,
Schmidt (2002) has argued that medical psychiatry was created precisely in
order politically to contain delusions of religious fervour. At this time the
method of ‘médecine rétrospective’ (Littre, 1860: 103) emerged. This involved
attempts to reinterpret, in the light of present-day medical knowledge, the
experiences of influential religious and philosophical figures such as Socrates,
Moses, Luther, St Teresa of Avila, and occasionally Swedenborg, whose
experiences had previously been predominantly situated in a religious dis-
course (T. James, 1995).
Two contrasting approaches to Swedenborg in the 19th century are parti-
cularly worthy of examination, those of Brierre de Boismont and Henry
Maudsley. De Boismont’s work on the relations between hallucinations and
sanity occurred as part of the debate among French proto-psychiatrists
(forerunners of contemporary medical psychiatry), as to the relation between
hallucinations and madness. This debate, which stretched over three decades
from approximately the 1830s onwards, culminated in the 1855 debate at the
Société Médico-Psychologique. During these debates a range of views were
proposed. At one extreme existed views such Leuret’s which argued that hallu-
cinations were inherently pathological, and an indisputable sign of madness
(T. James, 1995). De Boismont, a Roman Catholic doctor, was placed in an
awkward position by his colleagues’ arguments that all hallucinations were
signs of madness, and hence that the Christian prophets, and more recent
religious figures such as Luther and Joan of Arc, who had experienced
hearing voices and seeing visions, were insane. The need for a category of
hallucinations coexisting with sanity was for de Boismont crucial, as other-
wise, he notes, ‘we are compelled to admit that eminent men . . . must be
placed in the Pandemonium of the insane, if the diseased hallucination is the
only form that can be recognised’ (de Boismont, 1860: 369). He hence argued
TALKING BACK TO THE SPIRITS
13
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
that the opinion that turns celebrated men ‘into hallucinated lunatics, must
be rejected, and reason be permitted to claim these great men as their own’
(ibid.: 370).
In order to achieve this aim de Boismont noted that it was true that the
‘majority of the insane are subject to hallucinations; but it is equally certain,
that they may occur by themselves’ (ibid.: 261). From this flowed his argu-
ment that hallucinations were not necessarily a ‘sign of insanity’ (ibid.: xiv),
and that they may coexist with sanity. One influential case in supporting this
thesis was the oft-cited case of a German bookseller, Nicolai, who in 1799
experienced a number of visual hallucinations both of a known deceased
acquaintance and of other unknown figures. Nicolai was able to deal effec-
tively with the continuance of these hallucinations, to the point where he
came only to experience mild anxiety when they spoke to him. Upon seeking
medical assistance, he came to receive a diagnosis not of insanity, but ‘halluci-
nations compatible with reason’ (Berrios, 1996: 36).
De Boismont further subdivided his category of ‘hallucinations coexisting
with sanity’ (1860: 34) into hallucinations that were corrected by the judge-
ment, and those that were not. This distinction arose from de Boismont’s
division of hallucinations into ‘two distinct elements, the sensible idea and the
mental conception’ (ibid.: 259). The sensible idea referred to the raw experi-
ence (i.e. the hearing of the voice), whereas the mental conception referred to
how the experience was understood. In hallucinations corrected by the judge-
ment, individuals ‘correctly regarded their hallucinations as the offspring of
the imagination, or as arising from the unhealthy state of the body’ (ibid.: 74).
In hallucinations not corrected by judgment, individuals’ explanations were
‘led by their belief in the supernatural’ yet they ‘gave no evidence of a dis-
ordered intellect’ (ibid.). De Boismont claimed that the prominent religious
figures of the past fell into this latter category. He also noted the importance
of context, arguing that figures such as Luther’s interpretations of their experi-
ences belonged to ‘society and not the individual’ (ibid.: 363).
Although de Boismont considered a number of historical figures, he only
mentioned Swedenborg in passing, as part of a discussion on ‘hallucinations
in ecstasy’ (ibid.: 188). However, as is clear from the above, he does not appear
to have regarded figures such as Swedenborg as insane. A similar view was
taken by his contemporary Lélut. Lélut noted that there existed the view that
‘the insanity . . . of Swedenborg, is now generally admitted by all who have
added the study of mental disease to that of history and philosophy’ (Lélut
cited in De Boismont, 1860: 340). However Lélut admitted the possibility
that there could be
. . . more or less continual, chronic hallucinations, considered by the
hallucinators as real sensations, which are nevertheless compatible with
an apparently whole set of reason, and which allow the individual who
HISTORY OF THE HUMAN SCIENCES 21(1)14
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
suffers them, not only to manage to continue to live with his fellow
men, but even to bring to his conduct and the management of his inter-
ests all the soundness of judgement which is desirable. (Lélut, cited in
T. James, 1995: 91)
Lélut concluded that figures such as Swedenborg ‘were not mad, but they
were hallucinators, hallucinators such as no longer exist, nor can exist, hallu-
cinators whose visions were the visions of reason’ (ibid.: 92). Lélut recog-
nized that such historical figures had ‘hallucinations in a religious and
reforming mode which was fostered by the spirit of the age’ (ibid.: 91) and
that this spirit of the age, ‘incapable of understanding such a form of madness,
obliged the hallucinator and his witnesses to believe in the reality of his false
perceptions’ (ibid.).
Shortly after the French proto-psychiatrist debate, the agnostic and mate-
rialist English psychiatrist Henry Maudsley published a less favourable judge-
ment on Swedenborg’s sanity. Maudsley had the explicit aim to promote
science over religion (Leudar and Sharrock, 2003) and it was within this
context that he gave attention to the case of Swedenborg (Maudsley, 1869a,
1869b). Reflecting the influence of writers such as Kant, Maudsley began his
review by noting that ‘there are very few who have thought it worth their
while to study him [Swedenborg] at all seriously; he is commonly accounted
a madman’ (ibid.: 169). Maudsley then moved on to discredit Swedenborg’s
earlier scientific work, claiming it would be ‘unprofitable to attempt to give
here a summary of Swedenborg’s physiological views . . . among them wild
conjectures, fanciful theories, strange conceits and empty phrases’ (ibid.: 183).
Maudsley’s review of Swedenborg’s life relied on biographies and not on
Swedenborg’s original writings. As a consequence he placed weight on a
story of Swedenborg rolling around naked in mud, claiming to be the Messiah,
which today is considered apocryphal (Talbot, 1998). Maudsley concluded
that ‘though he [Swedenborg] was insane, he was capable of taking care of
himself sufficiently well, and of managing his affairs with prudence’
(Maudsley, 1869b: 434). This view appears to be in line with the French
proto-psychiatrists’ concept of Swedenborg’s hallucinations coexisting with
sanity, except that Maudsley saw Swedenborg’s hallucinations as being intrin-
sically constitutive of insanity. This seemingly paradoxical conclusion becomes
clearer when we note that, although Maudsley was aware of the existence of
hallucinations coexisting with sanity, he proposed that this concept applied
only to hypnagogic and hypnopompic hallucinations (Leudar and Thomas,
2000). Hallucinations, such as Swedenborg’s, occurring in clear conscious-
ness were understood by Maudsley as being pathological and indicative of
mental illness.
After Maudsley’s (1869b: 434) conclusion that Swedenborg was ‘capable
of taking care of himself sufficiently well, and of managing his affairs with
TALKING BACK TO THE SPIRITS
15
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
prudence’, it is somewhat troubling when he adds that ‘had he [Swedenborg]
lived at the present day in England it is very doubtful whether he would have
been left in undisturbed possession of his freedom and his property’ (ibid.).
Maudsley’s hypothetical scenario was later played out in court three decades
later when the hallucinations experienced by Daniel Paul Schreber, ‘the most
frequently quoted patient in psychiatry’ (Leudar and Thomas, 2000: 52),
resulted in a similar court case. In Schreber’s case the court ruled that although
Schreber was ‘insane’ this was not all-encompassing, being restricted to his
religious ‘fixed ideas’, and was hence not a reason to deprive him of his legal
rights (ibid.).
20th-century understanding of Swedenborg: consolidation of psychiatry
A number of events around the end of the 19th century provided further
impetus for considering Swedenborg’s experiences as non-pathological. First,
large-scale empirical investigations of hallucinatory experiences in the normal
population began. In a survey of 17,000 normal participants, the Report on
the Census of Hallucinations (Sidgwick et al., 1894: 33) asked the question
‘Have you ever, when believing yourself to be completely awake, had a vivid
impression of seeing or being touched by a living being or inanimate object,
or of hearing a voice: which impression, so far as you could discover, was not
due to any external physical cause?’ Ten per cent of respondents replied in the
affirmative, though approximately half reported these experiences occurring
as they were either falling asleep or waking up. In terms of the modality of
the experiences, 56 per cent were solely visual, 23 per cent solely auditory, and
6 per cent involved combined visual and auditory hallucinations.
Secondly, William James’s publication of The Varieties of Religious Experi-
ence in 1902 argued for alternative ways to understand religious experiences.
James (1960: 35) noted that what he termed medical materialism ‘finishes up
Saint Paul by calling his vision on the road to Damascus a discharging lesion
of the occipital cortex, he being an epileptic’, ‘snuffs out Saint Teresa as an
hysteric, [and] Saint Francis of Assisi as an hereditary degenerate’. James
claimed that such an approach was simple-minded, and argued that experi-
ences such as hallucinations should be dealt with not by ‘superficial medical
talk’ but by an inquiry into ‘their fruits for life’ (ibid.: 398).
Despite the existence of these conceptual tools, at the dawn of the 20th
century it was commonplace for hallucinations still to be seen as pathological
and indicative of mental illness (Leudar and Thomas, 2000). The psychiatric
framework remained the dominant method of understanding hallucinations,
and, inspired by Maudsley, psychiatry limited the phenomena conceptualized
as hallucinations of the sane to hypnagogic and hypnopompic hallucinations.
Hallucinations in clear consciousness were seen as implying mental disorder
(Leudar and Thomas, 2000), as they still often are today (American Psychi-
atric Association, 2000).
HISTORY OF THE HUMAN SCIENCES 21(1)16
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
With the further rise of medical psychiatry in the 20th century there were
a number of attempts to apply the French proto-psychiatrists’ approach of
retrospective medicine to Swedenborg and, partly due to Maudsley’s view on
hallucinations in clear consciousness, correspondingly fewer attempts to
understand his experiences as hallucinations coexisting with sanity. Sweden-
borg’s experiences accordingly continued to be predominantly understood
as a symptom of an illness, the most common proposals being schizophrenia
and epilepsy.
Claims for a diagnosis of schizophrenia
Jaspers (1923) argued that Swedenborg was suffering from schizophrenia on
the grounds of the hallucinations that he claimed Swedenborg experienced,
as well as the delusional messianic beliefs he understood him to espouse.
Jaspers considered the diagnosis of schizophrenia ‘a certainty’ (ibid.: 125)
even while conceding that ‘the available data do not suffice to render the
diagnosis absolutely foolproof’ (ibid.). Jaspers’ broad conception of schizo-
phrenia, encompassing ‘all irreversible processes which are not known as
organic cerebral processes or epilepsy’ (ibid.: 191), differed considerably
from contemporary definitions. However, some modern psychiatrists still
suggest that schizophrenia may be an appropriate diagnosis for Swedenborg
(e.g. J. Johnson, 1994). It is easy to see how some of Swedenborg’s experiences
could lead to such a conclusion. In addition to his apparent hallucinations and
delusions, statements such as ‘my hand was so manifestly directed to the
writing that the words hardly appeared to have been written by my hand;
my hand was being ruled’ (WE, n5741) could be interpreted as exemplifying
delusions of control associated with schizophrenia (Frith, 1992). A further
point of comparison concerns the intellectual quality of the voices and visions
Swedenborg experienced. Van Dusen (1990) claims to have been able to de-
lineate ‘higher’- and ‘lower’-order voices of patients with schizophrenia. The
lower-order voices he found to have a level of knowledge ‘far less than the
patient’s’ (ibid.: 154). Contrastingly, he found the higher-order voices to be
‘more gifted’ than the patient (ibid.: 150). As Van Dusen notes, Swedenborg
experienced both ‘higher’ and ‘lower’ voices according to this definition.
Claims for a diagnosis of epilepsy
In addition to views of Swedenborg as suffering from schizophrenia, there is
a long history of viewing his experiences through the lens of epilepsy, a
neurological disorder characterized by seizures resulting from abnormal or
excessive neuronal activity in the brain. There is a prima facie case to be made
for a diagnosis of epilepsy from the observations that at the onset of Sweden-
borg’s anomalous experiences he lost interest in sex (DD, n14), turned his
focus exclusively to religious matters, and started to produce voluminous
TALKING BACK TO THE SPIRITS
17
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
writings on this topic. These coincide with the three primary features of the
epileptic syndrome described by Geschwind (hyposexuality, hypergraphia,
and hyperreligiosity) which now bears his name (van Elst et al., 2003), although
the characterization of this as a personality disorder associated with epilepsy
remains controversial (Devinsky and Najjar, 1999).
Foote-Smith and Smith (1996) have also proposed that Swedenborg
suffered from epilepsy. They interpret Swedenborg’s report that, just prior
to experiencing a vision of Jesus, ‘there came over me a strong shuddering
from head to foot with a thunderous noise, as if many winds beat together;
which shook me; it was indescribable and prostrated me on my face’ (ibid.:
214) as being a generalized tonic-clonic epileptic seizure. They also claim
that Swedenborg demonstrated 8 out of the 18 possible behavioral correlates
of epilepsy described by Bear and Fedio (1977), as well as noting examples
of an ecstatic aura, typically associated with epilepsy, being reported by
Swedenborg.
Going beyond the evidence presented by Foote-Smith and Smith (1996),
a number of other facets of Swedenborg’s experiences could be seen as being
consistent with epilepsy. First, the presence of psychotic experiences such
as hallucinations and delusions, commonly associated with the positive
symptoms of schizophrenia, is a significant complication of epilepsy (Toone
et al., 1982). This is particularly the case in the most common form of epilepsy,
temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE), in which seizures originate in the temporal
lobe (ibid.). Psychotic experiences are found in many case reports of indi-
viduals diagnosed with epilepsy. For example, Oner et al. (2005) found almost
daily auditory verbal hallucinations, as well as brief visual hallucinations, in
an individual diagnosed with complex partial epilepsy. Furthermore, Hansen
and Brodtkorb (2003) report an instance of an epileptic patient who reported
the sensation of being in two different worlds. In one attack this patient
imagined encountering a wise woman who, in a non-verbal manner, attempted
to present to her the ultimate mission of her life. The patient was unable to
interpret the details of the message, save that it was related to saving children,
and was extremely important. The experience of being in another world, as
well as receiving an extremely important message, both characterize some
of the experiences of Swedenborg. Another patient reported by Hansen and
Brodtkorb (2003) related that during seizures his surroundings felt strange
and unfamiliar as if they were in another world.
The link between epilepsy and hyper-religiosity can be seen to provide
further support for a diagnosis of epilepsy. TLE has been found to be associ-
ated with an enhanced emotional response to specifically religious stimuli,
rather than neutral or sexual stimuli (Ramachandran and Blakeslee, 1998).
Furthermore, when the religious experiences of individuals with TLE with
prominent religious inclinations have been compared to healthy church-
goers, it has been found that hyper-religious epileptic individuals adopted
HISTORY OF THE HUMAN SCIENCES 21(1)18
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
significantly more non-mainstream (for the United Kingdom) religions than
healthy churchgoers (Trimble and Freeman, 2006). Hyper-religious epileptic
individuals are also significantly more likely to report the awareness of an
evil presence, the feeling of being punished by God, and sensory experience
of spiritual figures, as well as believing such experiences to be a source of
valid knowledge, which, although non-rational and insightful, are felt to be
not merely subjective (ibid.).
Swedenborg also reported a number of experiences that we may interpret
as simple partial epileptic seizures (seizures in which consciousness is not
impaired). First, he reported instances where he experienced unintended
facial muscular movements. For example, he writes that ‘there are spirits who
do not speak but [who] expressed their meaning by changes induced on my
face . . . This was affected by variations around the region of the lips extend-
ing thence into the face, and also around the eyes’ (AC, n1762, see also AC,
n5720). Elsewhere he states how a ‘region of my face was moved out of shape’
(SpD, n626). As Hopkins and Appleton (2003: 14) note, in partial seizures
cells in the motor cortex ‘which supply the corner of the mouth . . . are most
likely to be those in which a seizure discharge begins’. In a related vein
Swedenborg elsewhere notes that spirits ‘brought about an urge’ for him to
thrust his tongue between his teeth and so cut it off (SpD, n1361, see also
SpD, n1465). Second, Swedenborg notes that one evening spirits caused him
to smell ‘the odour of human excrement from the dry foods, and a foul
ruinous odour from the liquids’ (SpD, n618). Similarly he notes that ‘on two
occasions . . . sugar tasted almost like salt’, an experience which he attributed
to spirits from Jupiter (SpD, n645, see also WE, n4793). Both experiences
could be attributed to a seizure discharge in the anterior part of the temporal
lobe (Hopkins and Appleton, 2003). Third, Swedenborg reports an occasion
when ‘he [a spirit] induced a contraction or painful straitness in the lower
region of the belly’ (AC, n5388). Such epigastric sensations are also associ-
ated with epileptic seizures. Fourth, Swedenborg notes a number of experi-
ences in which he saw lights. Among these were small stars (SpD, n613,
n913), flashes of lightning (SpD, n914), and bright lights (AC, n1116, n6922).
Such experiences could be interpreted as elementary visual seizures (Ebner,
2001). Finally, Swedenborg also described out-of-body experiences, which
in addition are reported by some epileptic patients (Devine and Duncan,
2007).
Any interpretation of Swedenborg’s experiences as symptoms of epilepsy
would still leave open the question of what triggered his seizures. Benz (2002:
293) notes that ‘a great number of Swedenborg’s visions actually occurred
while he was sitting in his room and reflecting on a particular passage in the
Bible’. It could be speculated that Swedenborg experienced language-induced
epilepsy in which seizures can be precipitated by speaking, reading, or
writing (Valenti et al., 2006).
TALKING BACK TO THE SPIRITS
19
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Reconsidering diagnoses of schizophrenia and epilepsy
We may first reconsider the claim that Swedenborg was suffering from
schizophrenia. As noted above, Jasper’s (1923) claim that Swedenborg suffered
from schizophrenia was partly based on stories that Swedenborg claimed to
be the Messiah. As noted above, such claims appear apocryphal (Talbot,
1998). Furthermore, Swedenborg’s ‘delusions of control’ – feeling his hand
was moved by another power – occurred only for a short period. Foote-
Smith and Smith (1996: 216) have argued that the inappropriateness of a diag-
nosis of schizophrenia is self-evident from the fact of Swedenborg’s ‘lifelong
involvement in public affairs as a nobleman’, ‘his scientific achievements’, and
his membership of the Royal Academy of Sciences. Social and occupational
dysfunction is indeed one of the current diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia
(American Psychiatric Association, 2000), and, as the evidence presented
earlier has shown, Swedenborg was clearly able to function at a high level
after the onset of his anomalous experiences. It would hence appear that this
psychiatric diagnosis is not appropriate.
However, this does not necessarily validate the alternative conception that
Foote-Smith and Smith (1996) have put forward, namely that Swedenborg
suffered from epilepsy. A reexamination of Foote-Smith and Smith’s evidence
for Swedenborg’s epilepsy shows much that is anecdotal, selective and vague.
For example, examination of their suggestion that Swedenborg possessed
many of the eighteen possible behavioral correlates of epilepsy (Bear and
Fedio, 1977) shows that their claim of his alleged ‘sadness’ comes from a diary
entry where Swedenborg relates that he wept because he had not loved God.
The citing of a single episode of a transient emotional state in Swedenborg
does not amount to proof that he possessed sadness as a personality trait.
Evidence of the latter in Swedenborg is lacking, and as Pendleton (1998)
notes, Foote-Smith and Smith (1996: 215) themselves, elsewhere in their
article, cite a contemporary of Swedenborg describing him as ‘always
contented, never fretful or morose’.
There are also a number of aspects of Swedenborg’s account that suggest
that an explanation of his experiences as due to epilepsy is not appropriate.
First, Swedenborg never seemed to be overwhelmed by his experiences in
public. As Benz (2002: 307) notes, ‘not one of his friends or visitors report
sudden raptures, which might have overpowered him against his will at an
unsuitable moment, in company or before witnesses’. Second, Pendleton
(1998) has queried Foote-Smith and Smith’s (1996) interpretation of some of
Swedenborg’s experiences as generalized tonic-clonic seizures (GTCS).
Other aspects of Swedenborg’s experiences that question the appropriate-
ness of a diagnosis of epilepsy are their frequency, duration and length. First,
it is not common for patients with epilepsy to experience the multiplicity of
sensations that Swedenborg did (Landtblom, 2004). Although the contem-
porary experiences of individuals with epilepsy reviewed above show some
HISTORY OF THE HUMAN SCIENCES 21(1)20
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
similarities to Swedenborg’s experiences, none of these individuals presented
with the variety of experiences Swedenborg underwent. Second, as a recent
review concludes, the hallucinations of epilepsy are ‘usually brief, stereotyped
and fragmentary’ (Mamford and Andermann, 1998: 1826). Swedenborg’s
experiences were not like this. Furthermore, the hallucinations of epilepsy
usually follow a similar pattern as they most frequently are derived from the
same epileptic focus (Landtblom, 2004). The variety of Swedenborg’s experi-
ences is not consistent with this.
We may also reconsider the claim that Swedenborg’s hyper-religiosity was
a manifestation of TLE, as such an attitude may equally well be explained as
arising from the religious culture in which he was raised. The content of
hallucinations has been shown to be influenced by the culture in which the
individual is situated. For example, whereas much of the hallucinatory
content in Saudi Arabia is religious and superstitious, in the United Kingdom
hallucinations are more centred around instructional themes and running
commentaries (Kent and Wahass, 1996).
In a related hypothesis, Bradford (1999) has proposed that Swedenborg’s
experiences resulted from non-epileptic partial complex seizures associated
with vascular disease. From evidence that Swedenborg occasionally reported
experiencing the spirit world in the right portion of his visual field, Bradford
concludes that ‘Swedenborg experienced a right-superior quadrantanopia
which corresponds with damage along the temporooccipital geniculocalcarine
pathway’ (ibid.: 379). Unfortunately, like Maudsley, Bradford does not appear
to have consulted Swedenborg’s original writings, instead relying on repro-
duced quotations in Benz (2002). Swedenborg’s works actually show that he
also saw spirits in the left visual field (e.g. AC, n5180, n5387, n5390, n7170).
In conclusion, although cases exist where patients with epilepsy have experi-
enced either auditory, visual or olfactory hallucinations that individually bear
some resemblance to Swedenborg’s, we are not aware of reports of patients
experiencing all of these in clear consciousness and with no intellectual
impairment.
UNDERSTANDING SWEDENBORG’S EXPERIENCES
TODAY
Given the above review, Johnson’s (1994: 691) conclusion in the British
Journal of Psychiatry that ‘Swedenborg’s messianic psychosis was due to
acute schizophrenia or an epileptic psychosis’ is likely to be misplaced. The
attempts reviewed above to fit Swedenborg’s experiences into some form of
psychiatric diagnosis can be seen as operating in the context of a specific
understanding of the term ‘hallucination’. Despite the introduction of the
category of hallucinations of the sane by the French proto-psychiatrists, for
TALKING BACK TO THE SPIRITS
21
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
much of recent history the concept of hallucinations has been ‘pejorative,
coterminous with madness, lunacy, and schizophrenia’ (Sarbin, 1967: 379).
However, as Leon James (2001: 161) notes, there have always been those in
the arts who have seen Swedenborg’s experiences and writings ‘not as a
madman’s psychosis, but as intellectually valuable, convincing, and unique’.
The positive opinions of Swedenborg held by thinkers and writers such as
William Blake, Ralph Emerson, Carl Jung, August Strindberg and Henry
Thoreau have had little impact on psychiatric thinking about hallucinations
which has remained dominated by the 19th-century conceptions discussed
above (Berrios, 1991). Those outside of psychiatry have called for a re-
consideration of how we perceive Swedenborg’s experiences. For example, the
Reverend Brian Talbot (1998) notes that Swedenborgians are disappointed
that psychiatrists have not ‘considered a third option, and that is that Sweden-
borg was sane and his unusual experiences were genuine’. In the remainder of
this article, we consider how contemporary discourses about hallucinations
provide just such a ‘third way’ of understanding voice-hearing and related
experiences, through a revival of the concept of hallucinations in the sane.
A third way: hallucinations without mental disorder
The contemporary reconstruction of the 19th-century concept of hallucin-
ations in the sane may be traced back to the interactions of one psychiatrist,
Marius Romme, with a particular patient, Patsy Hague. Romme was led to
investigate the presence of specifically auditory hallucinations (voice-
hearing) in the general population after discussions with Hague who herself
heard voices. These meetings directly led to a television appearance by Romme
requesting voice-hearers to contact him. Of the 173 respondents who agreed
to complete a questionnaire, 76 were not in psychiatric care (Romme et al.,
1992). This and later work led to Romme’s being confronted by a large
number of ‘well-balanced, healthy people, who happened to hear voices’
(Romme and Escher, 1993: 59). This provided the impetus for a series of
studies into psychosis-like experiences in the general population, similar to
those at the end of the 19th century. These studies provided further evidence
that otherwise healthy individuals may experience hallucinations in the absence
of the social dysfunction or distress associated with clinical psychosis, and
that such experiences exist on a continuum stretching into the normal popu-
lation (Johns and van Os, 2001; Posey and Losch, 1983; Tien, 1991). The
annual prevalence of such hallucinatory experiences in the general population
(in both the visual and auditory modality) has been found to be 4 per cent
(Johns, Nazroo et al., 1998).
As a result of such work, a growing worldwide movement exists today
based around the view that hallucinating does not necessarily imply mental
illness (Romme and Escher, 1993). For example, in the United Kingdom this
has led to the creation of a large user-movement, the Hearing Voices Network,
HISTORY OF THE HUMAN SCIENCES 21(1)22
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
with parallel organizations now existing throughout the world. Although
such individuals’ experiences are predominantly auditory, members of the
Hearing Voices Network also experience hallucinations in a range of other
modalities; for example, visual and tactile (Romme and Escher, 1993). This
movement has resulted in a large number of such individuals rejecting the
label of ‘patient’, and the reinvention by such individuals, partly in partner-
ship with psychiatrists, of the paradigm of hallucinations in the sane. In order
to encompass the range of sensory modalities in which these experiences can
occur, we henceforth use the term ‘hallucinations without mental disorder’
(HwMD) to refer to these experiences.
There is a gathering consensus that whether an individual experiencing
voices and/or visions receives a psychiatric diagnosis depends much on the
emotional content of the experience, and her or his emotional response to it.
For example, voice-hearers’ interpretation of their voices, not just the experi-
ence of the voice-hearing per se, is an important determinant of the amount
of resulting distress and social/occupational disability (Krabbendam et al.,
2004). In this framework, social representations (Moscovici, 1988) of voices
and visions as being coterminous with madness and insanity may act as self-
fulfilling prophecies through their impact on how individuals who have these
experiences interpret them. Today many individuals who experience voices
and visions are able to lead productive lives, and encounter their experiences
in a non-psychiatric, non-mental illness discourse. Contemporary examples
of this include a prominent professor of Mathematics who reports receiving
beneficial mathematical insights from her voices (Malone, 2006) and a leading
pianist who hears a voice that illuminates his playing as well as experiencing
a number of anomalous visual experiences (ibid.). In line with such examples,
Leudar (David and Leudar, 2001: 256) has argued that ‘hearing voices (and
the experiences we may categorize as hallucinations) should be judged as sane
or insane in terms of their consequences for life. They are not in themselves
signs of madness, any more than, say, thinking and remembering.’
Given the religious background of Swedenborg, and the cultural milleu in
which he was situated, it seems appropriate for us today to consider his
experiences as those of an individual experiencing HwMD. It should be
stressed that such an account is not an explanation of Swedenborg’s experi-
ences, but rather offers an established framework within which to understand
them that does not invoke concepts of mental illness or insanity.
Swedenborg’s experiences in a modern context
It is interesting to compare modern characterizations of the experiences of
HwMD with what Swedenborg reported. As most research into such indivi-
duals’ hallucinations has focused on ‘hearing voices’, we focus our comparison
on Swedenborg’s early experiences in the mid-1740s, which, as noted above,
were largely auditory. The majority of individuals experiencing HwMD in a
TALKING BACK TO THE SPIRITS
23
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
sample interviewed by Leudar et al. (1997) reported being the target of the
voice, which spoke to them rather than addressing another voice or person.
Swedenborg’s experiences also included direct conversations with spirits, as
well as overhearing their conversations with each other. Secondly, Leudar
et al. found the majority of those experiencing HwMD reporting hearing
voices giving commands – for example, ‘Get the milk’ or ‘Go to the hospital’
(Nayani and David, 1996) – which typically focused on mundane, everyday
activities. Swedenborg’s early voices also gave him explicit directions, in his
case to write. Although it might seem odd to describe Swedenborg’s pro-
foundly spiritual experiences as being focused on mundane or everyday
activities, his voices were nevertheless able to go into great depth about the
specifics of life in a spiritual realm. It was this that led William Butler Yeats
to remark that he liked Swedenborg because he tells us that the angels don’t
use butter (Johnson, 1997).
Surveys of the numbers of different voices heard by individuals experi-
encing HwMD have found an average of 2–3 voices (Leudar et al., 1997).
Swedenborg heard many more than this number, indeed by 1748 he had
counted 60 different voices (SpD, n1499). Swedenborg also experienced voices
which themselves claimed to believe that other people could hear them.
While it is common for many experiencing HwMD initially to think that
their voices can also be heard by others (Romme and Escher, 1993), no
phenomenological surveys have reported examples of individuals reporting
that the voices themselves think that other people can hear them. This may
be seen to reflect Van Dusen’s (1990) observation that some voices can be
seen to be independent from the individual’s conscious self. Another facet
aspect of Swedenborg’s experience is that he claims to have often spoken
with spirits ‘by ideas of thought’ (AC, n1639). While the idea that we think,
not in natural language, but in a specific language of thought (today termed
‘mentalese’) is still a popular philosophical position (e.g. Fodor, 1975), it is
not known whether voices reported by those experiencing HwMD often
have this quality.
Currently, a lack of relevant evidence prevents us from comparing Sweden-
borg’s later experiences, in which he felt himself to ascend to the spirit world
and there converse with spirits, and similarly combined visual and auditory
experiences in modern accounts of HwMD. However, we would note that
personal accounts from those with HwMD indicate that when visually hallu-
cinating most experience themselves as firmly in this world, with their visual
hallucinations also embedded in it (Romme and Escher, 1993).
What can we learn from Swedenborg’s experiences?
Conceiving of Swedenborg’s experiences as those of an individual experi-
encing HwMD argues against previous proposals that it is not worthwhile
HISTORY OF THE HUMAN SCIENCES 21(1)24
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
‘to study him at all seriously; [as] he is commonly accounted a madman’
(Maudsley, 1869a: 169). When viewed as worthwhile of investigation, Sweden-
borg’s experiences present us with a number of questions. One such question
is the relation of the knowledge and intelligence of the voices and visions
experienced by an individual to his or her conscious persona. Swedenborg
was clearly able to hold dialogues with his voices and visions, while not
experiencing the answers or arguments they gave as his own. This raises the
question as to whether it is possible for voices and visions to have access to
information that the experiencer may not be able to consciously access. If so,
what are the cognitive mechanisms that allow voices to offer insightful, novel
information to those experiencing HwMD? This issue is highlighted by
contemporary cases, such as that of an otherwise healthy woman who heard
voices telling her to get a brain scan for a tumour, which was subsequently
found and removed, at which point the voices stopped (Azuonye, 1997). We
may also ask what the relation is between such experiences and the creative
process, where novel ideas enter into consciousness and are experienced as
generated by the self. Research showing an association between schizotypy
(non-clinical equivalents of the symptoms of schizophrenia) and creativity
(e.g. Brod, 1997; Nettle, 2006) suggests that the creative process may be linked
to ideas that are experienced as alien to the self (see also Taylor et al., 2004).
We may also consider what we can learn from the efforts of Swedenborg
and other historical figures to cope with their experiences. Swedenborg’s
talking back to the spirits may have been actively beneficial to him. One
experimental therapy for individuals experiencing HwMDs already acts
through treating the voices as separate entities with which the voice-hearer
can engage in a dialogue. This was explored in a case study by Davies et al.
(1999), in which one of the authors (Davies), a voice-hearer diagnosed with
schizophrenia, initially experienced two dominant voices. The first voice,
which she termed her ‘guardian angel’, commented on her behavior and often
ordered her to do ‘wicked’ things. Such commands were hard to resist. The
second voice she referred to as her ‘little devil’. This voice did not direct her
but gave her information necessary for performing the actions it suggested.
When, as part of a therapeutic intervention, Davies started to write out poten-
tial responses to the voices’ suggestions, as part of a dialogical engagement
with them, a new voice arose, which Davies termed her ‘holy angel’. This voice
reassured her, supported her, and had the effect of increasing her self-esteem.
CONCLUSION
This article first offered a summary of the experiences of Emanuel Sweden-
borg, and an account of how he and others have attempted to explain these
experiences. It was shown how the birth of psychiatry increased the range of
TALKING BACK TO THE SPIRITS
25
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
conceptual frameworks available for making sense of Swedenborg’s experi-
ences, including interpretations of his experiences both as a spiritual visionary
and a madman. However, as has happened with many other historical figures
(Leudar and Sharrock, 2003), Swedenborg’s experiences became translated
into pathological symptoms. In the 20th century this understanding, through
further medicalization of Swedenborg’s experiences, continued, with Sweden-
borg understood as suffering from epilepsy or schizophrenia. We have argued
that Swedenborg’s experiences are not consistent with such diagnoses, and
that instead they may be understood (though not may be explained) within
a non-medical framework. Specifically, we have proposed that Swedenborg
be understood as an individual experiencing HwMD, a discourse which can
exist outside of that of mental illness. David (David and Leudar, 2001: 256)
has claimed that ‘A [healthy] voice-hearer who is not in any distress, who
lives a fruitful and productive life according to commonsense criteria, would
never even enter the arena in which the possibility of mental illness was up
for discussion’. We suggest the frequent historical attempts to diagnose
Swedenborg with a psychiatric disorder have arisen due, not to the failure of
Swedenborg’s life to be fruitful and productive, but its failure to do so
‘according to commonsense criteria’.
We should also consider some possible limitations to our analysis. First, it
may be correctly noted that we have not consulted the writings of Sweden-
borg except in translation, and that the quotations we have used to support
our case are selective. In response to this we note that Swedenborg’s volumi-
nous output means that such a review must be to some extent selective. Second,
it may be noted that, just as Talbot (1998) has argued that some psychiatrists
may have an interest in promoting Swedenborg as an example of someone who
was insane, our account has been partly motivated by an attempt to demon-
strate that Swedenborg can be conceived of as experiencing HwMD. We are
happy to let the reader, and particularly those who have studied Swedenborg’s
writings more closely, judge whether our review of Swedenborg’s experiences
in the context of the psychiatric literature is fair. Even if others wish to
dispute our characterization of Swedenborg as experiencing HwMD, this
article has shown how a number of productive lines of enquiry relevant to
our contemporary understanding of hallucinations may be developed.
Finally, it appears from our review of Swedenborg’s experiences that the
formation of an active partnership between the scientific community and
those experiencing HwMD would be likely to be highly beneficial to both
parties. The active involvement of such individuals in the research process
should help to reduce the perception of such individuals as ‘patients’, and
instead promote a conception of them as rational, healthy individuals who
are attempting to understand experiences that are present, albeit in lesser
degree, in many of us. Such a partnership should also provide researchers with
a better acquaintance with the varied phenomenology of such experiences,
HISTORY OF THE HUMAN SCIENCES 21(1)26
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
TALKING BACK TO THE SPIRITS
27
forming a bridge between clinical and research aspects of psychology. This
approach would appear to be a good step towards what Romme (cited in
Bentall, 2003: 511) has termed the ‘liberation not cure’ of those experiencing
HwMD. Ignoring or sidelining the experiences of individuals undergoing
voices and visions, such as Swedenborg, may constitute a barrier both to
further understanding of the phenomenon and, if necessary, therapeutic
progress. Although consulting such individuals may at first raise more ques-
tions than answers, it is only by such a process that these most mysterious
of experiences can come to be more fully understood.
BIBLIOGRAPHY
American Psychiatric Association (2000) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, 4th edn, revised. Washington, DC: APA.
Azuonye, I. O. (1997) ‘A Difficult Case: Diagnosis Made by Hallucinatory Voices’,
British Medical Journal 315: 1685–6.
Bear, D. M. and Fedio, P. (1977) ‘Quantitative Analysis of Interictal Behaviour in
Temporal Lobe Epilepsy’, Archives of Neurology 34: 454–67.
Bentall, R. P. (2003) Madness Explained: Psychosis and Human Nature. London:
Penguin Books.
Benz, E. (2002) Emanuel Swedenborg: Visionary Savant in the Age of Reason, trans.
N. Goodrick-Clarke. West Chester, PA: Swedenborg Foundation.
Berrios, G. E. (1991) ‘British Psychopathology since the Early 20th Century’, in
G. E. Berrios and H. Freeman (eds) 150 Years of British Psychiatry 1841–1991.
London: Gaskell and the Royal College of Psychiatrists.
Berrios, G. E. (1996) The History of Mental Symptoms: Descriptive Psychopathology
since the Nineteenth Century. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Borch-Jacobsen, M. (2001) ‘Making Psychiatric History: Madness as Folie à Plusieurs’,
History of the Human Sciences 14: 19–38.
Bradford, D. T. (1999) ‘Neuropsychology of Swedenborg’s Visions’, Perceptual and
Motor Skills 88: 377–83.
Brod, J. H. (1997) ‘Creativity and Schizotypy’, in G. Claridge (ed.) Schizotypy: Impli-
cations for Illness and Health. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
David, T. and Leudar, I. (2001) ‘Head to Head: Is Hearing Voices a Sign of Mental
Illness?’, The Psychologist 14: 256–9.
Davies, P., Thomas, P. and Leudar, I. (1999) ‘Dialogical Engagement with Voices: a
Single Case Study’, British Journal of Medical Psychology 72: 179–87.
De Boismont, A. B. (1860) On Hallucinations, trans. R. T. Hulme. Columbus, OH:
Joseph H. Riley.
Devine, M. J. and Duncan, J. S. (2007) ‘Development of Psychogenic Non-epileptic
Seizures in Response to Auditory Hallucinations’, Seizure, 717–21.
Devinsky, O. and Najjar, S. (1999) ‘Evidence Against the Existence of a Temporal
Lobe Epilepsy Personality Syndrome’, Neurology 53: S13–25.
Ebner, A. (2001) ‘Epileptic Syndromes in Adults’, in M. Dunitz (ed.) Epilepsy: Compre-
hensive Review and Case Discussions. London: Martin Dunitz.
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Fodor, J. (1975) The Language of Thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Foote-Smith, E. and Smith, T. J. (1996) ‘Emanuel Swedenborg’, Epilepsia 37: 211–18.
Frith, C. D. (1992) The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia. Hove, Sx:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Gould, L. N. (1948) ‘Verbal Hallucinations and Activity of Vocal Musculature: an
Electromyographic Study’, American Journal of Psychiatry 105: 367–72.
Gross, C. G. (1997) ‘Emanuel Swedenborg: a Neuroscientist Before his Time’, The
Neuroscientist 3: 142–7.
Hacking, I. (2003) ‘Indeterminacy in the Past: on the Recent Discussion of Chapter
17 of Rewriting the Soul’, History of the Human Sciences 16: 117–24.
Hansen, A. B. and Brodtkorb, E. (2003) ‘Partial Epilepsy with “Ecstatic” Seizures’,
Epilepsy & Behavior 4: 667–73.
Hopkins, A. and Appleton, R. (2003) Epilepsy: The Facts. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Ingram, A. (1998) Patterns of Madness in the Eighteenth Century: A Reader. Liverpool:
Liverpool University Press.
Inouye, T. and Shimizu, A. (1970) ‘The Electromyographic Study of Verbal Halluci-
nation’, Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 151: 415–22.
James, L. (2001) ‘Overcoming Objections to Swedenborg’s Writings through the
Development of Scientific Dualism’, The New Philosophy. Retrieved 1 November
2006, from: http://www.newphilosophyonline.org/journal/article.php?page=1027
&issue=104b
James, T. (1995) Dreams, Creativity, and Madness in Nineteenth-Century France.
Oxford: Clarendon Press.
James, W. (1960[1902]) The Varieties of Religious Experience. London: Collins.
Jaspers, K. (1923[1962]) General Psychopathology. Manchester: Manchester University
Press.
Johns, L. C., Nazroo, J. Y., Bebbington, P. and Kuipers, E. (1998) ‘Occurrence of
Hallucinations in a Community Sample’, Schizophrenia Research 29: 23.
Johns, L. C. and van Os, J. (2001) ‘The Continuity of Psychotic Experiences in the
General Populations’, Clinical Psychology Review 21: 1125–41.
Johnson, G. R. (1997) ‘Kant on Swedenborg in the Lectures on Metaphysics, Part 1’,
Studia Swedenborgiana. Retrieved 20 November 2006, from: http://www.bayside
church.org/studia/studia.cfm?VolumeID=37&RID=061207_041506_7048636
Johnson, J. (1994) ‘Henry Maudsley on Swedenborg’s Messianic Psychosis’, British
Journal of Psychiatry 165: 690–1.
Jones, S. R. and Fernyhough, C. (2007) ‘Neural Correlates of Inner Speech and
Auditory Verbal Hallucinations: a Critical Review and Theoretical Integration’,
Clinical Psychology Review 27: 140–54.
Kant, I. (2002[1766]) Kant on Swedenborg: Dreams of a Spirit Seer and Other Writings,
trans. G. R. Johnson and G. A. Magee. West Chester, PA: Swedenborg Foundation.
Kent, G. and Wahass, S. (1996) ‘The Content and Characteristics of Auditory Halluci-
nations in Saudi Arabia and the UK: a Cross-cultural Comparison’, Acta Psychi-
atrica Scandinavica 94: 433–7.
Krabbendam, L., Myin-Germeys, I., Hanssen, M., Bijl, R. V., de Graaf, R., Volle-
bergh, W. et al. (2004) ‘Hallucinatory Experiences and Onset of Psychotic
HISTORY OF THE HUMAN SCIENCES 21(1)28
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Disorder: Evidence that the Risk is Mediated by Delusion Formation’, Acta
Psychiatrica Scandinavica 110: 264–72.
Kroll, J. and Bachrach, B. (1982) ‘Visions and Psychopathology in the Middle Ages’,
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 170: 41–9.
Landtblom, A.-M. (2004) ‘Did St Birgitta Suffer from Epilepsy? A Neuropathogra-
phy’, Seizure 13: 161–7.
Leudar, I. and Sharrock, W. (2002) ‘The Cases of John Bunyan, Part 1. Taine and
Royce?’, History of Psychiatry 13: 247–65.
Leudar, I. and Sharrock, W. (2003) ‘Changing the Past?’, History of the Human
Sciences 16: 105–21.
Leudar, I. and Thomas, P. (2000) Voices of Reason, Voices of Insanity: Studies of Verbal
Hallucinations. London: Routledge.
Leudar, I., Thomas, P., McNally, D. and Glinski, A. (1997) ‘What Voices can do with
Words: Pragmatics of Verbal Hallucinations’, Psychological Medicine 27: 885–98.
Littre, E. (1860) ‘Un Fragment de Médecine Rétrospective’, Philosophie Positive 5:
103–20.
Malone, D. (producer and director) (2006) Voices in My Head [television broadcast,
18 June]. London: Channel 4.
Mamford, M. and Andermann, F. (1998) ‘Complex Visual Hallucinations: Clinical
and Neurobiological Insights’, Brain 121: 1819–40.
Maudsley, H. (1869a) ‘Emanuel Swedenborg’, Journal of Mental Science 15: 169–96.
Maudsley, H. (1869b) ‘Emanuel Swedenborg’, Journal of Mental Science 15: 417–36.
Moscovici, S. (1988) ‘Notes Towards a Description of Social Representations’,
European Journal of Social Psychology 18: 211–50.
Nayani, T. H. and David, A. S. (1996) ‘The Auditory Hallucination: a Phenomeno-
logical Survey’, Psychological Medicine 26: 177–89.
Nettle, D. (2006) ‘Schizotypy and Mental Health Amongst Poets, Visual Artists, and
Mathematicians’, Journal of Research in Personality 40: 876–90.
Oner, O., Unal, O. and Deda, G. (2005) ‘A Case of Psychosis with Temporal Lobe
Epilepsy: SPECT Changes with Treatment’, Pediatric Neurology 32: 197–200.
Pendleton, J. L. (1998) ‘Diagnosing Emanuel Swedenborg’, The New Philosophy,
retrieved 15 November 2006, from: http://www.newphilosophyonline.org/
journal/article.php?issue=sanity&page=1000
Porter, R. (2002) Madness: A Brief History. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Posey, T. B. and Losch, M. E. (1983) ‘Auditory Hallucinations of Hearing Voices in
375 Normal Subjects’, Imagination, Cognition, and Personality 3: 99–113.
Ramachandran, V. S. and Blakeslee, S. (1998) Phantoms in the Brain: Probing the
Mysteries of the Human Mind. New York: Morrow.
Romme, M. and Escher, S. (1993) Accepting Voices. Shaftesbury: Mind.
Romme, M., Honig, A., Noorthoorn, E. O. and Escher, S. (1992) ‘Coping with Hearing
Voices: an Emancipatory Approach’, British Journal of Psychiatry 161: 99–103.
Sarbin, T. R. (1967) ‘The Concept of Hallucination’, Journal of Personality 35: 359–80.
Schmidt, L. E. (2002) Hearing Things: Religion, Illusion, and the American Enlighten-
ment. London: Harvard University Press.
Sidgwick, H., Johnson, A., Myers, F. W. H., Podmore, F. and Sidgwick, E. M. (1894)
‘Report on the Census of Hallucinations’, Proceedings of the Society for Psychical
Research 10: 24–422.
TALKING BACK TO THE SPIRITS
29
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Slade, P. and Bentall, R. (1988) Sensory Deception: A Scientific Analysis of Hallucina-
tion. London: Croom Helm.
Stanley, M (1988) Emanuel Swedenborg: Essential Readings. London: Aquarian Press.
Swedenborg, E. (1872[1749–56]) Arcana Coelestia, trans. J. Clowes. London: Sweden-
borg Society.
Swedenborg, E. (1883[1746–65]) The Spiritual Diary of Emanuel Swedenborg, trans.
G. Bush and J. H. Smithson. London: James Speirs.
Swedenborg, E. (1883[1771]) True Christian Religion. London: Swedenborg Society.
Swedenborg, E. (1928[1746–47]) The Word of the Old Testament Explained, trans.
A. Acton. Bryn Athyn, PA: Academy of the New Church.
Swedenborg, E. (1931[1758]) Heaven and Hell, trans. J. R. Rendell. London: Sweden-
borg Society.
Swedenborg, E. (1986[1859]) Emanuel Swedenborg’s Journal of Dreams, trans.
J. G. G. Wilkinson, commentary W. Van Dusen. New York: Swedenborg Foun-
dation.
Swedenborg, E. (2006[1758]) Life on Other Planets, trans. J. Chadwick. London:
Swedenborg Society.
Swift, E. (1883) Emanuel Swedenborg: The Man and His Critics. London: James
Speirs.
Talbot, B. M. (1998) ‘Swedenborg’s Alleged Insanity’, The New Philosophy. Retrieved
15 November 2006, from: http://www.newphilosophyonline.org/journal/ article.
php?issue=sanity&page=1000
Taylor, M., Carlson, S. A., Maring, B. L., Gerow, L. and Charley, C. M. (2004) ‘The
Characteristics and Correlates of Fantasy in School-age Children: Imaginary
Companions, Impersonation, and Social Understanding’, Developmental Psychol-
ogy 40: 1173–87.
Tien, A. Y. (1991) ‘Distributions of Hallucinations in the Population’, Social Psychiatry
and Psychiatric Epidemiology 26: 287–92.
Toksvig, S. (1948) Emanuel Swedenborg: Scientist and Mystic. London: Faber & Faber.
Toone, B. K., Garralda, M. E. and Ron, M. A. (1982) ‘The Psychoses of Epilepsy and
the Functional Psychoses: a Clinical and Phenomenological Comparison’, British
Journal of Psychiatry 141: 256–61.
Trimble, M. and Freeman, T. (2006) ‘An Investigation of Religiosity and the Gastaut-
Geschwind Syndrome in Patients with Temporal Lobe Epilepsy’, Epilepsy &
Behavior 9: 407–14.
Vaitl, D., Birbaumer, N., Gruzelier, J., Jamieson, G. A., Kotchoubey, B., Kubler, A.
et al. (2005) ‘Psychobiology of Altered States of Consciousness’, Psychological
Bulletin 131: 98–127.
Valenti, M. P., Rudolf, G., Carre, S., Vrielynck, P., Thibault, A., Szepetowski, P. et al.
(2006) ‘Language-induced Epilepsy, Acquired Stuttering, and Idiopathic Gener-
alized Epilepsy: Phenotypic Study of One Family’, Epilepsia 47: 766–72.
Van Dusen, W. (1990) The Natural Depth in Man. New York: Swedenborg Foun-
dation.
Van Elst, L. T., Krishnamoorthy, E. S., Baumer, D., Selai, C., von Gunten, A., Gene-
Cos, N. et al. (2003) ‘Psychopathological Profile in Patients with Severe Bilateral
Hippocampal Atrophy and Temporal Lobe Epilepsy: Evidence in Support of the
Geschwind Syndrome?’, Epilepsy & Behavior 4: 291–7.
HISTORY OF THE HUMAN SCIENCES 21(1)30
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Ward, W. R. (2003) ‘Emanuel Swedenborg: Visionary Savant in the Age of Reason’,
English Historical Review 118: 515–16.
Watkins, J. (1998) Hearing Voices: a Common Human Experience. Melbourne: Hill
of Content Publishing.
Wesley, J. (1986[1770]) The Works of John Wesley, vol. 22, Journals and Diaries, V,
1765–75. Nashville, TN: Abingdon Press.
Williams-Hogan, J. K. (1988) ‘Swedenborg: a Biography’, in E. J. Brock, E. B. Glenn,
C. C. Odhner, J. D. Odhner, C. H. Walker and J. K. Williams-Hogan (eds)
Swedenborg and his Influence. Bryn Athyn, PA: Academy of the New Church.
Young, A. (1995) The Harmony of Illusions: Inventing Post-traumatic Stress Disorder.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
BIOGRAPHICAL NOTE
SIMON R. JONES is a doctoral candidate in psychology at Durham University.
His research interests include hallucinations in clinical and non-clinical
populations, and the experience of conscious volition.
CHARLES FERNYHOUGH is a developmental psychologist with a particular
interest in Vygotsky’s theory of the relation between language and thought.
In recent years he has been exploring its implications for psychosis and other
forms of psychopathology. His book, The Baby in the Mirror: A Child’s
World from Birth to Three, is forthcoming from Granta.
Address: (Corresponding author) Simon R. Jones, Department of Psychology,
Durham University, South Road, Durham DH1 3LE, UK. Tel.: +44 1913
343240. [email: s.r.jones@durham.ac.uk]
TALKING BACK TO THE SPIRITS
31
© 2008 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Palace Green Section on April 6, 2008 http://hhs.sagepub.comDownloaded from
... In some near death experiences (NDEs), independent agents either show or describe content that later proves to be veridical (Lundahl, 2001). In dreams, independent agents have been reported to show information related to spiritual themes (Jones & Fernyhough, 2008). In very rare cases to be discussed later in this article, they do both. ...
... Swedish philosopher Emmanuel Swedenborg claimed to see and hear spirits and angels during sleep and waking states. These fi gures provided him with information about 'the afterlife' (Jones & Fernyhough, 2008). Similarly, the spirit guide(s) presented in Kardec's work (2010) offer unverifi able but intelligent information about the nature of the afterlife. ...
... There are four principal types of spirits or spirit guides found in the literature: ghosts, spirits of the living during sleep or meditation (Gurney, 2010), spirits with no known connection to a living or deceased person (Haraldsson & Stevenson, 1975), and higher order spirits, such as angels or named religious fi gures (Jones & Fernyhough, 2008). Sometimes, the literature provides examples where these dream fi gures furnish veridical information about distant events (Betty, 2006;Haraldsson & Gerding, 2010) and the future (Beischel & Rock, 2009). ...
Article
Full-text available
Mystical experiences and spiritual dreams have been studied to learn how to defi ne them, who has them, and why. Rarely is the content taken seriously except as evidence of physical or psychological pathology, religious beliefs, or the infl uence of local culture. A challenge posed by spiritual dream content and mystical experiences in general, is that spiritual experiences refer exclusively to things that cannot be tested because they supposedly exist in a non-physical continuum. For this reason, spiritual or mystical content is generally described as ‘subjective’ in the literature. However, there can be some overlap with objectively veridical psi content. This research utilizes a single dataset comprised of 34 dream journals containing 12,224 dream records produced by this author over the past 27 years to explore the relationship between veridical and spiritual content in dreams. The results suggest that it is unreasonable to characterize all spiritual content as subjective when veridical secondary evidence is available. It also suggests that so-called ‘folkloric’ or ‘primitive’ explanations for dreams are more consistent with the data than modern psychological, cultural, or neurological explanations.
... The voices and visions experienced by Swedenborg remain a topic of much debate in psychiatric literature despite Swedenborg's pioneering contributions to the understanding of brain function and neural anatomy [2][3][4][5]. Jones & Fernyhough [6] offer a reconsideration of Swedenborg's experiences in relation to changes in psychiatric practice. They examine the varying conceptualizations of Swedenborg's experiences that were suggested by his contemporaries, and by psychiatrists of later generations, arguing that Swedenborg's condition cannot be considered the result of schizophrenia, epilepsy, or other typical types of mental illness. ...
... Jung was intensely interested in characterizing the movement of objects in the non-spatial psychic world that could be accessed by spiritual consciousness only, which many have referred to as "visions and voices" [6]. Jung discovered that traveling in the psychic world called the collective unconscious is distinctly different from ordinary thinking and memory. ...
Article
Full-text available
This article provides some comparative data on the frequency of occurrence of key words that can be found in the complete set of collected works of Gustav Jung (1875-1961), Emanuel Swedenborg (1688-1772), and Sigmund Freud (1856-1939). These three historically important writers have each made significant contributions to the formulation and development of dynamic and analytic psychology. These data provide a comparison of the intensity in topical focus in the works of these three writers. There is visible overlap between all three writers in the intensity of focus on certain key concepts. The overlap is greater between Jung and Swedenborg than between Freud and the other two. An interpretation of this finding is presented in terms of the attitude of each author regarding the existence of a psychic “world” that is distinct from the physical world. Freud’s individual unconscious is contrasted with Jung’s collective unconscious and Swedenborg’s collective conscious. Full text available at: http://medcraveonline.com/JPCPY/JPCPY-03-00134.pdf
... Its emphasis on positive acceptance and individual meaning-making denotes significant departures from, but also some overlaps with, conventional views espoused in psychiatry and psychology. Other examples of subcultural or religious influences on voice-hearing include the repositioning of the hallucinatory experiences of historical figures 141,142 , or studying their attribution to jinn (invisible spirits) in Islam 143 . ...
Article
Full-text available
Hearing a voice in the absence of any speaker can be a significant feature of psychiatric illness, but is also increasingly acknowledged as an important aspect of everyday, non-pathological experience. This recognition has led to a growth of interest in voice-hearing in individuals without any psychiatric diagnosis, coupled with greater attention to the subjective experience of voice-hearing across diagnostic groups. Research has also focused on the overlap between some aspects of voice-hearing phenomenology and everyday experiences such as ‘hearing’ the voices of fictional characters and spiritual experience. In this Review, we synthesize research on the range of cognitive, neural, personal and sociocultural processes that contribute to voice-hearing as it occurs in clinical, non-clinical and everyday experience, with particular emphasis on linking mechanism to phenomenology. Heterogeneous forms of voice-hearing can be understood in terms of differing patterns of association among underlying mechanisms. We suggest an approach to hallucinatory experience that sees it as partly continuous with everyday inner experience, but which is critical regarding whether continuity of phenomenology across the clinical–non-clinical divide should be taken to entail continuity of mechanism. Hearing voices has long been associated with severe mental illness but also occurs in the general population. In this Review, Toh et al. describe the cognitive, neural, personal and sociocultural processes that contribute to voice-hearing in clinical, non-clinical and everyday experience, with emphasis on linking mechanism to phenomenology.
... Woe to him who builds a city with bloodshed and establishes a town by injustice!" (Chapter 2: [11][12]. The specific features of H. Pahutiak's mythological thinking are reflected in her animistic views about stones which the writer explicates in "Sentimental Journeys through Halychyna": "They seem alive to me. ...
... Veridical (psi) content not previously known to the person that is communicated by an ostensible independent (extra-mental) agent or source has also been documented as occurring in both near-death experiences (Lundahl, 2001) and in dreams (Jones & Fernyhough, 2008) in normal people. In addition, there is a substantial body of logical argument and empirical evidence that has accumulated over the more than 140 year history of survival research (i.e., survival of the personality after the death of the body)that makes the hypothesis that discarnate survival personalities are empirically real and exist independent of the mind of the perceiver, not only possibly true, but probably true to the open-minded scientist (Almeder, 1992;Beischel & Rock, 2009;Betty, 2006;Braude, 2003;Cardeña, Palmer, & Marcusson-Clavertz, 2015, Part 7;Carter, 2012;Fontana, 2005;Gauld, 1982;Rock, 2013). ...
... When Type II normal automatisms occur, the culture or community endorses and confi rms the normal dissociator as a valued member because, to a greater or lesser degree, the dissociative person's automatism (and/or its product or outcome) is always a validation of that community. Conversely, to the extent that the local community does not validate a person's Type II Normal Dissociation, that person risks being labeled as mad or insane, despite being otherwise quite functional (e.g., Jones & Fernyhough, 2008). Type II Normal Dissociation is involuntary. ...
Thesis
Настоящият труд включва философско и научно изследване на креативността във феномена „гранична ситуация“, както е развит във философията на Карл Ясперс. Трудът е разделен на три части: 1) Въведение във философията на Карл Ясперс и концепцията за „гранична ситуация“, 2) Описание на функциите на психо-соматичния комплекс в норма и патология и техните отношения към философията и философското съзнание и 3) Изследване на креативността преди, по време и след преживяванията на гранични ситуации. Първата част е историко-философска. Втората – научна, а третата е синтез между първите две, като двете линии – философската и научната – са развити паралелно. Използваните методи са: историко-философски анализ, сравнителен анализ, концептуален анализ. Излагат се редица подстъпи към едно сериозно изследване на граничните ситуации и на ролята на креативността по отношение на методите за тяхното превъзмогване. Включени са редица изследвания в рамките на конкретни области от познанието, свързани с ядрената тематика труда, като: понятието „креативност“, епилепсията на Достоевски, животът на Е. Сведенборг, фармакометафизиката, депресивно-песимистичния реализъм, психичните защити по А. и З. Фройд, понятието „Аз“, логотерапията на В. Франкъл, психозомиметиците и др. Фокусът е върху способите за превъзмогване на граничните ситуации, в частност каква роля играе креативността при тях. Предлага се богата съвкупност от перспективи за бъдещи изследвания в горепосочените области и проблемни точки. Глава 1: Понятието „гранична ситуация“ в екзистенциалната философия на Карл Ясперс В тази глава се анализира понятието „гранична ситуация“ в рамките на философията на Карл Ясперс или т. нар. Existenzphilosophie. Става въпрос не за вид екзистенциална философия, а за философия на Existenz. Разгледани са в детайли основните понятия на Existenzphilosophie, а именно: Всеобхватно, Existenz, Transcendenz, Разум, Операцията „трансцендиране“, Крушение и Философска вяра. Понятието „гранична ситуация“ е дискутирано, от тази перспектива, в следните главни точки: Същност, Кой попада в гранична ситуация?, Методи за превъзмогване (вж. Глава 3), Гранични ситуации и психопатология, Видове гранични ситуации и Навлизане в гранични ситуации. Разгледано е и понятието „мистично преживяване“ в рамките на Ясперсовата Existenzphilosophie. Приведени са голямо количество цитати от основните трудове на Ясперс (от преводите от немски на английски език), които не са преведени на български език и които сами по себе си представляват една миниатюрна антология на Ясперсовата Existenzphilosophie. Глава 2: Науката на психо-соматичния комплекс срещу философското съзнание В тази глава изложението включва главно дискусията на научни понятия и концепции, но са дадени и редица философски такива. Ясперс е считал, че за философстващия са от ключово значение наличните обширни научни познания. Това е така поради факта, че самата Existenzphilosophie или философската светова ориентация, така да се каже, надгражда научния светоглед или т. нар. от Ясперс научна светова ориентация. Поради този факт, главата започва с дискусия върху отношението, взаимовръзката и взаимоотношението между науката и философията. След това в детайли е анализирано понятието „съзнание“, както от научна, така и от философска перспектива. На читателя се предоставя и кратка научна дискусия върху функционалността на човешкото съзнание и човешката психика в норма и в патология; тук е включено и изложение върху т. нар. механизми за психична защита по А. и З. Фройд. Главата завършва с кратко изложение на главните постановки в когнитивната наука, вкл. и афективната наука: възприятие, познание, емоции и деятелност. Включено е и изложение, което представлява кратко въведение в основите на функционалната невроанатомия, както и основните принципи на невро- и психофармакологията. Емоционалността или афективността е разгледана и от философска перспектива в лицето на т. нар. екзистенциални чувства. Главата завършва с кратко, но богато изложение върху психологията и философията на креативността. Глава 3: Динамика на креативността във феномена „гранична ситуация“ Тази глава се базира на Глава 1 и Глава 2, като представлява синтез между двете глави. Фокусът е върху динамиката на креативността, която се анализира в рамките на преживяванията на гранични ситуации. Приведен е списък с възможните методи за превъзмогване на граничните ситуации, който е далеч по-пълен от този, представен в съответната секция в Глава 1. Разгледан е случаят със SARS-CoV-2 като актуална световна гранична ситуация. Впоследствие се въвежда понятието „миниатюра на гранична ситуация“, което представлява смес между креативността като процес (и черта) и граничните ситуации – самата креативност се разглежда като миниатюра на гранична ситуация. Към всичко това, ние сме привели и следните дискусии (като вид практическо приложение на знанието за граничните ситуации): Логотерапията на Виктор Франкъл (и изобщо екзистенциалната психотерапия) като метод за превъзмогване на граничните ситуации, Феноменология на психеделичното състояние (като метод за превъзмогване на гранични ситуации и като метод за индуциране на гранични ситуации, респ. мистични преживявания), Видовете светогледи и граничните ситуации – в частност т. нар. от нас Депресивно-песимистичен реализъм, Философията на човешкото когнитивно фармакологично подобрение – Фармакометафизика и граничните ситуации, Екстазната аура като мистично преживяване при епилепсията на Достоевски и граничните ситуации (вж. секцията за мистичното преживяване в Глава 1), Метафизиката на душата като парафрения на психиката – случаят на Емануел Сведенборг – граничните ситуации и обективацията на езика на Трансценденцията.
Article
Full-text available
The history of mankind comprises many unique cases of combinations of exceptional characteristics in one and the same individual. Most often, the combinations include positive as well as negative characteristics. The combinations are almost entirely random as they depend on too many variables, both endogenous and exogenous. In the current paper, a description of the personality of the scholar Emanuel Swedenborg, a truly unique personality in all investigational aspects, is offered. A psychiatric evaluation of the personality of Swedenborg is performed and it is concluded that his life is an example of a psyche, which has developed paraphrenia, i.e. a late-onset or involutional schizophrenia. Many of the fundamental moments from the writings of Swedenborg are quoted in the paper and a philosophical as well a as neurological-reductionistic analysis are performed. Attention is directed to two other cases involving paraphrenization of the psyche. The metaphysical world of the soul is revealed as a psychopathology of the psyche, an idiosyncratic paraphrenization of the inner world of man.
Article
Full-text available
There are many historical examples of people who heard voices or saw visions but were not classified as having a mental illness and who were supported by a religious community. The article offers a perspective for effective psychosocial supports for schizophrenia. The author analyzes data on 95 people who experienced verifiable persistent non-drug-assisted hallucinations in Europe, North America, and Australasia and discusses the life outcomes of 39 subjects. They include founders of religions, dysfunctional monarchs, persons with cosmological beliefs, and mental health workers. Their psychoses were intrinsic to their personalities and contributions. Hallucinations generated by psychosis were useful for cultural innovation, particularly in religion as many hallucinators were integrated into church history. Community, work, friendship, and supportive practices are discussed. A scientific study of effective psychosocial support to supplement medication for schizophrenia is outlined.
Article
Julia Ward Howe, c. 1887. Engraving by C. A. Powell, 34.7 x 24.8 cm, from a photograph by J. J. Hawes. From The Century Gallery of One Hundred Portraits Selected from "The Century Magazine" (New York: The Century Co., [1897]), plate no. 26. Courtesy of the Library of Congress Prints and Photographs Division, LC-USZ62-8221. In his 1768 exposition Conjugial Love, Emanuel Swedenborg (1688-1772) sketches the prospect of heavenly marriage, detailing an extended quest for monogamous union in the afterlife. Swedenborg theorizes that husbands and wives meet again after death to test the staying power of their earthly bonds. If they prove incompatible, they divorce and search for new, more suitable partners. In Swedenborg's heaven, the unattached spirit chases the transfigurative force of conjugial love, a force so strong that it ultimately collapses sexual difference and produces intersexed angels made one body by divine coupling. As he rhapsodizes about celestial unities, Swedenborg lets fly an acerbic critique of earthbound marriage, cataloguing an ambitious list of legitimate causes for separation, including lunacy, intemperance, intractability, deception, indiscretion, impropriety, venereal disease, and impotence. Although Swedenborg maintains that adultery is the only justifiable reason for civil dissolution, he acknowledges that mortal unions are often contentious, sorrowful, and unsatisfying. Swedenborg's conceptions of love and sex reverberate throughout nineteenth-century spiritualist understandings of marriage. Following Swedenborg, spiritualists "viewed conjugal love as an exalted ideal and appropriate sexual feelings as part of the natural order that drew affinities together." The free-love movement turned the Swedenborgian doctrine of celestial attraction into a moral barometer, so that sex within marriages that lacked the sanction of conjugial feeling were judged illicit. If low or libidinal impulses propel a partnership, free lovers argued, husband and wife should live platonically or divorce. Like Swedenborg, spiritualists prioritized soulful couplings and spiritual affections, deriding the power imbalances, pecuniary exploitation, and sexual violations that corrupt earthly marriage. Julia Ward Howe—poet, abolitionist, suffragette, and one of Margaret Fuller's first biographers—records her reading of Swedenborg in Reminiscences, 1819-1899 (published in 1899). Howe lists Conjugial Love among the writings that interest her most and says she is "much fascinated by [Swedenborg's] theories of spiritual life." Gary Williams notes that "her letters show she was already something of an authority on his work by 1847." For Howe, Swedenborg's depiction of divisive couplings would have resonated with a painful, proximate reality. Julia's marriage to Samuel Gridley Howe was tumultuous and strained from its beginning. According to Williams, Samuel's abiding romantic attachment to Charles Sumner immediately triangulated the marriage, and Julia's domestic inexperience, her isolation in their Boston home, and Samuel's immersion in his work fueled tensions between them. Moreover, Samuel was chronically contrary about Julia's literary efforts and infuriated by the 1854 publication of Passion-Flowers. Early on, Samuel told his wife he dreamed of marrying again, "'some young girl who would love him supremely,'" and he took a mistress decades later. In 1846, three years into the marriage, with two of her eventual six children just out of their infancy, Howe began writing an untitled novel Williams subsequently called The Hermaphrodite. Reading the never-finished text as an "encoded autobiography," Williams (who first brought it to print in 2004) argues that the closeted manuscript enabled Howe to process her volatile union. Conceived as a first-person narrative, the novel chronicles the family history, education, and star-crossed loves of its intersexed protagonist, Laurence. Williams recognizes Swedenborgian influences on Howe's vision of Laurence, suggesting that he might be "understood as an earthly incarnation of a Swedenborgian hermaphroditic angel." The manuscript stages Howe's creative engagement with Swedenborg's theories in other ways as well, especially in two interrelated narratives of spiritualized love. Topically, this essay centers primarily on a section of the manuscript that details Laurence's sojourn in Rome as the welcome guest of an aristocratic bachelor tutor, Berto, and his sisters, Briseida, Gigia, and Nina. In this interval, Laurence makes an ethnographic study of Italian gender while he masquerades as "Cecilia" and grows increasingly fascinated with Berto's youngest sister, Nina. Nina exists in a...
Article
The value of the notion of 'indeterminacy in the past' continues to be contested. Ian Hacking's claim that the notion is perspicuous in the examination of historical instances is questioned through discussion of the possibility of retrospective application of the relatively recent diagnostic category Post-traumatic stress disorder'. Kevin McMillan maintains that there are deeper philosophical merits to the idea - particularly with respect to questions of truth - but neither Hacking's treatment of historical cases nor McMillan's directly philosophical elaboration of Hacking's position sustain this claim.
Article
Jaynes' elaborate theory of the evolution of human consciousness speculates that unconscious language use by the right hemisphere produced frequent auditory hallucinations in primitive people [1]. Jaynes offers some explanation as to why hearing voices would now be less common. It is parsimonious, however, to predict that hearing voices is still common, although usually unreported, in the modern normal population. Some clinical literature gives support to this prediction. This study tested the prediction by means of surveying 375 college students with a two-part questionnaire. The first section presented fourteen different examples of auditory hallucinations and asked whether the subject had experienced such occurrences. The second section asked for information concerning the characteristics of any hallucinated voices and for information about the subject that might relate to cerebral laterality. The results support the prediction that hearing voices is common within the normal population. Overall, 71 percent of the sample reported some experience with brief, auditory hallucinations of the voice type in wakeful situations. Hypnagogic and hypnopompic hallucinations were also reported. The most frequent incidents were hearing a voice call one's name aloud when alone (36%) and hearing one's thoughts as if spoken aloud (39%). Interviews and MMPI results obtained from twenty selected subjects suggested that these reports of hearing voices were not related to pathology. Further findings of a significant relationship between high rates of auditory hallucinations and the extent to which subjects reported skills in music, art, and poetry were interpreted as weak support for Jaynes' speculation that right hemisphere activity may account for auditory hallucinations. Overall, the results are seen as supportive of several of Jaynes' theoretical points.