ArticlePDF Available

Urinary behavior of female domestic dogs (Canis familiaris): Influence of reproductive status, location, and age

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The urinary behavior of adult domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) is sexually dimorphic with respect to the posture (males lift a leg and females squat), frequency of urination (males urinate more frequently than females), and tendency to direct urine at specific objects in the environment (males are more likely than females to direct their urine). Such behavioral differences have led to the belief that urination functions largely, or exclusively, in elimination in female dogs, while having the additional function of scent marking in male dogs. In this study, we observed urinary behavior of six spayed and six non-estrous intact female Jack Russell Terriers during walks on and off their home area. The females ranged in age from 0.4 to 11.2 years. Frequency of urination was positively correlated with age, and females four or more years old directed the majority of their urinations at objects in the environment. Overall, females urinated more frequently and directed more of their urinations when walked off their home area than when walked within their home area. Spayed females were more likely than non-estrous intact females to ground-scratch following defecation; we detected a similar trend for ground-scratching after urination. There was, however, considerable variation among spayed females in the tendency to display ground-scratching behavior. Overall, the most common posture displayed by females while urinating was the squat-raise. Other postures, in order of their frequency of occurrence included squat, arch-raise, combination, and handstand. Females used the squat-raise and arch-raise postures more when off their home area than when on their home area. Overall, there was substantial individual variation among females in the postures used while urinating. Our data indicate that female urinary behavior varies with location and reproductive status, and that substantial individual differences exist among females for some patterns of behavior. Additionally, the large percentages of directed urinations by spayed (60.8%) and non-estrous intact females (56.7%) in our study suggest that urination in female dogs does not function solely in elimination, but that it also has a significant role in scent marking, even when females are not in estrus.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Applied Animal Behaviour Science 85 (2004) 335–348
Urinary behavior of female domestic
dogs (Canis familiaris): influence of
reproductive status, location,
and age
Sharon Cudd Wirant, Betty McGuire
Department of Biological Sciences, Clark Science Center, Smith College,
Northampton, MA 01063, USA
Accepted 26 September 2003
Abstract
The urinary behavior of adult domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) is sexually dimorphic with respect
to the posture (males lift a leg and females squat), frequency of urination (males urinate more fre-
quently than females), and tendency to direct urine at specific objects in the environment (males
are more likely than females to direct their urine). Such behavioral differences have led to the be-
lief that urination functions largely, or exclusively, in elimination in female dogs, while having the
additional function of scent marking in male dogs. In this study, we observed urinary behavior of
six spayed and six non-estrous intact female Jack Russell Terriers during walks on and off their
home area. The females ranged in age from 0.4 to 11.2 years. Frequency of urination was positively
correlated with age, and females four or more years old directed the majority of their urinations at
objects in the environment. Overall, females urinated more frequently and directed more of their
urinations when walked off their home area than when walked within their home area. Spayed fe-
males were more likely than non-estrous intact females to ground-scratch following defecation; we
detected a similar trend for ground-scratching after urination. There was, however, considerable vari-
ation among spayed females in the tendency to display ground-scratching behavior. Overall, the
most common posture displayed by females while urinating was the squat-raise. Other postures, in
order of their frequency of occurrence included squat, arch-raise, combination, and handstand. Fe-
males used the squat-raise and arch-raise postures more when off their home area than when on their
home area. Overall, there was substantial individual variation among females in the postures used
while urinating. Our data indicate that female urinary behavior varies with location and reproductive
status, and that substantial individual differences exist among females for some patterns of behav-
ior. Additionally, the large percentages of directed urinations by spayed (60.8%) and non-estrous
intact females (56.7%) in our study suggest that urination in female dogs does not function solely in
Corresponding author. Tel.: +1-413-585-3758; fax: +1-413-585-3786.
E-mail address: bmcguire@science.smith.edu (B. McGuire).
0168-1591/$ – see front matter © 2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2003.09.012
336 S.C. Wirant, B. McGuire /Applied Animal Behaviour Science 85 (2004) 335–348
elimination, but that it also has a significant role in scent marking, even when females are not in
estrus.
© 2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Dog; Urine marking; Urinary postures; Scent marking; Urinary behavior
1. Introduction
Urinary behavior in domestic dogs is sexually dimorphic (Sprague and Anisko, 1973;
Beach, 1974; Ranson and Beach, 1985). Adult males urinate more frequently than do
females, and are more likely than females to direct their urine at specific targets (Martins and
Valle, 1948; Bekoff, 1979a; Ranson and Beach, 1985). In addition, adult males typically
raise a hind leg to urinate while adult females usually squat (Martins and Valle, 1948;
Beach, 1974). While urination clearly functions in elimination for both sexes, its role in
scent marking is thought to be larger in male dogs than in female dogs (Martins and Valle,
1948; Bekoff, 1979a; Pal, 2003). Indeed, Kleiman (1966) suggested that female dogs mark
with urine only around the time they are in estrus.
A major focus of previous research has been on the development of these sex differences
in canine urinary behavior. Such research has indicated that sex steroid hormones around
the time of birth organize the adult male urinary posture, but are not required to activate
the posture in adulthood (Berg, 1944; Martins and Valle, 1948; Beach, 1974; Ranson and
Beach, 1985). These studies focused on the urinary behavior of male dogs, and typically
included small numbers of females that were treated with testosterone at various times in
development; even smaller numbers of untreated females served as controls (Berg, 1944;
Martins and Valle, 1948; Beach, 1974; Ranson, 1981; Ranson and Beach, 1985). Few studies
have investigated urinary behavior of free-ranging male and female dogs (Bekoff, 1979a;
Pal, 2003). Thus, although urinary behavior has been well-studied in male dogs, relatively
little is known about such behavior in female dogs.
The purpose of this study was to examine the urinary behavior of female dogs in relation
to reproductive status (spayed or non-estrous intact) and location (on or off home area).
With respect to reproductive status, our goal was to examine whether the simple presence
of gonadal hormones influenced urinary behavior; we did not compare behavior across the
estrous cycle in this study. Finally, we observed females of different ages, and thus were
able to provide preliminary data on age-related patterns of urinary behavior in female dogs.
2. Methods
We observed urinary behavior of 12 female Jack Russell Terriers (Table 1). Females
ranged in age from 0.4 to 11.2 years and were privately owned. The dogs were housed either
in the homes of their owners or in kennels outside the homes. Six of the females were spayed
and the other six were intact. We classified the six intact females as “non-estrous” during
the period of observation. This classification was based on owner reports for each female of
all available start dates of proestrus (first day of blood-tinged vaginal discharge) and estrus
S.C. Wirant, B. McGuire /Applied Animal Behaviour Science 85 (2004) 335–348 337
Table 1
Characteristics of the 12 female Jack Russell Terriers observed for urinary behavior
SubjectaAge
(years)
Parity ObservationsbStart date for last proestrus
before observationsc
Ownerd
S1 11.2 1 18–25 March A
S2 6.5 0 22 February–19 April B
S3 5.6 1 18–25 March C
S4 3.1 2 18–25 March C
S5 2.3 0 18–25 March D
S6 1.3 0 22 February–5 April B
I1 7.0 2 18–25 March 20 February 2002 A
I2 4.2 1 18–25 March 16 October 2001 C
I3 4.2 2 18–25 March 16 February 2002 A
I4 1.3 0 18–25 March 18 February 2002 A
I5 1.3 0 18–25 March 30 September 2001 A
I6 0.4 0 18–25 March A
aReproductive status is indicated in the subject name; S =spayed, I =non-estrous intact.
bAll testing occurred in 2002; females were observed during 14 walks within the time period shown (seven
walks on their home area and seven walks off their home area).
cDate represents first day of blood-tinged vaginal discharge. The next period of proestrus for I2 began on 28
April 2002 and for I5 on 13 May 2002.
dOwners and housing of dogs: A =Farmcliff Kennels (mix of kennel housing and home of owner); B =
Sharon Cudd Wirant (home of owner); C =Highwater Kennels (home of owner); D =Valerie Whiterock (home
of owner).
(first day receptive to male), as well as the length of estrus (first to last day receptive to
male). Owner reports indicated that none of the intact females was in proestrus at the time
we observed them (Table 1) and that two females (I1 and I4) had just completed estrus a few
days before observations began. More specifically, female I1 was judged nonreceptive by
her owners on 12 March 2002 and female I4 on 13 March 2002; we began our observations
of these females on 18 March 2002. Dogs within homes or kennels regularly interacted with
one another. Outside of this study, all of the dogs participated in performance sports (e.g.,
conformation, hunting, agility, flyball) and thus dogs from different homes and kennels had
occasionally interacted with one another at such events. Some of the dogs were also used
for breeding purposes, though not during the period of this study.
All observations occurred during individual walks of the dogs between 22 February and
19 April 2002 (Table 1). Each dog was taken by the first author (SCW) for 14 walks on
an 8.3 m leash (each walk was 15 min; total hours of observation across all dogs =42).
During walks, the dogs were allowed to freely investigate areas and were not pulled along
by SCW; the pace of the walk was set by the dog. Seven of the 14 walks occurred within
the dog’s home area (area in which the dog eliminates on a daily basis) and seven occurred
outside the dog’s home area. Each walk outside the dog’s home area occurred at a different
location. The latter areas were those in which the dog either had never been walked or had
been walked at most two times in the preceding year. We observed each dog once or twice a
day; a minimum of 4 h elapsed between walks on the same day. For 10 dogs, all observations
occurred on consecutive days, and for the remaining two dogs some, but not all, observations
occurred on consecutive days. This difference was due to subject availability. In all cases,
338 S.C. Wirant, B. McGuire /Applied Animal Behaviour Science 85 (2004) 335–348
the order of walks on and off home area was counterbalanced across days. For example, if
a dog was walked on its home area in the a.m. and off its home area in the p.m. 1 day, then
it was walked off its home area in the a.m. and on its home area in the p.m. during the next
day of observation.
During each 15 min walk we recorded frequencies of occurrence for the following patterns
of behavior: (1) urinating, (2) defecating, and (3) sniffing (female stopped walking and
inspected the ground or an object with her nose). We also noted whether urinations or
defecations were followed by ground-scratching (backward scraping of the ground with
the front feet, hind feet, or both feet; Fig. 1A) and whether urinations were directed at
an object in the environment (urine directed within 20 cm of an obvious target, such as a
tree, fence post, or clump of grass or leaves; the criterion of 20 cm was used by Ranson
and Beach, 1985). Finally, we recorded the posture(s) used by females when urinating:
squat, squat-raise, handstand, and arch-raise (Fig. 1B–E;Sprague and Anisko, 1973). We
Fig. 1. Urinary behavior and postures of female Jack Russell Terriers: (A) ground-scratching; (B) squat; (C)
squat-raise; (D) handstand; (E) arch-raise; and (F through H) the combination posture squat-raise into handstand
into squat-raise.
S.C. Wirant, B. McGuire /Applied Animal Behaviour Science 85 (2004) 335–348 339
occasionally observed females using two or three postures in sequence during a single
urination. For example, some females would begin urinating in the squat-raise posture and
then move into a handstand and then back into the squat-raise with no pause in urinating
(Fig. 1F–H). We described these occurrences collectively as “combination postures”, after
Ranson’s (1981) classification of serial combinations of postures in beagles as the “combo.”
For each female, we calculated a mean frequency of urinating, defecating, and sniffing
over the seven trials on home area and the seven trials off home area, and used these means
in statistical analyses. We analyzed the mean frequencies of urinating, defecating, and
sniffing with a two-way mixed ANOVA with reproductive status (spayed or non-estrous
intact) as the between-subjects variable and location (on home area or off home area) as
the within-subjects variable. We plotted several of our dependent variables against age
and ran either linear or nonlinear regression. For the variables measured (in percentage)
as proportions (directed urinations, urinations followed by ground-scratching, defecations
followed by ground-scratching, and urinations using specific postures), we plotted on home
area values against off home area values for each dog to examine patterns with respect to
location. In many cases, we calculated for each female a new variable “difference”, where
difference =her value for a particular behavior off home area (e.g., average number of
urinations or percentage directed urinations) minus her value for that behavior on home area.
Using “difference” as our dependent variable, we then ran ANCOVA with age, reproductive
status, and the interaction between age and reproductive status as explanatory variables.
We first examined models with all three explanatory variables, then models with age and
reproductive status, and finally models with either age or reproductive status. For subsequent
comparisons of spayed and non-estrous intact females we used the Mann–Whitney U-test
and for subsequent comparisons of behavior off and on home area we used Wilcoxon’s
Signed-Ranks Test (one-sample, paired observations). We used either BMDP 7.0 (BMDP
Statistical Software, Inc., Los Angeles, CA, USA) or Minitab 13.32 (Minitab, Inc., State
College, PA, USA) for statistical analyses.
3. Results
Overall, females urinated more frequently when off their home area than when on their
home area (Table 2;F=8.54, d.f.=1, 10, P=0.02); we did not detect an effect of
reproductive status (F=0.37, d.f.=1, 10, P=0.55) or a reproductive status by location
interaction for this behavior (F=2.17, d.f.=1, 10, P=0.17). We ran ANCOVA with
mean frequency of urination as our dependent variable and age, reproductive status, and the
interaction between age and reproductive status as explanatory variables; neither the inter-
action nor reproductive status was a significant predictor of mean frequency of urination.
However, age was a significant predictor of mean frequency of urination (F=12.45, d.f.=
1, 10, P=0.005, R2=0.54; Fig. 2A). We explored the possibility that age should be used
as a covariate in the ANOVA by calculating the variable “difference” for each female, where
difference =average number of urinations off home area minus the average number of uri-
nations on home area. Using “difference” as our dependent variable, we then ran ANCOVA
with the variables age, reproductive status, and the interaction between age and reproductive
status. In no model was age a significant predictor of the dependent variable “difference.”
340 S.C. Wirant, B. McGuire /Applied Animal Behaviour Science 85 (2004) 335–348
Table 2
Frequency of urinating, defecating, and sniffing in 12 female Jack Russell Terriers in relation to reproductive status
and location walked (n=6 spayed females; n=6 non-estrous intact females)
Reproductive status Location walked UrinateaDefecatebSniffc
Spayed On home area 3.60 ±1.19 0.60 ±0.14 20.74 ±1.47
Off home area 5.70 ±1.47 0.76 ±0.14 23.64 ±1.93
Non-estrous intact On home area 3.36 ±0.90 0.36 ±0.09 19.67 ±2.19
Off home area 4.05 ±0.91 0.52 ±0.09 20.95 ±1.77
Values represent mean frequency of occurrence per 15 min walk ±S.E.M.
aSignificant effect of location (P=0.02).
bNo significant effects.
cNo significant effects.
We found no significant effects for defecating or sniffing. The frequency of defecating
did not vary with respect to reproductive status (F=3.60, d.f.=1, 10, P=0.09) or
location (F=2.39, d.f.=1, 10, P=0.15), and we did not detect a reproductive status
by location interaction for this behavior (F=0.00, d.f.=1, 10, P=0.99; Table 2). The
frequency of sniffing did not vary with respect to reproductive status (F=0.78, d.f.=1,
10, P=0.40) or location (F=1.87, d.f.=1, 10, P=0.20), and we did not detect a
reproductive status by location interaction (F=0.28, d.f.=1, 10, P=0.61; Table 2).
Age was not a significant predictor of either mean frequency of defecating (F=0.09,
d.f.=1, 10, P=0.77, R2=0.01) or mean frequency of sniffing (F=3.17, d.f.=1, 10,
P=0.11, R2=0.24).
Of the 390 urinations by spayed females, 237 (60.8%) were directed at objects in the
environment. Similarly, of the 312 urinations by non-estrous intact females, 177 (56.7%)
were directed at objects. The overall relationship between percentage of total directed
urinations and age was nonlinear (Fig. 2B). In general, females four or more years old
directed the majority (70–80%) of their urinations while younger females did not. We fit
the following exponential model to the data: Co(1exp(at)) where the asymptote Co=
92.43, a=0.22, and t=age; R2=0.77. With the exception of the youngest female (I6)
who did not direct any of her urinations, all other females directed more of their urinations
when off their home area than when on their home area (Fig. 2C). We examined whether this
pattern was significant by first calculating “difference” for each female, where difference =
percentage directed urinations off home area minus percentage directed urinations on home
area. We then used Wilcoxon’s Signed-Ranks Test to test the null hypothesis that the median
“difference” =0.00. We were able to reject the null hypothesis (P=0.004). Finally, using
“difference” as our dependent variable, we then ran ANCOVA with the variables age,
reproductive status, and the interaction between age and reproductive status. None of the
explanatory variables was a significant predictor of “difference.”
We analyzed ground-scratching after urinating and after defecating separately. No clear
pattern with respect to location emerged from our plot of percentage urinations followed
by ground-scratching on home area against percentage urinations followed by ground-
scratching off home area. We calculated “difference”, where difference =percentage uri-
nations off home area followed by ground-scratching minus percentage urinations on home
S.C. Wirant, B. McGuire /Applied Animal Behaviour Science 85 (2004) 335–348 341
Fig. 2. Age and location-related patterns in the urinary behavior of 12 female Jack Russell Terriers. (A) Mean
frequency of urination was positively correlated with age (R2=0.54, P=0.005). (B) The relationship be-
tween percentage directed urinations and age was nonlinear and could be represented by an exponential model
(R2=0.77). (C) Females directed more of their urinations when off their home area than when on their home
area (Wilcoxon’s Signed-Ranks Test, P=0.004). Dashed line represents y=x; data points above the dashed
line signify females that directed a larger percentage of their urinations when off their home area than when on
their home area.
342 S.C. Wirant, B. McGuire /Applied Animal Behaviour Science 85 (2004) 335–348
area followed by ground-scratching. We ran ANCOVA with the variables age, reproductive
status, and the interaction between age and reproductive status. None of the explanatory
variables was a significant predictor of “difference.” We then ignored the variable location
and ran the ANCOVA with percentage total urinations followed by ground-scratching as
our dependent variable; reproductive status was a significant predictor of percentage total
urinations followed by ground-scratching (F=7.65, d.f.=1, 10, P=0.02, R2=0.43).
On average (±S.E.), spayed females ground-scratched following 30.2±9.8% of their uri-
nations and non-estrous intact females did so after 2.4±1.9% of their urinations. The differ-
ence between spayed and non-estrous intact females in percentage urinations followed by
ground-scratching failed to reach statistical significance (Mann–Whitney U-test, P=0.06).
The results for ground-scratching after defecating were similar to those for ground-
scratching after urinating. Again, no clear pattern emerged with respect to location and
none of the explanatory variables (age, reproductive status, interaction) was a significant
predictor of “difference” in the ANCOVA. When we ignored the variable location and ran
the ANCOVA with total percentage of defecations followed by ground-scratching as our
dependent variable, we found that reproductive status was a significant predictor of the total
defecations percentage followed by ground-scratching (F=10.31, d.f.=1, 10, P=0.01,
R2=0.51). On average, spayed females ground-scratched following 49.7±13.0% of their
defecations and non-estrous intact females did so after 6.3±4.3% of their defecations.
The difference between spayed and non-estrous intact females in percentage of defecations
followed by ground-scratching was significant (Mann–Whitney U-test, P=0.03).
Overall, squat-raise was the most common urinary posture displayed by females (55.1%;
387/702). The other postures, in order of their frequency of occurrence, included squat
(18.4%; 129/702), arch-raise (15.5%; 109/702), combination postures (10.0%; 70/702),
and handstand (1.0%; 7/702). We examined whether any pattern emerged with respect
to location and urinary posture by plotting for each posture the urinations percentage in
which the posture was used on home area against the urinations percentage in which the
posture was used off home area. Some females used squat-raise and arch-raise more when
urinating off their home area than when urinating on their home area (Fig. 3A and B).
We examined whether these patterns were significant by first calculating “difference” for
each female, where difference =percentage urinations off home area in which the female
used the posture minus percentage urinations on home area in which the female used the
posture. We then used Wilcoxon’s Signed-Ranks Test to test the null hypothesis that the
median “difference” =0.00. We were able to reject the null hypothesis for squat-raise
(P=0.03) and for arch-raise (P=0.02); we were unable to reject the null hypothesis for
Fig. 3. Location-related patterns in the urinary postures of 12 female Jack Russell Terriers. In each panel, the
dashed line represents y=x; data points above the dashed line signify females that used the particular posture
more when off their home area than when on their home area. (A) Females used the squat-raise posture more when
urinating off their home area than when urinating on their home area (Wilcoxon’s Signed-Ranks Test, P=0.03).
(B) Females used the arch-raise posture more when urinating off their home area than when urinating on their home
area (Wilcoxon’s Signed-Ranks Test, P=0.02). Two spayed females and four non-estrous intact females never
displayed this posture; see numbers in parentheses and arrow. (C) There was no pattern with respect to location
for combination postures (Wilcoxon’s Signed-Ranks Test, P=0.71). Four spayed females and one non-estrous
intact female never displayed combination postures.
S.C. Wirant, B. McGuire /Applied Animal Behaviour Science 85 (2004) 335–348 343
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
0 20 40 60 80 10010 30 50 70 90
Percent of Combinations Off Home Area
Percent of Combinations On Home Area
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
0 20 40 60 80 10010 30 50 70 90
Percent of Arch-Raises Off Home Area
Percent of Arch-Raises On Home Area
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
0 20 40 60 80 10010 30 50 70 90
Percent of Squat-Raises Off Home Area
Percent of Squat-Raises On Home Area
Spayed
Non-estrous intact
(2, 4)
(4, 1)
(A)
(B)
(C)
344 S.C. Wirant, B. McGuire /Applied Animal Behaviour Science 85 (2004) 335–348
squat (P=0.15), handstand (P=0.18), and combination postures (P=0.71; Fig. 3C).
Finally, using “difference” as our dependent variable, we ran ANCOVA for each of the
five postures with the variables age, reproductive status, and the interaction between age
and reproductive status. None of the explanatory variables was a significant predictor of
“difference” for any posture.
As can be seen from the three examples depicted in Fig. 3, individual females varied
in the postures displayed while urinating. For example, although squat-raise was the most
common posture displayed by females, there were two females (S5 and I6) that never
displayed this posture (Fig. 3A). Similarly, one female (I3) frequently used combination
postures (particularly the squat-raise into handstand into squat-raise) whereas most females
rarely performed combination postures (Fig. 3C).
4. Discussion
In our study, some patterns of behavior differed with age of females. Older females
urinated more frequently than did younger females, and females four or more years old
directed the majority of their urinations whereas younger females did not. Ranson and
Beach (1985) conducted a longitudinal study of the development of urinary behavior in
young beagles. These authors reported a marked increase from 4 to 14 months of age in
frequency of urination and percentage of directed urinations in males, and relatively small
increases in these behaviors in females. Our study, though cross-sectional in nature, suggests
that frequency of urination continues to increase in adult female dogs as they age, and that
this increase occurs in a linear fashion. We found a different age-related pattern for directing
urinations; directed urinations reached 70–80% by about 4 years of age and then this level
characterized all females older than 4 years. Ranson and Beach (1985) found substantial
increases from 4 to 14 months of age in investigatory behavior of males (measured as
the frequency with which they sniffed posts within the observation pen), and only slight
increases in this behavior in females. We did not find increases in the frequency of sniffing
with age. Although Ranson and Beach (1985) did not study frequency of defecation with
respect to age in beagles, studies with coyotes (Gese and Ruff, 1997) and wolves (Peters and
Mech, 1975) have indicated that rates of defecation do not differ between pups and adults.
Similarly, we found no age-related differences with respect to frequency of defecation. The
age-related data presented here are preliminary. Ideally, in future studies we would confirm
the age-related patterns we found for urination frequency and directing urine by studying a
number of females at each age and by conducting a long-term longitudinal study.
Female dogs in our study urinated more frequently and directed more of their urinations
when off their home area than when within their home area. They also used the squat-raise
and arch-raise urinary postures more when off their home area than when within their home
area. Sprague and Anisko (1973) found that adult male beagles directed their urine toward
the scent of other males or vertical objects while adult females urinated at random locations
within a relatively small pen. Nevertheless, location has been found to strongly influence
male and female urinary behavior of free-ranging dogs and other species of canids, though
no study is strictly comparable to our study. Free-ranging male and female dogs exhibited
higher rates of marking (defined as urinations directed at a target in the environment) in
S.C. Wirant, B. McGuire /Applied Animal Behaviour Science 85 (2004) 335–348 345
areas in which they spent the least amount of time (Bekoff, 1979a). Pal (2003) found that
free-ranging male dogs marked near territory boundaries whereas females marked most
frequently at nest sites. Wolves performed raised leg urinations more commonly at the
edges of their territory than at the center (Peters and Mech, 1975). Gese and Ruff (1997)
observed that adult coyotes urinated more frequently than expected along the periphery of
their territory as compared to the interior, and Allen et al. (1999) found a preponderance of
coyote scent marks at the periphery of territories. Additionally, rates of raised leg urinations
by coyotes were highest in areas of frequent intrusion (Wells and Bekoff, 1981). Location
did not influence the frequency of defecation in our study. Similarly, frequency of defecation
by coyotes did not differ between the periphery and interior of their territory (Gese and Ruff,
1997). Finally, we found that location did not influence the likelihood of ground-scratching
following urination or defecation. In coyotes, ground-scratching has been found to occur
randomly within territories (Wells and Bekoff, 1981) or more frequently at the periphery
relative to the interior (Bowen and Cowan, 1980; Gese and Ruff, 1997; Allen et al., 1999).
For the most part, the behavior of spayed and non-estrous intact females in our study was
similar. Spayed and non-estrous intact females urinated, defecated, and sniffed at similar
frequencies and did not differ in the percentage of directed urinations. We found a substantial
difference between the two groups of females in ground-scratching. Specifically, spayed
females ground-scratched more frequently than did non-estrous intact females following
defecation and there was a similar trend for ground-scratching following urination. To
our knowledge, only one previous study of dog urinary behavior explicitly compared the
behavior of spayed and intact females that were not manipulated in some way through
the injection of hormones. Martins and Valle (1948) found no differences between spayed
and intact females in urination frequency or posture. Unfortunately, these authors did not
monitor ground-scratching.
In dogs and other species of canids, ground-scratching may serve as a visual signal and
as a chemical signal (Peters and Mech, 1975; Bekoff, 1979b; Bekoff and Wells, 1986). The
visual component may involve the act of ground-scratching or the marks left on the ground
by such scratching (Kleiman and Eisenberg, 1973; Bekoff, 1979b). The chemical component
may involve either scent deposited from interdigital glands or the dispersion of olfactory
cues from deposited urine or feces (Peters and Mech, 1975; Fox and Cohen, 1977). Females
in our study were walked one at a time, and all incidents of ground-scratching occurred out
of sight of other dogs. Thus, the only potential recipient of the act of ground-scratching as
a visual signal was the researcher walking the dog. While dogs are known to use a variety
of signals, some of which are visual, when interacting with humans (Borchelt, 1983; Millot
and Filiatre, 1986), we were unable to find any published reports that ground-scratching
is such a signal. The performance of ground-scratching in wild canids has been linked
to increasing group size (Barrette and Messier, 1980; Bekoff and Wells, 1986) and high
social status (Peters and Mech, 1975; Gese and Ruff, 1997). At this time, it is unclear why
spayed females in our study were much more likely than intact females to ground-scratch.
However, given the data from wild canids, it is possible that ground-scratching is associated
with dominance status in dogs. Several studies have suggested that spayed dogs more
commonly display dominance aggression toward humans than do intact females (Wright
and Nesselrote, 1987; Podberscek and Serpell, 1996; Guy et al., 2001). Further, O’Farrell
and Peachey (1990) found that dominance aggression increased following spaying of female
346 S.C. Wirant, B. McGuire /Applied Animal Behaviour Science 85 (2004) 335–348
dogs that had shown a tendency to display such aggression when young. If ground-scratching
is a dominance display in dogs that may sometimes be directed at humans, then perhaps it
too is increased by spaying.
The most common urinary posture used by females in our study was the squat-raise;
this was followed by the squat, arch-raise, combination postures, and handstand. Previous
reports have suggested that raising the leg while urinating is unusual among female dogs
and may be associated with estrus (Hart, 1974, 1975), old age (Berg, 1944; Martins and
Valle, 1948), or ovariectomy (Berg, 1944). Beach (1974) found that the vast majority of
urinary postures used by female beagles were either squats (which he called “full squats”)
or squat-raises (which he called “squat lifts”), but he did not provide separate percentages
for the two postures. Sprague and Anisko (1973) found squats to be more common than
squat-raises among female beagles, and Bekoff (1979a) and Pal (2003) reported similar
findings for free-ranging female dogs of different breeds and crosses. We do not know if the
frequent use of the squat-raise posture by females in our study is characteristic of the Jack
Russell breed. Nevertheless, the predominant use of squat or squat-raise postures by female
dogs is consistent with descriptions of female urinary postures for wild canids (Kleiman,
1966; Bekoff and Wells, 1986; Gese and Ruff, 1997). Female dogs in our study infrequently
performed the handstand posture as the only posture during urination; female beagles also
rarely performed handstands (Sprague and Anisko, 1973). In contrast, female bush dogs
routinely performed handstands against a vertical surface (Kleiman, 1966). Finally, we noted
pronounced individual variation in the urinary postures used by females. Beach (1974) and
Sprague and Anisko (1973) also found substantial individual variation in the urinary postures
used by female beagles.
The main criterion used to differentiate scent marking from simple elimination in canids
is the directing of urine toward a conspicuous object (Kleiman, 1966; Peters and Mech,
1975; Bekoff, 1979a; Bowen and Cowan, 1980). Some authors have suggested additional
criteria for marking (Kleiman, 1966) while others have found the distinction between scent
marking and elimination unnecessary, at least for some species (Barrette and Messier,
1980; Wells and Bekoff, 1981). Martins and Valle (1948) viewed urination by female dogs
as simply elimination and urination by males as elimination and scent marking. However,
if we consider directing urine at a specific object as the prime criterion for marking, then
61% of the urinations performed by spayed females and 57% performed by non-estrous
intact females were scent marks in our study. These percentages are higher than the 18%
previously reported for free-ranging female dogs by Bekoff (1979a) and the 39% reported
by Pal (2003), and disagree with Kleiman’s (1966) observation that female dogs only mark
during proestrus and estrus. Thus, our data suggest that urination in female dogs serves
eliminatory and scent marking functions, and that marking is neither unusual nor limited to
certain reproductive states.
5. Conclusion
The urinary behavior of female Jack Russell Terriers varied with age, location, and
reproductive status. Additionally, there was considerable individual variation in certain
patterns of behavior, such as ground-scratching following urination and defecation and
S.C. Wirant, B. McGuire /Applied Animal Behaviour Science 85 (2004) 335–348 347
urinary posture. Nevertheless, spayed and non-estrous intact females directed more than
half of all urinations at specific targets in the environment. The high percentages of directed
urinations suggest to us that urination in female dogs functions in scent marking as well as
in elimination, even when females are not in estrus.
Acknowledgements
We thank the staff of the Clark Science Center and members of the Department of
Biological Sciences at Smith College for their enthusiastic support of this project. We also
thank Bob and Genie Franklin of Farmcliff Kennels, Ann Shepard of Highwater Kennels,
and Valerie Whiterock for allowing us to observe their Jack Russell Terriers. Finally, we
thank Katherine Halvorsen and Linjuan Qian for statistical advice and William Bemis for
help with the figures.
References
Allen, J.J., Bekoff, M., Crabtree, R.L., 1999. An observational study of coyote (Canis latrans) scent-marking and
territoriality in Yellowstone National Park. Ethology 105, 289–302.
Barrette, C., Messier, F., 1980. Scent-marking in free-ranging coyotes, Canis latrans. Anim. Behav. 28, 814–819.
Beach, F.A., 1974. Effects of gonadal hormones on urinary behavior in dogs. Physiol. Behav. 12, 1005–1013.
Bekoff, M., 1979a. Scent-marking by free-ranging domestic dogs. Biol. Behav. 4, 123–139.
Bekoff, M., 1979b. Ground-scratching by male domestic dogs: a composite signal. J. Mammal. 60, 847–848.
Bekoff, M., Wells, M.C., 1986. Social ecology and behavior of coyotes. Adv. Stud. Behav. 16, 251–338.
Berg, I.A., 1944. Development of behavior: the micturition pattern in the dog. J. Exp. Psychol. 34, 343–368.
Borchelt, P.L., 1983. Aggressive behavior of dogs kept as companion animals: classification and influence of sex,
reproductive status and breed. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 10, 45–61.
Bowen, W.D., Cowan, I.M., 1980. Scent marking in coyotes. Can. J. Zool. 58, 473–480.
Fox, M.W., Cohen, J.A., 1977. Canid communication. In: Sebeok, T. (Ed.), How Animals Communicate. Indiana
University Press Bloomington Indiana. pp. 728–748.
Gese, E.M., Ruff, R.L., 1997. Scent-marking by coyotes, Canis latrans: the influence of social and ecological
factors. Anim. Behav. 54, 1155–1166.
Guy, N.C., Luescher, U.A., Dohoo, S.E., Spangler, E., Miller, J.B., Dohoo, I.R., Bate, L.A., 2001. Demographic
and aggressive characteristics of dogs in a general veterinary caseload. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 74, 15–28.
Hart, B.L., 1974. Environmental and hormonal influences on urine marking behavior in the adult male dog. Behav.
Biol. 11, 167–176.
Hart, B.L., 1975. Gonadal hormones and behavior of the female dog. Canine Pract. Sep.-Oct. 8–12.
Kleiman, D., 1966. Scent marking in the canidae. Symp. Zool. Soc. Lond. 18, 167–177.
Kleiman, D., Eisenberg, J.F., 1973. Comparisons of canid and felid social systems from an evolutionary perspective.
Anim. Behav. 21, 637–659.
Martins, T., Valle, J.R., 1948. Hormonal regulation of the micturition behavior of the dog. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych.
41, 301–311.
Millot, J.L., Filiatre, J.C., 1986. The behavioral sequences in the communication system between the child and
his pet dog. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 16, 383–390.
O’Farrell, V., Peachey, E., 1990. Behavioral effects of ovariohysterectomy on bitches. J. Small Anim. Pract. 31,
595–598.
Pal, S.K., 2003. Urine marking by free-ranging dogs (Canis familiaris) in relation to sex, season, place and posture.
Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 80, 45–59.
Peters, R.P., Mech, D., 1975. Scent-marking in wolves. Am. Sci. 63, 628–637.
348 S.C. Wirant, B. McGuire /Applied Animal Behaviour Science 85 (2004) 335–348
Podberscek, A.L., Serpell, J.A., 1996. The English Cocker Spaniel: preliminary findings on aggressive behaviour.
Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 47, 75–89.
Ranson, E.W., Jr., 1981. A developmental analysis of urinary behavior in dogs. Ph.D. Dissertation. University of
California, Berkeley, CA.
Ranson, E., Beach, F.A., 1985. Effects of testosterone on ontogeny of urinary behavior in male and female dogs.
Horm. Behav. 19, 36–51.
Sprague, R.H., Anisko, J.J., 1973. Elimination patterns in the laboratory beagle. Behaviour 47, 257–267.
Wells, M.C., Bekoff, M., 1981. An observational study of scent-marking in coyotes, Canis latrans. Anim. Behav.
29, 332–350.
Wright, J.C., Nesselrote, M.S., 1987. Classification of behavior problems in dogs: distributions of age, breed, sex
and reproductive status. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 19, 169–178.
... With respect to studies based on owner interviews (Hopkins et al., 1976;Maarschalkerweerd et al., 1997), why castration would reduce urination in the house, but not outside the house, remains an open question. Spaying appears to have no effect on urinary behavior of female dogs (Balogh et al., 2018;Martins and Valle, 1948;O'Farrell and Peachey, 1990;Wirant and McGuire, 2004). ...
... To my knowledge, both possibilities remain untested for dogs and could explain individual variation in responses to castration seen here. My finding of no effect of spaying on rate of urination by female dogs agrees with results from both observational studies and studies based on owner interviews (Martins and Valle, 1948;O'Farrell and Peachey, 1990;Wirant and McGuire, 2004;Balogh et al., 2018). However, my results do not indicate that gonadal hormones have no effects on urinary behavior of female dogs. ...
... Effect of castration on likelihood of defecation by male dogs has not been examined previously, which makes it difficult to put my conflicting findings for males from the two shelters in context; this difficulty is further compounded by sample size differences between the shelters (number of intact males observed at the Cortland shelter was almost twice that at the Tompkins shelter). These findings of no difference between intact and spayed females with respect to likelihood of defecation at either shelter agree with an earlier report for privately owned female Jack Russell terriers (Wirant and McGuire, 2004). Other studies of scent-marking in dogs (McGuire, 2016;McGuire and Bemis, 2017;Sprague and Anisko, 1973) and coyotes (Gese and Ruff, 1997) reported no sex differences in frequency of defecation, which is consistent with what I found at the Tompkins shelter. ...
... Although dogs are repeatedly neutered due to marking behaviour [17], this study showed that marking behaviour of intact and neutered females did not differ significantly from each other. Other studies are in line with these results [1,19,26,27] and could not find a connection between marking behaviour and sexual hormones [26,27]. Both, in this study and in others, it has been shown that neutered females are significantly more anxious [24,28] and nervous [23] than neutered ones, whereas some other authors did not find a connection between reproductive status and anxiety [1,29,30]. ...
... Although dogs are repeatedly neutered due to marking behaviour [17], this study showed that marking behaviour of intact and neutered females did not differ significantly from each other. Other studies are in line with these results [1,19,26,27] and could not find a connection between marking behaviour and sexual hormones [26,27]. Both, in this study and in others, it has been shown that neutered females are significantly more anxious [24,28] and nervous [23] than neutered ones, whereas some other authors did not find a connection between reproductive status and anxiety [1,29,30]. ...
... Females in oestrus often emit a characteristic smell, which is perceived by sexually mature males. A bitch in heat attracts dogs from the neighbourhood even if she is kept inside (Wirant and McGuire, 2004). The characteristic smell emitted by female sheep and goats in oestrus makes it easier for males to find females ready for coitus in the herd (Chenoweth, 1981). ...
Article
Full-text available
The article contains a literature review of facts and views on the strategies of sexual behaviour in mammals, taking into account the role of the animal’s species, sex, and position in the herd. The role of the senses, brain, and hormones in the expression of animals’ sexual behaviour is discussed, as well as the role of induction of the coitus reflex and social behaviour in the reproduction of present-day wild and domesticated animals. The analysis shows that the predominant strategy of sexual behaviour in females is to attract multiple potential partners to acquire the best male. The predominant strategies in the sexual behaviour of males of most mammalian species are aimed at fertilizing as many females as possible. Expression of sexual behaviour requires the generation of a set of characteristic sexual reflexes that indicate readiness to copulate and are sexually stimulating. Animals are differ in expressing their sexual behaviour. Some individuals react quickly and dynamically to sexual stimuli and immediately begin coitus, while others require longer preparation for successful coitus. Sexual behaviour influences the status of individuals in some species. Wild mammals with high expression of the sexual behaviour typical of a given species usually occupy a dominant position, which gives them an advantage in the search for breeding partners, access to food, and the expression of preferred social behaviour. Expression of sexual behaviour is stimulated by stimuli from potential sexual partners, which induce copulatory reflexes. These reflexes have been described in numerous animal species. They should be generated in the proper order and proceed without disruption for successful coitus and fertilization to occur.
... On other hand, the sexes of these dogs were confirmed via direct observation of their genitals or via observing their sexually dimorphic urinary behaviour. Male generally lift their one hind leg whereas females squat while urinating (Wirant & McGuire, 2004). Faecal collection was performed on a daily basis early in the morning. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background The presence of intestinal parasites influences the growth and well‐being of canids. Additionally, infected dogs and their faeces with considerably higher eggs/oocysts released per gram (epg/opg) of zoonotic parasites contribute to parasitic spillover to humans, domestic animals and sympatric wildlife. Objectives The current study aimed to reveal the prevalence of intestinal parasites (protozoa and helminths) and to list the zoonotically significant parasites in free‐roaming street dogs in Lalitpur Metropolitan City, Nepal. Methods Fresh faecal samples (n = 332) were collected from feral dogs of varying ages and sexes and transported to the research laboratory. The copro microscopic examination was carried out via direct wet mount, formalin ethyl acetate sedimentation, saturated salt flotation, acid‐fast staining and sporulation techniques. Results Coproscopy revealed an overall 95.7% (318/332) prevalence rate with 23 diverse species of intestinal parasites (10 protozoa and 13 helminths). Among them, 5 protozoa and 11 helminths possessed zoonotic potential, and their overall prevalence was 92.5%. Helminth's overall prevalence was double that of the protozoa (87.7% vs. 43%). Polyparasitism was dominant over mono parasitism (79.5% vs. 16.3%), and co‐infection of up to seven species of parasites at a time was recorded. Conclusions Urban street dogs harboured a higher prevalence of intestinal parasites that varied with age and sex. Since most of the reported parasites are zoonotic, dog density and parasitic richness indicate a greater spillover risk to humans and domestic animals. Furthermore, this study also provides appropriate ‘baseline’ data for assessing effective control measures against parasitic infestations among street dogs and controlling their transmission to humans.
... Hormones have also been linked to changes in the fecal microbiome of women and could feasibly be altering the urine microbiota [84]. In addition, urinary behavior differs between male and female dogs with males urinating more frequently than females [85]. It is feasible that urine volume and retention time in the bladder could alter urine composition and the urine microbial community. ...
Article
Full-text available
The urinary microbiota is the collection of microbes present in urine that may play a role in host health. Studies of urine microbiota have traditionally relied upon culturing methods aimed at identifying pathogens. However, recent culture-free sequencing studies of the urine microbiota have determined that a diverse array of microbes is present in health and disease. To study these microbes and their potential role in diseases like bladder cancer or interstitial cystitis, consistent extraction and detection of bacterial DNA from urine is critical. However, urine is a low biomass substrate, requiring sensitive methods to capture DNA and making the risk of contamination high. To address this challenge, we collected urine samples from ten healthy dogs and extracted DNA from each sample using five different commercially available extraction methods. Extraction methods were compared based on total and bacterial DNA concentrations and bacterial community composition and diversity assessed through 16S rRNA gene sequencing. Significant differences in the urinary microbiota were observed by dog and sex but not extraction method. The Bacteremia Kit yielded the highest total DNA concentrations (Kruskal-Wallis, p = 0.165, not significant) and the highest bacterial DNA concentrations (Kruskal-Wallis, p = 0.044). Bacteremia also extracted bacterial DNA from the greatest number of samples. Taken together, these results suggest that the Bacteremia kit is an effective option for studying the urine microbiota. This work lays the foundation to study the urine microbiome in a wide range of urogenital diseases in dogs and other species.
... In line with previous studies [35,36], our results showed that frequency of urine marking is higher in males than in females. Wirant et al. [37] reported that behaviour of spayed and non-oestrous intact females was similar. Two studies have examined urine marking by neutered and intact males however, both had small sample sizes and found no significant difference [38,39]. ...
Article
The aim of this study was to evaluate the effects of intraspecific social exposure (socialization vs rest) and habituation factors on the levels of urinary serotonin and cortisol [corrected respectively for creatinine to give the serotonin-creatinine ratio (5-HT/Cr) and cortisol-creatinine ratio (C/Cr)] and how they fluctuate in dogs. The frequency of marking during social sessions was recorded to evaluate its relationship with physiological parameters. The effects of covariates on 5-HT/Cr and C/Cr were assessed using a linear mixed models. 5-HT/Cr values were higher at dawn than at dusk during resting days, however, this difference is less evident after socialization sessions. During rest days, there was a trend for a decrease in C/Cr between dawn and dusk, while during social exposure days there was an opposite trend. Significant interactions were found between social exposure vs rest (P = 0.0005) and social exposure vs sessions (P = 0.002). Urine marking was more frequent in male than female dogs. The frequency of urine marking also appeared to be positively associated with C/Cr. Non-invasive monitoring of physiological markers could be a useful tool in assessing behaviour modulation following intraspecific socialization exposure. C/Cr was characterized by high individual variability and interactions with examined factors. The identification of new markers such as serotonin for assessing welfare in dogs is highly desirable.
Chapter
Behavior can change as a result of medical problems or physiological changes, and behavior changes are likely to be the first signs of stress, disease, and poor welfare in any animal. If shelter operations, behavior, and/or medical staff identify behaviors that may have an underlying medical cause, they can be addressed immediately, relieving suffering and increasing the adoptability of the animal. Conversely, if medical conditions that cause or exacerbate problematic behaviors are missed, time may be wasted on training or attempted behavior modification, thus prolonging suffering and time spent in the shelter. Only by safeguarding both physical and emotional health can we improve overall quality of life for animals in our care, facilitate their placement in homes, and help prevent their return to the shelter.
Chapter
Domestic dogs are members of the class Mammalia, order Carnivora, and family Canidae. Although within the order Carnivora, dogs have evolved to eat an omnivorous diet. Their nutritional requirements include specific amino acids, glucose precursors, fatty acids, and dietary fibre are important dietary elements. Dogs are generally social animals. Most well‐socialised dogs are strongly motivated to establish contact and interact with other dogs, for example on a walk. Human contact has beneficial effects for many dogs. Importantly, a dog's need for, and reaction to, human company is affected by its temperament and early experiences. Rabies is an important disease internationally, affecting millions of dogs yearly, and extrapolating from human experiences, may cause respiratory distress and pain prior to death. The behavioural responses of an individual dog are influenced by its breed, type, rearing, and current environment. Dogs' responses to rewards and training may also indicate their mood or overall welfare.
Chapter
Behavioral patterns are subject to natural selection and behavior like any other attributes of an animal, which contributes to individual survival. The chapter summarizes a long-term study of coyotes that was conducted in the Grand Teton National Park, in the northwest comer of Wyoming. There is remarkable agreement in the results stemming from a limited number of field projects concerned with the social behavior and behavioral ecology of coyotes, and some general principles concerning social ecology, scent marking, predatory behavior, time budgeting, and reproductive and care-giving patterns can be developed that are applicable not only to coyotes but to many other carnivores.
Article
This study is based on the analysis of video films of spontaneous actions between children from 2 to 5 years of age and their pet dog (N = 20), and presents behavioural sequences which regulate the interactions between the child and his dog. The preliminary results indicate and compare the percentages of the different communication behaviours of the child and dog, and the behavioural modifications which followed. The discussion deals with the originality of the child-dog communication systems as compared with the relational systems of a young child with his peers.
Article
Carnivores use various scent-marking methods. Observations on the scent marking by urination of 16 free-ranging dogs from two neighbouring groups were recorded in the town of Katwa, West Bengal, India. The frequency of urine marking was higher in males than in females. The seasonal mean (±S.D.) number of markings for individual males varied with a minimum of 4.0 (±1.5) in summer, and a maximum of 31.5 (±9.1) in late monsoon. Similarly, the seasonal mean (±S.D.) number of markings for individual females varied with a minimum of 1.1 (±0.9) in summer, and a maximum of 5.9 (±2.5) in late monsoon. Therefore, the frequency of marking was not the same in every season. The incidence of urine marking was higher in the courting place as well as in the late monsoon. Occasionally, ‘possessive urine marking’ was observed among the alpha males. Urine marking seemed to be linked with scavenging behaviour. The incidence of urine marking mostly by the males near the territorial boundary during trespassing by neighbouring dog(s) showed the evidence of territorial defense. Perhaps to protect the pups, the females marked with a higher rate at the nest site. The dogs, especially the males, marked on strange objects/vehicles perhaps to familiarise the strange objects.Every male and every female performed High leg raising (HLR) and squat postures, respectively. Raised leg display (RLD) was performed only by the males and was influenced by the presence of other dogs. The males showed the RLD mostly in the courting place and also near the territory, perhaps to indicate their dominance, aggressiveness and to threaten the others.
Article
A retrospective cross-sectional survey of dog-owning veterinary clients was undertaken in 1996 in the three Canadian provinces of New Brunswick, Nova Scotia, and Prince Edward Island, to generate a population of dogs for future use in a more detailed survey on canine behaviour. The questionnaire was designed to detect which dogs had or had not bitten a person living in the same household, and included both demographic and behaviour questions. Twenty veterinary clinics were enlisted to administer the questionnaire to their clientele. Data was collected on 3226 dogs, a response rate of 81.4%. Dogs were predominantly purebred (60.1%) and neutered (71.6%). The Labrador Retriever was the most commonly reported of 110 breeds. There were slightly more female than male dogs, and significantly more female dogs were neutered (P