ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

In this article we argue that social discourse can affect the structure and content of autobiographical memory. In making this argument, we review literature documenting the impact of social factors, including culture, social roles, and social disclosure frequency, on aspects of autobiographical memory. We also describe several social norms that govern social discourse and speculate about the effect that such norms might have on autobiographical memory. In addition, we review the mental structures and processes that might serve to mediate the relation between social discourse and autobiographical memory and offer suggestions about how both social and cognitive factors might be integrated into a common model accounting for autobiographical memory.
Content may be subject to copyright.
SKOWRONSKIAND WALKERDESCRIBING AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL EVENTS
HOW DESCRIBING AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL
EVENTS CAN AFFECT AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL
MEMORIES
John J. Skowronski
Northern Illinois University
W. Richard Walker
Winston-Salem State University
In this article we argue that social discourse can affect the structure and content
of autobiographical memory. In making this argument, we review literature
documenting the impact of social factors, including culture, social roles, and
social disclosure frequency, on aspects of autobiographical memory. We also
describe several social norms that govern social discourse and speculate about
the effect that such norms might have on autobiographical memory. In addi-
tion, we review the mental structures and processes that might serve to mediate
the relation between social discourse and autobiographical memory and offer
suggestions about how both social and cognitive factors might be integrated
into a common model accounting for autobiographical memory.
Although autobiographical memory research had always captured the
interest of a few researchers (e.g., Cason, 1932; Colegrove, 1983/1899;
Waldfogel, 1948), the pace of research into this topic accelerated in the
late 1970s and early 1980s. Researchers who had a background in cogni
-
tive or experimental psychology published many of the important stud
-
ies during this time (Linton, 1986; Neisser, 1978; Thompson, 1982;
Wagenaar, 1986). Consequently, the variables of interest (e.g., delay, re
-
hearsal) that were explored in these studies often focused on variables
that are typically important to cognitive psychologists. The influence of
cognitive psychology continues to manifest itself in much of the contem
-
555
Social Cognition, Vol. 22, No. 5, 2004, pp. 555-590
We would like to thank Charles P. Thompson, Jeffrey A. Gibbons, and Rodney J. Vogl
for their comments made throughout the course of this project.
Address correspondence to John J. Skowronski, Department of Psychology, Northern
Illinois University, DeKalb, IL 60115; E-mail: jskowron@niu.edu.
porary theory and research that explores autobiographical memory
(e.g., Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000).
However, even within cognitive psychology there has been a consis
-
tent, albeit low-level, interest in how variables that tend to be of primary
interest to social psychologists affect autobiographical memory. For ex
-
ample, Bartlett’s classic research into memory distortion (1932; for a re
-
cent update, see Bergman & Roediger, 1999) grew out of his earlier
interest (1923) in social determinants of remembering and forgetting.
Similarly, Loftus’s well-known research on the misinformation effect
(e.g., Loftus, 1975; Loftus & Loftus, 1980) explored how socially trans
-
mitted information conveyed after an event has occurred can cause dis
-
tortion in the event memory. Despite its cognitive psychology origins,
Betz, Skowronski, and Ostrom (1996) argued that such research can eas
-
ily be viewed through the lens of social psychology. Adopting such a
perspective suggests that classic social psychology persuasion variables
such as the credibility of the speaker, the plausibility of the information,
and the extent to which the post-event information is consensually
shared can work to consolidate or distort event memories (also see
Hoffman, Granhag, Kwong See, & Loftus, 2001; Roediger, Meade, &
Bergman, 2001). For example, recent research shows that although peo-
ple can be persuaded to falsely “recall” fictitious autobiographical
events, the plausibility of the event influences the probability that the
event will be falsely recalled (Mazzoni, Loftus, & Kirsch, 2001;
McDermott & Roediger, 1998; Roediger, et al., 2001; Roediger &
McDermott, 1995).
Moreover, social psychologistsdidnotentirelyyield the field to cogni-
tive psychologists in their exploration of how these social variables af
-
fected autobiographical memory. Some social psychologists, such as
Mike Ross, conducted influential research during this seminal period.
Ross’searly autobiographical memory studies werestimulated by social
psychologists’ interest in the attitude-behavior relationship and ex
-
plored how attitudes might alter recollections of behavior (e.g., Ross,
McFarland, & Fletcher, 1981). At about the same time, Greenwald (1980)
published an influential article on the “totalitarian ego” in which he out
-
lined how the self-concept consists of a number of interrelated knowl
-
edge structures that, in conjunction with various motivations, served to
direct information processing in such a way as to bias the content of
autobiographical recall.
The linkage between social and cognitive psychology in this area is
further emphasized by the fact that the relation between social interac
-
tion and autobiographical memory has emerged as a primary theme in
autobiographical memory research. For example, influenced by think
-
ers such as Mead (1934) and by the social-developmental movement
556 SKOWRONSKI AND WALKER
epitomized by Vygotsky (1962), some researchers suggest that
mother-child interaction is crucial to the development of autobiographi
-
cal memory (for a review, see Reese, 2002). Some researchers also sug
-
gest that the ability to describe autobiographical memories to others is a
hallmark in the development of the self (Nelson & Fivush, 2000). Related
research explores cross-cultural differences in the kinds of autobio
-
graphical events that are recalled (Leichtman, Wang, & Pillemer, 2003).
The origins of these differences are again thought to emerge from early
mother-child interactions: Mothers in different cultures are thought to
encourage their children torecall different kinds of events (Wang, 2001).
Personality researchers have also explored how elements of social in
-
teractions might be related to autobiographical memory. Some research
explores the narratives that people use to describe their lives to others.
This research focuses on the meaning that is derived from described life
events and how that meaning can be used to construct or modify the self
(Barclay, 1996; McAdams, 2001; Singer & Bluck, 2001).
Researchers in health psychology and in clinical and counseling psy-
chology have also been interested in the relation between social interac-
tions and autobiographical memory. One of the areas of extreme
controversy in psychology is the veracity of so-called “recovered memo-
ries” and the extent to which therapists can “plant” such memories (Lof-
tus, 1993; Nadel & Jacobs, 1998). Research now suggests that although
some “recovered memories” do, indeed, reflect memory for seemingly
forgotten events, other “recovered memories” are fabrications planted
by clinicians (or researchers; see Ceci & Loftus, 1994) during social inter-
actions (see Conway, 1997). Other clinical and health psychology re
-
search has focused on how social interactions affect the way that people
feel about their memories. For example, a number of studies have shown
that while disclosure of negative events to others might have negative
short-term effects, in the long term, such disclosure generally reduces
the negative feelings that typically accompany the recall of such events
(e.g., Neiderhoffer & Pennebaker, 2002).
One intriguing aspect of this research is that much of the early work
that explored the impact of social interaction on autobiographical mem
-
ory originated in areas outside of social psychology. This state of affairs
is a bit puzzling given that, as Middleton and Edwards (1990) noted, the
idea that memory can be, in part, a collective phenomenon has a rela
-
tively long intellectual history. For example, the French sociologist
Durkheim (1895/1982) and his student Halbwachs (1951/1980) were
proponents of this collectivist position. The influence of these intellec
-
tual trailblazers is reflected in Middleton and Edwards’s (1990) edited
volume, Collective Remembering. That volume presented both theorizing
DESCRIBING AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL EVENTS 557
about the relation between social interaction and memory and empirical
data that explore this relation.
Explorations of the relationship between social interactions and auto
-
biographical memory have recently come from several different sources
within social psychology. One research theme has focused on group
memory, exploring such questions as how, when, and why the collective
memory of groups tends to be superior to the recall of single individuals
(Hinsz, 1990) and how common knowledge and information sharing af
-
fect the memory performance of groups (Tindale & Sheffey, 2002). A sec
-
ond theme explores the phenomenon of transactive memory
(Hollingshead, 1998, 2001; Wegner, Erber, & Raymond, 1991). This re
-
search suggests, among other things, that peoples’ roles in a relationship
help to determine what they remember and what they forget (e.g., a
wife’s role might be to remember everyone’s birth date, while a hus
-
band’s role might be to remember the car tune-up schedule). A third
theme in the research emphasizes how public discussions might mirror
mental processes that are important to remembering and forgetting. For
example, research by Ruscher and Hammer (1994) suggests that some
discussions with others tend to focus on information that is related to a
stigma and that such discussion can promote subsequent memory for
stigma-related information. These discussions are thought to mirror the
mental processes that affect memory for stereotype-relevant informa-
tion (Ford & Stangor, 1992; Sherman, 1996). Indeed, Pasupathi (2001)
has made a broader argument, suggesting that discussions of autobio-
graphical events serve to organize individual events and imbed those
events into a personal narrative. In this view, social discourse is key
factor in determining how (and if) people remember particular events.
GOALS OF THE PRESENT ARTICLE
One goal of the present article is to briefly review and summarize some
of the research that has explored relations between social interaction and
autobiographical memory. We believe that this review and summary is
helpful in at least two ways. First, it makes the point to the non-expert
that there is much more of this kind of research taking place than might
previously have been believed. Second, it helps researchers who explore
social influences on memory to keep track of the research going on in re
-
lated disciplines. The additional research that we present in the remain
-
der of this article will continue to work toward this goal.
One additional purpose of this article will be to focus on the extent to
which a speaker’s description of an event to others (or non-communica
-
tion of an event) affects the speaker’s later memory for that event. Specif
-
ically, we will focus on three issues. The first issue is the extent to which
558 SKOWRONSKI AND WALKER
people rehearse autobiographical events for social reasons relative to
the extent to which those events are rehearsed for other reasons (e.g., to
understand events). We will argue that event memories are frequently
rehearsed when disclosing events to others and that such social rehears
-
als may be one of the most frequent reasons for the rehearsal of
autobiographical events.
A second issue concerns the impact of those rehearsals on autobio
-
graphical memories. Some events are described to others—but not other
events. Some event details get included in the descriptions—but not
other details. Some details might be accurately described to others—but
other details might be distorted in the retelling. Some extra-event infor
-
mation (such as the statement, “he looked just like Robert Redford”)
might be included in some descriptions—but not in other descriptions.
The second issue of interest in this article concerns these biases in the
conveyance of autobiographical events and the impact of such biases.
We argue that a number of factors, including social norms and conven-
tions and self-presentational concerns, affect when one relates autobio-
graphical events to others, what is included in those descriptions, and
the manner in which those events are described. We also argue that be-
cause these social norms and self-presentational concerns alter the
“when, what, and how” of autobiographical event descriptions, and be-
cause these biased descriptions function as rehearsals of events, those
variables can ultimately have an impact on the conveyor’s later memory
for the events related in those descriptions. These memories might be af-
fectedin several ways. For example, the temporalcategory into whichan
event falls often needs to be recoded as an individual ages: The event
that happened an hour ago may be subsequently recategorized as an
event that occurred yesterday, then as occurring last week, then as oc
-
curring last month, and so forth. These temporal recategorizations
might be facilitated by describing the event to others. Another possible
outcome of social storytelling is that one’s tendency to retrieve events
might be altered by the contexts of social storytelling. As one recounts
the events from one’s own life, various situational cues and communica
-
tion goals might become linked to the autobiographical memories that
are recounted. Such cues and goals might prompt reinstatement of the
events when those goals are activated or those cues are encountered at a
later time. As events become linked with a greater number of cues, the
probability of retrieving such memories should increase. Hence,
diversity in storytelling settings and goals might also play an important
role in the extentto which an individual recalls autobiographical events.
A third general issue surrounding social rehearsals is the set of cogni
-
tive processes that are involved when one conveys events to others and
how these cognitive processes might affect memory for the event con
-
DESCRIBING AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL EVENTS 559
veyed. Obviously, one might expect that events that are often rehearsed
will tend to be better recalled than events that are not rehearsed. Hence,
one would also expect that events that are frequently conveyed to others
should be well recalled. However, it is possible that such social rehears
-
als might alter the content of the recalled events. For example, some re
-
trieved events have a rich sensory content, while others do not. The
accessible memory representation of autobiographical events may be
-
come more semantic in nature when events are frequently described to
others. Such “impoverished” memories have been referred to as autobio
-
graphical facts (Brewer, 1996) or repisodic memories (Neisser, 1981).
After providing a brief description of autobiographical memories and
of some characteristics of those memories, we shall elaborate on these
themes. In one section of this article we will describe recently collected
evidence suggesting that social rehearsals of autobiographical events
occur quite frequently. In a second section, we review several social
norms that govern self-descriptions and attempt to relate those norms to
some of the characteristics of autobiographical memory. In a third sec-
tion, we describe the cognitive mechanisms that may mediate the rela-
tion between the social norms affecting the communication of
autobiographical events and event recall.
AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORIES AND THEIR
CHARACTERISTICS
Autobiographical memory is a term that refers to memory for self-relevant
events. The arguments that we make in this article are intended to apply
to this entire class of memories. However, we recognize that autobio
-
graphical memories might come in different “flavors.” For example,
Brewer (1986, 1996) argues that there are at least two different types of
autobiographical memories. These types roughly correspond to the clas
-
sic distinction between episodic knowledge and semantic knowledge.
For example, when recalling an individual episode or event, an individ
-
ual might have access to the phenomenological details of the event: its
sights, sounds, and smells. The individual might also be triggered to feel
emotions prompted by the event, as well as bodily sensations experi
-
enced while the event was occurring. In Brewer’s terms, an event re
-
called in this way would be a recollective memory. On the other hand, an
individual might recall autobiographical facts. These are events in a
person’s life that are recalled, but without accompanying sensory or
experiential details.
The retrieval of such event-specific knowledge can have many conse
-
quences. Recall of such events might allow an individual to construct
self-schemata. Those schemata can provide an individual with a sense of
560 SKOWRONSKI AND WALKER
cohesiveness among subsequently retrieved life events, and can affect
the encoding and storage of new life events. Recalling an event might
serve to alter a person’s mood, help the person resolve a conflict with an
-
other person, or solve a current problem. In short, the recollection of an
event often causes a cascade of responses, including the subsequent ac
-
cess of additional memories. Thus, it can be argued that autobiographi
-
cal memory represents the single most complex type of human memory
(Conway, 1996).
One of the distinctive features of autobiographical memories is that
they are often accompanied by a general sense of when the event oc
-
curred. This general sense of an event’s location in time emerges in esti
-
mates of an event’s date (Betz & Skowronski, 1997; Brewer, 1988;
Skowronski & Thompson, 1990), estimates of an event’s age
(Huttenlocher, Hedges, & Prohaska, 1992), or in judgments of the order
in which two events occurred (Skowronski, Walker, & Betz, 2003). How
-
ever, Thompson, Skowronski, Larsen, and Betz (1996) noted that people
often remember the “core details” of an event memory, such as what
happened and where the event took place, but misremember “periph-
eral details,” such as exactly when the event occurred. Obviously, if the
time that an event occurred is not exactly recalled, people might instead
use estimation processes to judge that time. Predictably, then, a person’s
sense of an event’s age is certainly not infallible—errors in temporal
judgments are typical. Moreover, these errors are nonrandom: There is
often a considerable amount of reconstruction that accompanies the de-
termination of the time at which an event occurred (Thompson,
Skowronski, & Betz, 1993) and such reconstruction can lead to system-
atic biases in the placement of an event in time (Ross & Wilson, 2002).
Nonetheless, most people do have a reasonable sense of time in their
memories—indeed, the loss of this sense of time and order is often the
sign of serious psychopathology (Shimamura, Janowsky, & Squire,
1990).
One of the other distinctive features of autobiographical memories is
that they are constructions that integrate information from different lev
-
els of specificity (Brown & Schopflocher, 1998). For example, Conway
has suggested that autobiographical memories can incorporate three
types of information: Lifetime periods, general event knowledge, and
event-specific knowledge (e.g., Anderson & Conway, 1993; Conway,
1996; Conway & Bekerian, 1987). The lifetime period, such as “when I
was in graduate school,” represents general knowledge of a period in a
person’s life. General event knowledge, such as “visits to the zoo,” rep
-
resents generalized knowledge that can be derived from repeated
events. Event-specific knowledge contains the details of a specific event
memory, such as “seeing the new owl exhibit at the zoo’s grand open
-
DESCRIBING AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL EVENTS 561
ing.” Information from these different levels is often integrated into a co
-
herent recollection. The lifetime period themes and general event
knowledge can serve as thecontextual background for the event-specific
knowledge, allowing a specific memory to be placed into proper per
-
spective. Conway (1996) argued that this pattern of information
integration across levels of specificity may be the defining characteristic
of autobiographical memories.
The finding that most autobiographical memories are multiply struc
-
tured should not overlook one important finding: Most life events are
probably not incorporated into autobiographical memory. Brewer
(1988) gave participants a beeper and asked them to record autobio
-
graphical events that were occurring when they were randomly paged.
This procedure obviously produced many records of relatively mun
-
dane life events, and people had poor recall of such events. Similar find
-
ings come from research indicating that neutral events are remembered
more poorly than affectively toned events (Betz & Skowronski, 1997;
Skowronski, Betz, Thompson, & Shannon, 1991; Walker, Vogl, &
Thompson, 1997). The general finding that most neutral or insignificant
events are not well remembered should surprise no one. However, the
finding does suggest that the primary function of autobiographical
memory is not to retain accurate representations of all life events. In-
stead, the primary function of autobiographical memory may be to pro-
duce a record of important events, such as events that have personal
relevance to one’s goals (Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000).
EVENTS ARE OFTEN REHEARSED IN CONVERSATIONS WITH
OTHERS
Casual observation suggests that event descriptions might be important
to event memory simply because those descriptions are frequently com
-
municated. Job applicants try to impress potential employers by re
-
counting their positive, job-related behaviors (a job applicant might
relate the story of how her master’s thesis was her first independent pro
-
ject and it was published in JPSP). Spouses often share the events of the
day with each other over the dinner table (a faculty member might con
-
vey to her spouse the confrontation that she had with a student). On
dates, people try to “connect” with each other by relating stories about
themselves (a suitor might describe events from his past that are thought
to be informative about the suitor’s personality). When interacting with
people who are in emotional difficulty, one may try to express empathy
by relating a past event that has similar implications. People share
events that provoke laughter (a friend recounts his tale of falling into the
pond at a golf outing), tears (a family member recounts vacation events
562 SKOWRONSKI AND WALKER
with a sibling who died prematurely), and joy (a colleague recounts the
story of getting a paper accepted without a single revision).
Data examining the content of conversations confirms the observation
that people often relate autobiographical event descriptions to each
other in the course of those conversations (e.g., Hirst & Manier, 1996;
Hirst, Manier, & Apetroaia, 1997; Dritschel, 1991). Until recently, re
-
searchers had not documented the extent to which people engaged in
communication-based event rehearsals relative to non-social forms of
event rehearsal (but see Bluck, Alea, Habermas, & Rubin, in press) Such
data is seemingly important to the issue of the potential impact of social
rehearsals on communication. That is, if people think about events far
more often than they talk about them, then the possible impact of social
communications on event memory might not be worth pursuing. How
-
ever, if rehearsal via social communication occurs frequently enough,
then consideration of the effects of social rehearsals on autobiographical
memory might have a substantial payoff.
Recently collected data suggest that social rehearsals occur at least as
often as, and perhaps more often than, other rehearsal types (Walker,
Skowronski, Gibbons, & Vogl, 2004). For example, in each of two studies
participants were asked to list either four or six autobiographical events
that occurred within the last 6 months. After listing the events, partici-
pants were asked to estimate the number of times that they had re-
hearsed each event for one of several different reasons (e.g., to
remember the details of the events, to re-experience the emotions associ-
ated with the events, to better understand the events, to talk about the
events to others, or the events were thought of involuntarily). In both
studies, the most frequent reason that people rehearsed events was for
the purpose of talking to others (see Table 1), and its reported frequency
was significantly greater than any other reason.
We admit that there is reason to treat these data with a bit of skepti
-
cism. For example, because of the open-ended response format, a few re
-
hearsal estimates were quite high (e.g., over 1,000), requiring that data
transformations be used prior to inferential analyses (a log transforma
-
tion was used in Study 1 and a truncation of extreme frequencies to the
value of 100 was used in Study 2). However, despite these manipula
-
tions, it is informative that similar orderings of means emerge in both
data sets. Moreover, in a third study this ordering emerged when partic
-
ipants were asked to report their rehearsal frequencies on bounded rat
-
ing scales, eliminating the extreme value problem. The similarities in
our data across studies and across different data collection and treat
-
ment techniques suggest that there is considerable consistency in peo
-
ples’ retrospective perceptions of their event rehearsals: People perceive
DESCRIBING AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL EVENTS 563
that social rehearsals of memories occur often, perhaps more often than
any other single rehearsal type.
CONVERSATIONAL NORMS THAT MAY AFFECT
AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY
We next turn to an examination of the social rehearsals themselves and
to the conditions that can alter the communication of autobiographical
events. A number of questions immediately present themselves. Why
are people selective in the events that they convey to others? Why are
someevents described frequently while other eventsare described infre
-
quently or not at all? Why do people convey some event details and not
others? Why are some event details distorted in the conveyance while
others are conveyed accurately? What influences the insertion of
non-event information (such as metaphors) into event descriptions and
whatimpact does inclusion of such devices have on later event memory?
These questions are partly answered by referring to social norms that
govern how people communicate with each other. It has been argued
that these norms are part of the social contract between the speaker and
the listener, who both engage in conversation to achieve particular
goals. These norms determine what kind of information is discussed,
how the information is discussed, and which reactions are appropriate
(e.g., Clark, 1996; Clark & Schober, 1992; Grice 1975, Higgins, 1992;
McCann & Higgins, 1990; Sperber & Wilson, 1986). We will argue that
the operation of these norms can have an impact on the frequency of
event conveyance and the content of such conveyance. In pursuing this
564 SKOWRONSKI AND WALKER
TABLE 1. Rehearsal Estimates by Rehearsal Type for Events Occurring within the Past
Six Months
Rehearsal Type Study 1 Study 2
Storytelling 4.68
a
8.70
a
Remember Event Details 1.82
b
5.59
b,c
Re-Experience Event’s Emotion 1.95
b
5.90
b
Better Understand the Event .38
c
3.92
b,c
Rehearsal Was Involuntary 1.69
b
5.68
c
Don’t Know Why Rehearsed 2.05
d
Other .67
d
Note. Within each study, means that share a superscript are not significantly different.
idea, we explicitly build on the work of Pasupathi (2001). Pasupathi pro
-
posed that speaker qualities and listener qualities, as well as prior con
-
versational recollections, combine to affect the content of
autobiographical memory. According to Pasupathi’s model, these fac
-
tors create a social context that helps people select, organize, and inter
-
pret individual event memories. Figure 1 presents this principle of
co-construction.
We similarly argue that social norms, via their influence on social dis
-
closure, can affect autobiographical memory in several ways. These in
-
clude alterations in the recall of a given memory, the content of the
memory, the accuracy of the memory, the reconstructed temporal loca
-
tion of the memory, and emotions accompanying the memory. The sec
-
tions that follow describe various conversational norms and discuss the
potentialeffects these norms may have on autobiographical memories.
KEEP IT FRESH
When engaging in social discourse with others, one of the rules that a
speaker needs to follow is to be informative. This often takes the form of
discussing information that is thought to be unknown to the listener. For
example, the question “What’s new?” is typical when friends and family
reunite after a period apart.
An example of the tendency to focus on new events in social discourse
was noted by Pennebaker and Harber (1993). They surveyed: (1) 789
people in the San Francisco Bay area immediately after the Loma Prieta
earthquake, and (2) 2,188 people in Dallas, TX immediately after the
start of the Persian Gulf War. Their data suggested that these events
were discussed frequently by most people for approximately two weeks
after each event. After this two-week period, discussion of these events
dramatically declined.
One might also assume that individuals with diverse social networks
have many opportunities to discuss autobiographical memories, and
that these opportunities are likely to be spread across time and space.
Hence, one would expect the memories of these socially connected peo
-
ple to be more affected by their discourse-induced rehearsals than those
with impoverished social networks, who presumably do not have the
same opportunities to discuss the events in their lives with others. This
tendency to focus on new events in social interactions may also work to
the detriment of older events. The amount of time available for interac
-
tion may often take the form of a zero-sum game: When one discusses
the new, one does not rehearse the old. This lack of rehearsal is likely one
DESCRIBING AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL EVENTS 565
factor that is responsible for the dissipation of autobiographical
memories with the passage of time.
KEEP IT RELEVANT
Social discourse often has implicit agreed-upon parameters within
which speakers are required to stay. For example, in conversations
speakers might “know” that each memory that is reported should be rel
-
evant to the agreed-upon topic in the social discourse. This norm re
-
quires that the speaker understands the goals of the listener and
provides input that meets these goals. For example, Russell and Schober
(1999) asked pairs of individuals to describe abstract shapes to each
other. In some cases, the listener needed to be able to describe the shape
in general terms and, in other cases, the listener needed to describe the
shape in specific terms. The speaker was either aware or unaware of the
listener’s goals. When speakers were aware of the listener’s goals, they
tailored their speech to match the perceived goals of the listeners. This
helped the listener obtain the necessary information about the shape.
When speakers were unaware of the listener’s goals, they assumed that
566 SKOWRONSKI AND WALKER
FIGURE 1. The principle of co-construction.
Both listeners and speakers influence the rehearsal of events. This process is con-
strained by prior rehearsals (Pasupathi, 2001).
the listener’s goals were similar to their own and tailored their
communication accordingly.
One component of relevance is interest value. If a conversation is not
relevant to a listener, then a speaker runs the risk of boring the listener.
Hence, a speaker may attempt to present information that they think is
interesting to a listener. Autobiographical events that are thought to be
interesting are often those events that are extreme or novel. For example,
in our autobiographical memory research we examined the relation be
-
tween the emotional extremity of an event, the valence of an event, and
the event’s judged rehearsal frequency (Skowronski, Gibbons, Vogl, &
Walker, in press). Participants were asked to report the frequency with
which they rehearsed autobiographical events on a seven-point scale.
Participants also rated the positive emotion provoked by the event and
the negative emotion provoked by the event, also on seven-point scales.
As shown in Table 2, higher levels of both positive and negative emo
-
tions in events were related to high social rehearsal frequency. Thus,
people talk to others about events that they deem emotional, which
presumably are thought to be interesting to audiences.
The conveyance of emotion-provoking events also has implications for
the emotions that accompany event memories. In previous research, we
found that the negative emotions that accompany autobiographical mem-
ories seem to fade over time at a faster rate than the positive emotions that
accompany those memories. This differential fading of emotion has been
termed the fading affect bias. More recent research (Skowronski, et al., in
press) has linked this fading affect bias to the social rehearsal of event
memories. That is, increased social sharing of memories is related to (and,
indeed, appears to cause) a reduction in the fading of positive affect asso
-
ciated with positive event memories over time, but is also related to an in
-
crease in the fading of negative affect associated with negative event
memories. These results suggest that talking to others may help one to re
-
tain the “rosy glow” that results from positive events, but may take the
emotional “sting” out of negative events (also see Walker, Skowronski,
Gibbons, Vogl, & Thompson, in press).
Might such effects also be related to a bias in the content of discourse?
The data in Table 2 are not indicative of a tendency toward positivity in
social rehearsals. Such a bias would be convenient in a number of ways.
For example, if such a tendency existed, it might be related to the ten
-
dency for positive autobiographical events to be better recalled than
negative events (Holmes, 1970; Walker et al., 1997). Instead, our data has
consistently shown that positive and negative life events are perceived
to be communicated with roughly equal frequency.
This result is a surprise. There are a number of reasons to expect that
people would be more likely to communicate positive life events than
DESCRIBING AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL EVENTS 567
568 SKOWRONSKI AND WALKER
TABLE 2. Least-squared Means from Analyses of Rehearsal Frequency Estimates (Made on a Seven-Point Scale) by Positive and Negative
Emotions Induced by Events (Unpublished Data)
Extremity of Emotion Rating
1234567
To describe events to others Positive 2.33 2.38 2.41 2.50 2.71 3.12 3.54
Negative 2.26 2.39 2.54 2.64 2.77 2.93 3.45
So others can understand me better Positive 1.68 1.74 1.77 1.90 2.07 2.36 2.67
Negative 1.66 1.74 1.85 2.03 2.12 2.16 2.65
negative events. One of these reasons is simply event base rate. Walker,
Skowronski, and Thompson (2003) recently reviewed a number of stud
-
ies in autobiographical memory. These studies included: (a) diary stud
-
ies in which participants recorded a variety of events over a designated
recording period, (b) studies of childhood memories, and (c) retrospec
-
tive reports provided in controlled laboratory conditions. All of these
studies suggest that positive life events are perceived to outnumber
negative life events by a ratio of 2 to 1.
Another line of argument favoring positivity in discourse is that,
while social interactions can have negative consequences (see Rook,
1984), most people probably choose to engage in social discourse be
-
cause they find it enjoyable (Hartup & Stevens, 1997). Hence, one might
speculate that people prefer that the content of their social discourse be
positive rather than negative. After all, while it is certainly the case that
discussions need to be appropriate to the circumstances (students will
often commune in their sorrow after an exam that they perceived to be
difficult or unfair), it is also the case that people who excessively com-
plain about their lives to others are likely to receive social sanctions and
be excluded from future group conversations.
An oft-cited historical example of that preference can be found in the
public’s different reactions to Ronald Reagan and Jimmy Carter. Carter
was perceived as a person who was all too willing to describe the coun-
try’s events and circumstances in negative terms (e.g., his labeling of the
country as experiencing “malaise”). In comparison, Reagan’s public
messages about the state of our country were relentlessly upbeat, and he
peppered those messages with uplifting stories of the successes of spe
-
cific individuals. Hence, while Carter might have been perceived to be a
sincere, competent, and intelligent man, he was also perceived to be a bit
of a boor. In contrast, Reagan, who was relentlessly ridiculed for his sim
-
plistic thinking, was nonetheless able to garner tremendouspublic affec
-
tion. Such differential reactions, if generally applicable, would seem to
work toward inducing people to focus on the positive in their social
discourse.
Positivity in social discourse also might be expected from the self-pre
-
sentation literature. Theorists in that literature have described a number
of motivations that underlie the presentation of the self to others. While
self-presentations are undertaken to manipulate observers’ impres
-
sions, actors can also engage in self-presentations for the purpose of con
-
structing and maintaining the self and for the purpose of emotion
regulation (for a review see Leary, 1995). These motives for self-presen
-
tation suggest that people should more often want to depict themselves
positively to others than negatively. These considerations imply that
DESCRIBING AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL EVENTS 569
positivity should be more frequent than negativity in the description of
autobiographical events to others.
However, in several studies we have attempted to look for this
positivity effect in discourse and have yet to find evidence for it. The
data depicted in Table 2 are characteristic of our findings. Peoples’ per
-
ceptions of the extent to which they rehearse events are related to the
emotional extremity of the event but not to its valence. In considering
this finding, one might argue that, when discussing autobiographical
events, the need to be interesting and relevant to the discourse domi
-
nates, overriding other factors that might be expected to lead to
positivity in discourse content. After all, social psychologists have often
speculated that because negative events are non-normative, they are
perceived to be more informative than positive events (Jones & Davis,
1965; Ybarra, Stephan, & Schaberg, 2000). Successfully consuming a cup
of tea in the morning is pleasant, but is hardly a “newsworthy” event.
However, spilling that tea on one’s trousers might be newsworthy, ei-
ther as an excuse for being late to a meeting or as an amusing story to tell
to one’s friends at a party. Certainly, many news organizations seem to
have discovered that there is good reason to follow the maxim “if it
bleeds, it leads.” People who describe their autobiographical events to
others might similarly come to expect that negative events are often
more interesting to others than positive.
The focus on the interest value of an event might have at least one
other effect on the content of social discourse. This norm might induce a
speaker to alter or embellish the details of thestory to make the tale more
interesting to a listener. For example, a colleague of one of the authors is
fond of describing a pickup softball game in which the author threw the
colleague out at home as the colleague tried to score from second base on
a single. As that story has been retold over the years, the throw has taken
on mythic qualities, and there is no doubt that the colleague has come to
believe that the throw was a prodigious one. Unfortunately, the author
who made the throw remembers things quite differently. The single was
sharply hit, the ball was quickly corralled in medium-center field, and
the colleague was not the speediest runner on the base paths. Hence, the
author’s memory is that the throw, while good, was not especially diffi
-
cult or noteworthy (it is also interesting to note that the author does not
chooseto correct the colleague’s memory when he tells the story—which
must say something interesting about the low moral qualities of the au
-
thor). It is clear that the colleague’s attempt to make the story interesting
to an audience caused the colleague to exaggerate some details of the
story. Across time, these exaggerations, in the absence of corrective
feedback, probably become indistinguishable from the original memory
trace.
570 SKOWRONSKI AND WALKER
Such errors seem to reflect the idea that failures in reality monitoring
(Johnson, 1988) can lead to memory errors. However, because another
discourse norm is honesty in communications (see “Keep it Real, below),
an individual whose recollections too often deviated from the verifiable
truth would probably experience social approbation. Such approbation
would likely induce people to make some effort to distinguish between
reality and distortion in their memories. Nonetheless, it is certainly pos
-
sible that people might lose track of the distortions intentionally intro
-
duced into their stories and come to report them as ”truth," especially
when there is little possibility of corrective feedback from others. In fact,
one wonders whether in such cases the original information is retriev
-
able from memory, or whether it has been replaced or rendered inacces
-
sible by the repetition of the erroneous information in social discourse.
The issue of the retrievability of the original information in the face of
embellishments or distortions is an issue that has been long debated
with regard to the misinformation effect (e.g., Zaragoza & Koshmider,
1989).
A final point to be made about the norm of relevance in communica-
tions is that a focus on relevance in discourse might lead to possible vio-
lation of other conversational norms, such as the norm emphasizing the
conveyance of new information. For example, in some interactions, the
point of the conversation might be levity. In pursuit of this goal, one
might recount an amusing anecdote from one’s life, even though others
have already heard the anecdote. This repetition is tolerated because the
laughter provoked by the story is the main point of the communication,
not the information conveyed by the story’s content. Similarly, in trying
to comfort a disheartened colleague, one might relate a well-known epi
-
sode from one’s past in an attempt to make a point (e.g., “Remember
when I worked on that memory paradigm? It took years before I finally
got that to work correctly.”). These examples suggest that those autobio
-
graphical events that are thought to be particularly “useful” or “illustra
-
tive” are repeatedly used when circumstances are right, and the high
frequency with which such events are repeated should substantially
influence memory for those events.
KEEP IT REAL
Despite the fact that people sometimes introduce distortions into their
event descriptions for purposes such as entertainment or self-presenta
-
tion, conversational norms generally prescribe honesty in discourse.
When this norm is active, it can affect thecontent of an event description.
Certainly, such a norm might cause reluctance to describe events or
DESCRIBING AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL EVENTS 571
event details that are implausible unless they can be verified by external
sources (e.g., photographs) or other witnesses. For example, some mod
-
els of autobiographical memory suggest that autobiographical memo
-
ries are retrieved via a process of cyclic retrieval: The memory is
reconstructed online until retrieval is deemed complete (Conway, 1996).
This process implies that memories will sometimes not be deemed com
-
plete. Thus, when honesty norms are salient, people may be reluctant to
report events or event details of which they are unsure. It logically fol
-
lows that the events or details that are not described will not be subject to
the beneficial or biasing effects of public rehearsal.
However, as noted earlier, people sometimes violate this honesty
norm and lie about events in their lives. Vogl (2001) documented such
lies using a diary procedure in which people kept track of the lies they
told for several weeks. This research documented instances of lying
across an array of situations. Telling such lies is often an experience that
is associated with nervousness (of course, that is one principle that un-
derlies the science of lie detection). For example, White and Burgoon
(2001) found thatlying increased the anxiety levels of the speaker and re-
duced the involvement of both the listener and the speaker in the
conversation.
However, the impact of that arousal on memory is unclear. On the one
hand, the unpleasantness associated with the lie mightserve to suppress
further rehearsal of the event (either public or private). Hence, it logi-
cally follows that the events or details that are not described should not
be subject to the beneficial or biasing effects of public rehearsal. Alterna-
tively, the arousal associated with the lie (or with the lie’s potential con-
sequences) might serve as an additional motivation to make sure that
one is able to keep the lie and the original event description distinct in
memory. The information processing that is necessary to keep these two
memories distinct might serve to enhance memory for the original event
(or for certain event details).
One other complicating factor is that arousal itself has implications for
information processing (Kolanczyk, 2001). For example, some research
suggests that a high arousal level clouds information processing ability.
If this is the case, people who lie about an event in a public contest might
be more susceptible to misinformation-type effects in event memories.
For example, assume that one is involved in a discussion of an event. In
the course of this discussion, the other discussants might describe event
details that differ from the details that an individual recalls, or provide
details that the individual might not recall at all. Such publicly provided
details can become incorporated into a person’s event memory (Betz, et
al., 1996). This effect might be even more likely to occur as a result of the
arousal associated with telling an event-related lie. That is, because high
572 SKOWRONSKI AND WALKER
arousal might cause people to be less likely to cognitively counterargue
the event detail as it is encountered (Eagly, Kulesa, Brannon, Shaw, &
Hutson-Comeaux, 2000), that false detail might have a better chance of
becoming incorporated into a person’s reconstruction of the event. If the
details obtained from others are accurate, the veracity of the later event
memory reconstruction may increase as a result of the
socially-transmitted information; if not, the subsequent event memory
can become more distorted.
KEEP IT BRIEF
Because the social rehearsal of event memories consumes cognitive and
temporal resources, it makes sense that listeners do not want superflu
-
ous detail. Note that this does not mean that listeners do not want a
richly detailed narrative (after all, such details are often essential to a
good story), but they do expect that the discourse will proceed in a
timely manner. This norm allows the listener both to process the input
without becoming bored and to respond appropriately. Speakers who
violate this rule risk social approbation. For example, Pushkar et al.
(2000) asked 198 older adults to engage in the simple conversational task
of getting acquainted in small groups. Some of these people engaged in
very lengthy self-descriptions and were generally less interested in the
input of others. These individuals received significantly more negative
reactions from listeners than other speakers and tended to respond to
the negativity by cutting short their conversational input.
The emphasis on brevity in descriptions has implications for subse
-
quent event memory. One’s initial memory for an event might contain a
great deal of information: sights, sounds, smells, emotions, etc. Because
many of these kinds of cues are not central to the event, they may not be
conveyed in the description of the event. This should occur because of
the need to focus on the high points of the event and to ignore the periph
-
eral details when the event is discussed. Consequently, these kinds of
noncentral cues may tend to fall away from the memory with repeated
event retellings. One additional implication of this process is that even
though repeated discussion of an event might cause an event to be
highly accessible in memory, with repeated retelling the event might
tend to become increasingly semantic in nature. Such events seemingly
correspond to Brewer’s (1996) description of events that are autobio
-
graphical facts—events in a person’s life that are recalled, but without ac
-
companying sensory or experiential details. In comparison an event that
is rehearsed frequently, but only during private reflections, may retain
DESCRIBING AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL EVENTS 573
its sensory qualities and remain, to use Brewer’s term, a recollective
memory.
KEEP IT UNDERSTANDABLE
When conveying an event, a speaker has to take the characteristics of the
audience into consideration. Often, this means that an event has to be
communicatedin such a way that the audience can understand the event
(Krauss & Fussell, 1991). One implication of this idea is that a speaker
might include extra-event information in the communication, such as
comparisons or metaphors, that they think might be useful to a listener.
For example, in describing a recent blind date, a woman might describe
the date as a person who “looks like Brad Pitt.”
The inclusion of such comparisons or metaphors might influence later
memory for an event. For example, as illustrated by Bartlett’s (1932) work
on image reproduction, attaching a verbal label to an ambiguous stimulus
can distort later memory for the stimulus. We are proposing a similar pro-
cess here. That is, if asked to reconstruct the appearance of the blind date
after using the Brad Pitt metaphor in descriptions to others, the speaker
might later experience distortion in recalling the date’s image so that it
more closely resembles Brad Pitt.
ALLOW FOR FEEDBACK
Evidence suggests that feedback is important in conversation and has
benefits for both the speaker and the audience. Kraut, Lewis, & Swezy
(1982) asked participants in a study to watch a movie and later describe
the movie to either one or two listeners. When there were two listeners,
one was instructed to give feedback and the other was instructed to lis
-
ten passively. On a later test, the listener who provided feedback had
higher levels of comprehension of the film than the passive listener. Ad
-
ditional data suggested that when speakers received more feedback,
they were more likely to provide more details. This additional input al
-
lowed both listeners to understand the film better.
We suggest that such feedback can affect the memory of the event de
-
scriber as well as the memory of description of the listener. For example,
encouraging the production of greater numbers of event details obvi
-
ously increases the probability that those details will be later recalled.
However, encouraging description elaboration via listener feedback
may also increase the probability that an event memory would be sub
-
574 SKOWRONSKI AND WALKER
ject to any memory-distorting effects that might occur as a result of the
inclusion of false details in the event description.
Moreover, the accumulated impact of such feedback over time can
help to explain between-group differences in the kinds of autobiograph
-
ical memories that people report. For example, Eastern cultures may dis
-
courage event descriptions that focus on the self at the expense of the
group. Wang (2001) hypothesizes that mothers may convey this norm to
children by the feedback that they provide in the course of mother-child
interactions. Different events might be encouraged or discouraged by
mothers from Western cultures and Eastern cultures, leading children to
self-edit their event descriptions. Over time, these self-editing effects
may ultimately come to bias autobiographical memory. For example, in
Eastern cultures such editing may come to favor recall of group-relevant
events relative to recall of individual events. Similar effects might be a
cause of gender differences in autobiographical memory (Fivush, 1998).
For example, positive feedback in interactions with others may encour-
age males to remember male stereotype-consistent events while dis-
couraging memory for other events. Similar effects may bias females’
event memories, which may focus on interpersonal events relative to
achievement-oriented events.
One other potentially important aspect of feedback might have to do
with the emotions that are experienced when events are recalled. Recent
research (Walker et al., 1997; Walker et al., 2003) suggests that the ex-
tremity of affect associated with negative events fades faster than the ex-
tremity of affect associated with positive affect. Additional research
(Skowronski et al., in press) shows that this effect is moderated by social
discourse. When events are frequently discussed with others, the fading
affect bias is particularly large. When events are not frequently dis
-
cussed, the fading affect bias is small or nonexistent. Moreover, a similar
effect holds for the diversity of the audience for an event description. In
-
dependent of description frequency, the fading affect bias is particularly
large when the event was described to many different types of people;
the bias is small or nonexistent when the event was described only to a
few types of people.
Listener feedback may play a role in these effects. For example, imag
-
ine that a person describes a negative event to a friend. That friend might
respond to the event description with messages of support, entreaties to
avoid self-blame, and encouragement to blame the negative event on
other people or on bad luck that will not be repeated. On the other hand,
imagine that a person describes a positive event to a friend. That friend
might reply to that event with expressions of joy, attributing that good
event to the describer’s personal qualities and implying that the event
might be likely to occur again in the future. It is nothard to imagine how,
DESCRIBING AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL EVENTS 575
over time, such reactions might impact the affect associated with re
-
called events. That is, the soothing words of the listener might help to re
-
move the sting of negative events, while the joy of friends might help to
maintain the elation associated with positive events.
Of course, these social reactions may not be the only mechanism that
contributes to the fading affect bias. Pennebaker (1997a, 1997b) has
shown that merely describing negative events, even with no audience
present, can help to ease the sting of negative events across time.
Pennebaker speculates that the reduction in affect is a consequence of
the cognitive work that one exerts as one constructs the event descrip
-
tion. We do not dispute this mechanism. Instead, we offer the reactions
of listeners as an additional mechanism that can help to explain valence
differences in the extent to which the intensity of affect associated with
event memories changes over time.
HAVE A POINT
Listeners expect that event memories that are described during dis-
course should make a contribution to the consensually accepted pur-
pose of the interaction. One of the goals that speakers often have when
disclosing event memories is to convey to a listener a general sense of the
speaker’s traits and beliefs. Thus, in an attempt to be maximally infor-
mative, one might recount certain “critical” incidents from one’s life that
one thinks are especially indicative of one’s traits or goals, and use them
frequently in the course of attempting to convey one’s internal charac-
teristics to others (McAdams, 2001; Singer & Bluck, 2001).
This idea that the communication of events often conveys information
about the self is also reflected in the findings of Barbara Woike and her
colleagues (Woike, Gershkovich, Piorkowski, & Polo, 1999; Woike,
Lavezzary, & Barksy, 2001). Woike has distinguished between individu
-
als who are agentic (motivated by self-interests) and individuals who
are communal (motivated by group interests). In social discourse,
agentic individuals often report memories that reflect the themes of so
-
cial separation, self-mastery, anddifferentiation, highlighting their indi
-
viduality and their experiences as individuals. In comparison,
communal individuals report memories that reflect themes of friend
-
ship, love, and social connection, highlighting their connection to other
people.
We speculate that these attempts to describe “the self” to others
should have predictable effects on autobiographical memory. That is,
with increasing repetition such events should be more likely to be re
-
called as autobiographical facts than as recollective memories. Further
-
576 SKOWRONSKI AND WALKER
more, the memories of these events should also be more likely to
incorporate distortions if those distortions are included in the event de
-
scriptions provided to others. Finally, the intensity of affect associated
with such frequently described events should reflect the fading affect
bias: Recall of frequently described negative events should provoke less
negative affect, and frequently described positive events more positive
affect, than the affect provoked by events that are not described to
others.
COGNITION AND THE DESCRIPTION OF
AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL EVENT MEMORIES
It should by now be obvious that there are many cognitive mechanisms
than can help to explain how event retellings could alter one’s ability to
remember the event later (see Pasupathi, 2001). We have already re-
ferred to a number of different cognitive mechanisms that might play a
role in the alteration of autobiographical memories as a consequence of
event descriptions. At least five such mechanisms are relatively
straightforward.
First, it is an axiom of memory that rehearsal influences recall. Those
events that an individual chooses to convey to others should be better re-
called than events that are not conveyed. In fact, recent data that we have
collected suggests that social interaction might be particularly impor-
tant in prompting rehearsal (Walker et al., 2003). In several studies, we
have asked people to recall autobiographical events and to report how
often they have rehearsed those events. Participants in these studies
overwhelmingly reported that the most frequent reason for the re
-
hearsal of autobiographical events was to describe the event to others.
These social rehearsals may prompt a second cognitive mechanism
that can affect autobiographical memory. The retelling of autobiograph
-
ical stories often involves elaborative rehearsal of a memory, and it is an
axiom that such elaborative rehearsals are particularly good at building
memory for an event. However, these elaborations also have the poten
-
tial to shape or distort event memories. Consider the consequences of the
inclusion of extra-event information, such as metaphors or compari
-
sons, in event retellings. For example, in describing the car that ran him
off the road, a speaker might say: “I couldn’t identify the car, but it re
-
sembled a DeLorean.” When later memory is tested, reconstruction of
the car might be distorted to increasingly resemble the DeLorean. Fi
-
nally, these elaborations can also have consequences for the affect pro
-
voked by an event memory. As noted earlier, Pennebaker (1997a, 1997b)
speculates that the cognitive work that one exerts as one constructs a
DESCRIBING AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL EVENTS 577
description of a negative event can work to reduce the affect associated
with that event.
Third, in retelling events a speaker is selective in the retelling: Some
event details might be emphasized in preference to other details, some
might be altered, while still others might be omitted. Such selectivity ob
-
viously has the potential to shape an individual’s memory for an event.
Details excluded from descriptions should be less likely to be remem
-
bered, and false event details that are inserted in such descriptions
should be more likely to be erroneously “recalled” as being part of the
initial event.
Fourth, the process of describing events to others might alter the na
-
tureof the memory representation or might providea memory represen
-
tation that competes with the original memory trace. For example,
consider the phenomenon of verbal overshadowing (Dodson, Johnson,
& Schooler, 1997). One possible consequence of describing events is that
an event memory might have dual representation. One of these would
be the memory for the event description; the second would be the epi-
sodic memory of the original event. At recall, memory for the descrip-
tion might compete with the original memory trace. Hence, while the
occurrence of an oft-described event might be easily remembered, the
specific perceptual details of the event might become increasingly
difficult to retrieve.
Fifth, autobiographical memory might be shaped by the anticipation
of future events that are included in the course of conversations about
events that have not yet occurred. That is, when discussing what one is
going to do tomorrow, one can establish a cognitive structure that can
later be used to encode and retrieve relevant events. Memory research
suggests that events that are consistent with an established organiza
-
tional structure tend to be more easily encoded and retrieved in free re
-
call tasks; events that violate the structure tend to be given more
attention and elaboration, and might be better recalled in recognition
tasks. Hence, by discussing events that have not yet occurred with oth
-
ers, one is establishing a mental structure that might be used to process
information about the event when it occurs.
An understanding of how the conveyance of autobiographical events
affects memory also requires attention to the organization and structure
of memory. Indeed, a substantial body of evidence suggests that auto
-
biographical memory is highly organized (e.g., Conway, 1990; Conway
& Bekerian, 1987; Thompson, et al., 1996). For example, a large number
of studies seem to show that time is one variable that can serve to orga
-
nize recall. For example, Anderson and Conway (1993) found that peo
-
ple were better able to remember event memories when cued with the
beginning and end points of an event than when cued with other event
578 SKOWRONSKI AND WALKER
characteristics. The results of other studies suggest that events that are
widely separated in time are easier to order than events that are closely
spaced in time, an outcome known as the temporal distance effect (e.g.,
Underwood, 1977). Skowronski, Walker, and Betz (2003; 2004) have re
-
cently shown that this temporal distance effect occurs even when one
controls for event memory, suggesting that there is a temporal compo
-
nent to memory that is not entirely dependent on the details of the
memory itself.
The origin of this organization, however, is a point of some debate.
One currently popular model is the Self-Memory System Model (Conway
& Pleydell-Pearce, 2000). The strength of the Self-Memory System
Model is that it provides a definitive mentalstructure that serves to orga
-
nize and structure autobiographical memory and a clear set of processes
that are thought to operate on that structure. In this model, autobio
-
graphical memory is postulated to be a multiply structured knowledge
base. Proponents of this model argue that autobiographical memory
takes the form of partonomic hierarchical knowledgestructures: Knowl-
edge of a specific event is a part of knowledge about general events,
which itself may be a part of lifetime periods or lifetime themes (see Fig-
ure 2). As a consequence, descriptions of event memories often contain
all three kinds of information. An individual may provide a broad tem-
poral context (e.g., when I was in grade school), information about a gen-
eral event (e.g., I was serving as an altar boy) and specific event detail
(e.g., I sneaked a slug of sacramental wine).
Conway and Pleydell-Pearce suggest that events enter the autobio-
graphical knowledge base because of their relevance to the personal
goals present at event occurrence. Hence, events that are not relevant to
personal goals will not be well recalled. Moreover, rehearsal is thought
to be particularly important to one’s ability to access event-specific
knowledge after that knowledge has been stored (Burt, Watt, Mitchell, &
Conway, 1998). Even though an event might have been initially storedin
the event-specific knowledge base, in the absence of rehearsal access to
specific event detail is thought to become increasingly difficult.
It is important to note that this theory views autobiographical memories
as generated transitory constructions rather than stable event memories.
This conception of memory allows the self-memory system to be respon
-
sive to the changing goals of the self and to the situational demands of
event recall. At the heart of this theory is the process of cyclic retrieval.
Event details are retrieved in a cyclic pattern until the parameters of the
recall task are approximated. In other words, people keep attempting to
retrieve information until they judge that some criterion is reached.
Given the judgment component that is incorporated into the model, it
is not surprising that executive control processes play a central role in
DESCRIBING AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL EVENTS 579
the Working Self System Model. These processes lend structure to auto
-
biographical memory at input and regulate access to event-specific
knowledge during retrieval. These control processes are thought to be
responsible for transforming the goals of the self into temporal and the
-
matic structures in which to house the retrieved event details. These con
-
trol processes also are thought to operate during retrieval by regulating
how much effort is expended in accessing event-specific knowledge
during the process of memory reconstruction and by regulating deci
-
sions about the amount of event-specific knowledge that is required
580 SKOWRONSKI AND WALKER
FIGURE 2. The autobiographical memory knowledge base of the Working Self
Model.
Event-specific knowledge (ESK) is shown as an undifferentiated pool of knowledge,
regions of which (the circles) are activated by cues held at the general event level. The
small squares indicate activated ESK (Conway, 1996). Reprinted with permission.
prior to making a decision about whether an event has been
“remembered.”
Conway and Pleydell-Pearce (2000) attempted to pit the Self-Memory
System Model against a social interactionist view of autobiographical
memory, claiming that the Working Self Model can better account for a
number of memory-related phenomena. Examples that they provide
are: The relation of goals to memories, distortions of memories,
neuropsychological and clinical disruptions of autobiographical mem
-
ory, changes in autobiographical memory in adolescence, and different
types of autobiographical memory. While we agree with Conway and
Pleydell-Pearce about the utility of their formulation, we are somewhat
puzzled by the fact that they view the social interactionist position as in
-
compatible with their model. We obviously see the two positions as
quite compatible. There are a number of elements in the social
interactionist position that can be easily and directly placed into the
working self framework, with minimal modification to that framework.
The first element is obvious: Rehearsal. In the Self-Memory System
Model, rehearsal is a process that is crucial to the ability to recall event
details. We have similarly argued that rehearsal is important to the
maintenance of a memory. However, we have also argued that there are
different kinds of rehearsal, and these different rehearsal types might
have different effects on memory. We think that these differing effects
can be understood by reference to the different components of memory
in the Self-Memory System Model and whether those components are
repeatedly accessed together when reconstructing a memory. Such con-
temporaneous access is often the case when one recounts a story to oth-
ers: The story needs to be put in proper temporal and situational life
context, so if a story is repeatedly told these contextual elements will of
-
ten be activated and used in the memory reconstruction. It follows that
with repeated retellings these contextual elements will routinely be ac
-
cessed with the memory itself. In comparison, events that are not fre
-
quently conveyed to others, but that may be privately rehearsed, may
not acquire these contextual elements. Instead, the memories may retain
their sensory qualities and be less likely to be seen as part of a lifetime pe
-
riod when the event is recalled. These considerations lead to a straight
-
forward prediction: Events that are frequently recounted to others will
be more easily placed into a lifetime period than events that are not, even
equating for overall rehearsal frequency.
The social conveyance of an event’s temporal context also helps to
solve another conceptual problem with respect to the ability to place
events in time. How does an event get temporally recoded as the event
ages? That is, yesterday’s event becomes last week’s event, which be
-
comes last month’s event, which becomes last semester’s event, which
DESCRIBING AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL EVENTS 581
becomes last year’s event, which becomes that event that happened
while I was working at Ohio State. How does such recoding occur? The
Self-Memory System Model provides the structure for the temporal
recoding of such events by suggesting that the self works to structure
memory into meaningful lifetime periods. However, the Self-Memory
System Model has no ready mechanism to provide for recoding of
events into these meaningful time periods as an event (and a person)
ages. One can use the details of event-specific knowledge to reconstruct
the period during which an event occurred, but it would certainly be
cumbersome to do that every time a memory is accessed. Social dis
-
course provides a ready mechanism for this recoding. When telling sto
-
ries one must often place events in temporal context. These conveyed
temporal contexts will change with time (e.g., yesterday, last month, last
year, etc.) and will hence change the lifetime period associated with an
event as a story is retold.
This possibility leads to a couple of intriguing predictions. The first is
that events that are frequently rehearsed will be more easily subject to
within-period ordering confusion than events that are not. That is, if
each of two events is frequently described to others as occurring “during
college” it should be more difficult to order those events than to order
two events that are equivalent in temporal distance but that have not
been frequently disclosed to others. However, the situation ought to re-
verse for events that occur in different time periods. That is, because of
the easy accessibility of life theme labels, a high school event should be
more easily placed in time as coming before a college event if those two
events have been frequently discussed with others than if those two
events have not.
The control parameters that are built into the Self-Memory System
Model also seem to be a perfect vehicle for the operation of social norms
during recall. That is, the rules of discourse—keep it real, keep it brief,
keep it fresh, keep it relevant, have a point—can also be thought of as so
-
cial norms that might serve to partially govern the memory search and
reconstruction process. Hence, when asked to remember and recount an
autobiographical event, people have an internalized representation of
the level of detail and accuracy that is required to fulfill the memory re
-
quest and conduct their memory search until that level of detail and ac
-
curacy is reached. Moreover, the expected level of detail and accuracy
can be expected to change across situations, and via the internalization
of social norms the Self-Memory System Model’s control processes
would be sensitive to these variations. One might work harder to come
up with a more detailed and a more accurate retelling of events if one is
testifying at a trial than if one is responding to a stranger’s request for
event information. Furthermore, the decision criterion that is used to in
-
582 SKOWRONSKI AND WALKER
clude a fact as a part of a reconstructed memory representation might
change as a function of situational changes. Hence, errors in reality mon
-
itoring that might routinely be observed in laboratory contexts in which
relatively little is at stake might be less likely when social pressure
demands only verifiable facts.
Conway and Pleydell-Pearce’s Self-Memory System Model also em
-
phasizes the role of the self-concept in the storage and reconstruction of
autobiographical event memories. However, it is an axiom in social psy
-
chology that the self is partly a social construction. Because one of the
central concepts of the Self-Memory System Model is that the goal struc
-
ture of the working self is critical in both the encoding and retrieval of
autobiographical knowledge, it stands to reason that the social factors
that contribute to the self must also play a role in regulating autobio
-
graphical memory. For example, Conway and Pleydell-Pearce cite the
work of McAdams (1993) as evidence for their goal-oriented position.
However, it is hard not to notice that much of McAdams’s work con-
cerns the social themes of peoples’ lives, notably the themes of power
and intimacy. Moreover, given that intimacy and power are often goals
that people have in their social discourse, and given that Conway and
Pleydell-Pearce argue that the current state of the working self may de-
termine which autobiographical knowledge is accessed and how that
knowledge can be constructed into a memory, it again seems reasonable
to speculate that when these social motives are activated during the
course of conversation they will work to shape the recall of
autobiographical memory.
SUMMARY AND CONCLUSION
In this article we have attempted to articulate our view of the possible
impact of social discourse on autobiographical memory. We reviewed
literature documenting the impact of social factors, including culture,
social roles, and social discourse rehearsals, on aspects of autobiograph
-
ical memory. We described several social norms that govern social dis
-
course and speculated about the effect that such norms might have on
autobiographical memory. We reviewed the mental structures and pro
-
cesses that might serve to mediate the relation between social discourse
and autobiographical memory and offered suggestions about how both
social and cognitive factors might be integrated into a common model
accounting for autobiographical memory.
While others have treated this same topic, we think that our approach
is novel because of our attempt to provide a relatively integrated view of
the phenomenon. That is, we discussed how some factors that operate at
the group level (e.g., discourse norms) might regulate discourse, and
DESCRIBING AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL EVENTS 583
hence might impact autobiographical memory. However, at the same
time we also attempted to describe some of the mental processes and
structures that are activated during social discourse and how those
might work to alter autobiographical memory. Our pluralistic approach
to the issue offers a potential bridge between the ideas of those who fo
-
cus on the operation of cognitive mechanisms in autobiographical mem
-
ory (e.g., Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000) and those who focus on
social and cultural effects on autobiographical memory (e.g., Pasupathi,
2001).
Indeed, such a bridge might be long past due, especially in the eyes of
commentators who deride research in both social psychology and in
cognition for its focus on laboratory phenomena that may have little to
do with the social context of behavior (e.g., Woll, 2002). Here is a
real-world phenomenon—autobiographical memory—that can be rig
-
orously analyzed and that can serve as a proving ground for seminal
ideas in social psychology and cognitive psychology as well as serving
as a crucible for the discovery of new phenomena (such as the reminis-
cence bump—see Rubin & Schulkind, 1997). We do not claim to account
for all possible cognitive or social influences on autobiographical mem-
ory—future research will certainly show that variables other than those
discussed in the present article will certainly have an impact on autobio-
graphical memory. Nonetheless, we think that our attempt represents a
useful step toward understanding autobiographical memory, and we
hope that empirical investigation of the ideas and hypotheses embodied
in this article will spur a deeper understanding of the social, personal,
cognitive, and developmental mechanisms that contribute to our
memories about ourselves.
REFERENCES
Anderson, S.J., & Conway, M.A. (1993). Investigating the structure of autobiographi
-
cal memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cogni
-
tion, 19, 1178-1196.
Bahrick, H.P., Hall, L.K., & Berger, S.A. (1996). Accuracy and distortion in memory
for high school grades. Psychological Sciences, 7, 265-271.
Barclay, C.R. (1996). Autobiographical remembering: Narrative constraints on
objectified selves. In D.C. Rubin (Ed)., Remembering our past: Studies in autobio
-
graphical memory (pp. 94-125). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Bartlett, F.C. (1923). Psychology and primitive culture. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Uni
-
versity Press.
Bartlett, F.C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. New
York: Macmillan.
Bergman, E.T., & Roediger, H.L., III. (1999) Can Bartlett’s repeated reproduction ex
-
periments be replicated? Memory & Cognition, 27, 937-947.
584 SKOWRONSKI AND WALKER
Betz, A.L., & Skowronski, J.J. (1997). Self-events and other-events: Temporal dating
and event memory. Memory and Cognition, 25, 701-714.
Betz, A.L., Skowronski, J.J., & Ostrom, T.M. (1996). Shared realities: Social influence
and stimulus memory. Social Cognition, 14, 113-140.
Bluck, S. Alea, N., Habermas, T., & Rubin, D.C. (in press). A TALE of three functions:
Self-reported uses of autobiographical memory. Social Cognition.
Brewer, W. F. (1986). What is autobiographical memory? In D. Rubin (Ed.), Autobio
-
graphical memory (pp. 25-49). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Brewer, W. F. (1988). Memory for randomly sampled autobiographical events. In U.
Neisser & E. Winograd (Eds.), Remembering reconsidered: Ecological and tradi
-
tional approaches to the study of memory (pp. 21-90). New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Brewer, W. F. (1996). What is recollective memory? In D. C. Rubin (Ed.), Remembering
our past: Studies in autobiographical memory(pp.19-66).Cambridge, : Cambridge
University Press.
Brown, N.R., & Schopflocher, D. (1998). Event clusters: An organization of personal
events in autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 9, 470-475.
Burt, C.D.B., Watt, S.C., Mitchell, D.A., & Conway, M. A. (1998). Retrieving the se
-
quence of autobiographical event components. Applied Cognitive Psychology,
12, 321-338.
Cason, H. (1932). The learning and retention of pleasant and unpleasant activities.
Archives of Psychology, 134, 1-96.
Ceci, S. J., & Loftus, E. F. (1994). Memory work: The royal road to false memories? Ap-
plied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 351-364.
Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Clark, H. H., & Schober, M. F. (1992). Asking questions and influencing answers. In J.
M. Tanur (Ed.), Questions about questions: Inquiries into the cognitive bases of sur-
veys. New York: Russell Sage.
Colegrove, F. W. (1983/1899). The day they heard about Lincoln. In U. Neisser (Ed.),
Memory observed: Remembering in natural contexts (pp. 41-42). New York: W. H.
Freeman.
Conway, M.A. (1990). Autobiographical memory: An introduction. Buckingham, UK:
Open University Press.
Conway, M. A. (1996). Autobiographical memories and autobiographical knowl
-
edge. In D. C. Rubin (Ed.), Remembering our past: Studies in autobiographical
memory (pp. 67-93). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Conway, M.A. (Ed). (1997). Recovered memories and false memories. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Conway, M.A., & Bekerian, D.A. (1987). Organization in autobiographical memory.
Memory and Cognition, (15)1, 145-191.
Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographi
-
cal memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107, 261-288.
Dodson, C.S., Johnson, M.K., & Schooler, J.W. (1997). The verbal overshadowing ef
-
fect: Source confusion or strategy shift? Memory & Cognition, 25, 129-139.
Dritschel, B.H. (1991). Autobiographical memory in natural discourse: A method
-
ological note. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 5, 319-330.
Durkheim, E. (1895/1982). Therulesofsociologicalmethod. London: Macmillian Press.
Eagly, A.H., Kulesa, P., Brannon, L.A., Shaw, K., & Hutson-Comeaux, S. (2000). Why
DESCRIBING AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL EVENTS 585
counterattitudinal messages are as memorable as proattitudinal messages:
The importance of active defense against attack. Personality & Social Psychology
Bulletin, 26, 1392-1408.
Fivush, R. (1998). Gendered narratives: Elaboration, structure, and emotion in par
-
ent-child reminiscing across the preschool years. In C.P. Thompson, D.J.
Herrmann, D. Bruce, J.D. Read, D.G. Payne, & M. Toglia (Eds.), Autobiographi
-
cal memory: Theoretical and applied perspectives (pp. 79-104). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Ford, T.E., & Stangor, C. (1992). The role of diagnosticity in stereotype formation: Per
-
ceiving group means and variances. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
63, 356-367.
Greenwald, A.G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: The fabrication and revision of personal
history. American Psychologist, 35, 603-618.
Grice, H.P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J.L. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and
semantics 3: Speech acts. New York: Academic Press.
Halbwachs, M. (1951/1980). The collective memory. New York: Harper & Row.
Hartup, W. W., & Stevens, N. (1997). Friendship and adaptation in the life course.
Psychological Bulletin, 121, 355-370.
Higgins, E.T. (1992). Achieving “shared reality” in the communication game: A social
action that creates meaning. Journal ofLanguage & Social Psychology, 11, 107-131.
Hinsz, V.B. (1990). Cognitive and consensus processes in group recognition memory
performance. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 5, 705-718.
Hirst, W., & Manier, D. (1996). Remembering as communication: A family recounts
its past. In D.C. Rubin (Ed.), Remembering our past: Studies in autobiographical
memory (pp. 271-290). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Hirst, W., Manier, D., & Apetroaia, I. (1997). The social construction of the remem-
bered self: Family recounting. In J.G. Snodgrass & R.L. Thompson, (Eds.), The
self across psychology: Self-recognition, self-awareness, and the self concept. Annals of
the New York Academy of Sciences (Vol. 818, pp. 163-188). New York: New York
Academy of Sciences.
Hoffman, H.G., Granhag, P.A., Kwong See, S.T., & Loftus, E. F. (2001). Social influ
-
ences on reality-monitoring decisions. Memory & Cognition, 29, 394-404.
Hollingshead, A.B. (1998). Communication, learning, and retrieval in transactive
memory systems. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 423-442.
Hollingshead, A.B. (2001). Cognitive interdependence and convergent expectations
in transactive memory. Journal ofPersonality & Social Psychology, 81, 1080–1089.
Holmes, D. S. (1970). Differential change in affective intensity and the forgetting of
unpleasant personal experiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3,
234-239.
Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L.V., & Prohaska, V. (1992). Memory for day of the week: A
5 + 2 day cycle. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 313-325.
Johnson, M.K. (1988). Reality monitoring: An experimental phenomenological ap
-
proach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 390-394.
Jones, E.E., & Davis, K.E. (1965). From acts to dispositions: The attribution process in
person perception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimentalsocial psychol
-
ogy (Vol. 2, pp. 219-266). New York: Academic Press.
Kolanczyk, A. (2001). The role of affect and attention in preconscious and conscious
processing. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 32, 27-37.
586 SKOWRONSKI AND WALKER
Krauss, R.M., & Fussell, S.R. (1991). Perspective-taking in communication: Represen
-
tations of others’ knowledge in reference. Social Cognition, 9, 2-24.
Kraut, R.E., Lewis, S.H., & Swezy, L.W. (1982). Listener responsiveness and the coor
-
dination of conversation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43,
718-731.
Leary, M.R. (1995). Self-presentation: Impression management and interpersonal behavior.
Madison, WI: Brown & Benchmark.
Leichtman, M.D., Wang,Q., & Pillemer, D. (2003). Cultural variations in
interdepedence and autobiographical memory: Lessons from Korea, China,
India, and the United States. In R. Fivush, & C.A. Haden (Eds.), Autobiographi
-
cal memory and the construction of the narrative self: Developmental and cultural per
-
spectives (pp. 73-98). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Linton, M. (1986). Ways of searching and the contents of memory. In D.C. Rubin
(Ed.), Autobiographical memory (pp. 50-67). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Uni
-
versity Press.
Loftus, E.F. (1975). Leading questions and the eyewitness report. Cognitive Psychol
-
ogy, 7, 560-572.
Loftus, E.F. (1993). The reality of repressed memories. American Psychologist, 48,
518-537.
Loftus, E.F., & Loftus, G. R. (1980). On the permanence of stored information in the
human brain. American Psychologist, 35, 409-420.
Mazzoni, G.A.L., Loftus, E. F., & Kirsch, I. (2001). Changing beliefs about implausible
autobiographical events: A little plausibility goes a long way. Journal of Experi-
mental Psychology: Applied, 7, 51-59.
McAdams, D.P. (1993). Stories we live by: Personal myths and the making of the self. New
York: Morrow.
McAdams, D.P. (2001). The psychology of life stories. Review of General Psychology, 5,
100-122.
McCann, C. D., & Higgins, E. T. (1990). Social cognition and communication. In H.
Giles & W. P. Robinson (Eds.), Handbook of language and social psychology (pp.
13-32). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
McDermott, K.B., & Roediger, H.L. (1998). Attempting to avoid illusory memories:
Robust false recognition of associates persists under conditions of explicit
warningsand immediate testing. Journalof Memory and Language, 39, 508-520.
Mead, G.H. (1934). Mind, self and society from the standpoint of a social behaviourist. Chi
-
cago: University of Chicago Press.
Middleton, D., & Edwards, D. (Eds.) (1990). Collective remembering: Inquiries in social
construction. London: Sage Publications.
Middleton, D., & Edwards, D. (1990). Introduction. In D. Middleton & D. Edwards
(Eds.), Collective remembering: Inquiries in social construction (pp. 1-22). London:
Sage Publications.
Nadel, L., & Jacobs, W. J. (1998). Traumatic memory is special. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 7,154-157.
Neisser, U. (1978). Memory: What are the important questions? In M. M. Gruneberg,
P.E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory (pp. 3-14). London:
Academic Press.
Neisser, U. (1981). John Dean’s memory: A case study. Cognition, 9, 1-22.
Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2000). Socialization of memory. In E. Tulving, & F.I.M. Craik
DESCRIBING AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL EVENTS 587
(Eds), The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 283-295). Oxford, UK: Oxford Uni
-
versity Press.
Niederhoffer, K.G., & Pennebaker, J.W. (2002). Linguistic style matching in social in
-
teraction. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 21, 337-360.
Pasupathi, M. (2001). The social construction of the personal past and its implications
for adult development. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 651-672.
Pennebaker, J.W. (1997a). Opening up: The healing power of expressing emotions. New
York: Guilford Press.
Pennebaker, J. W. (1997b). Writing about emotional experiences as a therapeutic pro
-
cess. Psychological Science, 8, 162-166.
Pennebaker, J.W., & Harber, K.D. (1993). A social stage model of collective coping:
The Loma Prieta earthquake and the Persian Gulf War. Journal of Social Issues,
49, 125-145.
Pushkar, D., Basevitz, P., Arbuckle, T., Nohara-LeClair, M., Lapidus, S., & Peled, M.
(2000). Social behavior and off-target verbosity in elderly people. Psychology
and Aging, 15, 361-374.
Reese, E. (2002). Social factors in the development of autobiographical memory: The
state of the art. Social Development, 11, 124-142.
Roediger, H.L., & McDermott, K.B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering
words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Mem-
ory, and Cognition, 21, 803-814.
Roediger, H. L., III, Meade, M.L., & Bergman, E.T. (2001). Social contagion of mem-
ory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 365-371.
Rook, K. S. (1984). The negative side of social interaction: Impact on psychological
well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 1156-1166.
Ross, M., McFarland, C., & Fletcher, G.J. (1981). The effect of attitude on recall of per-
sonal histories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 627-634.
Ross, M., & Wilson, A.E. (2002). It feels like yesterday: Self-esteem, valence of per-
sonal past experiences, and judgments of subjective distance. Journal of Person
-
ality & Social Psychology, 82, 792-803.
Rubin, D.C., & Schulkind, M.D. (1997) The distribution of autobiographical memo
-
ries across the lifespan. Memory & Cognition, 25, 859-866.
Ruscher, J. B., & Hammer, E.D. (1994). Revising disrupted impressions through con
-
versation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 530-541.
Russell, A.W., & Schober, M.F. (1999). How beliefs about a partner’s goals affect refer
-
ring in goal-discrepant conversations. Discourse Processes, 27, 1-33.
Sherman, J.W. (1996). Development and mental representation of stereotypes. Journal
of Personality & Social Psychology, 70, 1126-1141.
Shimamura, A.P., Janowsky, J.S., & Squire, L.R. (1990). Memory for temporal order of
events in patients with frontal lobe and amnesiac lesions. Neuropsychologica,
28, 803-813.
Singer, J.A., & Bluck, S. (2001). New perspectives on autobiographical memory: The
integration of narrative processing and autobiographical reasoning. Review of
General Psychology, 5, 91-99.
Skowronski, J.J., Betz, A.L., Thompson, C.P., & Shannon, L. (1991). Social memory in
everyday life: Recall of self-events and other-events. Journal of Personality & So
-
cial Psychology, 60, 831-843.
Skowronski, J.J., Gibbons, J.A., Vogl, R.J., & Walker, W.R. (in press). Relations be
-
588 SKOWRONSKI AND WALKER
tween social discourse and the intensity of affect provoked by autobiographi
-
cal memories. Self & Identity.
Skowronski, J.J., & Thompson, C.P. (1990). Reconstructing the dates of personal
events: Gender differences in accuracy. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 4,
371-381.
Skowronski, J.J., Walker, W.R., & Betz, A.L. (2003). Ordering our world: An examina
-
tion of time in autobiographical memory. Memory, 11, 247-260.
Skowronski, J.J., Walker, W.R., & Betz, A.L. (2004). Who was I when that happened?
The timekeeping self in autobiographical memory. In D.R. Beike, J.M.
Lampien, & D.A. Behrand (Eds.). The self and memory (pp. 183-206). New York:
Psychology Press.
Sperber, D. & Wilson, D. (1986). Relevance: Communication and Cognition. Oxford,
UK: Blackwell.
Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: The mobili
-
zation-minimization hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 67-85.
Thompson, C.P. (1982). Memory for unique personal events: The roommate study.
Memory & Cognition, 10, 324-332.
Thompson, C.P., Skowronski, J.J., & Betz, A.L. (1993). The use of partial temporal in
-
formation in dating personal events. Memory & Cognition, 21, 352-360.
Thompson, C. P., Skowronski, J. J., Larsen, S. F., & Betz, A.L. (1996). Autobiographical
memory: Remembering what and remembering when. New York: Erlbaum.
Tindale, R.S., & Sheffey, S. (2002). Shared information, cognitive load, and group
memory. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 5, 5-18.
Underwood, B. J. (1977). Temporal codes for memories: Issues and problems. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Vogl, R.J. (2001). Memory for lies. Paper presented at the 2001 meeting of the Society for
Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, Kingston, ON.
Vygotsky, L.S. (1962). Thought and language. New York: Wiley.
Wagenaar, W.A. (1986). My memory: A study of autobiographical memory over six
years. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 225-252.
Waldfogel, S. (1948). The frequency and affective character of childhood memories.
Psychological Monographs, 62, Whole No. 291.
Walker, W.R., Skowronski, J.J., Gibbons, J.A., & Vogl, R.J. (2004). Why people rehearse
their memories: Frequency of use and effects on the intensity of emotions associated
with autobiographical memories. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Walker, W. R., Skowronski, J. J., Gibbons, J. A., Vogl, R. J., & Thompson, C. P. (in
press). On the emotions that accompany autobiographical memories:
Dysphoria disrupts the fading affect bias. Cognition and Emotion.
Walker, W.R., Skowronski, J.J., & Thompson, C.P. (2003). Life is pleasant and mem
-
ory makes it better! Review of General Psychology, 7, 203-210.
Walker, W. R., Vogl, R. J., & Thompson, C. P. (1997). Autobiographical memory: Un
-
pleasantness fades faster than pleasantness over time. Applied Cognitive Psy
-
chology, 11, 399-413.
Wang, Q. (2001). Culture effects on adults’ earliest childhood recollection and self-de
-
scription: Implications for the relation between memory and the self. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 220-233.
Wegner, D.M., Erber, R., & Raymond, P. (1991). Transactive memory in close rela
-
tionships. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 61, 923-929.
DESCRIBING AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL EVENTS 589
White, C.H., & Burgoon, J.K. (2001). Adaptation and communicative design: Patterns
of interaction in truthful and deceptive interactions. Human Communication Re
-
search, 27, 9-37.
Woike, B., Gershkovich, I., Piorkowski, R., & Polo, M. (1999). The role of motives in
the content and structure of autobiographical memory. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 76, 600-612.
Woike, B., Lavezzary, E., & Barsky, J. (2001). The influence of implicit motives on
memory processes. Journal of Personality Psychology, 81, 935-945.
Woll, S. (2002). Everyday thinking: Memory, reasoning and judgment in the real world.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ybarra, O., Stephan, W. G., & Schaberg, L. (2000). Misanthropic memory for the be
-
havior of group members. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 26,
1515-1525.
Zaragoza, M.S., & Koshmider, J.W. (1989). Misled subjects may know more than their
performance implies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, &
Cognition, 15, 246-255.
590 SKOWRONSKI AND WALKER
... The importance of a religious affiliation that adheres to a particular belief for FAB effects may be due to the official connection to an established group or an opportunity to share thoughts and beliefs with likeminded individuals. The opportunity to share one's events with other individuals is important in the FAB literature, as the FAB is positively related to increased social sharing [89], especially when sharing with attentive listeners [20]. Future research should determine the deeper underpinnings and explanatory mechanisms for these findings. ...
... Although the studies used to produce the data in the current study were not designed to examine many different types of mediators for its FAB effects across religious affiliations, they all included rehearsal (thinking or talking) ratings, which have been explanatory mechanisms for past FAB effects [3,29,20,88,89]. Therefore, we examined the importance of rehearsal ratings for the FAB effects across religious affiliations in the current study by statistically removing them as a covariate from the FAB effects for each religious affiliation in the current study. ...
... Although singleitem measures display some disadvantages compared to multi-item scales, single-item measures are efficient, they can yield comparable psychometric qualities to multi-item instruments [90], and they are commonly used in emotion research because of their economy and interpretability [91,92,93]. In addition, the method of measurement used in our study is a standard approach in the FAB literature [25,4,17,88,89,1]. If we adopted a different measurement system than the one commonly used in FAB research, our results would be questionable. ...
Article
Full-text available
The fading affect bias (FAB) is a robust phenomenon where unpleasant affect fades faster than pleasant affect. The FAB is believed to be coping mechanism designed to make life appear pleasant in the face of hardships and adversities. The FAB persists across several cultures and many event types (e.g., alcohol, religious, and death), even though low FAB has been demonstrated for social media events, videogame events, and events labeled as religious, but not spiritual. Although religion is also believed to make life more satisfying by providing existential meaning and social connectedness for their followers, research to date, has not examined religious differences in the FAB. Therefore, we examined the FAB using 2 measures of fading affect across participants’ self-reported religious affiliations and we found robust FAB effects for all categories except for an extremely small sample of Islamic followers. The FAB effects were strongest for Jewish and Buddhist affiliations and they were weakest for participants who did not report a well-known religious affiliation. The findings extend the literature on the FAB to religious belief systems. Future research should replicate the current study, examine the FAB for larger samples of Muslims, Buddhists, Hindus, and Jews, and test explanations for differential FAB effects across religious affiliations.
... There is indeed an effort component in conducting a personal conversation, because there are numerous conversational norms to pay attention to. A speaker, for example, is supposed to consider what the listener already knows and to be brief (Skowronski and Walker 2004;Westerman et al. 2020). Moreover, a conversation does not normally consist of a succession of disconnected remarks; the content is a function of cooperative efforts, calling on the participants to recognize a common purpose -which may or may not exist from the start (Grice 1975). ...
Article
Full-text available
Virtual agents (VAs) are used increasingly as representatives of the firm in retail and service settings-particularly in online environments. Existing studies indicate that the customer's experience is enhanced if VAs resemble humans, which seems to imply that what has been learned over the years in research about the influence of the human employee's behavior on customer satisfaction may be applicable also to VA behavior. This study explores one factor, effort, which has a positive impact on customer satisfaction when it characterizes the human employee in service encounters. Although a VA (i.e., a computer program) cannot experience effort, it was assumed that human sensitivity to other humans' effort, and a tendency to anthropomorphize non-human agents, would make human customers susceptible to effort-expending signals when they interact with a VA. To examine this assumption, data were collected from customers who had been interacting with existing VAs. The results indicate that three specific behaviors (engaging in personal conversation, listening, and display of warmth) boost the customer's perceptions of VA effort, and that perceived VA effort has a positive impact on customer satisfaction.
... Although participants generally made few contradictions, all made numerous additions and omissions and many adopted details and narrative structure from the confederate's script when there was no motivation to lie. Our findings are consistent with earlier work showing that inconsistency across memory accounts is normal and influenced by a range of factors besides direct outside suggestion (Barnier et al., 2008;Bond & Smith, 1996;Cialdini & Goldstein, 2004;Fisher et al., 2013;Harris et al., 2017;Koriat, Goldsmith, & Pansky, 2000;Pasupathi, 2001;Skowronski & Walker, 2004). ...
... As communicators consider the perspectives of their audience and use them to help validate their identities and past experiences (Hogg & Rinella, 2018), they may come to view the self and previous life events in ways that are consistent with the perspectives of others. Research demonstrates that people frequently turn to others to help them make sense of past events (Marsh & Tversky, 2004), and audience characteristics can have important consequences for how these events come to be remembered and incorporated into the self (Pasupathi & Billitteri, 2015;Pasupathi & Oldroyd, 2015;Skowronski & Walker, 2004). The present findings provide evidence to suggest that even brief experiences of shared reality with an audience may result in biased self-perceptions. ...
Article
The current investigation examined how experiencing a shared reality during the social reconstruction of the past might relate to communicators’ perceived authenticity and self-esteem. In two experiments, participants were randomly assigned to describe an autobiographical memory to an audience who had either a positive or negative attitude toward the memory topic. Participants then reported perceptions of shared reality, authenticity, and self-esteem. Across both experiments, experiencing a shared reality with an audience who held a positive attitude was associated with greater self-perceived authenticity and self-esteem, whereas experiencing a shared reality with an audience who held a negative attitude was associated with lower ratings of authenticity and lower self-esteem. The effect of shared reality on self-esteem was mediated by perceptions of authenticity.
... Since memories are now known to be malleable (Elsey & Kindt, 2016;Nader, 2016), certain interventions could focus on reconstructing or rescripting (Romano et al., 2020) sexual memories by activating (or reactivating) both the relational and the sexual subsystems. Through this reconstruction process, one endeavor would be to complement the memory with new mental representations (Hupbach et al., 2007;McKenzie & Eichenbaum, 2011;Skowronski & Walker, 2004). When a patient recalls a particular sexual memory, psychotherapists may help them to complement its reconstruction with some relational aspects. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Determinants of sexual well-being have often been identified as sexual and relational satisfaction, which both include a focus on partner relationship. Such a conceptualization excludes sexually active single individuals, while confusing sexual and relational satisfaction with sexual well-being as both determinant and outcome. In the present research, we propose an intrapersonal perspective focused on cognitive sexual and relational mental representations as determinants of sexual well-being. Based on behavioral systems theory, we suggest that a high level of integration of sexual and relational mental representations should be associated with sexual well-being indicators, whereas a weaker level of integration should correspond to a lower level of sexual well-being. We developed a novel codification scheme to measure the cognitive integration of sexual and relational mental representations in narratives of sexual autobiographical memories. We examined its associations with sexual well-being indicators (satisfaction, passion, and sociosexuality). In a sample of 142 students, results showed that integration of sexual and relational representations in memories was positively associated with sexual satisfaction and harmonious passion, and negatively with obsessive passion and sociosexual desire. Results suggest that an integrated cognitive organization of sexual and relational mental representations in autobiographical memories can reflect a greater sexual wellbeing. Clinical implications are discussed.
... Events that are rated more highly on these self-report measures may be linked to more life themes and knowledge structures that are then shared during the interview. It has been proposed that memories become more semanticized with more rehearsal (Skowronski & Walker, 2004). Consistent with prior work (Fuentes & Desrocher, 2013), external detail count was associated with self-reported rehearsal. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Autobiographical memory (AM) involves a rich phenomenological re-experiencing of a spatio-temporal event from the past, which is challenging to objectively quantify. The Autobiographical Interview (AI; Levine et al., 2002, Psychology & Aging) is a manualized performance-based assessment designed to quantify episodic (internal) and semantic (external) features of recalled and verbally conveyed prior experiences. The AI has been widely adopted yet has not undergone a comprehensive psychometric validation. We investigated the reliability, validity, association to individual differences measures, and factor structure in healthy younger and older adults (N=352). Evidence for the AI's reliability was strong: the subjective scoring protocol showed high inter-rater reliability and previously identified age effects were replicated. Internal consistency across timepoints was robust, suggesting stability in recollection. Central to our validation, internal AI scores were positively correlated with standard, performance-based measures of episodic memory, demonstrating convergent validity. The two-factor structure for the AI was not well-supported by confirmatory factor analysis. Adjusting internal and external detail scores for the number of words spoken (detail density) improved trait estimation of AM performance. Overall, the AI demonstrated sound psychometric properties for inquiry into the qualities of autobiographical remembering.
... Moreover, in general, the content of what is said in a conversation is governed by norms (Skowronski and Walker, 2004). Such norms exist for conversations in service encounters too (Choi et al., 2019), and one would expect that content with high as opposed to low warmth is expected from most customers. ...
Article
Purpose This study aims to examine humans’ reactions to service robots’ display of warmth in robot-to-robot interactions – a setting in which humans’ impressions of a service robot will not only be based on what this robot does in relation to humans, but also on what it does to other robots. Design/methodology/approach Service robot display of warmth was manipulated in an experimental setting in such a way that a service robot A expressed low versus high levels of warmth in relation to another service robot B. Findings The results indicate that a high level of warmth expressed by robot A vis-à-vis robot B boosted humans’ overall evaluations of A, and that this influence was mediated by the perceived humanness and the perceived happiness of A. Originality/value Numerous studies have examined humans’ reactions when they interact with a service robot or other synthetic agents that provide service. Future service encounters, however, will comprise also multi-robot systems, which means that there will be many opportunities for humans to be exposed to robot-to-robot interactions. Yet, this setting has hitherto rarely been examined in the service literature.
Virtual agents (VAs) are used increasingly as representatives of the firm in retail and service settings – particularly in online environments. Existing studies indicate that the customer’s experience is enhanced if VAs resemble humans, which seems to imply that what has been learned over the years in research about the influence of the human employee’s behavior on customer satisfaction may be applicable also to VA behavior. This study explores one factor, effort, which has a positive impact on customer satisfaction when it characterizes the human employee in service encounters. Although a VA (i.e., a computer program) cannot experience effort, it was assumed that human sensitivity to other humans’ effort, and a tendency to anthropomorphize non-human agents, would make human customers susceptible to effort-expending signals when they interact with a VA. To examine this assumption, data were collected from customers who had been interacting with existing VAs. The results indicate that three specific behaviors (engaging in personal conversation, listening, and display of warmth) boost the customer’s perceptions of VA effort, and that perceived VA effort has a positive impact on customer satisfaction.
Article
Full-text available
Across three studies we explored neuroticism in relation to the fading affect bias, which refers to the greater fading of unpleasant compared to pleasant emotions in autobiographical memory. With increasing neuroticism, there was an increase in the frequency of unpleasant event rehearsal, which was then linked to less fading of negative affect. Study 2 showed this effect was specific to reflective rehearsal, with Study 3 clarifying the mediating effect was due to increased frequency of the reflective sub-type of rumination. We offer new insights into the effects of neuroticism on autobiographical memory and suggest that reflective rumination can be linked to retention of negative affect in individuals with high neuroticism, which can be interpreted as indicative of maladaptive emotional processing.
Chapter
This chapter reviews the main theoretical approaches to the content and organisation of autobiographical memory, namely which information forms our remembered pasts and how this information links up. To this end, the author pays special attention to the contributions made by those psychologists who, in the wake of the cognitive revolution, enquired in depth into the cognitive processes and knowledge structures (e.g. lifetime periods, general events, event-specific knowledge) capable of mentally representing the autobiographical past. Other topics addressed in this chapter include the emerging “life story” construct, the techniques used to analyse the narrative organisation of autobiographical memories, and the relationship between the “autobiographical memory” and “autobiographical narrative” concepts.
Article
A laboratory experiment investigated the processes that underlie the development of transactive memory structures - the organizing schemes that connect knowledge held by individuals to knowledge held by others (D. M. Wegner, T. Guiliano, & P. T. Hertel, 1985). The design was a 2 × 4 factorial that controlled expectations about the partner's knowledge (similar or different from the participant's) and cognitive interdependence, the degree to which participants' outcomes depended on whether they recalled the same or different information as their partner (defined by 4 incentives). Transactive memory was most differentiated when individuals had different expertise and incentives to remember different information and most integrated when individuals had similar expertise and incentives to remember the same information. These findings may help to explain the impact of previous experience and relationships on the development of transactive memory.
Article
Supporting predictions from temporal self-appraisal theory, participants in 3 studies reported feeling farther from former selves and experiences with unfavorable implications for their current self-view than from equally distant selves and experiences with flattering implications. This distancing bias occurred when assignment to negative and positive pasts was random, for both achievement and social outcomes and for single episodes as well as longer term experiences. Consistent with a motivational interpretation, the distancing bias was stronger among high than low self-esteem participants and occurred for personal but not for acquaintances' past events. Frequency of rehearsal and ease of recall of past episodes also predicted feelings of distance, but these variables did not account for the Self-Esteem × Valence interaction on subjective distancing of personal events.
Article
Four studies tested the hypothesis that agentic and communal motives act as a channel for new knowledge and are linked to specific ways of organizing information that facilitate its accessibility. In Study 1, agentic and communal participants read an agentic or a communal vignette consisting of differentiated and integrated statements, performed a distraction task, then completed written recall and recognition tasks. Agentics recalled and recognized more differentiation in the agentic story; communals recalled and recognized more integration in the communal story. A computerized replication with randomized recognition items (Study 2) found the same pattern of recognition results. Studies 3 and 4 used implicit motive primes and found similar results in both written and computerized recognition tasks. These ways of organizing information have powerful implications for the encoding of autobiographical knowledge and its long-term organization.
Article
We examined the effects of retention intervals on the recollection of the emotional content of events. Memory for personal events was tested for three retention intervals: 3 months, 1 year, and 4.5 years. Participants made pleasantness ratings both at the time of recording the event and during testing of the events. Analyses of the data show that judgments of pleasantness or unpleasantness of an event became less extreme as retention interval increased. This effect was larger for unpleasant events than for pleasant events. Subsequent memory ratings of pleasant and unpleasant events showed a modest effect of pleasantness with pleasant events remembered slightly better than unpleasant events. The theoretical implications of these data are discussed. © 1997 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Article
Review of book: Recovered Memories and False Memories by Martin A. Conway, New York: Oxford University Press, 1997, 301 pp. Reviewed by C. Brooks Brenneis.
Article
Five experiments investigated the structure of autobiographical memories. In Exps 1–3, a timed production paradigm was used, and Ss listed details of memories in free recall, in forward order (from first to last), in reverse order, in terms of centrality, and in terms of the interest value of the details. Memory detail production rates were significantly higher under free recall and forward order listing, and details listed in free recall were associated with the most personally important detail in a memory. Exp 4 found that once a memory was accessed, both distinctive and beginning details of memories were available equally quickly, whereas Exp 5 found that distinctive details provided significantly faster access to individual memories than did beginning and ending details. These findings show that both personally significant knowledge and temporal knowledge play central roles in the organization of specific memories. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)