ArticlePDF Available

Reply to Pradeep J Nathan and Ben J HarrisonPronounced Cognitive Deficits Following an Intravenous L-Tryptophan Challenge in First-degree Relatives of Bipolar Patients Compared to Healthy Controls

Authors:

Abstract

Neuropsychopharmacology, the official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, publishing the highest quality original research and advancing our understanding of the brain and behavior.
Reply to Pradeep J Nathan and Ben J Harrison
Reply: Pronounced Cognitive Deficits Following an
Intravenous L-Tryptophan Challenge in First-degree Relatives
of Bipolar Patients Compared to Healthy Controls
S Sobczak
1
, A Honig*
,1
and Wim J Riedel
1
1
Department of Psychiatry, Institute of Brain and Behaviour, Maastricht University, University Hospital Maastricht (AZM), Maastricht, The
Netherlands
Neuropsychopharmacology (2003) 28, 2214– 2216, advance online publication, 10 September 2003; doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1300270
Sir
Thank you very much for your interest in our paper
‘Pronounced cognitive deficits following an intravenous
L-Tryptophan challenge in first-degree relatives of bipolar
patients compared to healthy controls’ (Sobczak et al,
2003b). We appreciate a critical review of the interpretation
of our research findings. Next, we will give our response on
the three major points of the letter.
GENERAL METHODOLOGICAL CONSIDERATION
If in an experiment, independent variable Xis manipulated
leading to a change in dependent variable Y, experimental
methodology only permits conclusions attributing the
change in Yto varying levels of X. Suppose that
the manipulation of Xinduces changes in Y, but these are
known to be actually accomplished by Z, in such a way that
it would have been more appropriate to manipulate Zrather
than X? One should attempt to either manipulate/control Z
or measure its known indicator(s). According to experi-
mental methodology, one may only speculate but not
conclude about the potential mediating influence of Z.
Obviously, Xrefers to tryptophan (Trp) and Yto cognitive
performance variables. Zrefers primarily to serotonin
(5-HT), as the fact that manipulating Trp primarily brings
about changes in 5-HT would be beyond dispute (but
see point 1). The aim of this experiment and a series of
other experiments was to study the role of serotonergic
vulnerability, expressed as responsivity to serotonergic
manipulations in healthy first-degree relatives of bipolar
patients (FH). We deliberately did not aim at assessing
dopaminergic effects as we assumed that the role of
dopamine in bipolar disorders is almost beyond dispute.
Therefore, it is only of methodological interest if we should
have considered dopaminergic factors in the present
experiment. Obviously, an experiment similar to the
present, in which dopaminergic turnover would be varied,
is also of great interest. Perhaps the best approach would be
when the two are combined.
However, several other downstream effects of manipula-
ting Trp should be considered as well. 5-HT is known to
have inhibitory influences on noradrenaline, acetylcholine,
and dopamine turnover (Robbins, 1997). In particular,
inhibitions of acetylcholine transmission brought about by
increased 5-HT turnover might explain the large effects of
Trp on delayed recall memory (Little et al, 1995). On the
basis of that, speculation about other potentially mediating
neurotransmitters to explain the effect of Trp on memory
would point much more into the direction of acetylcholine
than dopamine.
POINT 1: SPECIFICITY OF L-TRP CHALLENGE
The specificity of Trp as a serotonergic challenge has been
debated several times. Indeed, it has been suggested that
increasing Trp in the blood lowers tyrosine and phenyl-
alanine uptake due to competition for the amino-acid
transporter (van Praag et al, 1987). Dopamine lowers
prolactin release, and the increase in prolactin after Trp is
suggested to be due to a decrease in this inhibiting activity
of dopamine. Prolactin is only an indirect parameter of
brain dopaminergic activity. It has been shown that 5-HT
also has direct prolactin-stimulating effects via the 5-HT
2a/2c
receptors (Di Renzo et al, 1989, van de Kar et al, 1989). The
only way to investigate the central effects on these
neurotransmitters of Trp infusion is to assess 5-hydroxy-
indoleacetic acid (5-HIAA) and homovanillic acid (HVA) in
Online publication: 16 June 2003 at http://www.acnp.org/citations/
Npp061603030248/default.pdf
Received 04 June 2003; accepted 09 June 2003
*Correspondence: Dr A Honig, Department of Psychiatry, Institute
Brain and Behaviour, Maastricht University, University Hospital
Maastricht (AZM), PO Box 5800, Maastricht, The Netherlands, Tel:
+31 43 3877537, Fax: +31 43 3875444,
E-mail: adriaan.honig@spsy.azm.nl
Neuropsychopharmacology (2003) 28, 2214 2216
&
2003 Nature Publishing Group All rights reserved 0893-133X/03
$
25.00
www.neuropsychopharmacology.org
the cerebrospinal fluid. These are invasive methods and, to
our knowledge, such a study has never been described.
Another indirect method is measurement of HVA in blood.
For each HVA measurement, 1 ml blood is needed. As
the amount of blood taken from subjects is limited and
other blood parameters (ie hormones, amino acids) are
also important to assess, we chose not to include HVA
measurements.
Large neutral amino acids (LNAAs; leucine, valine, iso-
leucine, Trp, tyrosine, phenylalanine) compete for trans-
porter uptake at the level of the blood–brain barrier. The
ratios of Trp/LNAAs, phenylalanine/LNAAs, and tyrosine/
LNAAs might give another indication of the availability of
serotonergic, respectively dopaminergic, precursors in the
brain. In the present study, the ratio of Trp/LNAAs
increased significantly to a value of 2.3 from a baseline
value of 0.13. This is an increase of 1669%. The ratio
tyrosine/LNAAs decreased after Trp loading, but this was
not significant (F(1,42) ¼0.017, NS). The ratio phenylala-
nine/LNAAs was significantly lower after Trp
(F(1,42) ¼88.98, Po0.05). The ratio decreased from 0.11
at baseline to 0.0013. This is a decrease of 103%. As the
increase in the ratio of Trp is much higher than the decrease
in the ratio of phenylalanine, and the ratio of tyrosine did
not change at all, we suggest that the effects of the Trp
loading are more likely attributable to central serotonergic
effects than to dopaminergic effects.
5-HT acts as a neuromodulator, and important functional
interactions between brain 5-HT and dopaminergic systems
are known. 5-HT inhibits dopaminergic activity in the
mesolimbic system but stimulates this activity in nigro-
striatal structures (Manji and Potter, 1997). Thus, we cannot
fully exclude that other neurotransmitters are involved in
the cognitive effects of Trp and 5-HT.
Despite this we suggest that the cognitive effects of Trp
challenge are primary serotonergic mediated because the
detrimental effects of Trp on attention and planning in
healthy FH have also been found after acute tryptophan
depletion (ATD) (Sobczak et al, 2002c). Thus, it seems that
both an increase and decrease in Trp impair the same
cognitive domains that are regulated by the frontal lobe
(Sobczak et al, 2003b). The specificity of ATD has been
pointed out by Klaassen et al (1999). They found a decrease
in mood and impaired memory performance after ATD but
not after lysine depletion. This supports the hypothesis that
ATD affects brain 5-HT functioning and not brain protein
metabolism in general.
POINT 2: COGNITIVE DEFICITS ON BASELINE ARE
MEDIATED BY DOPAMINE
The second suggestion made by Nathan and Harrison
(2003) is that the baseline cognitive deficits in FH may
be a result of dysregulation of dopaminergic activity in
mesocortical regions.
It must be emphasized that the FH subjects in this study
were free of any clinical psychiatric symptoms. Thus,
cognitive impairments were not related to altered mood
states (Sobczak et al, 2003b). Therefore, if there was a
dopaminergic dysfunction in these subjects at baseline, it
would only have affected cognition and not mood.
Independent of Trp, cognitive performance was more
impaired in relatives of type I bipolar patients (FH I)
compared to relatives of type II patients (FH II). This
suboptimal baseline performance in FH I resulted in more
pronounced cognitive deficits after Trp (ie on planning).
These findings agree with an association of serotonergic
vulnerability and cognitive impairments in FH I subjects.
FH II subjects were characterized by mood changes after
ATD and Trp challenge (Sobczak et al, 2002a, b). Taking
these findings together, we speculate that FH II subjects
share more symptoms of primary affective disorders in
which 5-HT plays a prominent role, whereas FH I subjects
show characteristics of primary psychotic disorders in
which cognitive deficits persist and functional deficits in
other neurotransmitters like noradrenaline, acetylcholine
and dopamine, or even structural brain abnormalities, may
also be involved.
POINT 3: FUTURE STUDY ON THE ROLE OF 5-HT AND
DOPAMINE
Indeed the search for biological markers of bipolar
disorders is just in its infancy. The present findings must
first be replicated in a larger research population including
relatives of type I and type II bipolar patients. Of interest is
whether there is a biological distinction between type I and
type II patients and their relatives. The vulnerability to
serotonergic, dopaminergic, and noradrenergic dysfunction
should be investigated using ATD, specific serotonergic
challenge procedures, and a phenylalanine/tyrosine deple-
tion test. Abnormalities in cholesterol and fatty acids have
also been associated with altered brain neurotransmitter
activity and psychopathology (Sobczak et al, 2003a; Hibbeln
and Salem, 1995; Swartz, 1990). Thus, research into the
interaction of neurotransmitters, cholesterol, and fatty acids
in humans and their association with psychopathology is
still a challenge.
REFERENCES
Di Renzo G, Amoroso S, Taglialatela M, Canzoniero L, Basile V,
Fatatis A et al (1989). Pharmacological characterization of
serotonin receptors involved in the control of prolactin
secretion. Eur J Pharmacol 162: 371–373.
Hibbeln JR, Salem N (1995). Dietary polyunsaturated fatty acids
and depression: when cholesterol does not satisfy. Am J Clin
Nutr 62: 1–9.
Klaassen T, Riedel WJ, Deutz NEP, van Someren A, van Praag HM
(1999). Specificity of the tryptophan depletion method. Psycho-
pharmacology 141: 279–286.
Little JT, Broocks A, Martin A, Hill JL, Tune LE, Mack C et al
(1995). Serotonergic modulation of anticholinergic effects on
cognition and behavior in elderly humans. Psychopharmacology
120: 280–288.
Manji HK, Potter WZ (1997). Monoaminergic systems. In Young
LT, Joffe RT (eds). Bipolar Disorders. Biological Models and their
Clinical Application. Marcel Dekker Inc., New York.
Nathan PJ, Harrison BJ (2003). L-Tryptophan challenge and
cognitive deficits in bipolar disorder: evidence for hyper-
serotonergic and hypodopaminergic mechanisms (letter).
Neuropsychopharmacology (in press).
Robbins TW (1997). Arousal systems and attentional processes.
Biol Psychol 45: 57–71.
Reply
2215
Neuropsychopharmacology
Sobczak S, Honig A, Christophe A, Maes M, Helsdingen RWC, De
Vriese S et al (2003a). Lower high-density lipoprotein cholesterol
and increased o6 polyunsaturated fatty acids in first-degree
relatives of bipolar patients. Psychol Med in press.
Sobczak S, Honig A, van Duinen MA, Riedel WJ (2002a). Mood,
prolactin and cortisol responses following intravenous L-
tryptophan challenge; evidence for serotonergic vulnerability
in first-degree relatives of bipolar patients. Int J Neuropsycho-
pharmacol 5: 249–254.
Sobczak S, Honig A, Nicolson NA, Riedel WJ (2002b). Effects of
acute tryptophan depletion on mood and cortisol release in first-
degree relatives of type I and type II bipolar patients and healthy
matched controls. Neuropsychopharmacology 27: 834–842.
Sobczak S, Honig A, Schmitt JAJ, Riedel WJ (2003b). Pronounced
cognitive deficits following an intravenous L-tryptophan challenge
in first-degree relatives of bipolar patients compared to healthy
controls. Neuropsychopharmacology 28: 711–719.
Sobczak S, Riedel WJ, Booij L, Aanmet Rot M, Deutz NEP, Honig A
(2002c). Cognition following acute trytophan depletion: differ-
ences between first-degree relatives of bipolar disorder patients
and matched healthy control volunteers. Psycho Med 32:
503–515.
Swartz CM (1990). Albumin decrement in depression and
cholesterol decrement in mania. J Affect Disord 19: 173–176.
Van de Kar LD, Lorens SA, Urban JH, Bethea CL (1989). Effect of
selective serotonin (5-HT) agonists and 5-HT2 antagonist on
prolactin secretion. Neuropharmacology 28: 299–305.
Van Praag HM, Lemus C, Kahn R (1987). Hormonal probes of
central serotonergic activity: do they really exist? Biol Psychiatry
22: 86–98.
Reply
2216
Neuropsychopharmacology
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
Thirteen healthy subjects were subjected to tryptophan (TRP) depletion, lysine (LYS) depletion, and a placebo condition in a double blind cross-over study. The aim of the study was to test the specificity of psychological effects induced by TRP depletion. Subjects ingested a 100 g amino acid mixture with or without TRP or LYS. Six hours later, plasma TRP levels had decreased by 77% in the TRP depletion test and LYS levels by 51% in the LYS depletion condition. After 6 h of TRP depletion, subjects reported significantly more tiredness and lowering of mood, compared to subjects in the placebo group, and memory performance declined. After 6 h of LYS depletion, no significant differences in mood and memory compared to placebo were found. We conclude that the effects of TRP depletion on mood and memory are specific for the depletion of TRP and are not caused by the depletion of an amino acid per se. This supports the hypothesis that TRP depletion affects brain serotonin metabolism and not only brain protein metabolism in general.
Article
Within all psychiatric inpatients over a 4-year period, on admission depressives showed serum albumin 5.4% lower (P less than 0.001) than non-psychiatric controls. Similarly, manics showed cholesterol 10% lower (P less than 0.0005) and serum calcium/protein ratio 2.2% higher (P less than 0.05) than controls. These deviations suggest a tendency towards dietary aberrations in these patients of potential medical significance. These tests had shown the most distinct variation with diagnosis on split-half discriminant function analysis of routine chemistry and hematology tests of 107 patients with mania, 132 with depression, 67 with schizophrenia, 18 with schizoaffective illness, and 83 non-psychiatric controls admitted for elective surgery.
Article
The present study was undertaken to characterize the type of serotonin (5-HT) receptors involved in the control of prolactin (PRL) secretion in male rats. d-Fenfluramine (10 mg/kg i.p.), a potent 5-HT releaser and quipazine, (20 mg/kg i.p.) a 5-HT agonist, caused a marked increase in serum PRL levels. Ritanserin (200 micrograms/kg i.p.), a specific antagonist of 5-HT2 receptors, administered 1 h before the administration of d-fenfluramine or quipazine, completely prevented the PRL-releasing effect of these drugs. Furthermore, the administration of 8-hydroxy-2-(di-n-propylamino)tetralin (8-OH DPAT) (1.5, 3 and 6 mg/kg i.p.), a compound considered to be a prototypical 5-HT1A agonist, failed to induce any change in serum PRL levels. The same lack of effect on PRL secretion was observed after the administration of 5-methoxy-3-(1,2,3,6-tetrahydro-4-pyridin-4-yl)-1-H-indole (RU 24969) (1, 3 and 10 mg/kg i.p.), a compound which has been shown to possess a higher selectivity for 5-HT1B receptor subtypes than for 5-HT1A subtypes. These results suggest that 5-HT receptors involved in the control of PRL secretion are of the 5-HT2 type.
Article
The present study was undertaken to determine the involvement of serotonergic 5-HT1 and 5-HT2 receptor subtypes in stimulation of the secretion of prolactin. Several 5-HT agonists were administered, in a dose-response fashion, to conscious rats and the effect on the levels of prolactin in plasma was measured. The 5-HT1A + 5-HT1B agonist RU 24969 (5-methoxy-3[1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridin-4-yl]-1H-indole succinate) and the 5-HT1 + 5-HT2 agonist MK-212 (6-chloro-2-[1-piperazinyl]pirazine) increased levels of prolactin in plasma in a dose-dependent manner. In contrast, the selective 5-HT1A agonists 8-OH-DPAT (8-hydroxy-2-[di-n-propylamino]tetralin) and ipsapirone (2-[4-[4-(2-pyrimidinyl)-1-piperazinyl]butyl]-1,2-benzisothiazol-3 -(2H) one-1,1-dioxidehydrochloride) did not increase levels of prolactin in plasma at any dose. The 5-HT-releasing drug, fenfluramine, also increased the concentration of prolactin in plasma. Pretreatment with the selective 5-HT2 antagonist, LY53857 (6-methyl-1-[1-methylethyl]ergoline-8-carboxylic acid, 2-hydroxy-1-methyl propyl ester (Z)-2-butenedioate [1:1]), did not significantly diminish an increase in levels of prolactin in plasma, induced by injection of fenfluramine. The antagonist LY53857 inhibited, but did not block the MK-212- and RU 24969-induced increase in the levels of prolactin in plasma. By deduction, these data suggest that 5-HT1B receptors, or as yet undefined 5-HT receptor subtypes may be involved in the stimulation of the secretion of prolactin by endogenously released 5-HT, and that 5-HT2 receptors may play a minor role in the serotonergic regulation of the secretion of prolactin.
Article
The response of a hormone allegedly under 5-hydroxytryptaminergic (5-HT-ergic) control to a compound stimulating or inhibiting serotonergic activity has been used as a measure of the functional state of central serotonergic systems. The relevant literature is reviewed, and based on that, it is concluded that, as yet, no reliable hormonal 5-HT probe exists. The main problems are nonselectivity of the challengers and noncomparability of individual studies because of variations in dose and route of administration. An acceptable hormonal 5-HT probe should at least have passed the following three tests. The influence of the challenger on catecholaminergic (CA) systems must be rendered unlikely in humans to avoid the pitfalls of, say, the 5-HT precursors whose CA-ergic influences have been overlooked. Dose-response relationships must be established to avoid the confusion caused by different investigators using the challenger in different doses. It must be demonstrated that the effect of the challenger is counteracted by its functional opponent.
Article
Recent studies have both offered and contested the proposition that lowering plasma cholesterol by diet and medications increases suicide, homicide, and depression. Significant confounding factors include the quantity and distribution of dietary n-6 and n-3 polyunsaturated essential fatty acids that influence serum lipids and alter the biophysical and biochemical properties of cell membranes. Epidemiological studies in various countries and in the United States in the last century suggest that decreased n-3 fatty acid consumption correlates with increasing rates of depression. This is consistent with a well-established positive correlation between depression and coronary artery disease. Long-chain n-3 polyunsaturate deficiency may also contribute to depressive symptoms in alcoholism, multiple sclerosis, and post-partum depression. We postulate that adequate long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids, particularly docosahexaenoic acid, may reduce the development of depression just as n-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids may reduce coronary artery disease.
Article
Cholinergic neurotransmission is thought to be modulated by serotonin as documented in animal and human studies. We examined the effects of the muscarinic antagonist scopolamine (0.4 mg IV) given alone or together with the serotonin mixed agonist/antagonist m-chlorophenylpiperazine (m-CPP, 0.08 mg/kg IV), and the selective 5-HT3 receptor antagonist ondansetron (0.15 mg/kg IV). Ten normal elderly volunteers each received five separate pharmacologic challenges (placebo, ondansetron, scopolamine, scopolamine+ondansetron, and scopolamine+m-CPP). Cognitive, behavioral, and physiologic variables were analyzed using repeated measures analysis of variance. The acute effects of scopolamine in certain cognitive, behavioral, and physiological measures were significantly exaggerated by the addition of m-CPP. Scopolamine's cognitive effects were unaffected by ondansetron at the dose tested, nor did ondansetron given alone affect basal cognitive performance. This pilot study suggests that the serotonin mixed agonist/antagonist m-CPP may influence cholinergic neurotransmission. The changes associated with the combination of scopolamine and m-CPP do not appear to be secondary to simple pharmacokinetic alterations and suggest a complex interaction between the cholinergic and serotonergic systems centrally.
Article
Unitary concepts of arousal have outlived their usefulness and their psychological fractionation corresponds to a similar chemical differentiation of the reticular formation of the brain. Neurobiological characteristics of the monoaminergic and cholinergic systems can be described in terms of their anatomical, electrophysiological and neurochemical properties. Functional studies suggest that the coeruleo-cortical noradrenergic system, under certain circumstances, is implicated in processes of selective attention, that the mesolimbic and mesostriatal dopaminergic systems contribute to different forms of behavioural activation, and that the cortical cholinergic projections have fundamental roles in the cortical processing of signals, affecting attentional and mnemonic processes. The ascending serotoninergic systems contribute to behavioural inhibition and appear to oppose the functions of the other systems in several ways.
Article
Serotonergic circuits have been proposed to mediate cognitive processes, particularly learning and memory. Cognitive impairment is often seen in bipolar disorders in relation to a possible lowered serotonergic turnover. We investigated the effects of acute tryptophan depletion (ATD) on cognitive performance in healthy first-degree relatives of bipolar patients (FH) (N= 30) and matched controls (N= 15) in a placebo-controlled, double-blind cross-over design. Performance on planning, memory and attention tasks were assessed at baseline and 5 h after ATD. Following ATD, speed of information processing on the planning task was impaired in the FH group but not in the control group. FH subjects with a bipolar disorder type I relative (FH I) showed impairments in planning and memory, independent of ATD. In all subjects, ATD impaired long-term memory performance and speed of information processing. ATD did not affect short-term memory and focused and divided attention. The results suggest serotonergic vulnerability affecting frontal lobe areas in FH subjects, indicated by impaired planning. Biological vulnerability in FH I subjects is reflected in impaired planning and memory performance. In conclusion, the cognitive dysfunctions in FH subjects indicate an endophenotype constituting a possible biological marker in bipolar psychopathology. Serotonin appears to be involved in speed of information processing, verbal and visual memory and learning processes.
Article
Central serotonergic vulnerability indicated by altered mood and neuroendocrine responses to an intravenous (i.v.) tryptophan (Trp) challenge was assessed in healthy adult unaffected first-degree relatives of bipolar disorder (BD) patients (n = 30) (family history; FH). The effects of a single dose of 7.0 g Trp on mood, cortisol and prolactin (Prl) in FH subjects were compared with healthy matched controls (n = 15) in a placebo-controlled, double blind cross-over design. Prl and cortisol responses increased following Trp. Overall, Trp lowered mood. Hormonal and mood effects did not differ between FH and controls. Results show some evidence for differences in serotonergic vulnerability between relatives of type I BD patients and relatives of type II BD patients.