ArticlePublisher preview available

Choice as a disrupter of performance in paired-associate learning

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

Conducted 2 experiments in paired-associate learning in which 20 undergraduates were permitted to choose their responses from alternatives following the procedure of L. C. Perlmuter, R. A. Monty, and G. A. Kimble . The other 20 Ss were forced to learn the responses chosen by their yoked masters. Both groups of Ss were forced to learn an interposed A-C list following the choice-force procedure but prior to learning the A-B list. The performance of force Ss was reliably superior to that of choice Ss on A-C; however, when both groups learned A-B, the typical facilitative effect of choice was disturbed. In Exp II with 40 undergraduates, interposing a C-D list produced equivalent performance in both groups on C-D. Similarly, the facilitative effect of choice was not observed on the subsequent A-B trials. The degraded performance of choice Ss is discussed in terms of frustration as an additional source of motivation.
Journal
of
Experimental
Psychology
1974,
Vol. 102,
No. 1,
170-172
CHOICE
AS A
DISRUPTER
OF
PERFORMANCE
IN
PAIRED-ASSOCIATE
LEARNING1
LAWRENCE
C.
PERLMUTER
»
Bowdoin
College
RICHARD
A.
MONTY
Human
Engineering
Laboratory,
Aberdeen Proving Ground,
Aberdeen,
Maryland
PETER
M.
CROSS
Bowdoin
College
In 2
experiments,
half
of the 5s
were permitted
to
choose their responses
from
alternatives
following
the
1971 procedure
of
Perlmuter,
Monty,
and
Kimble.
The
other
5s
were forced
to
learn
the
responses chosen
by
their yoked
masters.
Both groups
of &
were forced
to
learn
an
interposed
A-C
list
following
the
choice-force procedure
but
prior
to
learning
the A-B
list.
The
performance
of
force
5s was
reliably superior
to
that
of
choice
5s on
A-C;
however, when both
groups learned A-B,
the
typical facilitative
effect
of
choice
was
disturbed.
In
a
second experiment, interposing
a
C-D
list produced equivalent perform-
ance
in
both groups
on
C-D. Similarly,
the
facilitative
effect
of
choice
was
not
observed
on the
subsequent
A-B
trials.
The
degraded performance
of
choice
5s was
discussed
in
terms
of
frustration
as an
additional source
of
motivation.
Allowing
5s the
opportunity
to
choose
a
portion
of
the
response materials
to be
learned
on a
subse-
quent paired-associate list
was
examined with
a
paradigm
that
manipulated both
the
amount
of
choice
allowed during
the
selection procedure
and
the
locus
or
point
at
which choice took place (Monty,
Rosenberger
&
Perlmuter, 1973).
The
amount
of
material
chosen
is not as
important
as the
point
at
which
choice
is
exercised,
as
indicated
by the
finding
that
even when only
3 out of 12
items were chosen,
performance
on the
entire list
was
nearly
as
good
as
that
when
all 12
items were chosen, provided
that
choice
was
allowed
early
in the
experiment. Such
an
observation ruled
out the
associative interpre-
tation
of the
choice
vs.
force
phenomenon
as
sug-
gested
by
Perlmuter, Monty,
and
Kimble (1971)
and
seemed
to
favor
a
motivational explanation.
Subsequently, Perlmuter
and
Monty (1973)
found
some preliminary evidence
that
motivational factors
may
even work
in a
detrimental
fashion
under
certain conditions,
A
rather extensive literature exists
on the
relation-
ship
between motivational level
and
performance.
It is
generally asserted that performance increases
as
motivation increases. However, excessively high
levels
of
motivation have also been implicated
in a
deterioration
of
performance (Brown, 1961). Thus,
the
purpose
of the
present
experiments
is to
study
further
the
role
of
choice
in
learning
and to
examine
the
possibility
that
choice
may
have
an
inhibitory
as
well
as a
facilitatory
effect
on
performance.
'This
research
was
supported
in
part
by
Grant
MH-15315
from
the
National
Institute
of
Mental
Health
to the first
author.
Experiment
I was
presented
at the
meeting
of the
Southeastern Psychological Association,
New
Orleans,
May
1973.
2
Requests
for
reprints
should
be
sent
to
Lawrence
C.
Perl-
muter,
who
is
now at the
Department
of
Psychology,
Virginia
Polytechnic
Institute
and
State
University,
Blacksburg,
Virginia
24,061.
EXPERIMENT
I
Employing
the
basic design
of
Perlmuter
et
al.
(1971),
half
of the 5s
chose their
A-B
responses
(choice
group),
while
the
other
5s
were
forced
to
learn
those responses chosen
by
their yoked masters
(force
group).
In
contrast
to
earlier experiments,
an
A-C
list
was
forced upon both groups prior
to
the
learning
of
A-B, with
the
expectation
that
inhibitory
effects
would cause
the
choice group
to be
inferior
to the
force
group
on A-C
trials,
while
motivation
produced
by
prior choosing would cause
them
to be
superior
to the
force group
on the
subse-
quent
A-B
trials. Thus,
the
analysis
of the A-B
trials should serve
to
determine
the
durability
of
the
beneficial
effect
of
choice.
Method.
The 5s
were
40
male
and
female
Uni-
versity
of
Delaware students, aged 19-25 yr.,
who
were
paid $2.00 each
for
participating
in the
experi-
ment.
They
were alternately assigned
to 2
groups
of
20 5s
each
in
order
of
their appearance.
The
apparatus
and
general procedures were
identical
to
those
employed
in
previous studies.
Briefly,
the
last
84
5-letter
words were taken
from
a
list generated
by
Taylor
and
Kimble (1967)
and
were
considered
to be of
relatively high meaning.
These
items were assembled into
12
groups
of 7
each. Within each group,
one
item
was
designated
arbitrarily
as the
stimulus word
and one
item
as the A-C
response word.
The
remaining
5
items
served
as the 5
potential
response
words
on the
A—B
list.
Each
5 was first
presented with
3
paired-
associate (PA) practice trials utilizing common
words.
The
choice group
was
then presented with
12
slides,
one at a
time. Each slide contained
a
stimulus
word centered
on the
left
and 5
potential
response words listed vertically
on the
right.
The
5 was
instructed
to
read
the
stimulus word
and the 5
potential response words aloud
and to
then select
170
... To account for the greater difficulty that choice subjects experienced in the learning of the nonchosen list, one additional assumption has to be added to the hypothesized relationship between choice and motivation. Namely, choice renders the chooser vulnerable to frustration (Perlmuter et al., 1974). Finally, when the choice subjects were subsequently allowed to learn their chosen materials, choice was no longer effective in enhancing performance. ...
Article
Full-text available
This report summarizes several years of research on how offering subjects a choice of task parameters leads to a perception of control over their environment, which in turn can lead to improved task performance. The development of a theoretical explanation of the phenomenon is traced, and implications for the design of displays are deduced and illustrated with hypothetical examples.
... force subjects perform better than choice subjects. These results (Perlmuter & Monty, 1973) could lead one to speculate that. in order for choice to benefit performance, it is necessary that subjects be permitted to choose the S-R pairs, presumably because it gives subjects the opportunity to form S-R associations during the choice procedure. The design of the present experiment prevents this and further adds to the body of lit era t u re con t es tin g t he ass ociative hookup interpretation in favor of a purely motivational interpretation. ...
Article
Four experiments are reported in which half of the subjects were permitted to choose either the stimulus or response members of S-R pairs from groupings of alternative stimuli or responses following the procedures of Perlmuter, Monty, and Kimble. The other subjects were exposed to the materials and were forced to learn the material chosen by their yoked partners. Approximately 24 h later, both groups learned PA lists comprised of either the chosen or forced material. Choosing either the stimulus or the response facilitated performance irrespective of whether the other member of the S-R pair was present at the time of choosing. This finding ruled out an associative hookup interpretation in favor of a purely motivational hypothesis and demonstrated the temporal durability of the effect of choosing.
... Other studies have shown that self-generated responses to items on memory tasks (Slamencka & Graf, 1978), the generation of information by subjects (Johnson & Raye, 1981), or the attributions of scores to subjects' own emotional reaction (Kanfer, Karoly, & Newman, 1974) result in significant advantages in recall and performance. Moreover, performance is lower for subjects who are deprived of a choice that they had previously believed to be accessible than for subjects offered no choice at all (Kanfer & Grimm, 1978;Perlmuter, Monty, & Cross, 1974). Subjects work harder when they have control over performance-outcome contingencies and types of rewards (Brigham, 1979). ...
Article
Full-text available
Goal setting has been widely used to enhance work motivation. This paper discusses the importance of goal acceptance in moderating goal setting effects and shows how workers' acceptance of goals can be influenced at various stages of the progression from goal setting to goal attainment. A heuristic organization of goal acceptance strategies is proposed as a basis for extending the theoretical framework underlying goal setting research.
Chapter
This chapter explains the sense of control in midlife. The chapter states that midlife is paradoxically a period in which individuals have many goals, roles, and responsibilities besides stress and lack of time for leisure. They also experience a peak in personal control and power. In midlife, adults may begin to recognize more external sources of control and adopt more accommodative (or secondary) control strategies, while maintaining their own sense of internal control and efficacy. To achieve a balanced mental state and attitude during midlife, when roles, goals, responsibilities, and stress tend to peak, individuals must believe in their own competence and effectiveness, and also be aware of their limitations and the influences of outside factors. According to the chapter, to understand individuals' sense of control in middle adulthood, one must study their sense of control within the context of the entire lifespan, as well as within the social and historical context.
Article
Full-text available
Learned helplessness theory places strong emphasis on the contingency between response and outcome as the main antecedent of perceived control, ignoring the relevance of freedom of choice. The current experiment was conducted to examine the behavioral effects of freedom to choose the way to perform a cognitive task on performance following unsolvable problems. Subjects were exposed to either no-feedback or failure in four unsolvable problems, and their performance was assessed in a subsequent Raven task. A group of subjects received the option to choose how to perform the Raven task, a second group did not receive such a facility, and a third group was explicitly denied the opportunity to make such a choice. Results indicated that subjects in the “forced” condition performed worse than subjects in the two other conditions. Furthermore, subjects in the “freedom” condition performed better than subjects in the two other conditions. Finally, the performance deficit produced by exposure to unsolvable problems was eliminated among subjects who were given the option to choose the way to perform the task. The beneficial effects of freedom of choice are discussed in terms of self-efficacy expectancies and changes in cognitive orientation.
Article
This study investigated the relationship between internal versus external locus of control of reinforcement and counter control or reactance in subjects in a verbal learning experiment. Internal and external controllers were given either a choice or no choice of material to be learned in a paired associate, anticipation task (after Monty & Perlmuter, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1972, 94, 235–238). As hypothesized, the lack of freedom of choice was associated with counter control (decreased recall) in internals. Being able to choose material lead to faster learning for both internals and externals after first trial recall.
Article
Although this concept has rarely been investigated systematically, the prison is an environment that severely limits inmates’personal control. This article applies theoretical and empirical advances in the area of personal control to the issue of inmate adjustment to prison. Personal control has three components: outcome control, choice, and predictability of future events. Research findings suggesting adverse impacts of limited control are discussed in light of their implications for prisoner adjustment. Several models of personal control, including the environmental/learned helplessness, individual difference/self-efficacy, and incongruency/reactance models, are applied to the process of prisoner adjustment. Using these models, a conceptual framework for integrating past research in the sociology and social psychology of corrections is proposed, and directions for future research are discussed.
Article
There has been an increasing emphasis in psychology on the perception of control over one''s environment and its effect on behavior and performance. In the present experiment, subjects were allowed to choose material they would like to learn if given the opportunity, or allowed to choose material for others to learn. Their learning of a nonchosen (forced) set of materials was then examined and compared with that of subjects given no opportunity to choose at all. It was found that the choice-for-self condition led to better performance than either the choice-for-other condition or the nochoice (forced) condition. However, when an additional group of subjects was given the opportunity to choose for others and their personal involvement was augmented by emphasizing their willingness to choose, their performance was enhanced relative to subjects offered no choice. The results were discussed in terms of motivational benefits stemming from perceived control.
Article
Permitting subjects to choose materials to be learned on a task enhances performance on that task. These results support the idea that choice increases the learner's perception of control, thereby enhancing motivation and performance. The purpose of the present experiments was to determine whether choosing responses to be learned on a paired-associate task would also benefit performance on a reaction-time task. Half of the subjects selected their responses to be learned on a paired-associate task while the remaining subjects were assigned responses. In one experiment, subjects then performed both the PA and RT tasks simultaneously, while in the second experiment the RT task was performed following the choice/force procedure. Providing the learner with the opportunity to choose produced a generalized increase in motivation, which resulted in significantly faster responding on the nonchosen RT task. Also discussed are the limitations of the effects of perceived control.
Article
Full-text available
Typescript (lithograph copy) Thesis (Ed. D.)--Oklahoma State University, 1995. Vita. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 79-88).
Article
Full-text available
Investigated the effect of giving 120 undergraduates the opportunity to choose the response materials they wish to learn in a paired-associate task. A paradigm was used which manipulated the amount of choice allowed during the selection procedure and the locus or point at which choice took place. Results show that locus of choice was an extremely powerful variable and that if properly manipulated, choosing only 3 responses in a 12-item list elevated performance to a level near that of a 12-item list in which all responses were chosen. Motivational mechanisms are postulated to account for these findings. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Examined the effect of giving Ss the opportunity to choose learning materials in a paired-associate paradigm. 4 studies using high- and low-meaning materials and employing between- and within-Ss designs were conducted with 40 undergraduates in the 1st 2 and 21 Ss in the others. Use of a between-Ss design showed that A-B learning was facilitated by choice with high-meaning but not low-meaning materials. Using a within-Ss design in which Ss chose 5 stimulus-response (S-R) pairs and were forced to learn 5 others, results show performance on the forced pairs was elevated to the level of the choice pairs. These data suggest that allowing Ss the opportunity to choose at least some S-R pairs generally facilitates performance through the operation of a proposed motivational mechanism which may also benefit learning of nonchosen S-R pairs. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Conducted 3 paired-associate learning experiments with a total of 120 male and female undergraduates. Under some conditions, a-b learning was facilitated when ss chose responses to be learned on a subsequent a-b list. Ss who chose their a-b responses and were subsequently forced to learn a competing set of material (a-c) showed a relatively greater disruption of learning than ss who did not have the opportunity to choose either a-b or a-c. All of these effects required that when ss chose their responses, the choosing occurred in the presence of their respective stimuli. Simply choosing responses in the absence of the stimuli produced performance which was not different from that resulting when ss were denied the opportunity to choose their responses. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Notes previous experiments showing that if Ss choose responses from among alternatives, subsequent paired-associate learning is facilitated if the choosing occurs in the presence of the respective stimuli. Choosing in the absence of the stimuli does not facilitate performance. In experiments with university students (N = 122), choosing the stimuli in the presence of the respective responses facilitated performance; however, choosing the stimuli in the absence of the respective response hindered performance. This lack of symmetry between choice of stimulus and choice of response is consistent with the finding that the effect of some variables may be different when either the stimulus or response items have been differentially familiarized. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
The method of Glaze was used to scale 320 words and paralogs for meaningfulness. One hundred Ss provided data from which three such measures were derived. Employing the most conventional of these measures (percentage of Ss responding in less than 2.5 sec) to select the items to be learned, a validating study demonstrated the usual relationship between association value and speed of learning. Other investigations have employed the materials successfully for purposes of control when the main interest of the experiment was in some other problem.
The motivation of behavior
  • J S Brown
BROWN, J. S. The motivation of behavior. New York: Mc-Graw-Hill, 1961.