ArticlePDF Available

Children’s Block Balancing Revisited: No Evidence for Representational Redescription

Authors:

Abstract

According to the theory of representational redescription (RR theory, Karmiloff-Smith, 1992), children’s reasoning is grounded on implicit knowledge. This initial knowledge is first consolidated and then subjected to reiterative cycles of representational redescription, leading to knowledge of increasing accessibility. As one important piece of evidence supporting RR theory a U-shaped developmental trend regarding children’s ability to balance asymmetrical blocks has been reported. To assess whether this trend is a robust phenomenon, we investigated how 4-, 5-, 6-, and 8-year-olds (N = 65) attempted to balance symmetrical and asymmetrical blocks on a narrow support. Independent from block type, we found quasi-linear improvements with age in all performance measures. These results question the robustness of the U-shaped developmental trend regarding children’s block balancing and fail to provide any evidence for representational redescription.
Children’s Block Balancing 1
Running head: CHILDREN’S BLOCK BALANCING
Children's Block Balancing Revisited:
No Evidence for Representational Redescription
Horst Krist, Holger Horz, and Tina Schönfeld
Department of Psychology, University of Greifswald, Germany
April 11, 2005
Children’s Block Balancing 2
Abstract
According to the theory of representational redescription (RR theory, Karmiloff-Smith,
1992), children's reasoning in a microdomain is grounded on implicit knowledge. This initial
knowledge is first consolidated and then subjected to reiterative cycles of representational
redescription, leading to knowledge of increasing accessibility. One of the microdomains for
which evidence supporting RR theory has been reported is block balancing. This evidence
consisted in a U-shaped developmental trend regarding children's ability to balance asymmetrical
blocks. To assess whether this trend is a robust phenomenon reflecting spontaneous
developmental change, we investigated how 4-, 5-, 6-, and 8-year-olds (N=65) attempted to
balance symmetrical and asymmetrical blocks on a narrow support. Independent from block
type, we found quasi-linear improvements with age in all performance measures. These results
question the robustness of the U-shaped developmental trend regarding children's block
balancing and fail to provide any evidence for representational redescription.
Key words: conceptual development, intuitive physics, representational redescription, U-shaped
growth
Children’s Block Balancing 3
Children’s Block Balancing Revisited:
No Evidence for Representational Redescription
Representational Redescription Theory
One of the most influential modern theories of cognitive development is Karmiloff-
Smith’s (1992) theory of representational redescription (RR theory). In her theory, she
synthesises the constructivist tradition of Piagetian provenience and recent nativist accounts
originating from infancy research, mainly on infants’ physical reasoning abilities (e.g., Spelke,
1994). From the constructivist perspective, she adapts the central assumption that cognitive
development is an active process, and from the nativist perspective, she adapts the claim that the
infant is equipped with pre-adapted structures and reasoning capabilities constituting certain core
domains, such as an intuitive physics or an intuitive psychology. Karmiloff-Smith resolves the
tension that the combination of these contrasting perspectives creates essentially by introducing
two main ideas: The first idea is that children are born with predispositions to attend to and
process certain inputs. These predispositions serve as constraints that lead to an increasing
modularity of the cognitive system, a gradual process that Karmiloff-Smith calls modularization.
The second, somehow opposite idea is that early knowledge is implicit but becomes more and
more generally accessible with development. Increasing accessibility of initially implicit
knowledge is achieved by the active and reiterative process that gives RR theory its name:
representational redescription.
Representational redescription is active because it is (mainly) internally driven and it is
reiterative because it repeats itself on increasingly higher levels of representation. According to
RR theory, repeated success, rather than failure, is most important for cognitive growth. More
specifically, behavioral mastery marks the beginning of representational development rather than
Children’s Block Balancing 4
its endpoint. Success on the behavioral plane is possible without any explicit representation of
the how and why of the successful behavior. For humans, this is evidenced by perceptual-motor
skills like riding a bicycle or throwing a ball at a target (e.g., Krist, Fieberg, & Wilkening, 1993).
Karmiloff-Smith conceptualizes the know-how underlying such skills, as well as infants’ early
cognitive competencies, as procedural rather than declarative knowledge. Procedural knowledge,
or implicit knowledge, in Karmiloff-Smith’s sense can only be used by running the respective
procedure, e.g., by riding a bicycle or throwing a ball. It is not available to consciousness or
verbal report, and it cannot be combined with or be influenced by other pieces of knowledge—it
is thus encapsulated or „bracketed”. Once the implicit knowledge has been consolidated,
representational redescription may occur, which leads to a re-representation of the implicit
knowledge on an explicit level. Besides the level of implicit representations, called level I,
Karmiloff-Smith postulates three levels of explicit representations: E1, E2, and E3.
On level E1, representations are available to cognitive operations, such as comparison,
negation, or abstraction, but not yet to conscious access or verbal report. For example, pretend
play requires this level of representation. On level E2, representations are encoded in a format
that is available to conscious access but not yet for verbal report. Physical imagery, for example,
can be considered as operating on level-E2 representations (although Karmiloff-Smith states that
empirical evidence for level-E2 representations is hard to find). It is only on level E3 that
representations are encoded in a format similar to linguistic expressions, so that they become
available to verbal report and hence to operations across different domains, such as physics and
mathematics. According to Karmiloff-Smith, level-I representations are first redescribed into
level-E1 representations, which are later redesribed into level-E2 representations, and eventually
into level-E3 representations. As with the transition from level-I format to level E1, the
Children’s Block Balancing 5
transitions from E1 to E2 and from E2 to E3 become possible only after consolidation of the
lower-level representation.
Representational redescription as conceived by Karmiloff-Smith is a domain-general
process, i.e., it is not restricted to certain domains such as language or physics. However, the
onset and timing of this process is not only assumed to be specific to domains but even to
microdomains, e.g., acquiring past tense verb forms or balancing blocks. This means that
representational redescription may occur at different ages in different microdomains. In one
microdomain, a child may only have level-I representations, while, in another microdomain, she
may have acquired representations up to level E3, for example.
RR theory offers a unifying framework for understanding both the amazing, yet
piecemeal competencies of young infants and the protracted, domain-specific course of
development culminating in theory-like knowledge structures and the potential for creative and
scientific thinking. But how can the theory be validated, and what empirical evidence supports
it? Two types of evidence may be distinguished, one is relatively weak and the other is relatively
strong. The weaker evidence concerns results suggesting a dissociation between different kinds
of knowledge within the same individual or a developmental progression from context-specific
(nonverbal) knowledge to more generally accessible (verbal) knowledge. We will not deal with
this kind of evidence here (see, e.g., Alibali & Goldin-Meadow, 1993; Clements & Perner, 1994;
Krist et al., 1993). Stronger evidence comes from studies showing that children go beyond
behavioral mastery, especially if this results in U-shaped behavioral growth. RR theory explains
certain U-shaped growth functions, i.e., the temporary decline of performance, by positing that,
after having achieved behavioral mastery, children no longer focus on the external data but rather
on their internal representations (for other accounts of U-shaped behavioral change, see Strauss,
Children’s Block Balancing 6
1982; Siegler, 2004). On the one hand, this shift of attention enables representational change, and
on the other hand it can lead to new errors and inflexibilities.
Among the stronger pieces of evidence Karmiloff-Smith (1992) refers to in her book, one
stands out as being particularly compelling and illustrative. It concerns the development of
children’s block balancing skills and concepts. In this microdomain a U-shaped behavioral
growth pattern was observed by Karmiloff-Smith and Inhelder (1974) suggesting
representational redescription. Children first balanced both symmetrical and asymmetrical
objects successfully (on a narrow support), then exhibited systematic errors with asymmetrical
objects by trying to balance them at their geometric center, before they mastered both types of
balancing problems again. Karmiloff-Smith (1992) interprets this U-shaped behavioral sequence
as follows: In the first phase, children deal with each object as a separate item by applying
implicit knowledge. The level-I knowledge consists of sensorimotor routines for balancing
individual objects based on proprioceptive feedback. In the second phase, a level-E1
representation is abstracted from the level-I knowledge. This representation comprises a simple
rule that correctly applies to most objects but leads to systematic errors in the case of
asymmetrical objects. It is only in the third phase that these objects are treated correctly again. In
the process of redescribing the level-E1 representation into an E2 or E3 format, this is achieved
by considering the anomalous items as such, i.e., as exceptions to the rule or as demanding a
more complex rule.
The present study focuses on the balancing task introduced by Karmiloff-Smith and
Inhelder (1974). Therefore, before presenting the aims of our own study, a closer look will be
taken at the original work, followed by a short review of follow-up studies reported in the
literature.
Children’s Block Balancing 7
The Development of Children’s Knowledge About Block Balancing
The research conducted by Karmiloff-Smith and Inhelder (1974) on children’s block
balancing is a pioneering work for the Genevan school, if not for cognitive developmentalists as
a whole. It has still great appeal for leading theoreticians in the field, as exemplified by a recent
reference to it by Siegler (2004, p. 7). Given the great theoretical influence of Karmiloff-Smith
and Inhelder’s original work, it is striking both how preliminary this work has been and how few
follow-up studies have been conducted during the past three decades.
The preliminary and exploratory character of the original study is acknowledged by the
authors themselves. With respect to the lack of a standardized procedure, they write:
"Indeed, just as the child was constructing a theory-in-action in his endeavor to balance
blocks, so we, too, were making on-the-spot hypotheses about the child’s theories and providing
opportunities for negative and positive responses in order to verify our theories!” (Karmiloff-
Smith & Inhelder, 1974, p. 197).
The research strategy chosen was thus similar to Piaget's method of the clinical interview
(Ginsburg, 1997) with the important difference that children's actions were the primary focus of
inquiry, rather than their verbal responses. Probably due to the "clinical" nature of Karmiloff-
Smith and Inhelder's research strategy, neither procedural details nor quantitative results are
reported in their article. This caveat has to be kept in mind when trying to interpret their
observations in terms of the RR theory or any other theoretical account. Nevertheless, these
observations have told a rich and interesting story about children's learning and development, the
main themes of which will be summarized next.
In the main part of their study, Karmiloff-Smith asked 23 children between 4 and 9 years
of age to balance various blocks on a narrow bar "so that they do not fall". There were various
Children’s Block Balancing 8
types of blocks belonging to four different categories: length blocks, conspicuous weight blocks,
inconspicuous weight blocks, and blocks that were only balanceable by using counterweights. In
length blocks, the center of gravity coincided with the block's geometric center, i.e., the weight
was evenly distributed in these blocks. Conspicuous weight blocks were asymmetrical along
their length axis, i.e., their center of gravity was shifted towards the heavier side. The latter was
also true for inconspicuous weight blocks, due to invisible weights inserted into one end of the
otherwise symmetrical blocks.
Although Karmiloff-Smith and Inhelder do not give exact figures regarding the mean age
or time of occurrence within a session at which particular behaviors were observed, three levels
can be discerned that were later taken as indicative of level-I, level-E1, and level-E2/3
representations by Karmiloff-Smith (1992). On the first level, characteristic of 4- and 5-year-
olds, children were highly successful in balancing both length and weight blocks irrespective of
their type. They appeared to treat each balancing trial as a separate task by using proprioceptive
information to adjust the point of support until equilibrium was achieved. Corrections were
rarely made in the wrong direction, i.e., away from the center of gravity. On the second level,
characteristic of 6- and 7-year olds, children appeared to have generalized the naive rule that all
blocks balance at their geometric center, the so-called geometric-center theory. They now
systematically tried all blocks first at their geometric center and, with conspicuous and
inconspicuous weight blocks, sometimes made adjustments in the direction of the geometric
center even if this lead to greater imbalance. Often children abandoned their attempts to balance
a weight block altogether, after having tried only a small region around the geometric center, and
declared the block as impossible to balance. Interestingly, the same children were reported to be
able to balance inconspicuous weight blocks easily with their eyes closed relying on
Children’s Block Balancing 9
proprioceptive feedback only. The rigid application of the geometric-center theory (with eyes
open) actually led to a decline in the number of successful trials at the age of approximately 5.5
years. Older children, 8 and 9 years of age, typically performed on the highest level. They were
again successful with both length and weight blocks, assessing the weight distribution of each
block in advance by lifting it, and immediately placing it close to the correct point of support.
Surprisingly few studies have tried to replicate or extend the original observations by
Karmiloff-Smith and Inhelder. To the best of our knowledge, there is only one line of such
research: a series of studies conducted by Pine and Messer (Pine & Messer, 1998, 1999, 2000,
2003; Pine, Lufkin, & Messer, 2004). While the focus of most of their research has been on
instructional issues, they have also studied children's block balancing from the developmental
perspective of RR theory. Only the latter part of their research is considered here. Pine and
Messer adapted the balancing paradigm and the stimuli from Karmiloff-Smith and Inhelder
(1974) using a semi-standardized procedure and objective criteria for identifying different levels
of representation. Children were asked to balance different symmetrical and asymmetrical
blocks. Additionally, they were encouraged to give explanations about how each block balanced
or not. In some of the studies (Pine & Messer, 1999, 2003) children were also asked for
predictions of whether a block would balance or not. Based on children's number of successful
trials with symmetrical and asymmetrical blocks, their tendency to try to balance all blocks at
their geometric center, and their verbal responses, Pine and Messer defined six levels of
representation that could be reliably coded by independent raters. Besides two transition levels,
they distinguished the following levels: Implicit (I), Abstraction Nonverbal, Abstraction Verbal,
and Explicit (E3). While the I and E3 levels correspond with those postulated by Karmiloff-
Smith, no evidence for level E2 was found, and level E1 was replaced by the two Abstraction
Children’s Block Balancing 10
levels. As already noted by Karmiloff-Smith and Inhelder (1974), but not predicted by RR
theory, some children are able to verbalize their geometric-center theory (e.g., "All things
balance in the middle"). These children are classified, according to Pine and Messer's scheme, as
having reached the Abstraction Verbal level, whereas children who only exhibit this theory in
their actions are classified as still operating with representations on the Abstraction Nonverbal
level.
In an earlier cross-sectional study conducted with more than 168 children aged between 5
and 9 years, Pine and Messer (1999) categorized children's verbal and nonverbal behavior into
the levels of representation defined by Karmiloff-Smith (1992). They found that about every
second child exhibited the naive center strategy, being successful with the symmetrical but not
with the asymmetrical blocks, placing all blocks onto the support at their midpoint, and often
declaring asymmetrical blocks as impossible to balance. However, there was only mixed
evidence for a developmental trend along the lines suggested by RR theory. Most 4-year-olds'
behavior was unclassifiable, evidence for implicit knowledge was most frequent among the 7-
year-olds, and the geometric-center theory was found with about the same frequency in 5-
through 9-year-olds. Only the frequency of level-E3 classifications exhibited the predicted age
trend, beginning in the group of 6-year-olds and increasing through the group of 9-year-olds.
Using their own, modified classification system, Pine and Messer (2003) obtained at least
partial support for the sequential ordering of the postulated levels of representation in a
longitudinal study covering a 5-day period. Twenty-five 5- to 6-year-olds were given the
balancing task on each of five consecutive days. On the second, third, and fourth day, the
balancing task was part of a free play session in which the children were encouraged to talk
about what they were learning. Apart from one child, all children changed levels at least once. Of
Children’s Block Balancing 11
all transitions 88% were to a higher and 12% to a lower level. By the fifth day, 80% of the
children had moved to a higher level, 8% had stayed on the same level, and 12% had moved to a
lower level. Skipping of at least one level occurred frequently, in 44% of the children, but no
child progressed directly from level I to the Explicit level (E3).
The Present Research
Pine and Messer have presented some evidence in support of RR theory and have shed
new light on the development of children's block balancing. However, their work has left open at
least the following important issue: Do children spontaneously develop the geometric-center
theory and regress to a lower rate of success with asymmetrical or invisibly weighted objects? In
other words, is there evidence for representational redescription occurring (within the
microdomain of block balancing but) outside the laboratory? Karmiloff-Smith and Inhelder's
original study does not answer this question because of its exploratory character and the fact that
children had extensive opportunities to explore the blocks and practice the task. Pine and
Messer's research does not answer this question either because their results are contaminated by
possible effects of repeatedly asking the child for a verbal explanation or prediction.
In the present study, we therefore combined a detailed observation of children's block
balancing with a standardized procedure of data collection, uncontaminated by repeated verbal
questioning. To cover the age range, during which the presumed developmental changes should
occur, we assessed the block balancing behavior of children between 4 and 9 years of age in a
cross-sectional study. Using a subset of the blocks originally employed by Karmiloff-Smith and
Inhelder (1974), including an invisibly weighted block, we carefully analyzed children's
balancing behavior in two series of five trials. The following empirical hypotheses were derived
from Karmiloff-Smith and Inhelder’s findings and tested in the present study:
Children’s Block Balancing 12
(1) The rate of success should follow a U-shaped age trend with asymmetrical (or
invisibly weighted) blocks, whereas it should remain constant (at a high level) or increase with
age with symmetrical blocks. (2) The time needed for successful performance should exhibit an
inverted U-shaped age trend with the asymmetrical but not the symmetrical blocks. (3) An
inverted U-shaped age trend should also be obtained for the frequency at which asymmetrical
blocks are first tried at their midpoint. (4) The relative frequency of correct adjustments should
again follow a U-shaped age trend, as far as the asymmetrical blocks are concerned.
In terms of RR theory, each of these four non-monotonic age trends occurs because the
externally driven level-I representations become redescribed into the level-E1 format—the
geometric-center theory—thereby disregarding negative action outcomes as well as
proprioceptive feedback. This leads to a temporary decline in performance, which is later
reversed when children begin to redescribe their level-E1 knowledge into the more flexible,
better integrated, and more adaptive E2 or E3 format.
Method
Participants. Sixty-five children participated in this experiment. There were four age
groups: 4-year-olds (12 boys, 3 girls, mean age: 4 years, 5 months), 5-year-olds (7 boys, 11 girls,
mean age: 5 years, 5 months), 6-year-olds (6 boys, 10 girls, mean age: 6 years, 8 months), and 8-
year-olds (6 boys, 10 girls, mean age: 8 years, 5 months). The 4- and 5-year-olds were recruited
from two kindergartens and the 6- and 8-year-olds from two elementary schools in Greifswald,
Germany. All children participated on a voluntary basis and with the consent of their parents.
One 4-year-old child had to be replaced because she obviously did not understand the task.
Materials. The balancing task was adopted from Karmiloff-Smith and Inhelder (1974).
Five different wooden blocks were to be balanced on a narrow bar (25 cm long, 1 cm wide, 3.5
Children’s Block Balancing 13
cm high) mounted onto a board (Figure 1). There were two symmetrical blocks (A and B), two
asymmetrical blocks (C and D), and one inconspicuous weight block (E). The symmetrical
blocks balanced at their geometric center, whereas the other blocks balanced off-center (block C:
2.8 cm; block D: 5.2 cm; block E: 1.7 cm). Block A was a single beam (25 cm long, 5.5 cm
wide, 2.5 cm high). Block B was made by overlapping two flat beams of the same size as block
A (total length: 37.5 cm). The asymmetrical blocks C and D consisted of a beam (block C: 25 cm
long, 5.5 cm wide, 2.8 cm high; block D: 25 cm long, 6 cm wide, 1 cm high) onto which a block
(6 cm long, 5.5 cm wide, 6 cm high) was glued on one end. The inconspicuous weight block E
was a single beam (23.5 cm long, 9 cm wide, 2.8 cm high) into which a metal plate was inserted
at one end. Blocks A and B were colored red; block C was yellow; block D was blue; and block
E was green. To allow for accurate readings of the blocks' placement on the bar from the video
recordings, each block had a measuring scale attached to it consisting of alternating black and
white stripes (0.5 cm wide; see Figure 1).
Video analysis. Children's balancing behavior was videotaped using a digital camcorder
(Sony DSR-PD150P) mounted on a tripod opposite to the child. A first observer coded the video
data according to a predefined coding system and by means of the computer program "Interact"
(Mangold Software & Consulting GmbH). To evaluate the inter-observer reliability, a second
observer coded a subset of the data (all trials from 4 children). The coding system included the
following categories: (1) Success was defined as the successful balancing of a block. (2) A
midpoint placement was coded if a block was first placed onto the support close to its geometric
center (+/- 1.25 cm). (3) Adjustments were defined as any corrections of the block's position
along its length axis. They were coded as correct if children moved the block towards the point
of balance, otherwise they were coded as incorrect. Additionally, response times were calculated
Children’s Block Balancing 14
for successful trials from the video data. The response time was defined as the time interval
between the child's first touch and her final release of the block.
Inter-observer reliability was greater than κ = .93 for the success and midpoint categories
and greater than r = .98 for the number of correct and incorrect adjustments and the response
times.
Procedure. Each child was tested individually in a suitable room of her kindergarten or
school by one experimenter. An experimental session took about 5 to 15 minutes. The child was
seated at a table at a right angle to the experimenter. The balancing board was placed on the table
in front of the child so that the length axis of the fulcrum was perpendicular to the child's frontal
plane. At the beginning of an experimental session, only block A was visible, and the child was
instructed as follows: "Today we are going to play with blocks. Did you ever play with blocks?
It's a special game and a little bit tricky. I am going to give you several blocks one at a time.
Please, try to put each block on this rod so that it will not fall down. I am not going to help you.
If you can't do it the first time, that's not a problem. You can try it again if you want."
Demonstrating and commenting on the orientation at which each block had to be put onto the bar
(i.e., with the block's length axis perpendicular to the fulcrum), the experimenter then presented
block A without giving any feedback about its equilibrium position. The child was asked to try to
balance the five blocks in the order A, B, C, D, E in two series of trials. The blocks were
presented separately with the remaining blocks remaining out of view. If a child paused after
having failed to balance a block, she was encouraged to try again. The next block was presented
when (a) success had been achieved, (b) the child gave up, or (c) she did not change the block's
position for at least two minutes.
Children’s Block Balancing 15
Results
The values of the performance scores obtained by analyzing the video recordings were
submitted to 2 (Symmetry) x 2 (Series) x 4 (Age) ANOVAs with repeated measures on the first
two factors. As a part of the ANOVAs, polynomial contrasts were computed for the age factor,
to assess the hypotheses of (inverted) U-shaped age trends for asymmetrical and invisibly
weighted blocks. A U-shaped trend should be revealed by a large and significant quadratic trend.
Success scores. For each series of trials, success scores were computed by counting the
successful balancing trials for each type of block (symmetrical: A1+B1, A2+B2; asymmetrical:
C1+D1, C2+D2). Therefore, success scores ranged between 0 and 2.
An ANOVA performed on the success scores revealed a large and significant age effect,
F(3, 61) = 8.334, p < .001, η 2
= .291. Four-year-old children had the lowest success scores
(averaged over both series: Msym = 1.67, Masym = 1.00), followed by the group of 5-year-olds
(Msym = 1.83, Masym = 1.69), whereas the 6-year-olds (Msym = 2.00, Masym = 1.81) and 8-year-olds
(Msym = 1.97, Masym = 2.00) performed close to ceiling.
As illustrated in Figure 2, a large effect of symmetry, F(1, 61) = 14.364, p < .001, η 2
= .
191, and a significant interaction between symmetry and age, F(3, 61) = 5.091, p = .003, η 2
= .
200, were also obtained. Overall, symmetrical blocks were more often balanced successfully
than asymmetrical blocks. Older children, especially the 8-year-olds, performed close to ceiling
with both types of blocks, however. The interaction of age and symmetry was largely attributable
to different slopes of the linear age trend, with the slope being steeper for asymmetrical than for
symmetrical blocks, p = 0.01. Neither the quadratic component of the age by symmetry
interaction, p = .265, nor the quadratic trend calculated for the asymmetrical blocks alone were
reliably different from chance, p = .099.
Children’s Block Balancing 16
Response times. The times children needed to complete a balancing trial successfully
were analyzed analogously to the success scores. Where only one of the two trials per series and
block type was performed successfully, the mean was calculated as the value from the other trial.
Children who failed on both symmetrical or both asymmetrical trials within a series where
excluded from the analysis. Among the remaining children, there were 7 4-year-olds, 17 5-year-
olds, 15 6-year-olds, and 16 8-year-olds.
Whereas the age effect just missed the significance level, F(3, 51) = 2.747, p = .052, there
was a significant series effect, F(1, 51) = 5.877, p = .019, η 2
= .103, as well as a significant
interaction between series and symmetry, F(1, 51) = 4.959, p = .030, η 2
= .089 (see Figure 3).
Mean response times (adjusted for age) were almost identical for symmetrical and asymmetrical
blocks in the first series (M1st,sym = 20.93 s, M1st,asym = 18.80 s). From the first to the second series,
the mean response time decreased substantially for symmetrical blocks but not for asymmetrical
blocks (M2nd,sym = 10.06 s, M2nd,asym = 18.36 s).
Midpoint scores. As described in the Method section, the initial placement of each block
was assessed by analyzing the video recordings. Midpoint scores were calculated, analogously to
the success scores, by counting the frequency of trials in which a block was first placed with its
geometric center close to the support.
Figure 4 depicts the mean midpoint scores as a function of age, symmetry, and series. An
ANOVA performed on the midpoint scores revealed no significant age trend, F(3, 61) = 1.721, p
= .172. Symmetry exhibited a very large effect, F(1, 61) = 80.708, p < .001, η 2
= .570, and the
interaction between age and symmetry also proved significant, F(3, 61) = 5.097, p = .003, η 2
= .
200. Midpoint scores were positively correlated with age for symmetrical blocks (M = 1.63,
1.75, 1.78, and 1.91 for the 4-, 5-, 6-, and 8-year-olds, respectively) and negatively correlated
Children’s Block Balancing 17
with age for asymmetrical blocks (M = 1.30, .972, .844, and .50 for the 4-, 5-, 6-, and 8-year-
olds, respectively). There was no significant quadratic component of the age by symmetry
interaction, p = .950, and there was no significant quadratic trend for the asymmetrical blocks
alone either, p = .959. The significance criterion was just missed by the series factor, F(1, 61) =
3.767, p = .057, and the interaction between series and symmetry, F(1, 61) = 3.535, p = .065.
Number of adjustments and percentage of correct adjustments. In most trials, children
adjusted their initial placement of the block in their balancing attempts. The number of
adjustments as well as the percentage of correct adjustments was calculated for symmetrical and
asymmetrical blocks of each series. Children who did not adjust in two trials of the same type
within a series had to be excluded from the analysis of the percentage of correct adjustments.
This pertained to two 4-year-olds, one 5-year-old, two 6-year-olds, and three 8-year-olds.
Figure 5 summarizes the results for the number of adjustments and the percentage of
correct adjustments. ANOVAs performed on both dependent measures revealed a significant age
effect in both cases, F(3, 61) = 5.311, p = .003, η 2
= .207, and F(3, 53) = 4.819, p = .001, η 2
= .
214, respectively. Four-year-olds (M = 14.63) and 5-year-olds (M = 14.58) made nearly twice as
much adjustments as 6-year-olds (M = 7.53) and 8-year-olds (M = 7.31). The percentage of
correct adjustments also increased with age (M = 73.4%, 76.3%, 85.4%, and 86.1% for the 4-, 5-,
6-, and 8-year-olds, respectively). Symmetry also had a significant effect both on the number of
corrections, F(1, 61) = 29.893, p < .001, η 2
= .329, and the percentage of correct adjustments,
F(3, 53) = 14.193, p < .001, η 2
= .211. Averaged over age and series, children adjusted less often
with symmetrical than with asymmetrical blocks (Msym = 7.45, Masym = 15.59) and had a higher
rate of correct adjustments (Msym = 83.7%, Masym = 76.9%).
Children’s Block Balancing 18
Regarding the number of adjustments, there was a significant interaction between
symmetry and age, F(3, 61) = 3.718, p = .016, η 2
= .115. The symmetry effect was noticeably
larger in the groups of 4-year-olds (Msym = 11.17, Masym = 18.10) and 5-year-olds (Msym = 7.58,
Masym = 21.58) than in the groups of 6-year-olds (Msym = 5.13, Masym = 9.94) and 8-year-olds
(Msym = 5.91, Masym = 8.72). There was neither a significant quadratic component of this
interaction, p = .088, nor a significant quadratic trend for the asymmetrical blocks considered
alone, p = .386. No significant age by symmetry interaction was found with respect to the
percentage of correct adjustments, F(3, 53) = 1.406, p = .251.
There was a significant main effect of series on the number of adjustments, F(1, 61) =
9.107, p = .004, η 2
= .130. Adjustments were more frequent in the first series (M = 12.71) than in
the second series (M = 9.32).
Inconspicuous weight block. Trials with the invisibly weighted block were analyzed like
the other trials, except that there was no symmetry factor and only one trial per series. For the
latter reason, success and midpoint scores were analyzed by applying χ 2
-tests for each series
separately. From the ANOVAs and χ 2
-tests performed on the success scores, response times,
midpoint scores, number of adjustments and percentage of correct adjustments, only a single
effect emerged which was statistically reliable: The percentage of correct adjustments tended to
increase with age (M = 73.6%, M = 78.0%, M = 89.9%, M = 85.3%, for the 4-, 5-, 6-, and 8-
year-olds, respectively), F(3, 50) = 3.497, p = .022, η 2
= .173. The small quadratic component of
this trend did not point in the predicted direction and remained statistically insignificant, p = .
241.
Discussion
Children’s Block Balancing 19
The results of this study call into question the robustness of the U-shaped developmental
trend regarding children's block balancing as originally reported by Karmiloff-Smith and
Inhelder (1974). Where performance was not already close to ceiling, we observed significant
quasi-linear improvements from age group to age group in almost all analyses: The rate of
success and the percentage of correct adjustments tended to increase, while the time to success
and the number of adjustments tended to decrease with age. As expected, symmetrical blocks
were easier to balance than asymmetrical blocks. However, there was no indication of any U-
shaped age trend as predicted for the asymmetrical blocks and the invisibly weighted block.
Neither the rate of success nor the percentage of correct adjustments followed a U-shaped age
trend; and neither the time needed for successful performance nor the frequency at which these
blocks were first tried at their geometric center exhibited an inverted U-shaped age trend.
Furthermore, there was no age by symmetry interaction effect on these four dependent measures,
as far as the quadratic age component (indicative of a U-shaped trend) was concerned.
Taken together, our results are at odds with each of the predictions derived from
Karmiloff-Smith and Inhelder's (1974) findings and the interpretation of these findings in terms
of RR theory. In the present study, the geometric-center theory, i.e., the naive rule that all blocks
balance in the middle, was apparently not used more frequently in 5- or 6-year-olds than in the
other age groups. How can this conclusion be reconciled with Karmiloff-Smith's original
demonstration of a U-shaped developmental trend and the replication of this result in Pine and
Messer's research (Pine & Messer, 1999, 2003)? In principle, two possibilities have to be
considered: First, the previous findings and/or the present results may lack internal validity; and
second, the discrepancies may be due to procedural differences. Each possibility will be
considered in turn.
Children’s Block Balancing 20
Concerning the issue of internal validity, our own results appear to be the least
questionable. We have employed a highly standardized procedure combined with detailed video
analyses of different aspects of children's actions, and the sample size was large enough (N=65)
to detect moderate age trends in the predicted direction. By contrast, Karmiloff-Smith and
Inhelder (1974) used a "clinical" procedure with a small sample (N=23) and analyzed children's
actions on the spot, apparently without any specific coding system. However, it would be
premature to dismiss the original findings by simply criticizing them on methodological grounds.
After all, the comprehensive research published by Pine and Messer has lent substantial support
to the developmental trend claimed by Karmiloff-Smith and Inhelder. Although Pine and Messer
did not analyze U-shaped age trends directly, such trends are implied by their definition of
different levels of representation in conjunction with evidence suggesting that these levels
constitute a developmental sequence (see Introduction). We see no reason to seriously question
the internal validity of the studies conducted by Pine and Messer. Therefore, we tentatively
conclude that the discrepancy between our results and previous findings is real and thus must
originate from procedural differences between the studies.
Although only few procedural details are reported by Karmiloff-Smith and Inhelder
(1974), it is obvious that their study differed in many respects from the present one. The most
important differences appear to concern the amount of practice children were given and the order
of presentation of the blocks. In Karmiloff-Smith and Inhelder's study, children practiced the
balancing task extensively using a variety of blocks, and they were more or less free to choose
which block to use on each trial. In Pine and Messer's research, the amount of practice given
varied from study to study, but evidence for inflexible use of the geometric-center theory was
also obtained when the number of trials was comparable to the present study (e.g., Pine &
Children’s Block Balancing 21
Messer, 2000). Similarly to the original study, children were free to select which block to try
next, but were asked to deal with each block. In another respect, Pine and Messer's procedure
differed, however, from both the original and the present study. To be able to infer the level of
representation a child used, they repeatedly asked the child for explanations and, in some studies
(Pine & Messer, 1999, 2000, 2003) also for predictions, e.g., "Why do you think that one
wouldn't balance?" (Pine & Messer, 1999, p. 21), or "Do you think it can be balanced?" (Pine &
Messer, 2000, p. 41).
Whether the less constrained (and perhaps the simultaneous) presentation of the blocks,
extensive practice, or repeated verbal questioning, either alone or in combination, are sufficient
to induce a U-shaped age trend in one or the other dependent measure, can and should be
clarified in future research. At present, one can only speculate about the reason why many
children of about 5 to 6 years of age appear to apply the geometric-center theory in one
experimental setting but not the other. One possibility, particularly suggested by the difference
between the present study and Pine and Messer's research, is that children of this age tend to
elaborate the geometric-center theory or a similar rule if they are induced to think about the
question of why some blocks balance and some do not. Note that such a question is actually
misleading because it erroneously presupposes that there are blocks which cannot be balanced,
and it probably rarely occurred to the children investigated in our study. The question that was
more likely induced by our procedure was about how blocks balance. Thus, even if children used
or elaborated the rule that some blocks balance in the middle, in our case, they apparently did not
generalize this rule to asymmetrical blocks and not even to the invisibly weighted block. Had
they been encouraged to categorize blocks into balanceable and not balanceable ones, they might
have framed a rule like "blocks balance in the middle, or not at all." The application of such a
Children’s Block Balancing 22
rule (combined with a notion of symmetry) would lead to just the kind of inflexibilities and
errors reported in the previous studies, although it is different from the presumed geometric-
center theory which says that all blocks balance in the middle.
Independently from these still speculative considerations, the present results strongly
suggest that the geometric-center theory does not develop spontaneously outside the laboratory.
Theoretically, it is possible, that children do develop such a representation but do (a) not readily
or (b) not exclusively apply it in our task context. However, at least, the former of these two
possibilities does not seem very likely, because the present task was well-suited to activate a
representation of how blocks balance, if it existed prior to the experiment. In sum, we can
conclude that there appears to be no stage in the development of knowledge about block
balancing in which children adhere to the geometric-center theory in an inflexible and exclusive
way. Hence, we were not able to find evidence for representational redescription in the
microdomain studied.
What are the implications for RR theory? To be sure, a single experiment cannot refute a
complex theory. Yet, strong evidence for representational redescription is sparse, and the present
results have severely undermined one important piece of evidence in support of RR theory. To
the extent that evidence for representational redescription can only be found in a few
microdomains and, within these microdomains, only under limited circumstances, RR theory
looses its explanatory power and heuristic value. It appears, however, both premature and unwise
to give up RR theory right away. Giving up RR theory, would be premature because RR theory
does not comprise a fixed set of axioms but a few core assumptions which can be specified in
different ways and, of course, because more pertinent research is needed. Giving up RR theory
would also be unwise because alternatives are almost completely lacking. Karmiloff-Smith
Children’s Block Balancing 23
herself seems to view modern connectionism combined with developmental neuropsychology as
an advancement of RR theory (Elman, Bates, Johnson, Karmiloff-Smith, Parisi, & Plunkett,
1996; Karmiloff-Smith & Inhelder, 1992). We are not so sure about that. The brilliance and
power of connectionist simulations of developmental change notwithstanding, it is not yet clear
how they could give a principled account of different levels of knowledge and the process of
representational redescription. Particularly the idea that development is mainly driven by success
rather than failure runs counter to the fact that standard simulation networks stop learning once
success has been achieved.
Better alternatives to RR theory appear to be offered by approaches which acknowledge
different kinds of knowledge or processes but do not claim strong ontogenetic relations between
these kinds of knowledge or processes (e.g., Gershkoff-Stowe & Thelen, 2004). In particular, the
constructivist assumption that conceptual knowledge is derived from action knowledge is
questionable. If at all, conceptual generalization of action knowledge appears to be achieved only
indirectly, via perceptual and imagery processes concerning actions (Krist, 2003). Applied to the
balancing paradigm, this would mean that the geometric-center theory is not directly abstracted
from the procedural skill of balancing blocks based on proprioceptive and visual feedback. In
fact, it is not clear how a generalization like "all objects balance in the middle" could ever be
derived from a negative-feedback loop alone. It is more plausible that the geometric-center
theory or a similar rule is derived from an analysis of the outcomes of successful balancing
attempts (cf., Mandler, 1988, 1992). In this sense, children may indeed go beyond behavioral
mastery. Exploring under which conditions they actually do and in which cases this results in U-
shaped behavioral change, is worth further scrutiny.
Children’s Block Balancing 24
References
Alibali, M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1993). Gesture-speech mismatch and mechanisms of
learning: What the hand reveals about a child's state of mind. Cognitive Psychology, 25,
468-523.
Clements, W. A., & Perner, J. (1994). Implicit understanding of belief. Cognitive
Development, 9, 377-395.
Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett,
K. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
Gershkoff-Stowe, L., & Thelen, E. (2004). U-shaped changes in behavior: A dynamic
systems perspective. Journal of Cognition and Development, 5, 11-36.
Ginsburg, H. P. (1997). Entering the child’s mind: The clinical interview in psychological
research and practice, Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on
cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Inhelder, B. (1974). If you want to get ahead, get a theory.
Cognition, 3, 195-212.
Krist, H. (2003). Knowing how to project objects: Probing the generality of children's
action knowledge. Journal of Cognition and Development, 4, 383-414.
Krist, H., Fieberg, E. L., & Wilkening, F. (1993). Intuitive physics in action and
judgment: The development of knowledge about projectile motion. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 952-966.
Children’s Block Balancing 25
Mandler, J. M. (1988). How to build a baby: On the development of an accessible
representational system. Cognitive Development, 3, 113-136.
Mandler, J. M. (1992). How to build a baby II: Conceptual primitives. Psychological
Review, 99, 587-604.
Pine, K. J., Lufkin, N., & Messer, D. (2004). More gestures than answers: Children
learning about balance. Developmental Psychology, 40, 1059-1067.
Pine, K. J., & Messer, D. (1998). Group collaboration effects and the explicitness of
children's knowledge. Cognitive Development, 13, 109-126.
Pine, K. J., & Messer, D. (1999). What children do and what children know: Looking
beyond success using Karmiloff-Smith's RR framework. New Ideas in Psychology, 17, 17-30.
Pine, K. J., & Messer, D. (2000). The effect of explaining another's actions on children's
implicit theories of balance. Cognition and Instruction, 18, 35-51.
Pine, K. J., & Messer, D. (2003). The development of representations as children learn
about balancing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 285-301.
Siegler, R. S. (2004). U-shaped interest in U-shaped development—and what it means.
Journal of Cognition and Development, 5, 1-10.
Spelke, S. (1994). Initial knowledge: Six suggestions. Cognition, 50, 431-445.
Strauss, S. (Ed.). (1982). U-shaped behavioral growth. New York: Academic Press.
Children’s Block Balancing 26
Author Note
We wish to thank Alexander Koch for assistance in data collection and coding.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Horst Krist, Institut für
Psychologie, Universität Greifswald, Franz-Mehring-Str. 47, D-17487 Greifswald, Germany. E-
mail: krist@uni-greifswald.de
Children’s Block Balancing 27
Figure Captions
Figure 1. Materials used in this study: Blocks A-E (left panel: front view; right panel: top view)
and balancing board (top panel, with block A).
Figure 2. Means and standard errors of the success scores achieved by 4-, 5-, 6-, and 8-year-old
children trying to balance symmetrical and asymmetrical blocks in two series of trials. The
maximum score is two.
Figure 3. Means and standard errors of the response times of 4-, 5-, 6-, and 8-year-old children
for successful attempts to balance symmetrical and asymmetrical blocks in two series of trials.
Figure 4. Means and standard errors of the midpoint scores of 4-, 5-, 6-, and 8-year-old children
trying to balance symmetrical and asymmetrical blocks in two series of trials. A score of one was
given for each trial in which the child first placed the block onto the support at its geometric
center. The maximum score is two.
Figure 5. Means and standard errors for number of adjustments (top panel) and percentage of
correct adjustments (bottom panel) observed with 4-, 5-, 6-, and 8-year-old children trying to
balance symmetrical and asymmetrical blocks in two series of trials.
... M. [Author] et al., 2020). The finding that most pre-school children do not use mass theory to predict stability is supported by other studies (e.g., Bonawitz et al., 2012;Krist, 2010Krist, , 2013Krist et al., 2005Krist et al., , 2018. Moreover, research has shown that children perform better on symmetrical objects than for asymmetrical ones (Krist, 2010;Krist et al., 2005). ...
... The finding that most pre-school children do not use mass theory to predict stability is supported by other studies (e.g., Bonawitz et al., 2012;Krist, 2010Krist, , 2013Krist et al., 2005Krist et al., , 2018. Moreover, research has shown that children perform better on symmetrical objects than for asymmetrical ones (Krist, 2010;Krist et al., 2005). Although the studies used different paradigms to assess children's knowledge about mass such as pictures (A. ...
... [Author] & [Author], 2020), real stimuli (Krist, 2010(Krist, , 2013, physical action (Bonawitz et al., 2012;Krist et al., 2005) or eye-tracking (Krist et al., 2018), they all reflect young children's limited capability to consider an object's mass when assessing stability. There is evidence that children's use of mass theory increases with age (Bonawitz et al., 2012;Krist et al., 2005Krist et al., , 2018 and can be fostered by playful interventions (Pine & Messer, 2003). ...
... Research indicated that asymmetrical building block constructions are more difficult to rate correctly than symmetrical building block constructions for 3-to 6-year-old children (Krist, 2010), indicating a lack of explicit stability knowledge. Additionally, Krist et al. (2005) found that 4-and 5-year-olds struggle to actively balance symmetrical building blocks on a beam scale, suggesting a lack of appropriate implicit knowledge. However, the development of stability knowledge between 4 and 8 years (Krist et al., 2005) can be fostered through interventions that address children's strategies to stabilize building blocks and their reasoning about it . ...
... Additionally, Krist et al. (2005) found that 4-and 5-year-olds struggle to actively balance symmetrical building blocks on a beam scale, suggesting a lack of appropriate implicit knowledge. However, the development of stability knowledge between 4 and 8 years (Krist et al., 2005) can be fostered through interventions that address children's strategies to stabilize building blocks and their reasoning about it . These studies indicated that domain-specific rule knowledge can be learned implicitly and is accessible implicitly and explicitly. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Problem-solving in early and middle childhood is of high relevance for cognitive developmental research and educational support. Previous research on science problem-solving has focussed on the process and strategies of children handling challenging tasks, but less on providing insights into the cognitive network that enables science problem-solving. Aims: In this study, we aimed to investigate whether performance in science problem-solving is mainly determined by domain-specific rule knowledge, by domain-general cog-nitive abilities or both. Methods: In our study, 215 6-to 8-year-old children completed a set of three domain-specific rule knowledge tasks and three corresponding problem-solving tasks that were content-coherent, as well as a vocabulary task, and a reasoning task. Results: Correlational and regression analyses revealed a negligible impact of domain-specific rule knowledge on corresponding problem-solving tasks. In contrast, the associations between problem-solving performance in different domains and the associations between problem-solving performance and domain-general abilities (vocabulary and reasoning) were comparably strong. Conclusions: The findings suggest that science problem-solving in primary school children primarily relies on domain-general cognitive abilities. Implications of these findings are discussed with regard to cognitive theories and early science education.
... It is for this reason that the presence of beliefs is commonly inferred from patterns of overt behavior (e.g., Kloos & Van Orden, 2005;Kohn, 1993;Penner & Klahr, 1996). For example, the mistaken belief that beams either balance at their geometric middle or not at all is commonly inferred from consistent attempts of a child to balance the beams at their geometric middle (Pine & Messer, 1999, 2003Krist, Horz, & Sch€ onfeld, 2005;Cheung & Wong, 2011). In this section, we describe an alternative for determining the presence of dynamic stability, using advances made in the area of motor behavior. ...
... It was observed in seven sessions: four eyes-open sessions (B1, G1) and three eyes-closed sessions (B1, G1, G2). These findings are consistent with previous research with this task: approximately half of the trials are typically characterized by mistakes in balancing (Krist, Horz, & Sch€ onfeld, 2005;Pine & Messer, 2000, 2003. ...
Article
Full-text available
Mistaken beliefs pose a barrier to science learning. For this reason, it is important to understand the circumstances in which they emerge and change. In the current paper, we apply complexity theory to shed light on the nature of mistaken beliefs. The strength of this approach lies in conceptualizing beliefs as dynamic stabilities, a well-defined construct that can be indexed precisely. For example, Recurrence Quantification Analysis (RQA) can determine the presence of dynamic stabilities by analyzing variability in time-series data. We applied this analytical tool to probe for mistaken beliefs in a beam-balancing task, a task that is known to elicit mistaken beliefs in preschoolers. Using a case-study design with four preschoolers, we tracked children’s hand position with motion sensors as they balanced various beams. The resulting time series of hand position was submitted to RQA, yielding two important results: First, we found that consistent mistakes in trying to balance the beams were not always accompanied by dynamic stability. This undermines the common assumption that overt consistency in task performance is sufficient to conclude the presence of beliefs. Second, we found strong individual differences over time, as children explored the balance beams. Applications to science education are discussed. • Highlights • A classical task of beam balancing was used to explore the underlying dynamics of children’s mistaken beliefs. • Moment-to-moment hand movements were tracked and subjected to a multi-dimensional recurrence quantification analysis (RQA). • Dynamic stability was captured through percent laminarity (%LAM), a measure of rigidity in children’s explorations. • The RQA measure of %LAM shed light on patterns of stability that were not available from the analysis of overt behavior. • In line with complexity theory, a model of persistent mistakes is offered that has important implications for science education.
... • Kinder konstruieren kontinuierlich Theorien über die Welt • Diese entsprechen nicht wissenschaftlichen Anforderungen • Ermöglichen Vorhersagen und Erklärungen (Gopnik & Wellman, 2012) • Können verändert werden, wenn Gegenevidenz gezeigt wird, die nicht in bisheriges Schema assimiliert werden kann (Piaget & Inhelder, 1969) • Kinder können ihre Theorien ändern, indem ihr Vorwissen auf Gegenevidenz bezogen wird (Griffiths, Sobel, Tenenbaum, & Gopnik, 2011;Koerber, Sodian, Thoermer, & Nett, 2005) • Kann man kindliche Theorien ändern, indem man sie gezielt mit Gegenevidenz konfrontiert? • Bessere Leistungen bei der Einschätzung symmetrischer als asymmetrischer Objekte (Krist, Horz & Schönfeld, 2005) • 5-und 6-Jährige haben Probleme mit asymmetrischen Objekten (Krist, 2010) Theoretischer Hintergrund 6 Forschungsstand • Bonawitz, van Schijndel, Friel, & Schulz (2012) • N = 95, Alter = 6-7 • Zentral für heterogene Gruppe der Vorschulkinder (Einsiedler, 2009;Sylva et al., 2007) • Modellieren als Methode in Kitas verwendet, andere Strategien zur Anregung höherer Denkprozesse sind eher selten (Leuchter, Saalbach, Studhalter, & Tettenborn, in review) • (Bonawitz, van Schijndel, Friel, & Schulz, 2012) • Entwicklungsgemäß (Krist, Horz, & Schönfeld, 2005) • Kinder der verbalen Scaffolding-Gruppe änderten ihre naiven Theorien früher als die anderen beiden Gruppen (Griffiths, Sobel, Tenenbaum, & Gopnik, 2011;Koerber, Sodian, Thoermer, & Nett, 2005) ...
... • Kinder konstruieren kontinuierlich Theorien über die Welt • Diese entsprechen nicht wissenschaftlichen Anforderungen • Ermöglichen Vorhersagen und Erklärungen (Gopnik & Wellman, 2012) • Können verändert werden, wenn Gegenevidenz gezeigt wird, die nicht in bisheriges Schema assimiliert werden kann (Piaget & Inhelder, 1969) • Kinder können ihre Theorien ändern, indem ihr Vorwissen auf Gegenevidenz bezogen wird (Griffiths, Sobel, Tenenbaum, & Gopnik, 2011;Koerber, Sodian, Thoermer, & Nett, 2005) • Kann man kindliche Theorien ändern, indem man sie gezielt mit Gegenevidenz konfrontiert? • Bessere Leistungen bei der Einschätzung symmetrischer als asymmetrischer Objekte (Krist, Horz & Schönfeld, 2005) • 5-und 6-Jährige haben Probleme mit asymmetrischen Objekten (Krist, 2010) Theoretischer Hintergrund 6 Forschungsstand • Bonawitz, van Schijndel, Friel, & Schulz (2012) • N = 95, Alter = 6-7 • Zentral für heterogene Gruppe der Vorschulkinder (Einsiedler, 2009;Sylva et al., 2007) • Modellieren als Methode in Kitas verwendet, andere Strategien zur Anregung höherer Denkprozesse sind eher selten (Leuchter, Saalbach, Studhalter, & Tettenborn, in review) • (Bonawitz, van Schijndel, Friel, & Schulz, 2012) • Entwicklungsgemäß (Krist, Horz, & Schönfeld, 2005) • Kinder der verbalen Scaffolding-Gruppe änderten ihre naiven Theorien früher als die anderen beiden Gruppen (Griffiths, Sobel, Tenenbaum, & Gopnik, 2011;Koerber, Sodian, Thoermer, & Nett, 2005) ...
Conference Paper
Das Spiel mit Bauklötzen ist besonders für junge Kinder sehr wichtig. Es verbessert ihr räumliches Vorstellungsvermögen (Casey et al., 2008) und macht die Kinder mit unterschied-lichen physikalischen Gesetzen vertraut (Adams & Nesmith, 1996). Kinder verfügen bereits früh über ein intuitives Wissen über physikalische Prinzipien (Baillargeon, 1995; Wilkening, Huber, & Cacchione, 2006). Karmiloff-Smith (1992) fand heraus, dass Vorschulkinder oft ein intuitives Wissen darüber besitzen, wie symmetrische und asymmetrische Objekte auf einer Balkenwaage balanciert werden können, ohne dass sie in der Lage wären, ihren Gedanken-gang zu verbalisieren. Mit zunehmender Erfahrung entwickeln sie eine verbalisierbare Mitte-basierte-Theorie und versuchen, alle Objekte mit ihrer geometrischen Mitte auf der Balken-waage zu platzieren. Dabei scheitern sie an asymmetrischen Objekten. Diese Theorie wird spä-ter modifiziert, indem die Kinder die Masse eines asymmetrischen Objekts einbeziehen, um es zu balancieren. Ab dann können die Kinder sowohl symmetrische als auch asymmetrische Ob-jekte balancieren. Dieser Prozess muss nicht im Kindergarten abgeschlossen sein, sondern kann sich bis in die Grundschulzeit und darüber hinaus fortsetzen (Bonawitz, van Schijndel, Friel, & Schulz, 2012). Dennoch hat die Forschung gezeigt, dass spezifische Interventionen, die Kindern erlauben, Erfahrungen mit dem Balancieren von Objekten zu sammeln, diese Entwicklung unterstützen können (Bonawitz et al., 2012; Pine, Messer, & Godfrey, 1999). Außerdem kann auch Scaffolding den Lernprozess von Kindern anregen (van de Pol, Volman, & Beishuizen, 2010). Die Untersuchung des kindlichen Verständnisses von Gleichgewicht kann zum Wissensgewinn über die Wandlung vom intuitiven Wissen hin zu verbalisierbaren Theorien beitragen. Forschungsfragen Es wird untersucht, (a) ob Kinder eine Masse- oder Mitte-basierte-Theorie nutzen, um Objekte zu balancieren, (b) ob strukturiertes Material ihr Stabilitätsverständnis verbessern kann und (c) ob sie zusätzlich von verbalen Erklärungen profitieren. Methode 167 Kinder zwischen fünf und sechs Jahren (M = 5.50, SD = 0.49) wurden in einem Messwiederholungsdesign mit Prä- und Posttest untersucht. Zu beiden Messzeitpunkten bear-beiteten die Kinder einen von (Plöger & Leuchter, in Vorb.) entwickeltes Verfahren zur Mes-sung der angewandten Strategie. Die Kinder in den beiden Experimentalgruppen erhielten strukturierte Materialien in Form von Bildkarten, anhand derer sie symmetrische und asym-metrische Zusammenstellungen von Bauklötzchen nachbauen sollten. Experimentalgruppe 1 (EG1, n = 55) erhielt nur die strukturierten Materialien, während die Kinder aus der zweiten Experimentalgruppe (EG2, n = 55) zusätzlich verbale Erklärungen über die Stabilität bzw. Instabilität der dargebotenen Bauklotzstrukturen erhielten. Die Kontrollgruppe (n = 57) spielte frei mit Bauklötzchen. Ergebnisse Im Prätest wendeten nur wenige Kinder eine Massemittelpunktstrategie an, um die Stabilität von Bauklotzkonstruktionen zu beurteilen, M = 8.52, SD = 4.18 von 23 Items. Eine moderierte Regression zeigte einen Anstieg des Statikverständnisses im Bereich Massemittel-punkt vom Prä- zum Posttest für die EG2: Material und verbale Unterstützung, β = .76, p < .001. Der Anstieg war zudem höher verglichen mit der EG1: Material, β = –.51, p < .05 und der KG, β = –.57, p < .05. Diskussion Nur wenige Kinder nutzten zunächst eine Massemittelpunktstrategie, was konsistent mit bisheriger Forschung ist (Bonawitz et al., 2012). Die Intervention verbesserte das Statik-verständnis der Kinder nur in der Gruppe mit zusätzlicher verbaler Unterstützung. Ein direktes Erleben des Materials reichte folglich nicht aus, sondern es bedarf zusätzlicher verbaler Scaf-folds.
... However, preschool children face problems with estimating asymmetrical objects' stabilities (Krist, 2010;Weber, Reuter, & Leuchter, 2020), overgeneralizing the geometrical center and ignoring an object's center of mass. Additionally, Krist and colleagues found that the form of presentation is irrelevant, as children between 4 and 8 years showed the same performance when actively balancing symmetrical and asymmetrical objects on a beam scale (Krist, Horz, & Schönfeld, 2005) or when being presented with photographs (Krist, 2010). Children can investigate the underlying reasons for stability and therefore acquire stability knowledge through their everyday play, e.g., when they play with building blocks (Borriello & Liben, 2018;. ...
Article
Full-text available
The study investigated preschool children's block building self-concepts in relation to their stability knowledge acquisition as implied by the reciprocal effects model and possible effects of different forms of play. We investigated three types of construction play: (a) guided play with verbal and material scaffolds, (b) guided play with material scaffolds, and (c) free play. We examined the effects of the different play forms on block building self-concept and stability knowledge acquisition as well as the reciprocal effects model's fit to preschool children. We implemented a pre-post-follow-up design, N = 183 German 5-to 6-year-olds (88 female). Block building self-concept declined in the free play group, but not in the guided play groups. Both guided play groups out-performed the free play group in stability knowledge acquisition. The reciprocal effects model was not supported. Guided play may be effective in fostering children's block building self-concepts and stability knowledge.
... However, Pine, Lufkin, Kirk, and Messer (2007) found that only few preschoolers even consider the weight, much less the distance, of objects on a balance beam. Krist, Horz, and Sch€ onfeld (2005) asked children between ages four and eight to actively balance symmetrical and asymmetrical objects on a beam scale. In another study, Krist (2010) applied a rating task with photographs. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background. Preschoolers’ knowledge of the principle of static equilibrium is an important research focus for understanding children’s science content knowledge. Hitherto studies have mainly used behavioural observation with small samples. Thus, extending these studies with a validated test instrument is desirable. Aims.The aim was to validate an instrument (the Centre-of-Mass Test), which is concerned with preschoolers’ knowledge of the principle of static equilibrium, using item response theory. In Study 1, the construct structure was tested, and in Study 2, its relationship with stabilities of symmetrical blocks, figural reasoning, figural perception, mental rotation, level of interest, self-concept, motivation, and language capacity was investigated. Samples. A total of 217 five- and six-year-old children participated in Study 1 and 166 five- and six-year-old children in Study 2. Methods. All tests were administered as paper–pencil picture tests in groups and single interviews. Results. In Study 1, the Centre-of-Mass Test’s conformity with a 1PL-testlet model with an overall knowledge of static equilibrium and with two subtests, estimation of stable and unstable constructions, was confirmed. Using a 95% binomial distribution, children were categorized into three knowledge categories: geometrical-centre, centre-of-mass, and undifferentiated knowledge. In Study 2, knowledge of the principle of static equilibrium showed positive correlations with figural perception and reasoning, language capacity, and estimation of the stabilities of symmetrical objects. Conclusions.The Centre-of-Mass Test measures knowledge of the principle of static equilibrium as a unidimensional construct and mirrors preschoolers’ estimations found in previous studies. The acquisition of a more sophisticated static equilibrium knowledge is related to spatial knowledge and language capacity.
... • Better performance in estimation of symmetrical compared to asymmetrical objects' stability • When balancing objects themselves (Krist, Horz, & Schönfeld, 2005) • When rating pictures (Krist, 2010) • With an eye tracking paradigm (Krist, Atlas, Fischer, & Wiese, 2018) Theoretical Background 6 State of research • Bonawitz, van Schijndel, Friel, & Schulz (2012) • N = 95, age = 6-7 ...
Conference Paper
Playing evokes positive emotions and intrinsic motivation and contains elements of choice for children (Einsiedler, 1999). In play, adults’ intervention may reduce children’s motivation (Bonawitz et al., 2011), however without intervention they may have little learning gain (Stipek, Feiler, Daniels, & Milburn, 1995). We investigate if different ways of intervention interact with motivation and learning gains in the domain of statics. 166 preschool children took part in a pre-post-follow-up-study. Experimental group 1 received an intervention with structured materials for guided block play with verbal scaffolds, experimental group 2 without verbal scaffolds and a control group played with blocks freely. Growth curve analysis showed no difference in development of motivation for the groups. Moreover, we revealed learning gains in statics knowledge for the scaffolding compared to the materials-only group. However, considering motivation no difference between the groups could be uncovered. Adult’s intervention through verbal scaffolding did not decrease children’s motivation for learning in the domain of statics. Furthermore, guided play with verbal scaffolding helped to increase children’s knowledge compared to a materials-only group. However, motivated children had learning gains in all groups.
Chapter
Full-text available
This essay brings together two lines of work—that of children’s cognition and that of complexity science. These two lines of work have been linked repeatedly in the past, including in the field of science education. Nevertheless, questions remain about how complexity constructs can be used to support children’s learning. This uncertainty is particularly troublesome given the ongoing controversy about how to promote children’s understanding of scientifically valid insights. We therefore seek to specify the knowledge–complexity link systematically. Our approach started with a preliminary step—namely, to consider issues of knowledge formation separately from issues of complexity. To this end, we defined central characteristics of knowledge formation (without considerations of complexity), and we defined central characteristics of complex systems (without considerations of cognition). This preliminary step allowed us to systematically explore the degree of alignment between these two lists of characteristics. The outcome of this analysis revealed a close correspondence between knowledge truisms and complexity constructs, though to various degrees. Equipped with this insight, we derive complexity answers to open questions relevant to science learning.
Article
Full-text available
Theory. Young children have an understanding of basic science concepts such as stability, yet their theoretical assumptions are often not concerned with stability. The literature on theory theory and theory-evidence coordination suggests that children construct intuitive theories about their environment that can be adjusted in the face of counterevidence that cannot be assimilated into the prior theory. With increasing age, children acquire a Center theory when balancing objects and try to balance every object at their middle, succeeding with symmetrical objects. Later, they acquire the basic science concept of stability through learning that the weight distribution of an object is of importance. Thus, they acquire a Mass theory and succeed in balancing asymmetrical objects as well. Fluid and crystallized intelligence might contribute to children’s acquisition of Mass theory. Moreover, their Mass theory might be supported by implementing a playful intervention including (a) material scaffolds and (b) verbal scaffolds. Aims. We investigated which theories children have about stability and whether these theories can be adjusted to Mass theory by implementing a playful intervention. Method. A total of 183 5- to 6-year-old children took part in the study with a pre-post-follow-up intervention design. Children’s Mass theory was assessed with an interview in which children explained constructions’ stabilities. The children received a playful intervention with two differing degrees of scaffolding (material scaffolds or material + verbal scaffolds) or no scaffolding. Results. At first few children used a Mass theory to explain their reasoning. However, after being confronted with counterevidence for the asymmetrical constructions, children changed their explanation and applied a Mass theory. More children in the play group with the highest degree of scaffolding, i.e., material + verbal scaffolds, acquired a Mass theory compared to the other groups. Fluid as well as crystallized intelligence contributed to children’s acquisition of a Mass theory. Discussion. Counterevidence can support children in their acquisition of a Mass theory. A playful intervention with scaffolding supports children even more.
Article
Full-text available
Children and adults often hold naive intuitive theories about how the physical world around them works, and their misconceptions can be difficult to change. Self-explanations have been found to be effective in producing better understanding of science (Chi, de Leeuw, Chiu, & LaVancher, 1994), and explaining another person's reasoning can also bring about cognitive change (Siegler, 1995). This study deals with one domain of physics-balance-and investigates the effects of 2 interventions on children who had either a procedure for balancing but could not explain it or had a naive theory. We pretested 140 children, ages 5 to 9 years, to assess their ability on a balance beam task and their knowledge about the principles of balance. These children were classified according to levels of representation derived from Karmiloff-Smith's (1992) Representational Redescription model. In this sample, 104 children could not explain the principles of balance or possessed a naive theory that all things had to balance in the center. These children were allocated to 1 of 2 intervention conditions. Approximately half of the children watched the experimenter model the correct solution to the balance task; the rest observed the model and were also encouraged to produce verbal explanations of what they saw. At posttest, a significantly higher number of children from the latter condition had improved their understanding of balance. The positive effects of interpersonal explanation are discussed in relation to Karmiloff-Smith's model of children's development, and the implications for teaching are highlighted.
Article
Full-text available
Disusses the articles in this special issue which raise three main questions: Why are U-shaped curves of interest? What do U-shaped curves tell us about the processes that produce cognitive development? And, what does the resurgence of interest in U-shaped curves tell us about where the field of cognitive development is going? (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Contrasts intuitive knowledge about projectile motion expressed in action with knowledge expressed in explicit judgments. In the action condition of Exp 1, children and adults threw a ball horizontally from different heights to hit targets on the floor; in the judgment condition the same Ss rated the respective launch speeds required. All age groups appropriately varied the launch speed with respect to both height of release and target distance in the action condition. In the judgment condition, however, kindergartners failed to integrate the relevant dimensions and even 4th graders and adults showed misconceptions of the speed–height relation. Exp 2 established that the speed gradations in the action condition did not critically depend on visual flight feedback or the availability of outcome information. It is concluded that perceptual-motor knowledge about projectile motion is distinct from naive, verbal-cognitive concepts of projectile motion and follows different developmental courses. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
The traditional view of development is stage-like progress toward increasing complexity of form. However, the literature cites many examples in which children do worse before they do better. A major challenge for developmental theory, therefore, is to explain both global progress and apparent regression. In this article, we situate U-shaped development as a special case of the nonlinearity that is characteristic of all developmental process. We use dynamic systems theory to show how behavioral regression can be understood as part of the ordinary mechanisms of change. Examples from our work in infant motor and language development illustrate the ways that U-shaped behavior arises from continuous changes in the collective dynamics of multiple, contingent processes. A central claim is that true regression is not possible because behavior exists continuously in time. Thus the current state of the system always depends on its past history. Instead, the appearance of regression reflects the concept of softly assembled behavior, or the ability of contributing components to self-organize in different configurations that depend upon the status of the components, the environment, and the task.
Article
Entering the Child's Mind teaches a powerful technique for gaining insight into a child's way of thinking. In the tradition of Piaget and Vygotsky, Dr Herbert P. Ginsburg argues that standardized instruments of evaluation often fail to meet the challenges of complex cognition. He presents the 'clinical interview' as a powerful tool that, when understood and used properly, can provide psychologists with a profound appreciation for children's thinking. Dr Ginsburg aims to convince the reader that the method has important applications in research, clinical practice, and education. The clinical interview offers a degree of flexibility that allows psychologists to more closely assess the thinking of a particular child.
Book
Rethinking Innateness asks the question, "What does it really mean to say that a behavior is innate?" The authors describe a new framework in which interactions, occurring at all levels, give rise to emergent forms and behaviors. These outcomes often may be highly constrained and universal, yet are not themselves directly contained in the genes in any domain-specific way. One of the key contributions of Rethinking Innateness is a taxonomy of ways in which a behavior can be innate. These include constraints at the level of representation, architecture, and timing; typically, behaviors arise through the interaction of constraints at several of these levels.The ideas are explored through dynamic models inspired by a new kind of "developmental connectionism," a marriage of connectionist models and developmental neurobiology, forming a new theoretical framework for the study of behavioral development. While relying heavily on the conceptual and computational tools provided by connectionism, Rethinking Innateness also identifies ways in which these tools need to be enriched by closer attention to biology. Bradford Books imprint
Article
The 2 studies that are reported in this paper challenge traditional notions about task success and failure by locating children's performance on a balance task within a Representational Redescription model proposed by A. Karmiloff-Smith (1992). The 1st study seeks to validate the levels of explicitness outline in the model by assessing the representations of 168 children, aged 4–9 yrs, on a balance beam task. It was found that children's success can be either implicit (without any conceptual understanding) or explicit (accompanied by understanding) and that these 2 representational levels are markedly different, even though both involve success with the task. The 2nd study, involving 58 children aged 6 and 7 yrs, used a similar procedure for the balance beam task used in the 1st study. Results show that the ability to explain successful performance on the task was not related to the language age of the Ss as measured by the British Picture Vocabulary Scale. The study also confirmed that children can have representations of the task which differ in explicitness. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Previous work on the development of intuitive knowledge about trajectories has shown a dissociation between young children's aimed throwing actions, in which they are highly sensitive to the physical laws of motion, and their explicit judgments, in which they exhibit misconceptions. This research investigated the generality of children's action knowledge by having the participants project a ball with a sling. In-stead of adjusting sling stretch, and hence sling force, correctly as a function of both the release height and the target distance, 5-and 6-year-olds (Experiment 1, N = 32) and, without flight feedback, even most 7-to 10-year-olds (Experiment 2, N = 96) considered distance only and ignored the height dimension. Similarly, children's judgments of the required sling stretch tended to follow a distance-only rule, espe-cially with children younger than 9 years of age. These results show that young chil-dren's action knowledge exhibited in throwing does not generalize to arbitrary means of force production. They further suggest that there is no developmental trend toward such generalization in the age range examined. Is there such a thing as action knowledge? Most people would probably think there is. But if so, how is it represented or implemented internally, how is it accessed, how does it develop, and what is its relation to cognitive development? These and related questions can be addressed only if one has some idea of what action knowledge is. Is action knowledge the same as "knowing how" (Ryle, 1949), sensorimotor intelli-gence (Piaget, 1952), or procedural knowledge (e.g., Squire, 1987)? Intuitively, these and similar terms share the same core meaning, but this core meaning and how it relates to the distinction between implicit and explicit knowledge has yet to be es-tablished precisely (e.g.
Article
The proposal that there is a simple dichotomy between implicit and explicit knowledge fails to fully explain why children and adults can perform a task with increasing awareness and efficiency before they are able to completely understand and explain their success. Karmiloff-Smith (1992) in her Representational Redescription (RR) model claims that representations of implicit procedures can be redescribed into further levels, E1, E2 and E3, which gradually afford increasing conscious and linguistic access. This study examines how 25 children's representations about balancing a beam on a fulcrum changed over time, as they attempted a set of tasks each day for five consecutive days. Children's behaviour was classified into seven levels, rather than the four identified by Karmiloff-Smith, and the path through these levels is traced as children progressed over the 5-day period. This provided one form of validation about the developmental sequence of the levels. Additional tasks assessed the flexibility of the children's representations, operationalized either as an ability to predict which beams would balance, or to identify which picture showed a beam that would balance on a fulcrum. The findings from these assessments shed light on the conscious accessibility and transportability of the children's knowledge, independent of their overt behaviour. The data also reveal a complexity in cognitive functioning which could not be identified from observations of performance alone. This research thus provides a theoretical endorsement and extension of Karmiloff-Smith's multi-representational framework of cognitive development and empirical support for a hypothesized path of development through the representational levels.