ArticlePDF Available

Values in action scale and the big 5: An empirical indication of structure. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(4), 787-799

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Within this study we used self-report measures completed by 123 undergraduate students from an Australian university to investigate the validity of Peterson and Seligman’s [Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P (2004). Character strengths and virtues. New York: Oxford.] classification system of 24 character strengths and six virtues. We also looked at how the 24 character strengths relate to the Five Factor Model of personality and to a measure of social desirability. Using a second order factor analysis of the 24 character strengths, we found that these 24 character strengths did not produce a factor structure consistent with the six higher order virtues as proposed by Peterson and Seligman [Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P (2004). Character strengths and virtues. New York: Oxford.]. Instead, the 24 character strengths were well represented by both a one and four factor solution. Patterns of significant relationships between each of the 24 character strengths, the one and four factor solutions and the Five Factor Model of personality were found. The results have implications for [Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P (2004). Character strengths and virtues. New York: Oxford.] classification.
Content may be subject to copyright.
This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached
copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research
and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution
and sharing with colleagues.
Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or
licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party
websites are prohibited.
In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the
article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or
institutional repository. Authors requiring further information
regarding Elsevier’s archiving and manuscript policies are
encouraged to visit:
http://www.elsevier.com/copyright
Author's personal copy
Values in action scale and the Big 5:
An empirical indication of structure
Craig Macdonald, Miles Bore
*
, Don Munro
School of Psychology, University of Newcastle, Callaghan, NSW 2308, Australia
Available online 4 December 2007
Abstract
Within this study we used self-report measures completed by 123 undergraduate students from an Australian university
to investigate the validity of Peterson and Seligman’s [Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P (2004). Character strengths and
virtues. New York: Oxford.] classification system of 24 character strengths and six virtues. We also looked at how the 24
character strengths relate to the Five Factor Model of personality and to a measure of social desirability. Using a second
order factor analysis of the 24 character strengths, we found that these 24 character strengths did not produce a factor
structure consistent with the six higher order virtues as proposed by Peterson and Seligman [Peterson, C., & Seligman,
M. E. P (2004). Character strengths and virtues. New York: Oxford.]. Instead, the 24 character strengths were well repre-
sented by both a one and four factor solution. Patterns of significant relationships between each of the 24 character
strengths, the one and four factor solutions and the Five Factor Model of personality were found. The results have impli-
cations for [Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P (2004). Character strengths and virtues. New York: Oxford.] classification.
Crown copyright !2007 Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Values; VIA scale; Big 5; Five Factor Model
1. Introduction
The field of positive psychology has the goal of helping people achieve an above normal or optimal level
of functioning, leading to a happier existence (Gable & Haidt, 2005; Wallis, 2005). Wallis (2005) suggests
that this is because much of psychological practice and theory has focused on helping people to recover
from a diminished level of functioning, and has largely neglected helping people achieve a higher level
of functioning.
Two of the main proponents of positive psychology are Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi (2000), who see
positive psychology encompassing subjective experience, individual traits, and societal interactions. With
regard to the area of individual dierences, Peterson and Seligman (2004) have developed a hierarchy of posi-
tive psychological character strengths. The hierarchy consists of 24 specific character strengths that are seen as
0092-6566/$ - see front matter Crown copyright !2007 Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2007.10.003
*
Corresponding author. Fax: +61 2 49216906.
E-mail address: Miles.Bore@newcastle.edu.au (M. Bore).
Available online at www.sciencedirect.com
Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 787–799
www.elsevier.com/locate/jrp
Author's personal copy
the psychological ingredients that make up six ‘‘virtues. These virtues are situated at a higher level of abstrac-
tion than character strengths, and are likened to constructs proposed by philosophers and religious figures
over many centuries. These six virtues and their associated character strengths are displayed in Table 1.
Peterson and Seligman (2004) analysed dierent religious, cultural and legal texts from around the world in
an attempt to achieve a universal classification for character strengths, and only included character strengths
and virtues that were found to be ubiquitous (Dahlsgaard, Peterson, & Seligman, 2005).
To measure and assess the 24 character strengths, Peterson and Seligman (2004) also developed the Virtues
In Action Scale (VIA). The VIA is a self-assessment measure of character strength requiring respondents to
rate how likely they are to participate in certain behaviours that are representative of the dierent character
strengths. It is important to note that the scale does not directly measure the six virtues they describe; these are
only linked conceptually to the character strengths by Peterson and Seligman (2004).
In addition to developing their classification system, Peterson and Seligman (2004) have also suggested how
their classification of character strengths and virtues is related to, but distinct from, already established the-
ories of values. For example, Peterson and Seligman (2004) see their classification of character strengths and
virtues as being related to Maslow’s (1973) idea of self-actualised individuals, the Five Factor Model (FFM) of
personality (McCrae & John, 1992; Costa & McCrae, 1994), Cawley’s virtue factors (Cawley, Martin, & John-
son, 2000), Buss’ evolutionary ideas about what is attractive in a mate [i.e. what character traits are essential
for survival and propagation, (Botwin, Buss, & Shackelford, 1997; Shackelford, Schmitt, & Buss, 2005)], and
Schwartz’s (1992) Universal Values.
Some research into establishing the validity of these claims has begun. Haslam, Bain, and Neal (2004)
found that both Schwartz’s (1992) Universal Values and the Five Factor Model (FFM) of personality were
conceptually linked to the 24 character strengths. However, as these constructs were defined and subsequently
measured by only one or two terms that were ranked and grouped together by participants on the basis of
conceptual likeness, more thorough research is needed before we can draw any firm conclusions.
Peterson and Seligman (2004) acknowledge that there are some clear correspondences between their
classification and the FFM. For example, Neuroticism could be seen as the conceptual opposite of Hope,
Table 1
Peterson and Seligman’s (2004) classification of character strengths and virtues
Virtue Character strengths
Wisdom and Knowledge Creativity
Curiosity
Open-mindedness
Love of Learning
Perspective
Courage Bravery
Persistence
Integrity
Vitality
Humanity Love
Kindness
Social Intelligence
Justice Citizenship
Fairness
Leadership
Temperance Forgiveness and Mercy
Humility/Modesty
Prudence
Self-control
Transcendence Appreciation of Beauty
Gratitude
Hope
Humour
Spirituality
Adapted from Table 1.1 in Peterson and Seligman (2004), pp 29–30.
788 C. Macdonald et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 787–799
Author's personal copy
and Extroversion could be a key to Leadership (Peterson & Seligman, 2004). They also show how their clas-
sification system as a whole corresponds to the FFM, by conceptually equating a factor analysis of their 24
character strengths to the five factors, although the FFM does not account for all of their classification
(see Table 2).
It is important to note that Peterson and Seligman (2004) did not empirically correlate their value strength
factors with the five factors of the FFM but only make these links conceptually. They also acknowledge that
the five factors found in their factor analysis of the VIA do not exactly reflect their hypothesized hierarchical
classification of the six virtues. Moreover, it is also interesting to note that only 19 of the 24 character strengths
are reported, raising the question of where the other five would load. This ambiguity brings into doubt both
the hierarchical link between the 24 character strengths and six virtues and the conceptual links between the
FFM and the character strengths. The proposed relationships are further brought into doubt when one
reviews other research into character strengths and the FFM. This research suggests that some of the character
strengths are related to combinations of FFM traits and not individual traits. For instance, creative people
have been shown to be high in Openness (O) and low in Agreeableness [A (King, Walker, & Broyles,
1996)]; honest and humble people have been found to be high in Agreeableness (Ashton & Lee, 2005) and also
high in Conscientiousness [C (Paunonen, 2003)]; Brose, Rye, Lutz-Zois, and Ross (2005) found forgiveness to
be both negatively correlated with Neuroticism (N) and positively correlated with Agreeableness and some-
times Extroversion (E). Also, a meta-analysis of a number of dierent studies by Suroglou (2000) found that
religiosity was related to high A, C and (to some extent) E.
Although most of the theoretical correlates predicted by Peterson and Seligman (2004) are reflected in this
research, there are often multiple predictors present as shown in the studies above. Our research pursues this
idea by investigating which combinations of FFM traits, rather than a single trait, are best related to each of
the 24 character strengths found within Peterson and Seligman’s (2004) classification.
In consideration of the factor analysis carried out by Peterson and Seligman (2004) it is important to reit-
erate that their results did not support their theory of particular combinations of character strengths as being
represented as the higher order virtues. Consequently, this issue is also examined, with the expectation that a
factor analysis of the 24 character strengths will not produce the six virtues proposed by Peterson and Selig-
man (2004).
A further area of interest we considered was whether the VIA is eected by social desirability. Peterson and
Seligman (2004) state that the 24 character strengths are socially desirable constructs themselves and as a
result the VIA should not be aected by social desirability. We take this to mean that the VIA will not be
aected by individual dierences in socially desirable responding. Although this may be true, the opposing
argument could also be made: as the 24 character strengths are socially desirable constructs, the VIA will
be highly aected by social desirability.
To summarise, the aim of this study is to further the understanding of Peterson and Seligman’s (2004) clas-
sification of 24 character strengths by examining the relationships between the character strengths themselves
and their relationship to the Five Factor Model of personality. To achieve this, the 24 character strengths are
Table 2
Factors found in the virtues in action scale and their correlates
Character strengths contained within the factor Name given to
factor
Reflected virtue Theoretical FFM correlate
Fairness, Humility, Mercy, Prudence Strengths of
restraint
Temperance Conscientiousness
Creativity, Curiosity, Love of Learning,
Appreciation of Beauty
Intellectual
strengths
Wisdom and
Knowledge
Openness
Kindness, Love, Leadership, Teamwork,
Playfulness
Interpersonal
strengths
Humanity and
Justice
Agreeableness
Bravery, Hope, Self-control, Zest Emotional
strengths
Courage Opposite of Neuroticism (Emotional
Stability)
Gratitude, Spirituality Theological
strengths
Transcendence No FFM correlate
Constructed from information found in Peterson and Seligman (2004), pp. 632–633.
C. Macdonald et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 787–799 789
Author's personal copy
first factor analysed with the expectation that the extracted factors will not neatly represent the six virtues pro-
posed by Peterson and Seligman (2004). The 24 character strengths will then be compared to the FFM in order
to investigate the relationships between them. It is hypothesized that there will not be a one-to-one relation-
ship between for the majority of the character strengths and the FFM personality traits. Rather it is expected
that most of the character strengths will show relationships to more than one of the FFM constructs. Follow-
ing Peterson and Seligman’s (2004) view, we expect that the VIA will not be influenced by socially desirable
responding.
2. Method
2.1. Participants
The participants for this study were 123 first year psychology students enrolled in an Australian university.
There were 28 males and 86 females, with 9 participants not indicating their gender. Ages ranged from 18 to 57
years, with a mean of 21.51 years and a standard deviation of 6.57 years. All participants volunteered to take
part in the study and were given course credit for doing so.
2.2. Materials
The materials used were question booklets containing a battery of ten psychometric tests and answer book-
lets. Four versions of the question booklets were used with the questionnaires presented in dierent orders in
each version to account for any possible order eects. The psychometric tests not relevant to the current
research will be reported elsewhere.
To measure the 24 character strengths, Peterson and Seligman’s (2004) Virtues In Action Scale (VIA) was
used. This test uses a five-point Likert scale (A = very much like me, B = like me, C = neutral, D = unlike me,
E = very much unlike me) to measure how frequently one perceives oneself as exhibiting certain behaviours.
These behaviours are representative of the 24 individual character strengths. The actual VIA scale used was a
213-item edition obtained from the IPIP web site (International Personality Item Pool, 2001). Items were com-
piled in the test booklet pseudo-randomly to ensure that items representing individual character strengths and
positively and negatively worded items were distributed evenly throughout the questionnaire. Using data
obtained from over 150,000 adult respondents completing the VIA over the Internet, the measure was found
to have acceptable internal reliability (all alphas >.7) and temporal reliability (test/retest >.7) (Peterson &
Seligman, 2004).
The measure of the Five Factor Model of Personality was Goldberg’s (1999) Big Five scale obtained from
the IPIP web site (International Personality Item Pool, 2001). This measure has 20 self-report items per scale
and participants respond using a four-point Likert scale (F = definitely false, f = false on the whole, t = true
on the whole, T = definitely true). This instrument has been found to be both a reliable and valid measure of
the FFM (see Goldberg, 1999; Goldberg et al., 2006).
The 20 item Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability scale (Strahan & Gerbasi, 1972) was also included in the
battery in order to examine and potentially account for social desirability eects. Strahan and Gerbasi
reported internal reliabilities ranging from.73 to.83 for the 20 item short-form of the scale. The scale uses a
true/false response option for each question. The scale is balance with half the items being reverse scored.
2.3. Procedure
Testing was done in multiple testing sessions. Upon entering the testing room the participants were ran-
domly given one of the four dierently ordered questionnaire booklets and its accompanying answer booklet.
Participants sat where they chose and were instructed to complete all the tests presented the question booklet
in the order they appeared. They were also told that all information recorded was confidential. All tests were
answered in a separate answer booklet using either a pen or pencil. Both booklets were collected and partic-
ipants were allowed to leave when they had finished. The time taken to complete the entire battery was
between 90 and 120 min.
790 C. Macdonald et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 787–799
Author's personal copy
3. Results
Each questionnaire was first assessed for missing data; 18 item non-responses from the VIA scale and 17
from the FFM scale were replaced with the middle response of their corresponding Likert scale. Two VIA
respondents and one FFM scale respondent were removed for not responding correctly. Each measure was
scored and descriptive statistics and histograms were generated. These were then inspected for normality
and were all found to be within acceptable limits. T-tests were calculated between mean scores for each
FFM scale and norms derived by the second author (MB) from data collected for teaching purposes from
undergraduate psychology students (n= 565). No significant dierences were found (p> .05). VIA norms
have not been published therefore no comparison was possible. In addition, t-tests were calculated to assess
dierences in age and gender; and all tests conducted were non-significant. Order eects possibly resulting
from the dierent presentation order of the tests were also found to be non-significant.
Internal reliabilities for all the measures were then assessed via the generation of Cronbach Alpha coe-
cients. These were compared to alpha coecients reported in the literature for each of the individual measures
(see Table 3).
The alpha coecient for all 213 VIA scale items taken together (reverse scored where appropriate) was .96.
This showed that the behaviours related to the character strengths were responded to in a highly consistent
Table 3
Alpha coecients for the VIA scale, the IPIP FFM scale, and the social desirability scale compared to the expected alpha coecients
obtained from published findings
Character strength Our sample Previously reported No. items
Appreciation of Beauty .70 .77 8
Capacity for Love .68 .70 9
Citizenship/Teamwork .62 .78 9
Curiosity .83 .78 10
Equity/Fairness .80 .70 9
Forgiveness/Mercy .85 .76 9
Gratitude .76 .79 8
Hope/Optimism .58 .73 8
Humor/Playfulness .86 .84 9
Industry/Perseverance/Persistence .83 .81 8
Integrity/Honesty/Authenticity .56 .72 9
Judgment/Open-mindedness .80 .80 9
Kindness/Generosity .75 .72 10
Leadership .43 .77 7
Love of Learning .74 .77 10
Modesty/Humility .68 .70 9
Originality/Creativity .83 .85 8
Perspective/Wisdom .76 .75 9
Prudence .57 .73 9
Self-regulation/Self-control .68 .75 11
Social/Personal/Emotional Intel .75 .76 7
Spirituality/Religiousness .89 .91 7
Valor/Bravery/Courage .61 .75 10
Zest/Enthusiasm/Vitality .80 .78 9
Total character strength .96 n.a 213
IPIP Big 5
Agreeableness .88 .91 20
Conscientiousness .90 .88 20
Extroversion .92 .88 20
Neuroticism .91 .91 20
Openness .87 .90 20
Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability .74 .73 to .83 20
Notes: For published alpha reliabilities see: VIA, Peterson and Seligman (2004); IPIP Big 5, Goldberg et al. (2006); Marlowe-Crowne
Social Desirability, Strahan and Gerbasi (1972).
C. Macdonald et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 787–799 791
Author's personal copy
fashion regardless of the separate scales with which they are theoretically associated. This overall measure was
included in subsequent analyses as a variable called Total Character Strength.
A second order factor analysis of the 24 character strengths derived from the VIA scale was then carried out
using a Principle Components analysis with a Varimax rotation. An initial extraction revealed five compo-
nents with an eigenvalue >1, however, the scree plot (shown in Fig. 1) indicated that three components might
also yield a simple solution.
The loadings of the character strengths in the five factor solution were not easily interpretable and many
high cross-loadings between the factors were evident. This was also observed when using an oblique (Direct
Oblimin) rotation. A four factor solution was then generated (see Table 4) and found to be more interpretable.
The four factors were tentatively labeled Positivity, Intellect, Conscientiousness and Niceness. The only nota-
ble reduction in communality between the five and four factor solutions was with the variable of Spirituality/
Religiosity. This four factor structure also had the clearest pattern of correlations with the FFM constructs
(see Table 4).
Social Desirability scores were correlated with each of the VIA character strength scores and the five factor
scores (also shown in Table 4). The correlations indicated that the factors were dierentially related to Social
Desirability. Factors 2 and 3 (Intellect and Conscientiousness) were not significantly related to Social Desir-
ability scores, however, Factor 1 (Positivity) was weakly and positively related to Social Desirability and Fac-
tor 3 (Niceness) moderately positively related. The correlations between Social Desirability and each of the 24
character strengths reflect this pattern, with the six character strengths that loaded on Factor 4 (Niceness) all
correlating significantly with Social Desirability.
The two and three factor solutions were also run but were found to produce pronounced high cross-load-
ings and disjointed relationships to the FFM scales. Therefore these solutions were not considered further. A
single component solution was produced on which all the character strengths except Modesty and Prudence
loaded strongly (Table 5). However, eight variables showed very low communalities (<.3) and are therefore
not well represented by this solution.
Given the significant correlations between Social Desirability and several of the VIA character strength
scores, the data was reanalysed after ‘centering’ the 24 character strength scores for each participant. This
was done by dividing each character strength score by the number of items that made up each score, calcu-
lating the mean score for each participant across all 24 character strengths and then subtracting this mean
from each participant’s score for each character strength. The eect of centering is to remove the influence
of mean response set dierences between participants (but not individual variability); in this case, individual
dierences in the tendency to agree more strongly with socially desirable items.
Fig. 1. Scree plot for the 24 character strength raw scores.
792 C. Macdonald et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 787–799
Author's personal copy
A second order Principle Components factor analysis of the centered scores of the 24 character strengths
derived from the VIA scale was conducted. The initial extraction revealed nine factors with an eigenvalue >1
(see Fig. 2). Several solutions were examined, but none were clearly interpretable. Given the elbow in the Scree
Plot at factor 6 a five factor solution is presented here as an example (see Table 6). We could arrive at no
meaningful interpretation of this factor structure, particularly with regard to some of the negative loadings
observed. The correlations observed between social desirability and each of the centred character strength
scores appeared to be systematically reduced compared to the same correlations with character strength
raw scores reported above. Thus, the technique of centering reduced but did not eliminate the influence of
social desirability.
Stepwise regression analyses were conducted with the FFM traits and social desirability as predictor vari-
ables and the raw and the centralised character strength scores as criterion variables. After adjustment to
account for family-wise error a working significance level of p6.01 was used. Only values for the constructs
that had a pvalue 601 are presented in the results of this analysis (see Table 7).
Table 4
Varimax rotated four factor solution of 24 character strength scores and correlations with social desirability and Big 5 scores
Variable Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 Communality SocDes correlation
Positivity
Citizenship/Teamwork .78 .64 .13
Capacity for Love .74 .58 .19
*
Hope/Optimism .73 .68 .26
**
Humor/Playfulness .63 !.43 .60 .18
Zest/Enthusiasm/Vitality .62 !.40 !.48 .77 .28
**
Leadership .50 !.44 .38 .58 .15
Intellect
Originality/Creativity !.78 .69 .02
Appreciation of Beauty !.72 .55 .15
Curiosity .49 !.61 .69 .32
**
Love of Learning !.60 !.43 .61 .15
Social/Personal/Emotional Intel .55 !.58 .67 .23
*
Perspective/Wisdom .35 !.53 !.47 .65 .12
Valor/Bravery/Courage .42 !.44 .46 .22
*
Conscientiousness
Self-regulation/Self-control !.75 .62 .12
Industry/Perseverance/Persist !.73 .66 .17
Judgment/Open-mindedness !.59 .34 .58 !.05
Integrity/Honesty/Authenticity !.58 .39 .57 .33
***
Prudence !.53 .50 .60 .16
Niceness
Modesty/Humility .75 .72 .28
**
Equity/Fairness .36 !.31 .64 .68 .53
***
Kindness/Generosity .35 .64 .65 .45
***
Forgiveness/Mercy .35 .62 .53 .55
***
Spirituality/Religiousness .50 .34 .25
**
Gratitude .42 !.41 .45 .59 .29
**
Proportion of Variance .19 .16 .13 .13 .61
Factor correlations
Social Desirability .24
**
!.04 !.06 .46
***
Agreeableness .20
*
!.08 .14 .57
***
.60
***
Conscientiousness .12 .14 .71
***
.15 .18
*
Extraversion .71
***
.26
**
!.06 !.10 .20
*
Neuroticism !.50
***
!.17 !.12 !.30 !.27
**
Openness .03 .68
***
.11 .04 .16
Notes: Loadings <.3 not shown. SocDes = Social Desirability.
For correlations: "p< .05; ""p< .01; """p< .001.
C. Macdonald et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 787–799 793
Author's personal copy
Table 5
One factor solution of 24 character strength scores and correlations with social desirability and Big 5 scores
Character strength Factor 1 Communality
Zest/Enthusiasm/Vitality .81 .65
Curiosity .79 .63
Perspective/Wisdom .76 .57
Social/Personal/Emotional Intelligence .75 .56
Gratitude .74 .55
Hope/Optimism .74 .54
Equity/Fairness .72 .52
Kindness/Generosity .70 .49
Humor/Playfulness .68 .46
Leadership .68 .46
Industry/Perseverance/Persistence .63 .39
Integrity/Honesty/Authenticity .63 .39
Valor/Bravery/Courage .63 .39
Capacity for Love .57 .32
Citizenship/Teamwork .57 .33
Originality/Creativity .56 .31
Love of Learning .54 .29
Forgiveness/Mercy .53 .28
Appreciation of Beauty .52 .27
Self-regulation/Self-control .49 .24
Judgment/Open-mindedness .45 .20
Spirituality/Religiousness .39 .16
Prudence .24 .06
Modesty/Humility .19 .04
Proportion of Variance .38 .38
Factor correlations
Social Desirability .37
***
Agreeableness .37
***
Conscientiousness .52
***
Extroversion .51
***
Neuroticism !.45
***
Openness .45
***
Notes: For correlations "p< .05; ""p< .01; """p< .001.
Fig. 2. Scree plot for the 24 character strengths centred scores.
794 C. Macdonald et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 787–799
Author's personal copy
The results of this analysis found that no consistent pattern of predictors was evident. For example, in the
raw score analysis, Neuroticism was only predictive of Hope/Optimism and Valour/Bravery/Courage while
Spirituality/Religiosity was only weakly predicted by Agreeableness. Social Desirability was found to be a sig-
nificant predictor of just 3 of the 24 character traits. Conducting the stepwise regression analysis on the char-
acter strength centred scores did produce somewhat dierent predictor patterns but no consistency in the
pattern of predictors was apparent.
4. Discussion
As hypothesised, the results of a second order Principal Components factor analysis of the 24 character
strengths were not consistent with Peterson and Seligman’s (2004) theory of how they relate to the six virtues.
For their theory to have been supported the 24 character strengths needed to have produced a clean six com-
ponent solution, whereas only five components with eigenvalues greater than one were found. At first glance
the five component solution appears to be similar to the five factor solution obtained by Peterson and Selig-
man (2004), but it is apparent on closer inspection that they are made up of dierent clusters of character
Table 6
Varimax rotated four component solution of centred scores and correlations with social desirability and Big 5 scores
Variable Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 Factor 5 Communality SocDes correlation
Curiosity .75 .65 .12
Prudence !.70 !.36 .64 .09
Zest/Enthusiasm/Vitality .67 .31 !.32 .67 .08
Modesty/Humility !.59 !.47 !.30 .66 .03
Judgment/Open-mindedness !.59 .32 .54 !.32
***
Hope/Optimism .55 !.41 .50 .01
Forgiveness/Mercy !.72 .54 .44
***
Equity/Fairness !.72 .54 .43
***
Kindness/Generosity !.55 .38 .27
**
Perspective/Wisdom .47 .30 !.20
*
Social/Personal/Emotional Intel .62 .47 !.04
Leadership .58 !.31 .53 !.21
*
Self-regulation/Self-control !.57 .46 !.13
Industry/Perseverance/Persist .43 !.56 .51 !.04
Humor/Playfulness .51 !.30 .48 !.04
Originality/Creativity .36 .44 .35 !.40 .67 !.21
*
Citizenship/Teamwork !.75 .59 !.17
Love of Learning .66 .47 !.10
Capacity for Love !.63 .43 !.06
Appreciation of Beauty .30 .39 .34 !.06
Gratitude .70 .55 .09
Spirituality/Religiousness .69 .50 .12
Integrity/Honesty/Authenticity !.34 !.42 .42 .05
Valor/Bravery/Courage !.38 .21 !.07
Proportion Of Variance .12 .11 .10 .10 .07 .50
Factor Correlations
Social Desirability .25
**
!.49
***
!.09 .06 .14
Agreeableness .16 !.57
***
!.20
*
.04 .11
Conscientiousness .10 .18
*
!.49
***
.17 .06
Extroversion .49
***
.14 .28
**
!.33
***
.07
Neuroticism !.49
***
.04 .04 .13 .07
Openness .31
***
.09 .24
**
.48
***
!.10
Notes: Loadings <.3 not shown. SocDes = Social Desirability.
For correlations: "p< .05; ""p< .01; """p< .001.
C. Macdonald et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 787–799 795
Author's personal copy
strengths (compare Table 2 with Table 4). Furthermore, none of the clusters of character strengths contained
within the components were completely representative of the clusters needed to make up any of the six virtues,
thus providing additional evidence against Peterson and Seligman’s (2004) theoretical relationships.
Many of the VIA character strengths loaded strongly on multiple components making interpretability ques-
tionable. Similar problems of cross-loading may have been the reason that Peterson and Seligman (2004) orig-
inally left out five of the character strengths when reporting the results of their factor analysis. Even so, they
must have found cross-loadings dierent to those contained in our component solution, because many of the
character strengths remaining in Peterson and Seligman’s (2004) solution showed cross-loadings in ours.
In an attempt to find a solution that was representative of independent higher order constructs, two, three
and four component solutions of the 24 character strengths were produced. Each of these solutions also had
multiple character strengths cross-loading on dierent factors. The two and three factor solutions were unin-
terpretable, whereas the four-factor solution made conceptual sense and is discussed below. An oblique rota-
tion was also carried out to allow the factors to correlate more strongly with each other, but the results did not
dramatically enhance the interpretability of the solution.
Lastly a single component solution was generated. Only two character strengths did not load strongly: Pru-
dence and Modesty. Several others were also poorly represented, as indicated by the squared loadings. How-
Table 7
Stepwise regression analysis with FFM scales and social desirability as predictor variables of raw and centred character strength scores
Character strength Proportion of raw score accounted for Proportion of centred score accounted for
A C E N O Soc
Des
A C E N O Soc
Des
Appreciation of Beauty — — — — 27.6
**
— 4.41
*
— — — 10.1
**
Capacity for Love 24.97
**
— — — 8.01
*
6.49
**
— 4.79
*
Citizenship/Teamwork — 34.7
**
— — — 7.53
**
17.49
**
— 13.59
**
Curiosity — 5.06
*
19.06
**
— 11.28
**
— — — — 5.62
*
Equity/Fairness 30.46
**
— — — — 5.52
*
20.02
**
— — 5.17
*
— 3.79
*
Forgiveness/Mercy 44.89
**
— — — — 2.66
*
33.43
**
3.4
*
————
Gratitude — 11.24
**
6.55
*
—————————
Hope/Optimism — — 3.21
*
35.51
**
— — — — — 18.18
**
——
Humour/Playfulness — 34.73
**
— 4.22
**
— — 8.22
**
20.43
**
———
Industry/Perseverance/
Persistence
— 58.67
**
— — — — — 37.66
**
— 3.7
*
——
Integrity/Honesty/
Authenticity
— 17.28
**
— — — 6.44
*
— — 5.28
*
———
Judgment/Open-
mindedness
— 22.83
**
2.8
*
— 4.58
*
— — 6.82
**
15.26
**
— — 7.11
*
Kindness/Generosity 20.67
**
— 3.79
*
— — — 6.98
**
— — 7.34
*
——
Leadership — — 19.75
**
— — — 6.95
*
————
Love of Learning 9.22
**
— — 25.67
**
— — 12.04
**
— 8.01
**
Modesty/Humility 22.85
**
— 17.79
**
— — — 11.39
**
— 32.12
**
— 6.35
**
Originality/Creativity 34.2
**
— — — — — 10.97
**
— — — 17.8
**
Perspective/Wisdom — 21.82
**
— — 13.5
**
— 7.13
*
— — — 5.07
*
Prudence — 18.8
**
11.21
**
— — — 4.72
*
29.41
**
2.9
*
6.66
**
Self-control — 29.02
**
— — — — 5.82
*
————
Social/Personal/
Emotional Intel
— — 32.9
**
— 6.46
**
— — 15.46
**
8.42
**
——
Spirituality/
Religiousness
5.72
*
———————————
Valour/Bravery/
Courage
— — — 16.59
**
5.84
*
— 6.96
**
— — 6.77
*
——
Zest/Enthusiasm/
Vitality
— 25.99
**
17.76
**
— — — 5.37
**
10.78
**
———
Total Character
Strength
— 29.56
**
7.13
**
— 12.08
**
4.61
*
Note: ""p< .001, "p6.01.
796 C. Macdonald et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 787–799
Author's personal copy
ever, the idea that the character strengths are best represented by one overarching factor was further sup-
ported by the very high Cronbach Alpha coecient of .96 that was obtained for the Total Character Strength
score. Although this variable is not the one suggested for the VIA scale by Peterson and Seligman (2004), it
may provide a useful addition to a theory of character.
In order to further investigate the VIA structure we centered the data and again conducted factor analyses.
None of the resulting factor solutions were supportive of the proposed relationship between the 24 character
strengths and the six virtues. Therefore, regardless of which factor solution might be considered most appro-
priate from these results, none of them supports at the next level of abstraction the six virtues as proposed by
Peterson and Seligman (2004). But that does not mean that these six virtues are meaningless. Like the 24 char-
acter strengths, they were originally found ubiquitously in cross-cultural and cross-generational literature. The
interpretation of the virtues as higher order representations of a large number of character strengths was
purely theoretical. The results of the factor analyses within this study, and even the results of Peterson and
Seligman’s (2004) own factor analyses, reveals that their theory does not hold up under empirical investiga-
tion. The question now open for consideration is what to do with the six virtues Peterson and Seligman
(2004) found to be ubiquitous in various forms of literature. Potentially the six virtues could be better dealt
with as a stand-alone classification with a separate instrument needing to be developed to measure them, or
simply treated as additional character strengths themselves that could be added to Peterson and Seligman’s
classification of character strengths.
Another question raised by the factor analyses is how to interpret the overarching factor that was found in
the raw data factor structure but not found (or minimized) in the centered scores factor structure. Centering
the data appeared to removed the influence of the large first factor. This then raises the question of what this
factor was representing. Centering is generally thought to remove the influence of socially desirable respond-
ing. However, one suggestion we make here is that social desirability in this instance is itself a character
strength (rather than a response set) with anti-social tendencies being its antithesis. As such, it would seem
inappropriate to remove a critical aspect of the construct the VIA sets out to measure.
The most simple interpretation might be that this factor is representing some sort of ‘‘goodnessas a
whole, thus raising the question of whether a person possessing large amounts of one character strength is
also likely to possess large amounts of multiple character strengths. Another interpretation may be that
because these character strengths are measured via self-assessment they might be subject to self-enhancing
halo eects. Therefore, self-perception of one’s overall character might be what is being measured by this
factor.
The correlations between the Marlow-Crowne Social Desirability scores and the character strengths and
factor scores partly support these suggestions. However, Social Desirability was found to be significantly
related to some character strengths but not all character strengths. This clearly does not support Peterson
and Seligman’s (2004) assertion that the VIA is free of social desirability eects because all the items of the
VIA are socially desirable. Rather the dierential relationships observed suggest that some character strengths
are more socially desirable than others. Perhaps not surprisingly all the character traits that loaded on the fac-
tor of Niceness were all significantly related to social desirability scores.
A further complication is that centering the character strength scores reduced but did not remove the influ-
ence of social desirability. This finding, together with the dierential relationships between social desirability
scores and the character strengths and factor scores, and the presence of a possible single overriding factor,
strongly suggest that research into the predictive validity of the VIA is required. Findings from such research
might be able to indicate which scores are predictive of positive behaviour: raw character strength scores, cen-
tered scores or scores with social desirability partialed out.
Within considering various factor analyses of the VIA it is important not to lose sight of the individual
character strengths. Given that the majority of the 24 constructs had alpha coecients over .6 (as shown in
Table 3) and that they are all highly recognized constructs within society, one should not ignore them as indi-
vidual constructs. The only character strength with very low internal consistency was Leadership. The reason
for this may have been the young age of the majority of participants and their lack of opportunities to exhibit
leadership. Prudence, Integrity and Hope also had reliabilities of less than .60. One may argue, for example,
that Prudence is not a concept that is familiar to the current generation of younger people. This leads to the
suggestion that the cultural context of character needs to be considered.
C. Macdonald et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 787–799 797
Author's personal copy
As hypothesised, some of the predictors of the character strengths consisted of more than one FFM con-
struct, which brings into doubt Peterson and Seligman’s (2004) idea that each character strength should be
represented by one FFM construct. Furthermore, some FFM scales were only weakly related to the character
strengths. In particular, Neuroticism was only predictive (negatively) of Hope/Optimism and Valour/Bravery/
Courage. A similar trend was seen with the regressions conducted on the centralized data. Within this, some
dierences in predictors were seen between the two analyses although the stronger predictors remained con-
stant between the two data sets. Moreover, changes between the data sets did not show any consistent pattern,
with some FFM traits increasing in predictiveness when moving from the original to the centralized data sets,
whereas others decreased. A better understanding of what latent construct is being removed when centering
the data is needed before this can be discussed further. What is evident from both of these data sets is that
as hypothesized, for a majority of the character strengths there was more than one FFM predictor.
None of the factor analyses were entirely ‘clean’ in terms of cross-loadings. The cross-loadings suggest that
such character strengths might be positioned in factor space between two orthogonal factors or between two
(or more) of the FFM traits as suggested in the AB5C personality model (Hofstee, de Raad, & Goldberg,
1992). Other strengths, such as spirituality, might be related to a sixth ‘big trait’ (e.g., see Piedmont,
1999).
1
Another way of relating the FFM to character strength is to look at how the FFM relates to the fac-
tors obtained in each solution attempted. When this was done with the four factor solution of the character
strength scores (not centered) one could tentatively interpret the factors as representing higher traits such as
Positivity, Intellect, Conscientiousness and Niceness. The first component was positively correlated with
Extroversion and negatively with Neuroticism, with the character strengths reflecting the idea of being positive
(or engaged) and active in life (and the workplace). The second component reflects character strengths mainly
related to the activity of the mind (except Valour/Bravery/Courage) and was positively correlated with Open-
ness. The third component was highly positively correlated with Conscientiousness and reflected self-regula-
tion of one’s attitudes and actions. The fourth component (Niceness) was highly positively correlated with
Agreeableness and the individual character strengths are all related to interaction between individuals.
A limitation of our research was the small sample size. It would be interesting to see how the 213 individual
items of the VIA scale factored together as opposed to the 24 character strengths analysed here. To do this,
over 1000 participants would be needed to allow for at least five participants per item. From this analysis one
could further investigate the influence of the overarching factor. Furthermore it is important that relationships
between the VIA and other psychological constructs continue to be investigated in order to elaborate the con-
struct validity of the character strengths themselves. Peer ratings of character strength could also be used in
future research to provide some validation for the VIA. In our study, both a single factor and a four factor
solution of the non-centered VIA character strength scores were most interpretable perhaps suggesting that
the latter represents the influence of the Big 5 on character strengths whereas the former may represent some
other global influence.
References
Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2005). Honesty–humility, the big five and the five-factor model. Journal of Personality, 73(5), 1321–1353.
Botwin, M. D., Buss, D. M., & Shackelford, T. K. (1997). Personality and mate preferences: Five factors in mate selection and marital
satisfaction. Journal of Personality, 65(1), 107–136.
Brose, L. A., Rye, M. S., Lutz-Zois, C., & Ross, S. R. (2005). Forgiveness and personality traits. Personality and Individual Dierences, 39,
35–46.
Cawley, M. J., Martin, J. E., & Johnson, J. A. (2000). A virtues approach to personality. Personality and Individual Dierences, 28(5),
997–1013.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1994). Stability and change in personality from adolescence through adulthood. In C. F. Halverson,
Jr., R. P. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood
(pp. 138–149). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaun Associates.
Dahlsgaard, K., Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2005). Shared virtue: The convergence of valued human strengths across culture and
history. Review of General Psychology, 9(3), 203–213.
Gable, S. L., & Haidt, J. (2005). What (and why) is positive psychology? General Review of Psychology, 9(2), 103–110.
1
We are indebted to one of the reviewers for these two suggestions.
798 C. Macdonald et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 787–799
Author's personal copy
Goldberg, L. R. (1999). A broad-bandwidth, public domain, personality inventory measuring the lower-level facets of several five-factor
models. In I. Mervielde, I. Deary, F. De Fruyt, & F. Ostendorf (Eds.). Personality Psychology in Europe (vol. 7, pp. 7–28). Tilburg,
The Netherlands: Tilburg University Press.
Goldberg, L. R., Johnson, J. A., Eber, H. W., Hogan, R., Ashton, M. C., Cloninger, C. R., et al. (2006). The international personality item
pool and the future of public-domain personality measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 84–96.
Hofstee, W. K. B., de Raad, B., & Goldberg, L. R. (1992). Integration of the big five and circumplex approaches to trait structure. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 146–163.
Haslam, N., Bain, P., & Neal, D. (2004). The implicit structure of positive characteristics. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(4),
529–541.
International Personality Item Pool (2001).A scientific collaboratory for the development of advanced measures of personality traits and
other individual dierences. Available from <http://ipip.ori.org>, accessed:19/03/06.
King, L. A., Walker, L. M., & Broyles, S. J. (1996). Creativity and the five-factor model. Journal of Research in Personality, 30, 189–203.
Paunonen, S. V. (2003). Big five factors of personality and replicated predictions of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
84(2), 411–424.
Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Character strengths and virtues. New York: Oxford.
Piedmont, R. L. (1999). Does spirituality represent the sixth factor of personality? Spiritual transcendence and the five-factor model.
Journal of Personality, 67, 985–1013.
Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction. American Psychologist, 55, 5–14.
Shackelford, T. K., Schmitt, D. P., & Buss, D. M. (2005). Universal dimensions of human mate preferences. Personality and Individual
Dierences, 39(2), 447–458.
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries.
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 1–65.
Strahan, R., & Gerbasi, K. (1972). Short, homogenous version of the Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scale. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 28, 191–193.
Suroglou, V. (2000). Religion and the five factors of personality: A meta-analytic review. Personality and Individual Dierences, 32, 15–25.
Maslow, A. H. (1973). Dominance, self-esteem, self-actualization: Germinal papers of A.H. Maslow. Monterey: Brooks/Cole.
McCrae, R. R., & John, O. P. (1992). An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications. Journal of Personality, 60(2), 175–215.
Wallis, C. (2005). The new science of happiness. Reprinted from Time, January 17.
C. Macdonald et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 787–799 799
... However, only moderate support has been found for the original theorybased classi cations of character traits and virtues within the VIA, as factor analytic studies suggest that positive character traits may be best represented by one (Macdonald, Bore, & Munro, 2008), three (Shryack, Steger, Krueger, & Kallie, 2010), four (Macdonald et al., 2008), or ve virtue factors . The higher order virtue constructs identi ed in these factor analytic studies have been subjectively labeled and described in such diverse terms as cautiousness, conscientiousness, courage, intellect/ intellectual strengths, justice/humanity, niceness, positivity, temperance, transcendence, wisdom/knowledge, and vitality. ...
... However, only moderate support has been found for the original theorybased classi cations of character traits and virtues within the VIA, as factor analytic studies suggest that positive character traits may be best represented by one (Macdonald, Bore, & Munro, 2008), three (Shryack, Steger, Krueger, & Kallie, 2010), four (Macdonald et al., 2008), or ve virtue factors . The higher order virtue constructs identi ed in these factor analytic studies have been subjectively labeled and described in such diverse terms as cautiousness, conscientiousness, courage, intellect/ intellectual strengths, justice/humanity, niceness, positivity, temperance, transcendence, wisdom/knowledge, and vitality. ...
... In contrast, the virtues are least related to openness to new experiences (i.e., 2 of 12 virtues). Consistent with previous studies, there were no consistent relationships identi ed between character strengths and BFI personality constructs (Macdonald et al., 2008). ...
Article
Virtues and character traits are increasingly recognized as impacting health outcomes, although distinctions between these constructs remain unclear. In order for Christian social workers to most effectively incorporate virtues-based interventions into their clinical practices, there is a need to identify the distinct nature of the different virtues and their relationships to health outcomes. In Part I, a principal components factor analysis of six character traits (i.e., altruism, empathy, forgiveness, gratitude, hope, and humility) based on 402 students primarily from Christian universities determined the empirical validity of these constructs (i.e., whether they are best conceptualized as distinct constructs, dimensions of higher order constructs, or one overall “goodness” virtue). Results identified 12 distinct character traits, suggesting a need to focus on specific character traits rather than general virtues. In Part II, hierarchical regressions indicated that personality variables predicted 37% of variance in mental health, with only gratitude and lack of resentment toward others (i.e., two of the 12 identified character traits) predicting an additional 8% of the variance (no variables predicted physical health). The results suggest the need to conceptualize character traits as distinct constructs, and that interventions to increase gratitude and reduce resentment may be most effective in improving mental health outcomes in Christian college students.
... To facilitate empirical research on their classification, they developed the self-report Virtues In Action (VIA) scale. Validation studies however, have not supported the underlying factor structure they proposed (Macdonald & Bore, 2008;Shryack, Steger, Krueger & Kallie, 2010). We live in a world where extreme, outrages, and risky behaviors are sometimes encouraged and rewarded . ...
Chapter
While robust conceptualizations of Islamic leadership are covered in Islamic studies and the humanities, its nascence within Business and Management and the Social Sciences presents both arguments and opportunities for further investigation. In response, this paper explores the notion of balanced leadership. We aim to fill a gap in knowledge through delineating Islamic perspectives: first, delving into the origins of balance within the Western and Eastern traditions; second, examining how such concepts are understood within the field of psychology; and third, exploring the notion of balance within the field of management by going through character-centered leadership theories. Finally, a conceptualized Islamic perspective on balanced leadership is presented in detail.
... To facilitate empirical research on their classification, they developed the self-report Virtues In Action (VIA) scale. Validation studies however, have not supported the underlying factor structure they proposed (Macdonald & Bore, 2008;Shryack, Steger, Krueger & Kallie, 2010). We live in a world where extreme, outrages, and risky behaviors are sometimes encouraged and rewarded . ...
Book
Despite continuous public interest in leadership studies as well as the abundance of extant literature, the world continues to face leadership predicaments. Given the global crises mankind has been recently exposed to, which include the financial crisis of 2008 as well as the COVID-19 pandemic, further exploration of leadership concepts seems warranted. This edited book aims at introducing the concept of Balanced Leadership from an Islamic perspective. Drawing on both leadership and Islamic studies, the book synchronizes between secular and religious knowledge domains whilst introducing the notion of balance to mainstream leadership literature. The conceptualizations presented in the book serve to dissect the leadership literature, present historical and philosophical accounts of the balanced leadership concept and infuse that with Islamic theology. The book is useful for practitioners interested in leadership studies as well as scholars and researchers aiming at extending the leadership literature. Unprecedented in compiling scholarship on the balanced leadership topic especially from an Islamic perspective. Tackles the notion of Islamic Balanced Leadership from several perspectives.
... The Big Five Model or the Five-Factor Model (FFM) assumes the way in which personality can be divided into a smaller number of fundamental constructs (Macdonald, Bore, & Munro, 2008). Following that theory, personality can be described by means of five factors: extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, emotional stability and openness to experience (Pervin & John, 1997). ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
This paper discusses the effects of additional strength and force training on the improvement of swimming performance in swimmers aged 10 to 14 years. The objective of the paper is to determine the effects of a program of additional strength training on dry land. The category observed in this study comprised swimmers aged 10‐14 years. Additional strength training offers swimmers of this age group the opportunity to enhance their motor skills, explosive strength, speed and speed endurance, which in turn yields an improvement in swimming results. A search of electronic databases returned a set of studies which met the established criteria. The effects of the applied studies, lasting between six weeks and one year, and taking place in sessions between 20 and 60 minutes in duration, with a frequency of two to five times a week, indicate a significant improvement in swimming ability, sprint performance, stroke strength, stroke tempo, jump height, start time at 10m, absolute swimming speed, stroke frequency, turn time, stroke length and stroke efficiency. The scarcity of available studies into this problem affords researchers the opportunity for further research. The significance for sport and pedagogy lies in the effect of additional dry‐land strength training in swimmers aged 10 to 14, which also warrants consideration regarding strategic planning for developing swimming performance with a view to improving swimming results.
... The Big Five Model or the Five-Factor Model (FFM) assumes the way in which personality can be divided into a smaller number of fundamental constructs (Macdonald, Bore, & Munro, 2008). Following that theory, personality can be described by means of five factors: extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, emotional stability and openness to experience (Pervin & John, 1997). ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Restricted blood flow resistance training (RBFRT/KAATSU) is a relatively new method of training that leads to positive effects in the strength gain and hypertrophy of muscle tissue such as high intensity resistance training of ≤70 % of 1RM but for much shorter period and without risks of injuries. KAATSU Training consists of performing low‐intensity resistance training while a relatively light and flexible cuff is placed on the proximal part of one’s lower or upper limbs, which provides appropriate superficial pressure. KAATSU Training does not induce ischemia within skeletal muscle, but rather promotes a state of blood pooling in the capillaries within the limb musculature. Applied basics and clinical research conducted over the past 15 years has demonstrated that KAATSU Training not only improves muscle mass and strength in healthy volunteers and athletes but also benefits patients with cardiovascular and orthopedic conditions, even astronauts.
... The Big Five Personality Factors is a hierarchical model which is descriptive and taxonomic and is based on personality traits. It assumes the way in which personality can be divided into a smaller number of fundamental constructs (Macdonald, Bore, & Munro, 2008). Modified by Lewis Goldberg, the Big Five Model summarizes the most human personality differences into 5 dimensions; namely, Neuroticism (N), Extraversion (E), Conscientiousness (C), Agreeableness(A) and Openness (Goldberg, 1990). ...
... "Appendix A" lists the page numbers for each section of text included in our analysis. While Table 2 organizes the virtues and strengths according to the structure proposed by Peterson and Seligman (2004), this structure has been called into question by empirical work supporting 3-5 factor solutions (Brdar & Kashdan, 2010;Macdonald et al., 2008;McGrath, 2015;Ng et al., 2018). Within each excerpt, we coded sentences that defined the relevant trait, excluding text that provided theoretical, etymological, or historic background to the traits. ...
Article
Full-text available
The theoretical relationship between personality and character, as two approaches to conceptualizing individual differences, remains poorly defined. Attempts to clarify the relationship by analyzing leading personality and character frameworks—NEO-PI-3 and Peterson and Seligman’s Character Strengths and Virtues (CSV)—have yielded divergent, unclear results. In this paper, we use a qualitative thematic analysis method to systematically compare NEO-PI-3 and CSV trait descriptions. Our analysis found 35 overlapping facet-strength trait pairs, including 22 morally-toned facet-strength relationships. For the CSV, the overlap was spread out among the strengths, while the overlap for NEO-PI-3 was mostly located in the factors of Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness. We also found traits that were uniquely evaluated or emphasized by each framework. NEO-PI-3 tended to emphasize traits related to emotions, tasks, and socializing; the CSV tended to emphasize self-management, prosocial, and worldview traits. Our analysis indicates that the two frameworks share substantial description of traits—including moral traits—but are not redundant. Our conceptual results were largely supported by empirical studies. Future research should continue to synthesize conceptual and empirical scholarship to advance understanding of the relationship between these two frameworks for evaluating individual differences.
... Interestingly, although Park and Peterson (2006) proposed six virtues, i.e., wisdom and knowledge, courage, humanity, justice, temperance and transcendence, constituted by the 24 character strengths at the conceptual level, results from factor analysis in empirical studies varied. For example, those studies have reported different numbers of latent factors, which imply candidates for virtues (Macdonald et al., 2008;McGrath & Walker, 2016;Peterson et al., 2008). Although a recent study conducted by McGrath et al. (2021) presented a three-factor model that was cross-validated across diverse representative samples, the discrepancy in the factor structure between the conceptually-proposed and empirically-supported virtue structure models measured by the VIA instrument may warrant further investigations. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
We explored the relationship between 24 character strengths measured by the Global Assessment of Character Strengths (GACS), which was revised from the original VIA instrument, and moral functioning comprising postconventional moral reasoning, empathic traits and moral identity. Bayesian Model Averaging (BMA) was employed to explore the best models, which were more parsimonious than full regression models estimated through frequentist regression, predicting moral functioning indicators with the 24 candidate character strength predictors. Our exploration was conducted with a dataset collected from 666 college students at a public university in the Southern United States. Results showed that character strengths as measured by GACS partially predicted relevant moral functioning indicators. Performance evaluation results demonstrated that the best models identified by BMA performed significantly better than the full models estimated by frequentist regression in terms of AIC, BIC, and cross-validation accuracy. We discuss theoretical and methodological implications of the findings for future studies addressing character strengths and moral functioning.
Article
Full-text available
يهدف علم النفس الايجابي إلى تحقيق الرفاهية الشخصية، إذ تستند عقيدته المركزية في ذلك على تحديد ثم تفعيل وتنمية مكامن القوى الشخصية، على اعتبار أنها بُنى نفسية متعددة الأبعاد تساعد على فهم وتنمية خصائص الشخصية الجيدة، وهو ما يساعد نحو التطوير الإيجابي للشباب ما قد يحقق السعادة والازدهار؛ وهي نتائج أكدتها العديد من الدراسات العالمية. وعليه، تعتبر هذه الورقة البحثية كامتداد لهذه الدراسات، والتي نهدف من خلالها إلى التَحَقُق من موثوقية وصحة النسخة العربية من مقياس مكامن القوة الشخصية والقيم/أو الفضائل العاملة، على عينة من الشباب الجامعي من مختلف الولايات بالجزائر. والذين بلغ عددهم (180) فرد، 154 اناث و26 ذكور. وقد توصلت نتائج هذه الدراسة إلى أن بصمة القوى الشخصية التي تميز الشباب الجامعي تتمثل في: قوة الانصاف، قوة الاستقامة، قوة العطف والكرم، قوة التواضع وقوة الروحانية/التدين، وأنه لا توجد فروق بين الجنسين، حيث بينت العديد من الدراسات السابقة أن نتائج الدراسة الحالية ترتبط ايجابا مع الرفاه الشخصي، الرضا عن الحياة الجامعية والتفوق في الأداء الدراسي الجامعي. ـــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــ ـــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــAbstractــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــ Positive psychology aims to achieve flourishing. It central tenet’s is based on identifying and activating and developing personal strengths, as a multidimensional construct that helps to understand and develop the characteristics of good personality, which contributes towards the positive development of individuals which may achieve happiness and well-being, These results have been confirmed by many international studies. Accordingly, this current study is considered as an extension of these studies in which we aim to verify the reliability and validity of the Arabic version of the VIA-IS, The study sample consisted of a sample of university youth from different states in Algeria of (n= 180) individual, 154 females and 26 males. The results of this study found that the personal strengths of the university youth are: the strength of equity, strength of integrity, strength of kindness and generosity, strength of humility and strength of spirituality/religiosity, and that there are no differences between the sexes. The current relationship is positively related to personal well-being, satisfaction with university life and excellence in university academic performance. Keywords: Positive psychology ; Good Character ; Character Strengths and Virtues
Article
Purpose This study aims to review the development of strengths assessment in mental health services and evaluate the features of those newly developed strengths assessments. Method A systematic literature review approach was adopted to identify relevant articles from September 2010 to August 2021. Results The results reveals that 1) the Value in Action Inventory of Strengths is the most widely studied strengths assessment but contains shortcomings in the application of mental health services; 2) 12 strengths assessments have been newly developed, which demonstrated quantitative measure, brief questionnaire, and contextual consideration are the tendency of measure development; 3) domains of strengths assessments were categorized into individual strengths, environmental strengths, and interpersonal strengths, in which individual strengths was the most commonly focus; 4) strengths conceptualization and psychometric properties are two remained limitations of existing strengths assessments. Discussion Developing a brief, quantitative, holistic, contextualized, recovery-oriented, psychometrically sound, and strengths-based assessment for people with mental illnesses is promising.
Article
Full-text available
This study reports on the development of the Spiritual Transcendence Scale, a measure designed to capture aspects of the individual that are independent of the qualities contained in the Five-Factor Model of Personality (FFM). Using 2 separate samples of undergraduate students including both self-report ( Ns = 379 and 356) and observer data ( N = 279), it was shown that Spiritual Transcendence: (a) was independent of measures of the FFM; (b) evidenced good cross-observer convergence; and (c) predicted a wide range of psychologically salient outcomes, even after controlling for the predictive effects of personality. Given the long theoretical pedigree of Transcendence in the psychological literature, it was argued that Spiritual Transcendence represents a broad-based motivational domain of comparable breadth to those constructs contained in the FFM and ought to be considered a potential sixth major dimension of personality. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Most UK medical schools use aptitude tests during student selection, but large-scale studies of predictive validity are rare. This study assesses the United Kingdom Clinical Aptitude Test (UKCAT), and its four sub-scales, along with measures of educational attainment, individual and contextual socio-economic background factors, as predictors of performance in the first year of medical school training. A prospective study of 4,811 students in 12 UK medical schools taking the UKCAT from 2006 to 2008 as a part of the medical school application, for whom first year medical school examination results were available in 2008 to 2010. UKCAT scores and educational attainment measures (General Certificate of Education (GCE): A-levels, and so on; or Scottish Qualifications Authority (SQA): Scottish Highers, and so on) were significant predictors of outcome. UKCAT predicted outcome better in female students than male students, and better in mature than non-mature students. Incremental validity of UKCAT taking educational attainment into account was significant, but small. Medical school performance was also affected by sex (male students performing less well), ethnicity (non-White students performing less well), and a contextual measure of secondary schooling, students from secondary schools with greater average attainment at A-level (irrespective of public or private sector) performing less well. Multilevel modeling showed no differences between medical schools in predictive ability of the various measures. UKCAT sub-scales predicted similarly, except that Verbal Reasoning correlated positively with performance on Theory examinations, but negatively with Skills assessments. This collaborative study in 12 medical schools shows the power of large-scale studies of medical education for answering previously unanswerable but important questions about medical student selection, education and training. UKCAT has predictive validity as a predictor of medical school outcome, particularly in mature applicants to medical school. UKCAT offers small but significant incremental validity which is operationally valuable where medical schools are making selection decisions based on incomplete measures of educational attainment. The study confirms the validity of using all the existing measures of educational attainment in full at the time of selection decision-making. Contextual measures provide little additional predictive value, except that students from high attaining secondary schools perform less well, an effect previously shown for UK universities in general.
Article
Full-text available
Measures used for medical student selection should predict future performance during training. A problem for any selection study is that predictor-outcome correlations are known only in those who have been selected, whereas selectors need to know how measures would predict in the entire pool of applicants. That problem of interpretation can be solved by calculating construct-level predictive validity, an estimate of true predictor-outcome correlation across the range of applicant abilities. Construct-level predictive validities were calculated in six cohort studies of medical student selection and training (student entry, 1972 to 2009) for a range of predictors, including A-levels, General Certificates of Secondary Education (GCSEs)/O-levels, and aptitude tests (AH5 and UK Clinical Aptitude Test (UKCAT)). Outcomes included undergraduate basic medical science and finals assessments, as well as postgraduate measures of Membership of the Royal Colleges of Physicians of the United Kingdom (MRCP(UK)) performance and entry in the Specialist Register. Construct-level predictive validity was calculated with the method of Hunter, Schmidt and Le (2006), adapted to correct for right-censorship of examination results due to grade inflation. Meta-regression analyzed 57 separate predictor-outcome correlations (POCs) and construct-level predictive validities (CLPVs). Mean CLPVs are substantially higher (.450) than mean POCs (.171). Mean CLPVs for first-year examinations, were high for A-levels (.809; CI: .501 to .935), and lower for GCSEs/O-levels (.332; CI: .024 to .583) and UKCAT (mean = .245; CI: .207 to .276). A-levels had higher CLPVs for all undergraduate and postgraduate assessments than did GCSEs/O-levels and intellectual aptitude tests. CLPVs of educational attainment measures decline somewhat during training, but continue to predict postgraduate performance. Intellectual aptitude tests have lower CLPVs than A-levels or GCSEs/O-levels. Educational attainment has strong CLPVs for undergraduate and postgraduate performance, accounting for perhaps 65% of true variance in first year performance. Such CLPVs justify the use of educational attainment measure in selection, but also raise a key theoretical question concerning the remaining 35% of variance (and measurement error, range restriction and right-censorship have been taken into account). Just as in astrophysics, 'dark matter' and 'dark energy' are posited to balance various theoretical equations, so medical student selection must also have its 'dark variance', whose nature is not yet properly characterized, but explains a third of the variation in performance during training. Some variance probably relates to factors which are unpredictable at selection, such as illness or other life events, but some is probably also associated with factors such as personality, motivation or study skills.
Article
Industry has seen massive changes in the field of management and employee labor relations during the past century -from a period of strikes and confrontation to the evolution of labor law that includes workers' rights and safety considerations. To meet the challenges for continued performance improvement, leaders seek further improvement in management of employee relations and commitment. With the evolution of positive psychology, enlightened leaders are seeking a better understanding of what positive mindsets can have on the organization and a better understanding of motivation.
Article
Comments on W. J. Camara and D. L. Schneider's (see record 1994-23870-001) summary of reports on integrity tests (ITs). It is argued that the construct of honesty or integrity remains vague and ill-defined. Evidence for the criterion validation of ITs is not very compelling. It seems unlikely that the present applications of ITs will ever meet established American Psychological Association ethical standards.
Article
In this article, we seek to summarize current practice concerning situational judgment tests in personnel selection. We begin by describing the manner in which situational judgment tests are developed and examining the diverse ways in which situational items are presented and scored. We then offer speculation concerning constructs assessed by situational judgment tests as well as discuss the legal aspect of situational judgment measures. We also review meta-analytic evidence concerning the construct validity of situational judgment tests and offer several new meta-analytic findings. Situational judgment tests are shown to be typically correlated moderately with general mental ability. Their primary personality correlates are emotional stability, conscientiousness, and agreeableness. Situational test scores also tend to increase with increasing years of job experience. The article concludes with a list of areas that need addressed in future research.
Article
Recent lexical studies of personality structure suggest that there are 6, not just 5, basic dimensions of human personality variation. The most distinguishing feature of this new 6-dimensional Structure, known as the HEXACO model, is the addition of a new factor named Honesty-Humility. We demonstrate that this new dimension has important implications in personnel psychology. Specifically, the HEXACO model substantially outperformed the Five-factor model (FFM) in predicting workplace delinquency based on samples obtained in 3 different countries, namely, Australia, Canada, and the Netherlands. In addition, the HEXACO model substantially outperformed the FFM in predicting scores on an overt integrity test, due to the inclusion of the Honesty-Humility dimension.