ArticlePDF Available

Using a two-factor theory of achievement motivation to examine Performance-based outcomes and self-regulatory processes

Authors:

Abstract

Many researchers agree that achievement motivation represents a multidimensional concept. We employed a two-factor theory of achievement motivation (intrinsic and extrinsic factors) to predict three achievement-related factors: generalized expectancy for success, need for cognition, and self-reinforcement. As predicted, intrinsic achievement motivation was positively associated with scores on all three achievement-related factors, whereas extrinsic achievement motivation was positively related only to generalized expectancy for success. Subsequent regression analyses revealed that intrinsic achievement motivation better predicted all three factors than did extrinsic achievement motivation. Implications for employing a two-factor model of achievement motivation are presented for both basic and applied applications.
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 1
Using a Two-factor Theory of Achievement Motivation to Examine Performance-Based
Outcomes and Self-regulatory processes
Paul A. Story1, Jason W. Hart2, Mark F. Stasson3, & John M. Mahoney4
1 Auburn University at Montgomery
2 Christopher Newport University
3 Metropolitan State University
4 Virginia Commonwealth University
Author Note
Paul A. Story https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2220-1105
Paul A. Story is now at the Department of Psychological Science, Kennesaw State
University.
John M. Mahoney passed away in December of 2007 and this article was published in his
honor as he was a contributing author.
We have no conflict of interest to disclose.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Paul A. Story, Kennesaw
State University, 1000 Chastain Road, Kennesaw, GA 30144, United States. Email:
pstory@kennesaw.edu
This is the accepted manuscript of an article published in Personality and Individual
Differences. The final published version is available at:
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.10.023
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 2
Licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0
International License: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 3
Abstract
Many researchers agree that achievement motivation represents a multidimensional concept
(e.g., Cassidy & Lynn, 1989; Hart, Stasson, Mahoney, & Story, 2007). We employed a two-
factor theory of achievement motivation (intrinsic and extrinsic factors) to predict three
achievement-related factors: generalized expectancy for success, need for cognition, and self-
reinforcement. As predicted, intrinsic achievement motivation was positively associated with
scores on all three achievement-related factors, whereas extrinsic achievement motivation was
positively related only to generalized expectancy for success. Subsequent regression analyses
revealed that intrinsic achievement motivation better predicted all three factors than did extrinsic
achievement motivation. Implications for employing a two-factor model of achievement
motivation are presented for both basic and applied applications.
Keywords: achievement motivation, intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, need for
cognition, generalized expectancy for success, frequency of self-reinforcement,
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 4
1. Introduction
In the last decade, researchers have increasingly examined relations among achievement
motivation, personality, and behavior (e.g., Busato, Prins, Elshout, & Hamaker, 2000; Farsides &
Woodfield, 2003; Hart, Stasson, Mahoney, & Story, 2007; Major, Turner, & Fletcher, 2006;
Paspalanov, 1984; Phillips, Abraham, & Bond, 2003; Zsolnai, 2002). Although many researchers
agree that achievement motivation represents a complex, multi-dimensional concept, there is
frequently disagreement about the exact number of factors and how to best conceptualize the
construct (e.g., Cassidy & Lynn, 1989; Deci & Ryan, 1985; Harackiewicz, Barron, Tauer, &
Elliot, 2002; Herzberg, 2003; Vroom, 1964; Woo, Gibbons, & Thornton, 2007). Current research
proposes that theories of achievement motivation can be summarized well by two theoretically
and empirically supported motivational factors: intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (Ryan & Deci,
2000).
The distinction between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation has been exemplified in classic
theories of motivation. For example, Herzberg, Mausner, and Snyderman (1959) found that
employees often cited intrinsic related reasons for being satisfied with work, whereas extrinsic
related reasons were reported as sources of dissatisfaction with work. McGregor (1960)
recommended that managers motivate employees based on whether the worker is either
externally or intrinsically motivated. Vroom (1964) stated that one's motivation to work is a
multiplicative function of three factors: expectancy, instrumentality, and outcome valence. The
distinction between extrinsic and intrinsic motivational factors becomes especially meaningful
for outcome valence, as some people focus on extrinsic outcomes (e.g., money), whereas others
focus on intrinsic outcomes (e.g., satisfaction from mastering a task). These classic motivation
theories are consistent with a two-factor approach.
Achievement motivation refers to the tendency to set and work toward personal goals
and/or standards (Cassidy & Lynn, 1989). Working from a multidimensional view of
achievement motivation, the Cassidy and Lynn Achievement Motivation Scale (CLAMS) was
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 5
developed to assesses achievement motivation on seven interrelated, but conceptually distinct,
factors: Work Ethic (effort based on enjoying the work for its own sake), Acquisitiveness for
money and material wealth (effort based on a need to obtain valued material objects and
money), Dominance (effort based on the need to exert influence on others), Pursuit of Excellence
(effort based on the need to meet a personal standard of excellence), Competitiveness (effort
based on the need to outperform others), Status (effort based on the need to climb the "social
ladder"), and Mastery (effort based on the need to solve or perform challenging tasks).
Although each factor offers unique insight into the complexity of achievement motivation, a
clearer understanding of achievement motivation may emerge by collapsing the seven factors
into a parsimonious two-factor model.
Influenced by Deci and Ryan’s (1985; 2008) self-determination theory (SDT), our earlier
research (Hart et al., 2007; Story, Stasson, Mahoney, & Hart, 2008) found that the CLAMS fits
well within a two-factor model of achievement motivation: intrinsic and extrinsic achievement
motivation. Consistent with our approach, Deci and Ryan (2008) argued against a unified
construct of motivation, but rather stated that motivation is either autonomous or controlled.
Autonomous motivation refers to types of motivation that involve internal processes through
which people come to identify with and internalize the value of an activity, whereas controlled
motivation refers to types of motivation that are the result of external demands. Based on this
information, it appears that autonomous motivation seems consistent with our notion of intrinsic
achievement motivation, whereas controlled motivation is analogous to extrinsic achievement
motivation.
Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation has been shown to relate to academic performance.
College students who adopt external, performance-oriented goals, worry about how their
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 6
performance compares to others, receive higher grades, and show increased performance
compared to students who adopt internal, learning-focused goals (Barron & Harackiewicz, 2001;
Elliot & Church, 1997; Harackiewicz, Barron, Carter, Lehto, & Elliot, 1997; Harackiewicz,
Barron, Tauer et al., 2002). On the other hand, intrinsically motivated students focus more on
mastering material than extrinsically motivated students, which leads to increased persistence
and involvement in the classroom (Barron & Harackiewicz, 2001; Elliot & Church, 1997; Elliot
& Harackiewicz, 1996). While the research is mixed (Elliot & Church, 1997; Harackiewicz et
al., 1997), some studies have found that intrinsic motivation predicts final course grades (Phillips
et al., 2003) and performance on standardized tests (Gottfried, 1985).
Intrinsically motivated students use more complex cognitive strategies than extrinsically
motivated students. For example, students who enjoy learning were more likely to report using
deeper level processing strategies compared to students focused on competitive performance
(Nolen, 1988). Students instructed to focus on mastering a task use deeper level processing
strategies compared to those instructed to focus on competing with others (Graham & Golan,
1991). Seventh grade students who enjoyed their classes used a wider variety of cognitive
strategies when studying (Pintrich & De Groot, 1990). Junior high students adopting a mastery
orientation used more cognitive strategies than students not using this intrinsic-based orientation
Patrick, Ryan, & Pintrich, 1999). Similar results have been found among college students
(Greene & Miller, 1996; Walker, Greene, & Mansell, 2006); suggesting that intrinsically
motivated students enjoy thinking and have a desire to engage in effortful thinking.
Studies have found that intrinsic motivation is associated with better self-regulatory
strategies, not surprising given these individuals are oriented towards mastering and learning the
material (Ryan & Deci, 2000). Even after controlling for prior achievement, students high in
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 7
intrinsic motivation are better at self-regulating their behavior, and work harder to learn the
material than those low in intrinsic motivation (Patrick et al., 1999; Pintrich & de Groot, 1990).
Extrinsic motivation on the other hand is negatively associated with self-regulatory strategies
(Patrick et al., 1999). For example, extrinsically motivated individuals tend to prefer easier tasks
compared to challenging ones (Ames, 1992; Gottfried, 1985). Intrinsically motivated students
then, seem to reinforce their own behavior whereas extrinsically motivated students do not.
Research also shows that intrinsic motivation is positively associated with a belief in
one’s ability to succeed. Students high in intrinsic motivation perceive themselves as more
competent compared to children low in intrinsic motivation (Gottfried, 1985; Pintrich & de
Groot, 1990). Junior high school students adopting a mastery orientation have increased levels of
self-efficacy (Patrick et al., 1999). Intrinsically motivated college students are confident in their
abilities and expect to succeed (Garcia & Pintrich, 1996; Greene & Miller, 1996; Walker et al.,
2006). Thus, intrinsic motivation is positively associated with expectations of success.
Extrinsic motivation serves as an underlying component of performance goals (Lepper,
Corpus, & Iyengar, 2005). Numerous studies find that performance goals are predictive of valued
achievement performance outcomes (Barron & Harackiewicz, 2001; Harackiewicz, Barron,
Pintrich, Elliot, & Thrash, 2002). For example, Harackiewicz and colleagues (1997) found that
the academic performance of students was positively associated with performance goals, but not
with mastery goals. Taken as a whole, extrinsic motivation appears to be especially important in
contexts in which performance outcomes are salient.
The research summarized above links extrinsic motivation to performance outcomes and
intrinsic motivation to cognitive and self-regulatory processes. Much of the research on intrinsic
and extrinsic motivation contrasts these motivation types as two ends of a continuum or as two
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 8
distinct categories, one is either intrinsically or extrinsically motivated. However, it seems
possible that across a wide range of situations one could be motivated by both intrinsic and
extrinsic factors. For example, while writing a term paper, a student's motivation may be a
product of the desire to receive a good grade in combination with his or her passion for writing.
In this study, we examine the utility of a two-factor model that examines the separate effects of
intrinsic and extrinsic motivation.
The achievement motivation literature is impressive in scope and depth, but many
important questions remain unanswered. In the present study, we use a two-factor model of
achievement motivation that provides a parsimonious, yet informative, lens through which to
view the construct and its relationship to a number of achievement-related constructs. Using a
two-dimensional conceptualization of motivation we assess the independent impacts of intrinsic
and extrinsic motivation, as well as possible interactive effects. This may allow for a richer
understanding of how achievement motivation relates to other achievement variables.
Extrinsic achievement motivation (EAM) and intrinsic achievement motivation (IAM)
were used to predict three achievement-related constructs: need for cognition, frequency of self-
reinforcement, and generalized expectancy for success. We choose need for cognition (NFC;
Cacioppo, Petty, & Kao, 1984), because this construct measures the degree to which individuals
engage in and enjoy effortful thinking. Past research (e.g., Nolen, 1988) has shown those
classified as intrinsically motivated to show a preference for effortful thinking, so our first
hypothesis (H1) is that we expect IAM to be positively related to NFC. It is less clear if and how
EAM might be predictive of NFC, so we make no definitive prediction for EAM.
Frequency of self-reinforcement (FSR; Heiby, 1982) was included as a measure of self-
reported self-regulation. Prior research has shown that intrinsic motivation is positively
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 9
associated with self-regulation (Patrick et al., 1999; Pintrich & de Groot, 1990), so our second
hypothesis (H2) is that IAM will be positively related to FSR, because self-reinforcement is very
much an internal process. Given that those high in EAM look to the environment for rewards and
reinforcers, it seems unlikely that EAM would be predictive of an internal process such as FSR.
Generalized expectancy for success (GESS; Fibel & Hale, 1978) is the degree to which
one expects one will attain valued outcomes and/or goals. We included this variable because it
provides us with a self-report of future performance, which should be associated with
environmental motivators. Thus, our third hypothesis (H3) is that EAM will be positively related
to GESS. However, because some goals and outcomes may also be more “internal” (e.g.,
mastering a task), our fourth hypothesis (H4) is that IAM should also be positively related to
GESS.
2. Method
2.1 Participants
Introductory psychology students (total N = 340) took part in the research to complete
part of a course requirement. In this study, participants completed several individual differences
measures during mass testing sessions, at multiple time points in a semester. All participants
were treated in a manner consistent with the American Psychological Association Code of
Professional Ethics (APA, 2002).
2.2 Measures
The 49-item CLAMS measures seven achievement motives: Work Ethic, Acquisitiveness,
Dominance, Pursuit of Excellence, Competitiveness, Status Aspiration, and Mastery. Participants
responded to each item (e.g., I find satisfaction in working as well as I can), using a five-point
Likert-type scale (1 - never to 5 - always). The seven subscales were collapsed into two higher-
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 10
level dimensions (see Story et al., 2008). Work ethic, pursuit of excellence, and mastery were
combined to form Intrinsic Achievement Motivation (IAM; α = .71) and acquisitiveness,
dominance, competitiveness, and status aspiration subscales formed the measure of Extrinsic
Achievement Motivation (EAM; α = .64).
The Generalized Expectancy for Success Scale (Fibel & Hale, 1978) measures the degree
to which an individual tends to expect that he or she will be able to attain valued outcomes
and/or goals (α = .91). The scale consists of 30 items (e.g., In the future I expect that I will
succeed in the projects I undertake; In the future I expect that I will get the promotions I
deserve), rated on five-point scales (1 = highly improbable to 5 = highly probable).
The 18-item Need for Cognition Scale (Cacioppo et al., 1984) measures the degree to
which individuals take pleasure engaging in effortful thinking (α = .85).
The 30-item Frequency of Self Reinforcement Questionnaire (Heiby, 1982) assesses how
often individuals self-reinforce and their feelings towards success and failure (α = .77).
2.3. Procedure
Participants completed a number of personality and individual difference measures at
multiple time points during one semester. At the first session, all participants filled out the
achievement motivation measures, Generalized Expectancy for Success Scale, and Need for
Cognition scale. One hundred nineteen of the original participants returned to a second session at
which they filled out the Frequency of Self-reinforcement Questionnaire. At the end of each
session, participants were debriefed and thanked for their participation.
3. Results
Simple correlations between each achievement motivation measure and the GESS, NFC,
and FSRQ scores are displayed in Table 1. Supporting H1, IAM scores were positively
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 11
correlated with scores on the NFC scale (r = .61, p <. 01), greater levels of intrinsic motivation
were associated with a higher need for cognition. On the other hand, EAM was not significantly
related to NFC. Consistent with H2, IAM was positively correlated with FSR (r = .32, p < .01),
showing that those with higher levels of intrinsic motivation reported higher levels of self-
regulation. However, there was no significant correlation between EAM and FSR. As predicted,
GESS was positively correlated with both EAM (r = .28, p < .01) and IAM (r = .45, p < .01),
supporting H3 and H4. Higher levels of extrinsic and intrinsic achievement motivation were each
predictive of higher expectations of success.
IAM and EAM were positively correlated with one another (r = .22, p < .01), but the
modest correlation allows for the possibility that they could have unique contributions in
explaining the other achievement variables. To explore the predictive utility of using the two-
factor model of achievement motivation, we performed regressions to predict NFC, FSR, and
GESS from the IAM and EAM measures. We used centered scores for IAM and EAM to
minimize multicollinearity problems (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). In addition, the regression
models included the interaction between IAM and EAM to examine possible non-additive effects
of these variables. A summary of the regression results are in Table 2.
Consistent with H1, IAM (β = .62, p < .01) was the strongest predictor of need for
cognition. However, EAM (β = -.13, p < .01) had a significant negative relation with NFC after
controlling for IAM, suggesting that higher levels of extrinsic motivation are associated with
lower levels of cognitive activity. A significant interaction between IAM and EAM (β = .11, p <
.05) qualified the main effect results. We examined the interaction effect using simple slopes
predicting NFC for low (1 SD below the mean), average (mean), and high (1 SD above the mean)
values of IAM and EAM (see Figure 1). Generally, those high in IAM had high levels of NFC
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 12
that were not impacted by EAM. On the other hand, those who were low in IAM had relatively
low levels of NFC and were susceptible to the inverse effect of EAM.
As predicted in H2, IAM was positively related to frequency of self-reinforcement (β =
.32, p < .01), those with higher levels of intrinsic motivation reported higher levels of self-
reinforcement and self-regulation. EAM was not related to FRSQ as the EAM main effect and
interaction term were not significant.
The regression predicting GESS supported both H3 and H4, with main effects for both
EAM (β = .21, p < .01) and IAM (β = .43, p < .01) showing an independent positive effect for
each type of motivation. The interaction between EAM and IAM was also significant (β = -.15, p
< .01), and can be understood by examining predicted GESS scores for combinations of low,
average, and high EAM and IAM values (see Figure 2). Generally, the effect of either motivation
is highest when a person is low on the other type of motivation; whereas at high levels of either
IAM or EAM, the other motivation factor has a relatively small effect. In short, if one is high on
either IAM or EAM that person will have a relatively high expectation of success.
4. Discussion
The results of the present study suggest that a two-factor model of achievement
motivation offers a useful and straightforward method in which to examine the relations among
achievement-related constructs. IAM was positively related to self-reported levels of internal
processes such as cognitive activity (need for cognition) and self-regulation (frequency of self-
reinforcement), while both EAM and IAM were related to a more performance based (outcome)
variable, generalized expectancy for success. In particular, the usefulness of the two-factor
achievement motivation model, as opposed to a single bipolar dimension, was exhibited best
with generalized expectancy for success in that both IAM and EAM, as well as their interaction,
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 13
were significant predictors of performance expectations. Specifically, the results indicated that a
person can have high expectations for success if one is high in either EAM or IAM, and being
high in both types of achievement motivation provides little rise in success expectations over
being high in just one type of achievement motivation.
Generally, these results are consistent with existing literature showing that intrinsically
motivated individuals not only enjoy engaging in effortful thinking (Graham & Golan, 1991;
Nolen, 1988; Pintrich & de Groot, 1990; Walker et al., 2006), but they are also more likely to
self-reinforce and regulate their behaviors (Patrick et al., 1999; Pintrich & de Groot, 1990).
Intrinsic motivation is also associated with higher self-efficacy and expectations of success
(Garcia & Pintrich, 1996; Greene & Miller, 1996; Walker et al., 2006). Although researchers
have not always found a statistically meaningful relationship between both types of achievement
motivation and expectation of success, we found positive relationships. Of course, comparisons
to studies that define intrinsic and extrinsic motivation as opposite poles of a single dimension
cannot be made easily with our conceptualization of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation as
separate, independent constructs (see Deci & Ryan, 2008).
Our results suggest one’s dispositional levels of motivations affect cognitive activities,
self-regulatory strategies, and expectations for success. Those generally high in intrinsic
motivation seem to prefer challenging cognitive tasks and can self-regulate their behaviors, so
offering rewards, setting external goals, or deadlines will do little for them, unless they are also
high in extrinsic motivation. Offering rewards and assigning goals/deadlines would be strategies
best suited for the person high in extrinsic motivation, who is more focused on the outcome than
the process. For those with high intrinsic motivation, emphasis could be placed on the engaging
nature of the task and encouragement of self-set goals and deadlines. Implications from our study
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 14
are that some may be intrinsically and extrinsically motivated, and would profit from a mixture
of these approaches. Furthermore, there may be some individuals who are not motivated in either
way, and may require help learning how to work on their own, perhaps with focused tasks that
can build intrinsic or extrinsic motivation. For example, a common recommendation for
educators seems to be that teachers should appeal to both types of motivation, and our results
support this approach, especially when teachers do not know their students’ level of achievement
motivation (Barron & Harackiewicz, 2001; Komarraju & Karau, 2005).
One implication of our study is that extrinsic and intrinsic motivation are not always
antagonistic in nature (i.e., extrinsic motivation may actually operate synergistically with
intrinsic motivation in a positive direction). The idea that extrinsic and intrinsic motivations are
not in conflict with each other is not new (e.g., Covington & Müeller, 2001). Guay, Ratelle, and
Chanal (2008) found that positive academic outcomes are associated with both autonomous and
controlled motivations. In sum, individuals may not be limited to just one type of motivation as
they tackle life's demands (Vallerand, Pelletier, & Koestner, 2008).
The study reported here is not without limitations. All of the data collected were
correlational and were measures of dispositional motivation. We did not manipulate goals or
motivation levels. Furthermore, we acknowledge that interventions can focus individuals on one
type of motivation, perhaps overriding the other type in that context (as in the classic over-
justification effect, see Deci & Ryan, 2008). This study included only self-report measures and
we did not collect performance outcomes. Also, students’ perceptions of how they are motivated
may be different from what actually motivates them (Nisbett & Wilson, 1977), and expectations
of success may not be related perfectly to actual achieved levels of success. Finally, it is worth
noting that the frequency of self-reinforcement data were collected at a different time point than
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 15
the other measures, and not all participants returned to provide data. It is possible that students
who returned at the second time point were different from those who did not, though we cannot
predict how this might impact the results.
Future studies should continue to explore the utility of this two-factor model of
achievement motivation as it may provide more detail than the intrinsic-extrinsic dichotomy.
One approach might be suggested by SDT, in which sub-theories such as organismic integration
theory identify particular dimensions of controlled (extrinsic) motivation (Ryan & Deci, 2000).
In the academic domain, the Academic Motivations Inventory (Moen & Doyle, 1977) or
Academic Motivation Scale (Vallerand & Bissonnette, 1992) measure multiple facets of
achievement motivation. The CLAMS (Cassidy & Lynn, 1989) measure can also distinguish
sub-types of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation and be applied to a wide range of work, school,
and other settings. More generally, the two-factor model of motivation should be tested across a
variety of different performance contexts, and we would hope to see this two-factor approach
lead to the development of techniques that enhance performance of individuals from a wide
range of motivational types. The challenge is to determine what motivates different individuals
in particular contexts thus selecting a strategy that will enable each person to be successful.
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 16
References
American Psychological Association (2002). Ethical principles of psychologists and code of
conduct. American Psychologist, 57, 1060-1073.
Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student motivation. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 84, 261-271.
Barron, K. E., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2001). Achievement goals and optimal motivation:
Testing multiple goal models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 706-
722.
Busato, V. V., Prins, F. J., Elshout, J. J., & Hamaker, C. (2000). Intellectual ability, learning
style, personality, achievement motivation and academic success of psychology students
in higher education. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 1057-1068.
Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., & Kao, C. F. (1984). The efficient assessment of need for cognition.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 306-307.
Cassidy, T., & Lynn, R. (1989). A multifactorial approach to achievement motivation: The
development of a comprehensive measure. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 62, 301-
312.
Covington, M. V., & Müeller, K. J. (2001). Intrinsic versus extrinsic motivation: An
approach/avoidance reformulation. Educational Psychology Review, 13, 157-176.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human
behavior. New York: Plenum Press.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008). Facilitating optimal motivation and psychological well-being
across life’s domains. Canadian Psychology, 49, 14-23.
Elliot, A. J., & Church, M. A. (1997). A hierarchical model of approach and avoidance
achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 218-232.
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 17
Elliot, A. J., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). Approach and avoidance achievement goals and
intrinsic motivation: A mediational analysis. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 70, 461-475.
Farsides, T., & Woodfield, R. (2003). Individual differences and undergraduate academic
success: The roles of personality, intelligence, and application. Personality and
Individual Differences, 34, 1225-1243.
Fibel, B., & Hale, W. D. (1978). The generalized expectancy for success scale: A new measure.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 924-931.
Garcia, T., & Pintrich, P. R. (1996). The effects of autonomy on motivation and performance in
the college classroom. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 21, 477-486.
Gottfried, A. E. (1985). Academic intrinsic motivation in elementary and junior high school
students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 631-645.
Graham, S., & Golan, S. (1991). Motivational influences on cognition: Task involvement, ego
involvement, and depth of information processing. Journal of Educational Psychology,
83, 187-194.
Greene, B. A., & Miller, R. B. (1996). Influences on achievement: Goals, perceived ability, and
cognitive engagement. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 21, 181-192.
Guay, F., Ratelle, C. F., & Chanal, J. (2008). Optimal learning in optimal contexts: The role of
self-determination in education. Canadian Psychology, 49, 233-240.
Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Carter, S. M., Lehto, A. T., & Elliot, A. J. (1997). Predictors
and consequences of achievement goals in the college classroom: Maintaining interest
and making the grade. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1284-1295.
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 18
Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Pintrich, P. R., Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2002).
Revision of achievement goal theory: Necessary and illuminating. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 94, 638-645.
Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Tauer, J. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2002). Predicting success in
college: A longitudinal study of achievement goals and ability measures as predictors of
interest and performance from freshman year through graduation. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 94, 562-575.
Hart, J. W., Stasson, M. F., Mahoney, J. M., & Story, P. (2007). The Big Five and achievement
motivation: Exploring the relationship between personality and a two-factor model of
motivation. Individual Differences Research, 5, 267-274.
Heiby, E. M. (1982). A self-reinforcement questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 20,
397-401.
Herzberg, F. (2003). One more time: How do you motivate employees? Harvard Business
Review, 81, 87-96.
Herzberg, F., Mausner, B., & Snyderman, B. (1959). The motivation to work (2nd ed.): John
Wiley.
Komarraju, M., & Karau, S. J. (2005). The relationship between the big five personality traits
and academic motivation. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 557-567.
Lepper, M. R., Corpus, J. H., & Iyengar, S. S. (2005). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivational
orientations in the classroom: Age differences and academic correlates. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 97, 184-196.
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 19
Major, D. A., Turner, J. E., & Fletcher, T. D. (2006). Linking proactive personality and the big
five to motivation to learn and development activity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91,
927-935.
McGregor, D. (1960). The human side of enterprise: McGraw-Hill.
Moen, R., & Doyle, K. O. (1977). Construction and development of the academic motivations
inventory (AMI). Educational and Psychological Measurement, 37, 509-512.
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on
mental processes. Psychological Review, 84(3), 231-259.
Nolen, S. B. (1988). Reasons for studying: Motivational orientations and study strategies.
Cognition and Instruction, 5(4), 269-287.
Paspalanov, I. (1984). The relation of nAch to extraversion, emotional instability and level of
anxiety in people of different social status and success. Personality and Individual
Differences, 5, 383-388.
Patrick, H., Ryan, A. M., & Pintrich, P. R. (1999). The differential impact of extrinsic and
mastery goal orientations on males' and females' self-regulated learning. Learning and
Individual Differences, 11(2), 153-171.
Phillips, P., Abraham, C., & Bond, R. (2003). Personality, cognition, and university students'
examination performance. European Journal of Personality, 17, 435-448.
Pintrich, P. R., & de Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of
classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(1), 33-40.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic
motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68-78.
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 20
Story, P. A., Stasson, M. F., Mahoney, J. M., & Hart, J. W. (2008). A two-factor model of
achievement motivation. Social Behavior and Personality, 36, 707-708.
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston, MA:
Allyn & Bacon/Pearson Education.
Vallerand, R. J., & Bissonnette, R. (1992). Intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivational styles as
predictors of behavior: A prospective study. Journal of Personality, 60, 599-620.
Vallerand, R. J., Pelletier, L. G., Koestner, R. (2008). Reflections on self-determination theory.
Canadian Psychology, 49, 257-262.
Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation. New York: Wiley.
Walker, C. O., Greene, B. A., & Mansell, R. A. (2006). Identification with academics,
intrinsic/extrinsic motivation, and self-efficacy as predictors of cognitive engagement.
Learning and Individual Differences, 16, 1-12.
Woo, S. E., Gibbons, A. M., & Thornton, G. C., III. (2007). Latent mean differences in the facets
of achievement motivation of undergraduate students and adult workers in the US.
Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 1687-1697.
Zsolnai, A. (2002). Relationship between children's social competence, learning motivation and
school achievement. Educational Psychology, 22, 317-330.
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 21
Table 1
Correlations between achievement motivation scales and NFC, GESS, & FRSQ.
Need for Generalized Expectancy Frequency of
Cognition† for Success† self-reinforcement‡
Intrinsic AM .61** .45** .32**
Extrinsic AM .03 .28** .04
†Ns range from 318 to 339
‡Ns range from 112 to 116
** p < .01
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 22
Table 2.
Regression Analyses Predicting Need For Cognition, Generalized Expectancy for Success, and
Frequency of Self-Reinforcement from Intrinsic and Extrinsic Achievement Motivations.
Need for Generalized Frequency of
cognition expectancy for self-reinforcement
success
Predictor Beta R2Beta R2Beta R2
Intrinsic AM .62** .39** .43** .25** .32** .09**
Extrinsic AM -.13** .21** .04
IAM x EAM .11* -.15** -.09
** p < .01, *p <.05
Notes: Variables were centered prior to analysis, R2 values are adjusted, and all Betas are
standardized coefficients. Regressions that excluded the interaction term showed similar results
for the main effects. N = 323 for NFC, N = 315 for GESS, N = 111 for FSR
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 23
Figure Captions
Figure 1. Regression slopes of IAM and EAM predicting need for cognition. Low IAM and
EAM = 1 SD below the mean; high IAM and EAM = 1 SD above the mean.
Figure 2. Regression slopes of IAM and EAM predicting expectations for success. Low IAM
and EAM = 1 SD below the mean; high IAM and EAM = 1 SD above the mean.
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 24
TWO-FACTOR THEORY OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 25
... The level of job satisfaction positively relates to the motivation of public service. Story, et al. [38] 2009 Work ethic, the pursuit of excellence, and skills should be considered as components of internal motivation to achieve it: the desire for ownership and profit, the need for dominance, and the desire for competition and prestige. This feeling depends on the value that employees receive from their work, as well as the actual and expected profits. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study examines the comparison of job satisfaction between government and private sector employees. Labor is a legal category that defines an employee's employment. Employee satisfaction is defined as the evaluation of their performance and work environment. It includes not only employee satisfaction with salary and benefits but also satisfaction with job content, excellent leadership, relationships with coworkers, and so on. The article describes the labor market model, the private sector model, and the public sector model. Upon comparing the private sector and public sector models in the labor market, we discovered that each sector possesses unique characteristics. This study focuses on the differential impact of each type of fit on outcomes, dividing personality fit with a profession into two levels: education fit and competency fit. Correlation analysis, t-test, and multi-regression analysis were carried out to test a number of hypotheses. Using a survey, an analysis of the influence of the public and private sectors and a comparative analysis were conducted. We examined nine job satisfaction factors and found that public sector workers were more satisfied than their private sector counterparts. Education and technology boost careers, while supportive social policies enhance job satisfaction in the private sector.
... It includes the desire for success (a feeling of competence and accomplishment upon achieving goals) and the avoidance of failure (a strong aversion to taking risks and experiencing failure) (Collins et al., 2004). The inclination to pursue success exerts a positive influence on problem-solving and creativity (Story et al., 2009) and ranks among the most relevant individual factors associated with creativity (Schoen, 2015). Moreover, Yao and Ma (2021) have highlighted that challenge research stressors, such as time constraints, high research innovation requirements, and substantial research workloads, can positively predict postgraduates' achievement motivation. ...
Article
Full-text available
The research creativity of doctoral students is not solely fueled by their intrinsic motivation, but also thrives in an environment that offers challenging research opportunities, substantial support, and feedback from significant others. Based on the job demands-resources model, this study aims to explore the impact of challenge research stressors on the research creativity of Chinese doctoral students. A mediated moderation model was constructed to examine the mediating effect of achievement motivation and the moderating effect of supervisor developmental feedback on the relationship between challenge research stressors and research creativity. A total of 538 valid questionnaires were collected from doctoral students using convenience sampling and snowball sampling. The questionnaires included the Challenge Research Stressors Scale, the Research Creativity Scale, the Achievement Motivation Scale, and the Supervisor Developmental Feedback Scale. Regression analyses, bootstrap testing, and simple slope analyses were used to estimate the various relationships. The findings indicated that challenge research stressors had a positive effect on doctoral students’ research creativity. Supervisor developmental feedback positively moderated the impact of challenge research stressors on the achievement motivation and research creativity of doctoral students. Achievement motivation partially mediated the influence of challenge research stressors on doctoral students’ research creativity, and further fully mediated the interaction effect of challenge research stressors and supervisor developmental feedback on doctoral students’ research creativity. These findings contribute not only to our understanding of the mechanisms and boundary conditions through which challenge research stressors impact the research creativity of doctoral students, but also provide valuable insights into how to stimulate and maintain their research creativity.
... As one of the crucial components of students' learning motivation (Han and Lu, 2018), achievement motivation is the driving force behind an individual's efforts to put energy into what he or she perceives to be valuable and meaningful to achieve a desired outcome (Story et al., 2009). It can be considered as achievement motivation when an individual's behavior involves "competing at a standard of excellence" (Brunstein and Heckhausen, 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction The present study aimed to examine the effects of online game addiction on reduced academic achievement motivation, and the mediating role of learning engagement among Chinese college students to investigate the relationships between the three variables. Methods The study used convenience sampling to recruit Chinese university students to participate voluntarily. A total of 443 valid questionnaires were collected through the Questionnaire Star application. The average age of the participants was 18.77 years old, with 157 males and 286 females. Statistical analysis was conducted using SPSS and AMOS. Results (1) Chinese college students’ online game addiction negatively affected their behavioral, emotional, and cognitive engagement (the three dimensions of learning engagement); (2) behavioral, emotional, and cognitive engagement negatively affected their reduced academic achievement motivation; (3) learning engagement mediated the relationship between online game addiction and reduced academic achievement motivation.
Article
Innovation is an important strategic imperative that is necessary for organizations to effectively navigate the increasingly tumultuous market conditions they face. As such, it is critical that organizations have clarity about how to best foster employee innovative behavior, namely, idea generation, idea promotion, and idea realization. Disperse research has identified a number of antecedents of employee innovative behavior. However there is little clarity on the relative importance of these antecedents and if there is an antecedent that is most important for bringing about employee innovative behavior. In this field study, the researchers included three different acknowledged antecedents of employee innovative behavior in the same model (namely employee goal orientation, leadership effectiveness, and psychological safety) to determine the degree of their relative importance to employee innovative behavior. The findings revealed that only the employees’ personal characteristics of learning goal orientation had significant relationships with the three different types of innovative behavior (idea generation, idea promotion, and idea realization). These findings should serve to elevate the precision of the theoretical and practical attention given to the commonly studied antecedents of innovative behavior.
Article
Full-text available
This study aims to analyze the motivation level of the nurses related to their job performance and examine the impact of intrinsic motivation on the job performance of the nurses. Based on the questionnaire survey, data are collected from a sample of 155 nurses from various health institutions located in Lalitpur and Kathmandu. The descriptive analysis reported a moderate presence of intrinsic motivation in health institutions. The result of regression analysis revealed a positive and significant effect of responsibility and the nature of work on job performance. However, no significant impact of recognition and opportunity for growth was found on job performance. The findings of this study help evaluate and improve existing motivational policies adopted by the institutions for better job performance due to increased job satisfaction and engagement.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction and purpose: Students' perceptions of the learning environment play a vital role in the process of education, buoyancy, and academic performance. This study aimed to investigate the capability of explaining the academic buoyancy and academic performance of health students of Semnan University of Medical Sciences, Semnan, Iran, based on their perception of learning environment. Methods: The statistical population of this descriptive-correlational study consisted of all health students who were studying at Semnan University of Medical Sciences, Semnan, Iran, in the academic year 2018-19. A total number of 138 students were selected through systematic random sampling. The data were collected using a Dundee Ready Educational Environment Measure (DREEM), academic buoyancy questionnaires, and grade point average. Moreover, simple and multiple regressions were used for data analysis. Results: The results showed that 16.1% (R=0.408, R 2 =0.167, Adjusted R 2 =0.161, F=27.212, P<0.01) of the variance of academic buoyancy can be explained by the overall score of students' perception of the learning environment. Furthermore, according to the regression coefficients, the subscale of students' perception of educational climate had a significant role in explaining academic buoyancy. According to the findings of the study, 13.3% (R=0.373, R 2 =0.139, Adjusted R 2 =0.133, F=21.971, P<0.01) of the variance in academic performance could be explained by the overall score of students' perception of the learning environment. In addition, the subscales of students' perceptions of learning and social conditions of education had a significant role in explaining academic performance. Conclusion: Given the positive role of perception of the learning environment in explaining academic buoyancy and academic performance based on the study findings, it is very important to conduct comprehensive assessments, identify strengths and weaknesses, and perform educational interventions to enhance students' perception of the learning environment.
Article
Full-text available
Article Info Introduction: Students' perceptions of their educational environment play a very crucial role in the learning process, academic buoyancy and performance. This study aimed to investigate health sciences students' perceptions of the educational environment and evaluated the predictability of academic buoyancy through their perceptions of the educational environment. Methods: This research was a descriptive-correlational study. The statistical population consisted of all health students who were studying in Ilam, Arak, Birjand, Semnan and Hamadan Universities of Medical Sciences in the academic year 2018-19. A total number of 475 students were selected through multistage random sampling method and the data of 400 participants were analyzed (response rate = 84.2%). Data were gathered using a Dundee Ready Educational Environment Measure (DREEM) and academic buoyancy questionnaires. In this study, while using SPSS v.22 software, independent samples t-test, one-way ANOVA, simple and multiple regressions were employed for data analysis. Results: The mean overall DREEM score was 114.1±22.92 (maximum, 200). The result showed that students' perceptions of the educational environment would predict 9% of the variance of their academic buoyancy (R=0.304, R 2 =0.092, Adjusted R 2 =0.09, F=40.507, p<0.01). Also, regression coefficients showed that the components of students' perceptions of the educational environment, except the component of students' perceptions of teachers, had a significant role in predicting academic buoyancy. Conclusion: Health students' perceptions of their educational environment were at a moderate level shows that the educational environment was found to be more positive than negative. Nevertheless, addressing accurate assessments, identifying strengths and weaknesses, and conducting educational interventions are essential to enhance students' perceptions of the educational environment and, consequently, improve academic buoyancy .
Article
The main goal of this research is to analyze and explain the effect of job promotion on work motivation, to analyze and explain the job promotion effect on employee performance, to analyze and explain the job promotion effect on employee performance through work motivation, to analyze and explain the influence of motivation to work towards employee performance of PT.BPR Gunung Ringgit Malang.In this research, there are 3 variables: job promotion variable (X), work motivation(Z) and employee performance (Y), which can be classified into two variables, are independent variables and dependent variable. The independent variables consist of the job promotion (X). The dependent variable consists of work motivation (Z) and employee performance (Y). Several data collection methods are used, such as questionnaire, interview and documentation. Data analysis method is Ordinary Least Square (OLS). Supported by SPSS software version 15.0. The respondent is 80 persons. Data analysis results show that the job promotion variable (X) has a positive and significant effect on work motivation (Z). Job promotion variable (X) have an effect on positive and significant impact on employee performance variable (Y). Work motivation variable (Z) has a positive and significant impact on employee performance variable (Y).
Article
Full-text available
The present study investigated whether there are differences in intrinsic and extrinsic motivation and obsessive-compulsive symptoms by gender, academic stream and grade level. The sample of the study consisted of 512 high school students in Northern Almazar District, Jordan. Data were collected using three scales measuring intrinsic and extrinsic motivation and obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Three-way ANOVA, and three-way MANOVA were used. This study used an analytical-descriptive methodology. Results showed that males scored significantly higher on the intrinsic motivation scale, scientific stream students scored significantly higher on the intrinsic motivation scale, literary stream students scored significantly higher on the extrinsic motivation scale, twelfth grade students scored significantly higher on the extrinsic motivation scale, literary stream students scored significantly higher on the slowness subscale of the obsessive-compulsive scale, and twelfth grade students scored significantly higher on the doubting subscale of the obsessive-compulsive scale.
Article
Full-text available
Aim: the purpose of current study is to determine the effectiveness of the emotional education model based on Davanloo's psychoanalysis on academic self-regulation and academic achievement. Method: The design of this quasi-experimental study was a pre-test/post-test with the control group. In the second semester of 2018-2019, which had a grade point average (two consecutive semesters) of less than 12, 40 students were selected as the research sample. They were then randomly divided into two groups of 20 as experiments and controls. 40 students from Tehran University of Medical Sciences, School of Rehabilitation, and University of Tehran psychology and educational sciences faculty were selected by available sampling method in the second semester of 1397-98. All participants had a grade point average (GPA) of less than 12 (out of 20) in their last two consecutive semesters. They were randomly assigned into one experimental and one control group (20 participants each). The research instruments were the academic self-regulatory questionnaire (Ryan and Connell, 1989), the grade point average, and the researcher-made emotion education protocol based on Dovanloo psychodynamics. Research data were analyzed using descriptive indicators and analysis of covariance. Results: The findings showed that the mean post-test scores in the experimental group increased in the overall scores of academic self-regulation, identified regulation, and intrinsic motivation, as well as the grade point average of academic achievement. On the other hand, the internal and external adjustment variables are reduced (p <0.01). Conclusion: By educating and increasing people's awareness of feelings, anxiety, and defense mechanisms, academic self-regulation, and academic achievement can be enhanced in them.
Article
Full-text available
The authors investigated personality predictors of achievement goals in an introductory psychology class, as well as the consequences of these goals for the motivation and performance of 311 undergraduates. Two dimensions of achievement motivation (workmastery and competitive orientations; J. T. Spence & R. L. Helmreich, 1983) predicted the goals endorsed. Individuals high in workmastery were more likely to adopt mastery goals and less likely to adopt work avoidance goals, whereas competitive individuals were more likely to endorse performance and work avoidance goals. Students adopting mastery goals were more interested in the class, but students adopting performance goals achieved higher levels of performance. These results suggest that both mastery and performance goals can lead to important positive outcomes in college classes.
Article
Full-text available
Currently, there is a debate about which types of achievement goals promote optimal motivation. A number of theorists argue for a mastery goal perspective focusing on the adaptive consequences of mastery goals and the maladaptive consequences of performance goals. Others endorse a multiple goal perspective in which both mastery and performance goals can be beneficial. The purpose of the present investigation was to review why this debate has emerged and to offer a comprehensive test of the mastery vs. multiple goal perspectives. In Study 1, a correlational approach was employed to identify the optimal goals for college participants to adopt for a learning activity. In Study 2, an experimental approach was employed to identify the optimal goals to assign for the same activity. Each study revealed benefits of both mastery and performance goals, providing support for the multiple goal perspective.
Article
Full-text available
The present study examined the relationship between the Big Five and a two-factor model of achievement motivation. Participants (N = 777) completed the Big Five Inventory (John & Srivastava, 1999) and a multidimensional achievement motivation scale (Cassidy & Lynn, 1989). Correlation and multiple regression analyses disclosed relations among the Big Five and achievement motivation. Conscientiousness, openness, and extraversion were positively associated with intrinsic achievement motivation, whereas extraversion, conscientiousness, and neuroticism were positively related to extrinsic achievement motivation. Agreeableness was also found to be negatively associated with extrinsic achievement motivation. Conscientiousness was anomalous in that it was positively related to both intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. Results suggest that both forms of motivation may be more complex than originally expected.
Article
Full-text available
Most contemporary achievement goal conceptualizations consist of a performance goal versus mastery goal dichotomy. The present research offers an alternative framework by partitioning the performance goal orientation into independent approach and avoidance motivational orientations. Two experiments investigated the predictive utility of the proposed approach-avoidance achievement goal conceptualization in the intrinsic motivation domain. Results from both experiments supported the proposed framework; only performance goals grounded in the avoidance of failure undermined intrinsic motivation. Task involvement was validated as a mediator of the observed effects on intrinsic motivation. Ramifications for the achievement goal approach to achievement motivation and future research avenues are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Book
I: Background.- 1. An Introduction.- 2. Conceptualizations of Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination.- II: Self-Determination Theory.- 3. Cognitive Evaluation Theory: Perceived Causality and Perceived Competence.- 4. Cognitive Evaluation Theory: Interpersonal Communication and Intrapersonal Regulation.- 5. Toward an Organismic Integration Theory: Motivation and Development.- 6. Causality Orientations Theory: Personality Influences on Motivation.- III: Alternative Approaches.- 7. Operant and Attributional Theories.- 8. Information-Processing Theories.- IV: Applications and Implications.- 9. Education.- 10. Psychotherapy.- 11. Work.- 12. Sports.- References.- Author Index.
Article
Evidence is reviewed which suggests that there may be little or no direct introspective access to higher order cognitive processes. Subjects are sometimes (a) unaware of the existence of a stimulus that importantly influenced a response, (b) unaware of the existence of the response, and (c) unaware that the stimulus has affected the response. It is proposed that when people attempt to report on their cognitive processes, that is, on the processes mediating the effects of a stimulus on a response, they do not do so on the basis of any true introspection. Instead, their reports are based on a priori, implicit causal theories, or judgments about the extent to which a particular stimulus is a plausible cause of a given response. This suggests that though people may not be able to observe directly their cognitive processes, they will sometimes be able to report accurately about them. Accurate reports will occur when influential stimuli are salient and are plausible causes of the responses they produce, and will not occur when stimuli are not salient or are not plausible causes.
Article
A correlational study examined relationships between motivational orientation, self-regulated learning, and classroom academic performance for 173 seventh graders from eight science and seven English classes. A self-report measure of student self-efficacy, intrinsic value, test anxiety, self-regulation, and use of learning strategies was administered, and performance data were obtained from work on classroom assignments. Self-efficacy and intrinsic value were positively related to cognitive engagement and performance. Regression analyses revealed that, depending on the outcome measure, self-regulation, self-efficacy, and test anxiety emerged as the best predictors of performance. Intrinsic value did not have a direct influence on performance but was strongly related to self-regulation and cognitive strategy use, regardless of prior achievement level. The implications of individual differences in motivational orientation for cognitive engagement and self-regulation in the classroom are discussed.