Article

The Impact of Olfactory Stimulation on Dreams

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

Problem Only a limited number of trials is available regarding the impact of external and internal stimuli on human dreams. Current research focuses on the question whether these stimuli (acoustic or mildly painful stimuli e.g.) are incorporated into dreams and whether they influence their emotional coloring. In a recently published trial we were able to demonstrate that isolated olfactory stimulation does not lead to arousals. Aim of the present study was to investigate whether olfactory stimuli of different hedonic characteristics influence dreams in humans. Methods 15 young healthy female volunteers were investigated during 30 nights of testing. Standardized awakenings were performed during REM sleep. During REM phases, subjects were exposed to non-odorous control, a positive odor (PEA, 20%) and a negative odor (H2S, 4 ppm) for 10 seconds each in a randomized fashion using a computer olfactometer. After the awakening, subjects were advised to report the content of their dream in a standardized fashion and to assess the emotional coloration (positive and negative) on a four digit scale (0/1/2/3: no / little / moderate / strong feelings) to calculate the overall emotional coloration. Results In all subjects, three REM awakenings were performed. For 40 out of 45 awakenings, a dream was reported by the subjects. Mean emotional coloration after control stimulation was slightly positive (+0.5). After negative stimulation, the mean emotional coloration was shifted to negative values (-0.4) while the mean emotional coloration was significantly more positive after positive stimulation (+1.2). Conclusion Standardized REM awakenings can be performed successfully even under the conditions of an olfactory laboratory with intransal stimulation. With olfactory stimulation, the emotional coloration of dreams can be significantly influenced in accordance with the hedonic aspect of the stimulant. Significance The study opens a potential field of therapeutic intervention with nocturnal olfactory stimulation.

No full-text available

Request Full-text Paper PDF

To read the full-text of this research,
you can request a copy directly from the authors.

ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
The notion that dreaming is isolated from sensory activity is challenged by demonstrations that somatosensory stimuli are frequently incorporated into dream content. To further study such effects, four volunteers were administered pressure stimulation to either the left or the right leg during REM sleep and awakened to report their dreams. These dreams were rated and compared to non-stimulated dreams. Stimulated dreams more frequently contained leg sensations and references to the pressure stimulus than did non-stimulated dreams; dreamed leg activity, but not dreamed arm activity, was also rated as more intense. Incorporations of the stimulus were typically simple, direct kinesthetic sensations of pressure or squeezing but were also sometimes embedded in more extended ‘problem-solving’ sequences. Stimulation also increased bodily bizarreness. The latter included changes in kinesthetic quality of movement, instabilities of posture and the environment, as well as visual-kinesthetic synthesias. Although micro-arousals may be an explanatory factor, the results suggest that somatosensory stimulation influences ‘kinesthetic fantasy’, a dimension of dreaming associated with both central and peripheral sources of kinesthetic activity.
Article
Full-text available
Investigated the proposed link between vestibular activation and dream lucidity in 8 university students who spent 2 consecutive nights sleeping in a hammock in the sleep laboratory. For each night, during the 2nd, 3rd, and 4th REM sleep periods, the hammock was either stationary (control condition), or rocked at a constant frequency (experimental condition) stimulating the S's vestibular system. Ss were awakened after 10 min of REM sleep and asked to provide a mentation report that was later analyzed on a number of scales and physiological measures. Results indicate that there was a pronounced increase in both peak self-reflectiveness and lucidity mentation continuum scores associated with rocking in the early morning. Also, rocking can be incorporated into dream imagery, leading to a higher incidence of vestibular imagery and dream bizarreness. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Rapid-eye-movement (REM) sleep is associated with intense neuronal activity, ocular saccades, muscular atonia and dreaming. The function of REM sleep remains elusive and its neural correlates have not been characterized precisely in man. Here we use positron emission tomography and statistical parametric mapping to study the brain state associated with REM sleep in humans. We report a group study of seven subjects who maintained steady REM sleep during brain scanning and recalled dreams upon awakening. The results show that regional cerebral blood flow is positively correlated with REM sleep in pontine tegmentum, left thalamus, both amygdaloid complexes, anterior cingulate cortex and right parietal operculum. Negative correlations between regional cerebral blood flow and REM sleep are observed bilaterally, in a vast area of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, in parietal cortex (supramarginal gyrus) as well as in posterior cingulate cortex and precuneus. Given the role of the amygdaloid complexes in the acquisition of emotionally influenced memories, the pattern of activation in the amygdala and the cortical areas provides a biological basis for the processing of some types of memory during REM sleep.
Article
Full-text available
"Sniffin' Sticks" is a test of nasal chemosensory performance that is based on penlike odor-dispensing devices. It is comprised of three tests of olfactory function: tests for odor threshold, discrimination and identification. Previous work has already established its test-retest reliability and validity in comparison to established measures of olfactory sensitivity. The results of this test are presented as a composite TDI score--i.e., the sum of results obtained for threshold, discrimination and identification measures. The present multicenter investigation aimed at providing normative values in relation to different age groups. To this end, 966 patients were investigated in 11 centers. An additional study tried to establish values for the identification of anosmic patients, with 70 anosmics investigated in five specialized centers where the presence of anosmia was confirmed by means of olfactory evoked potentials. For healthy subjects, the TDI score at the 10th percentile was 24.5 in subjects younger than 15 years, 30.3 for ages from 16 to 35 years, 28.8 for ages from 36 to 55 years and 27.5 for subjects older than 55 years. While these data can be used to estimate individual olfactory abilities in relation to a subject's age, hyposmia was defined as the 10th percentile score of 16- to 35-year-old subjects. Our latter study revealed that none of 70 anosmics reached a TDI score higher than 15. This score of 15 is regarded as the cut-off value for functional anosmia. These results provide the basis for the routine clinical evaluation of patients with olfactory disorders using "Sniffin' Sticks."
Article
Full-text available
The amount of observed eye movement was related to the degree of participation of Ss in the events of the dreams. The last eye movement before awakening corresponded in direction to the last reported fixation of the dreamer. Certain external and internal stimuli did not influence the dream content. The course of time in the dream was comparable to the time elapsing for that activity while awake. The implications of these findings are discussed. 15 references. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2006 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
To examine olfactory arousal threshold during sleep in comparison to an auditory tone. On night 1, participants rated odor intensity when awake and experienced olfactory stimuli during stage 1 sleep. Night 2 involved stage 2, stage 4, and rapid-eye-movement (REM) sleep trials using the "staircase" threshold-detection method. Electroencephalogram, electrooculogram, electromyogram, electrocardiogram, and respiration were recorded along with behavioral response. An 800-Hz tone was given on trials when odors failed to arouse. Participants slept in individual rooms. Stimulus-delivery systems were operated from a separate room, where an experimenter observed physiologic recordings and behavioral responses. Three healthy men and 3 women aged 20 to 25 years (mean, 22 years). Two odorants, peppermint and pyridine, at 4 concentrations were presented through nasal cannulas using an air-dilution olfactometer. Tones were played over a speaker. Behavioral (button press and oral) responses, electroencephalographic activation, and changes in breathing and heart rate were assessed. Participants responded to odors on 92% of stage 1 sleep trials. Peppermint was ineffective in stages 2, 4, and REM sleep. Pyridine produced behavioral threshold on 45% of stage 2 trials, none in stage 4, and one third of REM sleep trials. Tones were effective on at least 75% of trials. Heart rate increased significantly only following behavioral responses to odors or tones across sleep stages. The data indicate that human olfaction is not reliably capable of alerting a sleeper.
Article
Full-text available
Sleep facilitates memory consolidation. A widely held model assumes that this is because newly encoded memories undergo covert reactivation during sleep. We cued new memories in humans during sleep by presenting an odor that had been presented as context during prior learning, and so showed that reactivation indeed causes memory consolidation during sleep. Re-exposure to the odor during slow-wave sleep (SWS) improved the retention of hippocampus-dependent declarative memories but not of hippocampus-independent procedural memories. Odor re-exposure was ineffective during rapid eye movement sleep or wakefulness or when the odor had been omitted during prior learning. Concurring with these findings, functional magnetic resonance imaging revealed significant hippocampal activation in response to odor re-exposure during SWS.
Article
Content analysis is an important and a frequently applied tool in dream research. Hall and Van de Castle (1) stressed the importance of interrater reliability in the application of content analytic scales, i.e., how good is the agreement between two judges scoring the same dream material independently? The present study investigated the effect of rater training on the interrater reliability of scales developed by Schredl (2). Three samples of 100 dream reports each have been analyzed by two inexperienced judges who received two training sessions after coding 100 and 200 dreams. The results indicate that tie training of raters has a positive effect on interrater reliability and the mean differences of some scales (nominal and interval scales) but not for ordinal scales (e.g., dream emotions). It remains unclear how much training is necessary for different scales and whether it might be necessary to improve the scales themselves if extensive training does not result in a desired improvement in interrater reliability. Thus, more studies investigating rater training for different systems of dream content analysis are needed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Whereas many researchers see a heuristic potential in the covert REM sleep model for explaining NREM sleep mentation and associated phenomena, many others are unconvinced of its value. At present, there is much circumstantial support for the model, but validation is lacking on many points. Supportive findings from several additional studies are summarized with results from two new studies showing (1) NREM mentation is correlated with duration of prior REM sleep, and (2) REM sleep signs (eye movements, phasic EMG) occur frequently in NREM sleep. The covert REM sleep model represents one class of explanatory models that combines the two assumptions of mind-body isomorphism and a 1-gen mentation generator; its future development will depend largely upon a more detailed understanding of sleep state interactions and their contribution to mind-body isomorphisms.
Article
This review summarizes studies of evoked potentials (EPs) applied to the investigation of human sleep and of sleep disorders. The first part is devoted to studies dealing with the nature, mechanisms and extent of information processing during sleep. EP studies suggest that the brain's ability to detect salient stimuli persists during even the deepest sleep stages, while discrimination of the stimulus' intrinsic significance and/or semantic content may persist only in stage II and paradoxical sleep (PS). Deviance detection in non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep is reflected by amplitude recovery of K-complexes elicited by stimulations that differ from the background. The evoked K-complex appears to be formed by two functionally different modules. The first may be related to the discrimination of relevant information. The second appears more sensitive to stimulus salience than to its intrinsic significance. In PS, the EP signs of stimulus discrimination are similar to those observed during waking. Thus, if the possibilities of information processing are fairly similar during stage II and PS, their respective neural mechanisms are not the same, as judged by their electrophysiological counterparts.The second part of the paper reviews clinical application of EPs to the study of sleep/wake disorders. While early potentials are of little use for diagnosis of sleep disorders, the cognitive P300 may help to quantify cognitive slowing and pathological sleepiness. However, intersubject variability restrains the use of these techniques in individual patients. A promising approach is the utilization of late responses to the study of sleep inertia with the use of “forced awakening” recording paradigms.
Article
The present article briefly reviews the literature on gender differences in dream content. The results confirm earlier findings that men dream more often about men, physical aggression and sexuality than women. Women's dreams, on the other hand, contain an equal proportion of male and female characters, more aggression turned inwardly and themes of depression. In regard to formal features, e.g., dream realism, dream length, occurrence of verbal and physical interaction, dreams of men and women are quite similar. These findings are related to meta-analyses compiling studies of gender differences in waking-life. Since men, for example, were found to be more aggressive in waking-life and women tend to be more vulnerable to depression, the assumption of continuity between waking-life and dreaming is supported.
Article
THE NUMEROUS anecdotal accounts of incorporations of naturally occurring external and internal stimuli (falling out of bed, thunder, stomachaches, etc) into dreams,1-4 as well as some early, systematic studies,5-7 attest to man's continuing interest in the nature of his commerce with the environment during sleep. Dement and Wolpert8 studied the effects of three different types of stimuli (water spray, light, and pure tone) on dreams. They presented the various stimuli during rapid eye movement (REM) periods, the stage of sleep from which dream reports are most often elicited on awakening,9-12 and found the tactile stimulus (water) to be the most potent, being incorporated 42% of the time as compared to 9% for the tone and 23% for the light. Berger13 obtained over 50% incorporation for both meaningful and nonmeaningful names. The immense discrepancy between the amount of incorporation of an auditory
Article
'Sniffin' Sticks' is a new test of nasal chemosensory performance based on pen-like odor dispensing devices. It comprises three tests of olfactory function, namely tests for odor threshold (n-butanol, testing by means of a single staircase), odor discrimination (16 pairs of odorants, triple forced choice) and odor identification (16 common odorants, multiple forced choice from four verbal items per test odorant). After extensive preliminary investigations the tests were applied to a group of 104 healthy volunteers (52 female, 52 male, mean age 49.5 years, range 18-84 years) in order to establish test-retest reliability and to compare them with an established measure of olfactory performance (the Connecticut Chemosensory Clinical Research Center Test, CCCRC). Performance decreased with increasing age of the subjects (P < 0.001). Coefficients of correlation between sessions 1 and 2 were 0.61 for thresholds, 0.54 for discrimination and 0.73 for identification. Butanol thresholds as obtained with the CCCRC increased as a function of age; this relation to the subjects' age was not found for the CCCRC odor identification task. The test-retest reliability for CCCRC thresholds was 0.36, for odor identification it was 0.60. It is concluded that 'Sniffin' Sticks' may be suited for the routine clinical assessment of olfactory performance.
Article
Recent PET data on human sleep suggest functional interactions between the amygdala and the cortex during REM sleep. This amygdalo-cortical interplay might reflect the processing of some types of memory traces.
Article
Even though various investigations found a preponderance of negative emotions in dreams, the conclusion that human dream life is, in general, negatively toned is limited by several methodological issues. The present study made use of three different approaches to measure dream emotions: dream intensity rated by the dreamer, intensity rated by a judge, and scoring of explicitly mentioned emotions (Hall & Van de Castle, 1966). Results indicate that only in the case of external raters' estimates do negative emotions outweigh the positive ones; but in the case of self-ratings (i.e., those made by the dreamer himself/herself), the ratio was balanced. Analyses showed that this is mainly due to the underestimation of positive emotions in the external ratings. Additionally, a positive correlation was found between the intensity of dream emotions and dream recall frequency, whereas gender differences were nonsignificant as regards the emotional tone of diary dreams.
Article
Olfactory event-related potentials (OERPs) were recorded in 14 young and 14 older adults, with odor strength of isoamyl acetate manipulated to assess olfactory stimulus intensity. Young participants produced significantly larger N1/P2, N2/P3 amplitudes and shorter N1, P2 and N2 latencies than older participants. Medium- and high-odor concentrations elicited significantly shorter P2 and N2 latencies than the lowest concentration for both age groups. Odor concentration appears to affect the speed of olfactory stimulus information processing regardless of age.
Article
The olfactory test battery "Sniffin'Sticks" comprises a perception threshold test, an odour discrimination test and an odour identification test. The purpose of this study was to examine the suitability of the Sniffin'Sticks for use in everyday practice and to obtain (at least provisional) normal values. Thirty normosmic and 15 anosmic volunteers were examined with the Sniffin'Sticks and the "University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test" (UPSIT). All three Sniffin'Sticks tests distinguish between normosmics and anosmics in a highly significant manner. The good correlation of the individual tests with each other and with the results of the UPSIT documents the reliability of the test results. Critical mention must be made of the overly complex determination of the olfactory threshold. In conclusion, the Sniffin'Sticks test battery provides a validated instrument adapted to European conditions for the examination of olfactory disorders. It has proven successful in everyday clinical practice and constitutes a major aid for compiling medical certificates.
Article
In animal models, ponto-geniculo-occipital waves appear as an early sign of rapid eye movement sleep and may be functionally significant for brain plasticity processes. In this pilot study, we use a combined polysomnographic and functional magnetic resonance imaging approach, and show distinct magnetic resonance imaging signal increases in the posterior thalamus and occipital cortex in close temporal relationship to rapid eye movements during human rapid eye movement sleep. These findings are consistent with cell recordings in animal experiments and demonstrate that functional magnetic resonance imaging can be utilized to detect ponto-geniculo-occipital-like activity in humans. Studying intact neuronal networks underlying sleep regulation is no longer confined to animal models, but has been shown to be feasible in humans by a combined functional magnetic resonance imaging and electroencephalograph approach.
Article
For chemosensory event-related potentials (ERP) significant effects of age and sex have been demonstrated. The aim of the present study was to assess the effects of stimulus concentration, side of stimulation, and sex on the topographical distribution of chemosensory ERP in a large group of subjects stratified for different age groups. In addition, psychophysical measures of both olfactory and trigeminal function should be assessed in greater detail compared to previous work. A total of 95 healthy subjects participated in the study. Olfactory functions were tested using the 'Sniffin' Sticks' comprising tests of odor identification, odor discrimination, and odor threshold. Trigeminal sensitivity was assessed on a psychophysical level using a lateralization paradigm. ERP to the olfactory stimulant H2S and the trigeminal irritant CO2 were recorded; stimuli were presented in different concentrations to the left and right nostril. Olfactory thresholds exhibited an age-related increase while the outcome of psychophysical trigeminal tests was not significantly affected by age. In contrast, there was no significant main effect of the factor 'sex' for olfactory tests, while women scored higher than men in the trigeminal task. ERP to olfactory and trigeminal stimuli exhibited a relationship to stimulus concentration, age, and sex with youngest women showing largest amplitudes and shortest latencies. There was no significant main effect of left- or right-sided stimulation on ERP. Measures of olfactory function were found to correlate with parameters of olfactory ERP even when controlling for the subject's age. In addition, correlations between scores in the lateralization task and parameters of the trigeminal ERP were found. Based on electrophysiological data obtained in a large sample size the present results established an age-related loss of olfactory and trigeminal function, which appears to be almost linear. Further, the present results emphasize that responses to chemosensory stimuli are related to sex, while the side of stimulation does not play a major role in the presently used paradigm. Finally, these data establish the lateralization paradigm as a psychophysical tool to investigate intranasal trigeminal function. The present results obtained in a representative group of healthy subjects establishes a comprehensive set of data, which will serve as reference for future work in this area of research.
Article
This chapter focuses on central olfactory processing in the human brain. As the psychophysiology of human olfactory function is important for appreciating its underlying neurophysiology, the chapter will begin with a brief overview of what the human nose can do, contesting notions that human olfaction is a second-rate system. It will be followed by an anatomical survey of the principal recipients of olfactory bulb input, with some comments on the unique organizing properties that distinguish olfaction from other sensory modalities. The final section will cover the neural correlates of human olfactory function, including aspects of basic chemosensory processing (odor detection, sniffing, intensity, valence) and higher-order olfactory operations (learning, memory, crossmodal integration), with particular emphasis on functional imaging data, though human lesion studies and intracranial recordings will also be discussed.
Article
To give an overview on the theoretical and practical applications of chemosensory event-related potentials. Chemosensory event-related potentials (ERPs) may be elicited by brief and precisely defined odorous stimuli. Based on the principles of air-dilution olfactometry, a stimulator was developed in the late 1970s, which allows stimulation of the olfactory neuroepithelium and the nasal mucosa with no concomitant mechanical stimulation. Chemosensory ERPs were obtained after stimulation of the olfactory nerve (olfactory ERPs) or the trigeminal nerve (somatosensory or trigeminal ERPs). The characteristics of the stimulator for chemosensory research as well as the variables influencing the responses are discussed in this paper. Implementation and normative data from our department are reported with different clinical examples from otorhinolaryngologic clinic. The bulk of the evoked response consists of a large negative component (often referred to as N1), which occurs between 320 and 450 ms after stimulus onset. This component is followed by a large positive component, often referred to as P2, occurring between 530 and 800 ms after stimulus onset. Absence of olfactory ERPs and presence (even with subtle changes) of somatosensory ERPs is a strong indicator of the presence of an olfactory dysfunction. This review examines and discusses the methods of chemosensory stimulation as well as the electrophysiological correlates elicited by such stimuli. The clinical applications of chemosensory ERPs in neurology and otorhinolaryngology are outlined.
Article
Aim of the present pilot study was to investigate whether cortically generated chemosensory event-related potentials (ERPs) can be recorded during sleep. Chemosensory function during sleep was assessed in 14 healthy female volunteers. An overnight polysomnography was performed to assess nocturnal sleep and to classify sleep stages. Chemosensory ERPs were recorded using air-dilution olfactometry. H2S (4ppm) was used for olfactory and CO2 (40%, v/v) for trigeminal stimulation. Chemosensory ERPs could be recorded during sleep for both olfactory and trigeminal stimuli in some but not all subjects. Compared to baseline, latencies of olfactory ERPs were longer and amplitudes were larger during light sleep and slow wave sleep (SWS). For trigeminal stimulation N1 latencies were longest during REM sleep. These results indicate that both trigeminal and olfactory ERPs can be recorded during sleep suggesting that chemosensory stimuli are processed on a cortical level during sleep.
Article
The interaction of sensory physiology and sleep has been studied for various sensory systems. Nevertheless, the question whether chemosensory (especially olfactory) stimuli may lead to arousals during sleep remains under discussion. Specifically, the central processing of olfactory information shows fundamental differences compared to other sensory systems. Prospective controlled trial. Sleep research facility, University Hospital. Five young healthy, normosmic volunteers. Intranasal chemosensory stimulation during sleep was based on air-dilution olfactometry. For olfactory stimulation H2S (smell of rotten eggs) was used in 4 concentrations (1, 2, 4, and 8 ppm). For trigeminal stimulation CO2 (stinging sensation) was also administered in 4 concentrations (10%, 20%, 40%, and 60% v/v) while odorless stimuli were used for control. Arousal reactions due to chemosensory stimulation were assessed during overnight polysomnography 30 seconds after the presentation of every stimulus during 23 nights of testing. For olfactory testing, an average number of 703 olfactory stimuli and 157 odorless controls were used for analysis per subject. Even the highest stimulus concentration did not produce an increase in arousal frequency. For trigeminal testing, an average number of 405 stimuli and 79 controls were used for analysis per subject, and an increase in arousal frequency was observed following the increase of stimulus concentration. With the present results we were able to demonstrate that, in contrast to trigeminal stimulation, the presentation of a strong but selective olfactory stimulus does not lead to arousals during nocturnal sleep in humans.
Elektrophysiologische Untersuchungen des menschlichen Geruchssinns
  • G Kobal
Kobal, G. Elektrophysiologische Untersuchungen des menschlichen Geruchssinns. Thieme Verlag, Stuttgart, 1981.
  • T A Nielsen
  • D L Mcgregor
  • A Zadra
  • D Ilnicki
  • L Ouellet
Nielsen, T. A., McGregor, D. L., Zadra, A., Ilnicki, D. and Ouellet, L. Pain in dreams. Sleep, 1993, 16: 490-498.
Dreams and How to Guide Them (1867)
  • H Saint-Denys
  • De
Saint-Denys, H. de. Dreams and How to Guide Them (1867).
Chemical senses: taste and olfaction
  • D V Smith
  • G M Shepherd
Smith, D. V. and Shepherd, G. M. Chemical senses: taste and olfaction. In: L. R. Squire, J. L. Roberts, N. C. Spitzer, M. J. Zigmond, S. K. McConnell and F. E. Bloom (Eds) Fundamental Neuroscience, 2nd edn. Academic Press, Amsterdam, 2003: 631-666.