Content uploaded by Marvin H. Berman
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Marvin H. Berman on Apr 15, 2019
Content may be subject to copyright.
Efficacy of neurofeedback for executive and memory function in dementia
Marvin H. Berman, Ph.D., Principal Investigator Jon Frederick, Ph.D., Project Director
Abstract
Background
Previous studies have shown that dementia is associated with quantitative EEG (QEEG) abnormalities
including increased slow activity and a reduction in the dominant alpha frequency.
Objective
This study tested whether using EEG biofeedback (neurofeedback) training to normalize abnormal
EEG activity could improve measures of memory and executive function.
Methods
Participants were randomly assigned to immediate treatment or to a waiting-list control group. All
participants received neuropsychological and QEEG assessments before and after treatment or control
conditions. Each participant’s pre-treatment QEEG was compared to a normative database, and
neurofeedback protocols were customized to normalize EEG activity at significantly deviant (>1.0±SD)
frequencies and specific scalp locations. Treatment consisted of 30 or 40, 30-minute neurofeedback
training sessions involving operant conditioning of the EEG using simultaneous visual, auditory, and
tactile reinforcements. To date, 16 subjects and 11 waitlist controls have completed treatment.
Results
Pre- and posttreatment scores displayed significant improvements in verbal memory (mean Mini
Mental Status Exam [MMSE] orientation and recall, Memory Assessment Scales’ (MAS) list and prose
memory, p< .05); visual memory (mean MAS Visual and Rey Figure recall, p< .05); Behavioral Rating
Inventory of Executive Function (mean self and informant General Executive Composite, p< .05);
Immediate Visual and Auditory (IVA) continuous performance test response control (p< .05). Trends
toward improvement were seen in the MMSE (p= .072) and psychiatric distress measured by the
Symptom Checklist 90-Revised (p= .085). A number of executive function measures did not
significantly improve, including the IVA Attention, Wisconsin Card Sort, and Delis-Kaplan Executive
Function System (with the exception of verbal fluency, p< .05). In the treatment group, the standardized
mean treatment effect on variables that improved (at p< .10) correlated significantly with the
pretreatment MAS Global Memory index (r= .71, p< .01). In the control group, this correlation was -.06.
Conclusion
These results support QEEG-based neurofeedback training as a “possibly efficacious” treatment for
dementia. The strong correlation of efficacy with pretreatment memory suggests the importance of
learning and memory in this treatment’s mechanism of action, and suggests that neurofeedback is
more strongly indicated as an earlier stage intervention.
Introduction
Dementia is associated with a diversity of EEG abnormalities, including increased power in delta (1-3.5
Hz) and theta (4-7 Hz) bands, decreased power in the alpha (8-12 Hz) and beta (13-30 Hz) bands
(Jackson & Snyder, 2008), and a reduction in the peak alpha frequency (Passant et al., 2005, Chan et
al., 2004).
In EEG biofeedback (neurofeedback), an individual’s real-time EEG is presented continuously as a
visual or auditory signal, and desired variations are rewarded. A standard practice in neurofeedback is
to analyze a baseline quantitative EEG (QEEG) during an initial assessment, and build custom
neurofeedback protocols designed to reward the normalization of each client’s individual abnormalities
(Lubar, 2004). Neurofeedback has been shown to be Possibly Efficacious for posttraumatic stress
disorder (effective in one study with waiting list controls; Chambless and Holon, 1998); Efficacious for
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (two or more studies; Monastra, 2005) and Substance Abuse
Disorders (Nelson, 2003), and Efficacious and Specific for Seizure Disorders (two or more studies with
placebo controls; Nelson, 2003).
A recent double-blind controlled study (Angelakis et al., 2007) showed that neurofeedback training that
rewarded increases in the dominant alpha frequency improved cognitive processing speed and
executive function in a small sample of normal elderly adults. The present study measures whether
neurofeedback provides similar benefits to adults with mild to moderate dementia.
Methods
The following criteria were required for inclusion in the study: (1) a diagnosis of dementia by a
physician or clinical psychologist; (2) MMSE >/ =20; (3) independence or minimal assistance in
activities of daily living; (4) age < 80; (5) BRIEF-A >90th percentile. Exclusion criteria included
psychosis, a history of seizures, drug dependence, or greater than moderate dementia.
Measures of psychological function included:
(1) Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult Version (BRIEF-A; self and informant report).
(2) Symptom Checklist 90-R.
(3) Williams’ Memory Assessment Scale, (PAR Inc).
(4) Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure Task.
(5) Wisconsin Card Sort Test.
(6) Integrated Visual and Auditory Continuous Performance Test (IVA).
(7) Delis-Kaplan Executive Function Battery (omitting Proverbs, the second Card Sort set, and the
Tower test).
A 19-channel referential EEG was recorded using a Mitsar amplifier. Five-minute eyes-open and eyes-
closed recordings were compared to a normative database us ing Neuroguide (Applied Neuroscience,
Inc.). Neurofeedback protocols were customized based on the specific frequencies and locations
identified as abnormal in each individual. Generally, rewards were provided for decreased 0-8 Hz and
22-35 Hz and increased 10-18 Hz amplitude.
Participants were randomly assigned to either an immediate treatment or waiting list group. Sixteen
completed treatment and 12 completed the waiting period. The waiting period was determined by the
average treatment completion time. Treatment consisted of 30 or 40 sessions of EEG biofeedback
using a Pendant EEG and Bioexplorer software. Desired patterns in the EEG were rewarded by the
presentation of a DVD movie. When the EEG varied outside of the desired range, reinforcement was
withdrawn by reducing the brightness and volume to 15%.
Psychological and EEG measures were repeated upon completion of treatment or the waiting period.
Aggregate Variable Variable Description Tx Control P
Executive IVA, BRIEFA, DKEFS, MAS clustering, WCST, MMSE att/calc 3.72 1.66 0.140
Global Memory MMSE orientation and recall, MAS Verbal and MAS Visual 4.45 -2.70 0.009
MAS Global Memory MAS Verbal + MAS Visual 4.17 -1.13 0.045
Verbal Memory MMSE orientation and recall, MAS list and prose 2.81 -3.69 0.024
Visual Memory MAS Visual and Rey Figure 6.36 -0.51 0.014
Results
To optimize statistical power in this small sample, individual test scores were standardized to the sample mean and standard
deviation and averaged into categories, as shown in Table 1. Changes in individual test scores are illustrated in figures 1, 2, and 3.
Table 1. One tailed t-test comparisons of pre- and posttreatment effect (standard score differences) vs controls.
When the differences in the mean standard score of all tests were analyzed, 6/16 participants in the treatment group showed
negligible improvement or a decline, compared to 10/12 in the control group. To determine whether some factors made some
participants better candidates for neurofeedback than others, pretreatment scores were correlated with the standardized mean
treatment effect on variables that improved (at p< .10). Among 45 variables considered, the only significant correlations (at p<.01)
were with MAS Verbal Memory (r=.632), Global Memory (r=.635), and MAS Global Memory (r=.714). However, when Fisher Z-
transforms were compared to the control group, only MAS global memory was significantly different (p<.01).
Improvement was predicted by 8-18 Hz amplitude in the pretreatment QEEG baselines, but this correlation was not significantly
greater than that seen in controls. However, when the average pre-post amplitude differences were compared by t-test, the treated
group showed significantly greater amplitude changes in all frequencies higher than 10 Hz (figure 4).
Improvement was also related to a reduction in 1-4 Hz amplitude between pre- and posttreatment eyes-closed QEEG at most
locations. This correlation was significantly greater than controls at F4 and C4. This difference is illustrated in figure 5, comparing
the average amplitude maps for those who improved most and least in each group.
Discussion
This study showed that neurofeedback training resulted in significant improvement in memory and some aspects of executive
function, compared to a waiting list control, suggesting that neurofeedback is a “possibly efficacious” treatment for dementia. The
finding that the efficacy of neurofeedback is greater in persons with more intact memory function suggests that this intervention is
more strongly indicated for earlier stage cases. It also suggests that learning and memory are involved in neurofeedback’s
mechanism of action.
While eyes-closed EEG baselines did not predict the efficacy of this treatment, improvement correlated with lower 1-4 Hz
amplitudes more in trained participants than in controls, and training resulted in significantly higher amplitudes above 10 Hz. These
observations may support the view that neurofeedback works by normalizing abnormal brain rhythms in the direction of training.
The eyes-open QEEGs and the session recordings, not yet analyzed, may provide concurrent validation of these results.
The low subject number in this study made many significant findings borderline and many promising trends undetectable.
*
Figure 1. Changes in executive function in treated participants vs controls.
Figure 2. Changes in visual memory in treated participants vs controls.
Figure 3. Changes in verbal memory in treated participants vs controls.
Figure 4. Compared to controls, treated participants showed greater changes in EEG
amplitude above 10 Hz in the eyes-closed baselines (red circles denote positive p<.05).
Figure 5. Compared to controls, improvement in treated subjects showed a significantly
greater correlation with reduction in slow wave activity.
Top Five Tx
Top Four Controls
Lowest Four Controls
Lowest Five Tx
-5
0
5
10
15
IVA
RCQ IVA
AQ BRIEF
-A WCST
Sets MMSE
Att/Calc DKEFS
Trails DKEFS
Errors DKEFS
VFlu DKEFS
DFlu DKEF
Stroop DKEFS
Sort DKEFS
20Q DKEFS
WC
Standard Score Difference
Treatment
Control
-10
-5
0
5
10
MAS
Vis
Reprod
MAS
Imm
Vis
Recog
MAS
Del
Vis
Recog
REY
Imm
Reprod
REY
Del
Reprod
Rey
Recognition
Standard Score Difference
Treatment
Control
* p<.05
** p<.01
Figure 6. Composite postest-pretest QEEG differences of 5 most and least improved subjects.
-20
-15
-10
-5
0
5
10
MMSE
Orient
Time
MMSE
Orient
Place
MMSE
Recall MAS
Imm
List
Recall
MAS
Del
List
Recall
MAS
Imm
Prose
Recall
MAS
Del
Prose
Recall
Standard Score Difference
Treatment
Control
** *
*
*
*
* p<.05
** p<.01
w w w . q u i e t m i n d f d n . o r g