ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to understand how men and women define sexual desire and sexual arousal and how they distinguish between the two. The authors conducted 32 semi-structured interviews with individuals in South East England, using a purposive sampling strategy to maximize the variation in experience of sexual function across the group. The authors identified three criteria that participants used to define and distinguish between desire and arousal: the sequence in which they occurred; whether the mind or the body (or both) were engaged; and the extent to which feelings of desire or arousal were responsive (in response to person or stimulus) and motivational (oriented toward a goal). Most participants attempted to distinguish between desire and arousal when prompted, but often with difficulty. Participants commonly felt that desire preceded arousal; some felt that desire was "mind" and arousal "body"; and many felt that both desire and arousal were responsive and motivational. However, the authors identified numerous times when these distinctions were reversed or the differences between terms were blurred. The results support recent proposals to merge the two diagnostic categories of female sexual arousal disorder and hyposexual desire disorder into a single diagnostic category.
How do men and women define sexual desire and sexual arousal?
Abstract
The purpose of this study was to understand how men and women define sexual desire and
sexual arousal and how they distinguish between the two. We conducted 32 semi-structured
interviews with individuals in South East England, using a purposive sampling strategy to
maximise the variation in experience of sexual function across the group. We identified three
criteria that participants used to define and distinguish between desire and arousal: the
sequence in which they occurred; whether the mind or the body (or both) were engaged; and
the extent to which feelings of desire or arousal were responsive (in response to person or
stimulus) and motivational (orientated towards a goal). Most participants attempted to
distinguish between desire and arousal when prompted, but often with difficulty. Participants
commonly felt that desire preceded arousal; some felt that desire was ‘mind’ and arousal
‘body’; and many felt that both desire and arousal were responsive and motivational.
However, we identified numerous times when these distinctions were reversed or the
differences between terms were blurred. Our results support recent proposals to merge the
two diagnostic categories of Female Sexual Arousal Disorder and Hyposexual Desire
Disorder into a single diagnostic category.
Introduction
Sexual desire and sexual arousal are widely acknowledged as complex constructs and the
relationship between them as intricate (Ågmo, 2011; Janssen, 2011; Meana, 2010); despite
their ubiquity, there is little consensus on how best to define them (Janssen, 2011).
An understanding of patient and research participant accounts of sexual desire and
arousal is important because of the reliance on self-report in diagnosing and measuring sexual
dysfunction. Patient accounts of other types of dysfunction often differ significantly from
scientific or professional definitions and theories (Furnham, 1988) and this appears true of
sexual dysfunction (see Bancroft et al., 2003; King et al., 2007).
Although there is now a small qualitative literature on women’s (and, to a lesser
extent, men’s) sexual experiences, little work has been done on how men and women
understand the terms “sexual desire” and “sexual arousal.” In this paper we briefly outline
prominent expert definitions of desire and arousal in relation to sexual response. We then
present data from a qualitative study designed to address the following research questions:
How do men and women define sexual desire and sexual arousal? Do they make a distinction
between the two? In exploring participant accounts of sexual desire and arousal we seek to
contribute to a conundrum currently troubling experts: are these two concepts distinct or are
they different facets of the same construct? (Binik et al, 2010). By exploring the accounts of
men as well as women, we will also address the current paucity of research into men’s
understanding of desire and arousal (Brotto, 2010b).
Expert definitions
Sexual desire has been broadly described as “the sum of the forces that lean us toward
and push us away from sexual behavior” Levine (2003, p. 285). Regan and Berscheid (1999)
proposed that sexual desire is “a psychological state subjectively experienced by the
individual as an awareness that he or she wants or wishes to attain a (presumably pleasurable)
sexual goal that is currently unattainable” (p. 15).
Most expert definitions of sexual arousal are similarly broad, incorporating a number
of different components of arousal beyond genital response. For example, Bancroft (2005)
conceptualized sexual arousal as “a state motivated towards the experience of sexual pleasure
2
2
and possibly orgasm” (p. 411). He proposed that arousal included a range of physical and
mental processes, including information processing of relevant stimuli, arousal in a general
sense, incentive motivation, and genital response. Bancroft’s definition incorporates ideas of
both mind and body, as well as the idea of motivation towards a goal. Janssen (2011) defined
sexual arousal as “an emotional/motivational state that can be triggered by internal and
external stimuli and that can be inferred from central (including verbal), peripheral (including
genital), and behavioral (including action tendencies and motor preparation) responses.” (p.
708). Ågmo (2011) recently suggested that the term “sexual arousal” should be abandoned, or
restricted in its use to refer only to genital arousal, arguing that “subjective sexual arousal” is
a vague and unhelpful concept.
In a recent conceptual review of sexual arousal in men, Janssen (2011) concluded that
sexual desire has traditionally been described as a motivational state, and sexual arousal
mainly as an emotional state (“an embodied affective state”), but that there is increasing
evidence of “considerable overlap” between the two.
Expert models of sexual response
Where attempts have been made to establish the chronological order of the two terms,
desire has generally been held to precede arousal. Kaplan (1979) and Lief (1977), for
example, both positioned sexual desire as the first stage of Masters and Johnson’s Human
Sexual Response Cycle, before sexual arousal. Kaplan (1979), for instance, defined sexual
desire as “a sensation that moved the individual to seek out, or become receptive to, sexual
experiences” (p. 10). These traditional models have characterised sexual response as
essentially a linear progression from sexual desire to sexual arousal, orgasm, and ultimately
resolution.
Although this linear model has received much criticism, particular with regard to its
relevance for women (Levin, 2008; Mitchell & Graham, 2008; Tiefer, 1991), it nevertheless
3
3
forms the basis of the current Diagnostic and Statistical Manual classification of discrete
desire, arousal, and orgasm disorders (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association [APA],
2000). More recent models of sexual response have not conceptualised sexual desire and
arousal as discrete “stages.” Basson (2000, 2001) proposed instead a circular model of sexual
response, in which sexual neutrality is the starting point, and desire is primarily responsive
(for instance, to cues from a partner or other stimulus) rather than spontaneous. A second
model - the Incentive Motivation model (Laan & Both, 2008; Laan & Everaerd, 1995; Laan
& Janssen, 2007) – argues that desire actually reflects early stages of arousal in response to a
sexually relevant stimulus, and this model has received considerable empirical support
(Toates, 2009). These models see arousal and desire as closely interlinked, and allow that the
experience of sexual desire may follow rather than precede sexual excitement (Laan & Both,
2008).
Reviewing the evidence for linear vs. circular models of female sexual desire and
arousal, Hayes (2011) concluded that although there was better support for linear models of
sexual response than for circular models, there was also growing evidence that “responsive
desire” (desire in response to a sexual stimulus) is a feature of many women’s sexual
experiences. To our knowledge, there have been no comparable studies involving men.
Much of the existing literature has involved “fitting” expert models to participant
experiences (e.g., Giles & McCabe, 2009; Sand & Fisher, 2007). In this study we took a more
inductive approach, allowing participants to define and describe their experiences of desire
and arousal without reference to any expert models. The data presented here come from a
larger qualitative study whose overall aim was to explore the meaning of sexual (dys)function
for individuals with and without sexual problems.
Method
Sampling strategy
4
4
We used a purposive sampling strategy that sought to maximize variation in
experience of sexual function by recruiting individuals from three groups, each with a
different likelihood of experiencing sexual difficulties. Our sampling strategy was designed
to capture roughly equal numbers of those with and without significant problems; as Table
one shows, this aim was achieved. The first group consisted of those who had sought
professional help for their difficulties. The group comprised consecutive patients attending an
NHS sexual problems clinic in London (n=6)), who were introduced to the first author by the
doctor, following their appointment. Those expressing interest were given a recruitment pack
and interviews were arranged with those who chose to take part. The second group comprised
individuals who, because of underlying health conditions (diabetes, depression and HIV),
were likely to be experiencing some difficulties but not necessarily to have sought help for
them or to have self-identified as having problems (sub-clinical). On behalf of the authors,
staff at a London general practice (GP) practice selected names randomly from their diabetes
and depression clinic lists, and invited these patients to participate by letter (n=13). This
group also included three HIV-positive participants, recruited via a staff member from an
HIV charity in a regional town. The third group served as a proxy to the general population,
with some individuals experiencing difficulties and others not. These individuals were
recruited in person by the first author from the waiting room of the same GP practice (n=10).
The waiting room was small, such that during a series of recruitment sessions, every
unaccompanied individual was approached. Non-English speakers and those under age 18
were excluded. All those taking part were given time to consider their decision and were
reassured that they could change their mind at any point. Written informed consent was
obtained prior to each interview. Although we expected, and were prepared for, some
participants to feel upset when relating their experiences, this only happened once. In this
case, and in several others, the experience of relating personal stories appeared therapeutic
5
5
for the participant.
We examined each participant account closely and categorized them subjectively
according to their reported experience of sexual difficulties (see Table 1). Individuals who
did not describe any significant frustration or difficulty were categorized as “functional;”
those who expressed minor frustrations and/or difficulties but no significant concern about
these experiences were categorized as “dissatisfied;” and those who described significant
problem(s), some level of distress and had also either sought or considered professional help
were categorized as “problematic.”
[Table 1 about here]
Data Collection
Interviews lasting between 45 minutes and two hours were carried out by the first author
(30) and second author (2) and were audio-taped (with permission). Interviews were held in a
private room in the clinics from which participants were recruited, except if the participant
requested an interview at home (2) or by phone (1), because of poor health. The interview
was guided by a topic guide that sought to draw out an individual’s priorities for a functional
sex life, and included the following questions:
What do you understand by the term sexual desire (wish for sex/sexual drive/libido)?
What do you understand by the term sexual arousal (readiness for sex)?
Is arousal different from desire? In what way?
Analysis
The interviews were transcribed verbatim by an audio-typist. The first author listened
to all the tapes and checked the transcripts as part of the familiarization process. The data
were then indexed and charted according to broad themes (Ritchie & Lewis, 2003).
Following principles of grounded theory analysis (Charmaz, 2006), the first and second
6
6
authors then undertook intensive open coding of excerpts of transcripts that had been charted
under the themes of sexual desire and sexual arousal. We generated a set of provisional codes
that conceptually labeled the ways in which participants talked about desire and arousal.
Through discussion, and with the aid of a mind map (diagrammatic representation of linkages
between codes) we began to develop categories (made up of interlinked codes), each of
which described a key criterion that participants used to distinguish between desire and
arousal (whether explicitly or implicitly). We then used these categories to selectively code
the data. The analysis developed iteratively; clarifications and refinements to categories
continued into the writing stage, through discussion between analysts, feedback on successive
drafts and by returning regularly to the transcripts.
Ethical approval
Ethical approval for the study was obtained from Camden and Islington Local
Research Ethics Committee and the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine Ethics
Committee. Governance approval was obtained from Camden and Islington Primary Care
Trust and Camden and Islington Mental Health and Social Care Trust.
Results
Difficulty making a distinction between sexual desire and sexual arousal
Participants were asked what they understood by the terms sexual desire and arousal,
and some spontaneously proffered a distinction between the two. Most attempted to do so
when prompted, but with often with difficulty (“I find that a very difficult question to
answer”; man aged 78).
Man aged 39: [Pause]… linguistically you could just as well say one is totally
interchangeable for the other, but if I had to make a distinction in English language I
would probably separate them [out as follows….].
7
7
For these participants, difficulty in distinguishing between concepts stemmed from
the view that they were intertwined, and the difference was “very subtle; it’s a matter of
degree really” (woman aged 54).
I: I’m interested a little bit in the relationship between these two – arousal and desire.
How do you see the relationship between those two?
Woman aged 47: I would see them as very closely linked. I’m not sure if you can
unpick them.
Temporal sequence
Desire before arousal
Amongst those participants who referred to a time dimension to distinguish between desire
and arousal, many saw desire as occurring before arousal. For some, the sequence – desire
first, arousal after – was clear. A sense of chronology was conveyed by terms such as
“preceding” or “coming before” in the case of desire, or as “following” or “coming after” in
the case of arousal.
P (participant): I think the arousal comes after the desire. The desire is there first
and then if the touching goes on any longer…
I (Interviewer): So you experience desire first and then that can lead to…?
P…arousal.
I: A sort of second stage.
P: I think so yes. I might not have thought too clearly about it, but I think so.
[Woman aged 64]
Woman aged 51: Well I suppose desire is a little twinge that would be followed up
by something … If you saw a bloke and you thought “oh”… and you want to get to
know them..., or there would be something physically that you wanted to see more
of ... you get the little bit of interest and then you build on that to see if there’s
anything to stoke it up with.
8
8
For some participants, desire was seen not only as a precursor but as a prerequisite to arousal.
One man (aged 56), for example, referred to sexual desire as the “physical and psychological
requirement/need to experience sexual satisfaction” and so for him desire was a condition
without which, sine qua non, arousal could not occur. This sense is echoed in descriptions of
desire as “potential” (man aged 56) and “opportunity” (man aged 78). For some, however,
sexual desire was a necessary, but not always a sufficient - condition for arousal. The not
infrequent use of the conditional tense “ I would like to... ,” in the context of desire,
indicated that sexual arousal was seen as not only in the future, but as conditional on other
favourable conditions occurring before arousal could take place:
Man aged 65:“...the attraction that you’re seeing ... That comes first and after that
there has to be something else… to continue your desire. It doesn’t happen – it could
be cut off immediately”.
By contrast, arousal was seen as more proximate to sexual activity, and contingent on sexual
activity occurring. Where desire was expectant or hopeful, arousal was in the moment, and
contiguous with sexual activity.
Woman aged 42: Desire would be wanting to have sex, and arousal or being turned
on is the act of being with somebody where sex is imminent and they were doing… if
I was attracted to them and they were doing the things that would make me feel I
wanted to have sex.
Man aged 56: [Pause]. Sexual desire is physically thinking “I would like to have
sex”. Sexual arousal is actually when sex has begun to take place – your changed,
altered state.
Woman aged 23... for me, it’s more the there and then… yes – there and then, being
aroused. (Emphasis added)
9
9
Arousal before desire
A smaller number of participants saw the order as reversed; arousal preceded, and in one case
at least, was a prerequisite to desire: “You get a total sense of desire for a person because
you’re totally aroused” (man aged 55; emphasis added). A woman aged 47 described the
reciprocal relationship between the two, desire increasing with heightened arousal and vice
versa:
I: Some people argue that they happen sequentially, so you desire sex and then you
become aroused. Would you agree with that or would you think they can happen in
all sorts of order?
P: I think I might say I desire sex because I am aroused. I might become more
aroused.
I: So for you there isn’t a particular order necessarily?
P: … I think maybe there is, to the extent that desire can probably proceed from quite
a low level of arousal. If there was absolutely no arousal I don’t think the desire
would be there… and as the desire sets in you become more aroused.
[Woman aged 47]
Temporal sequence - blurred distinction
By no means all participants, however, saw the temporal order of desire and arousal
so unequivocally. For some, the two conditions were seen as occurring simultaneously and,
at least in terms of sequence, were therefore indistinguishable. For others, there was no clear
pattern:
I: Do you think these happen in sequence – the sexual arousal and desire – or does
one happen before the other, or might they happen at the same time, or does it
change?
P: I think it can change. I think it can happen… [pause]. It comes in different ways I
think.
I: There’s not necessarily an order to it?
P: No, no.
[Man aged 33]
10
10
Factors influencing temporal sequence
A key factor determining whether desire was seen as preceding or following arousal appeared
to be the nature of the relationship. One man, whose sexual script emphasised the erotic (see
Mitchell et al., 2011), described the sequencing of desire and arousal in the context of the
social distance between the self and the object of desire. He saw arousal as subsumed by
desire when the person is less familiar:
Man aged 55: Arousal on that scale is not there with someone that you just don’t
know, because that’s instantaneous. The arousal’s gone in desire, it doesn’t come into
it – that is just pure sexual desire.
By contrast, this man felt that with a familiar partner, a gradual build up of arousal leads to
desire, rather than the reverse:
P: “... you know the person – you feel more at ease with them and I’d include
foreplay but not a sexual foreplay. It’s the evening – making the dinner, sitting down
just touching the person, and sitting there just talking to them. You’re looking at
them and maybe you just touch them, so you’re just slowly, slowly building up to it.
I: Would [desire and arousal] happen at the same time or one before the other, or in
different circumstances?
P: I think the arousal could go on through the evening, and then maybe you just walk
into the bedroom and she might just drop whatever and be stood there – stocking and
suspenders. For my age that was the normal, and that is how I picture most women –
to be like that. Then you get total sexual desire for the person because you’re totally
aroused.
I: So, in a way, arousal comes first and then the desire?
P: Yes – in a long-term relationship. The desire is the erection; the arousal is
making sure everything’s alright beforehand; you might not have an erection but that
you know that something’s going on chemically inside yourself. (Emphasis added)
[man aged 55]
Mind/body engagement
Desire as mind, arousal as body
11
11
Participants frequently defined desire and arousal in terms of what was happening both in the
mind and body. Where participants distinguished between the two, desire tended to be
associated with the mind (thinking about or wanting sex) and arousal with body (physical
signs and sensations). However, the distinction was often not clear-cut:
Woman aged 34: Desire I suppose is, not always, but it’s more of a mental process
and arousal is more of a physical thing. It’s not a strict demarcation but it’s
[something like that].
The “thinking” words that participants associated with desire often signified motivation and
aspiration (“wanting”, “fancy”, “liking”). In contrast, arousal was associated with an
embodied “altered state”; an “awakening of the senses” (man aged 56); and “a state of
perpetual readiness for sex” (woman aged 51). Several participants described being “turned
on,” a term that suggests a “switch” or a move to a new physical state. In contrast to desire,
arousal was associated with a set of distinct physical signs, of which the most salient were an
erection (for a man), and lubrication (for a woman):
Woman aged 27: For a man it’s getting an erection. For a woman it’s getting
lubricated. For both it’s… I don’t know, it’s everything else that happens –
quickened heart beat, and that kind of thing.
The woman quoted below describes both a change of state as well as distinct physical signs:
Woman aged 54: I suppose you feel some sort of inner tingling or whatever, if you
want to be very exact about it – you do. Your lips start to swell a little bit and all
these changes, but I think it’s a general openness in your whole stance…...
This woman expressed frustration at being asked to describe signs of arousal as she assumed
there was a normative set with which the interviewer would be familiar. She was surprised to
learn that the signs varied from one person to another. For some men, the erection became
the definitive sign, without which it was not possible to “achieve” arousal:
P: Oh well, of course in a man [the sign of arousal is] primarily erection isn’t it,
12
12
which of course I can’t do.
I: Would you say that you’re no longer able to feel sexually aroused then?
P: Not properly.
[man aged 78]
Desire as body; arousal as mind
At times this distinction was reversed: desire was associated with body and action, and
arousal with mind and thinking. Desire was described by one man as “related to the loins […]
you can look at a woman and you maybe get a tingling around there…” (man aged 55).
Desire was associated with an altered embodied state: “something that wells up in you and
one gets into a heightened state” (woman aged 64). And the converse, a loss of desire, was
described by one man as “the loss of a physical drive, so it’s your body telling you that you
want to have sex” (man aged 39, emphasis added).
On the other hand, arousal was described as an altered state of mind: “a slight loss of
rational thinking, inability to concentrate on other things…” (man aged 39), or as an
aspiration, originating in the mind:
I: […] how do you know if you’re feeling sexually aroused?
P: Something happens in you so you feel that “God, actually I wouldn’t mind some
of that”.
I: So it’s a thought – it’s a thinking thing?
P: Yes it’s always in the mind isn’t it? It starts from the mind doesn’t it? I think it
starts in the mind.
[man aged 65]
Mind-body - blurred distinction
The accounts showed that the distinction between mind and body was not always easily made
For instance, terms such as “urge” and “impulse” were often used in association with sexual
desire and an urge might originate from the mind or the body or both together. Arousal was
also sometimes described as of both mind and body: “[you know you’re aroused because] the
13
13
whole body is telling you – your mind, everything.” (woman aged 38). And when asked to
describe the signs of arousal, some definitions included both mind (aspirations/motivations)
as well as bodily signs:
I: What are the signs of that physical arousal?
P: Quickening heart rates, am…pure excitement, in some cases erectile function, a
draw towards another person – a real sort of want of that person ….
[man aged 48]
The view that both mind and body are simultaneously engaged is nicely illustrated by this
woman:
Woman aged 47: Sexual desire is physically thinking “I would like to have sex”
Motivational/emotional states
This final criterion relates to the extent to which desire and arousal are motivated
towards a goal, and the extent to which they are felt spontaneously or arise in response to
cues. These criteria were used, not so much to distinguish between the terms but to define
each one. Indeed, desire and arousal were usually both viewed as responsive and
motivational, and the distinctions between them on this criterion were particularly blurred.
Desire and arousal as responsive and motivational
Participants frequently talked about desire and arousal as responsive (a response to a person
or stimulus) and motivational (orientated towards a goal). Desire was often described as a
response to a cue, such as an invitation from an intimate partner:
Woman aged 64: I think sexual desire is all to do with having a loving partner who
wants to touch you and when that person touches you, you experience sexual desire.
Equally, desire could be triggered in response to a stranger whose attributes or behaviour are
14
14
attractive: “there can be a desire just with the movement of her bottom, the way she walks, or
the way she smiles…” (man aged 55). The stranger here does not necessarily intend to trigger
this desire, and although the felt desire may be motivational, it may be accompanied by an
acceptance that these feelings will not translate into action.
Desire was often defined in motivational terms (“wanting to have sex”; man aged 56).
The motivation might be orientated towards the physical rewards of sex (“a keenness to have
sex with penetration”; man aged 60); or might pertain to a relational goal (“there is this desire
to disappear into each other which I think is very important”; man aged 61). Arousal was also
described as a response. Again the trigger might come from a partner or from some subtle
attribute of a stranger (“something she’s wearing or the way she sits, the way she is playing
or her smile”; man aged 65).
Like desire, arousal was expressed as a motivational state, orientated towards a
physical goal (“getting to the point where they feel like they want to have sex”; man aged
60); or a relational goal (“a real sort of want of that person”; man aged 48). But the
motivation was often expressed with more urgency: “It’s an urgency of wanting to go to bed
with the person – either make love or have sex with them” (man aged 55).
Comparisons between the two terms sometimes depicted arousal as a response to a
partner or someone known well and desire as an instant and fleeting response to a stranger or
stimulus (such as an image). There was a sense in which arousal required more than just an
image; there needed to be “something to continue your desire” (man aged 65). As one woman
put it:
P: You see I wouldn’t think pictures of someone like Lucian Freud… they’re sexy but
they wouldn’t make me feel sexy. Sometimes I think you can see someone and you
can think “Cor blimey!” – just a glance or… a type of person.
I: Would that be desire or arousal?
P: That would be desire I think.
I: So when would the arousal…? What would need to be there for arousal to
15
15
happen?
P: Well I think you’d have to know that person a bit deeper.
[Woman aged 51]
This association of arousal with someone known well was, however, by no means
universal. On several occasions, a sexual response to erotic images in magazines or scenes in
films was described in terms of arousal:
Woman aged 51: Have you ever seen that film ‘Don’t Look Now’? […]. There’s an
immensely erotic scene. That aroused me.
Desire as not motivational and not responsive
Desire was also talked about as an emotional state or an ongoing appetite that was
neither goal oriented (motivational) nor responsive. When talking about desire in this way,
participants used terms such as need, drive and urges.
Several participants talked about sexual desire in terms of a libido or drive, an
ongoing appetite, need or sense: “It’s like the sixth sense to me. You’ve got your natural five
senses and you’ve also got this other need within your body, and it doesn’t do any good to try
and suppress that need totally” (man aged 48). On several occasions, sexual desire was
expressed as “one of the constituents of the partnership” (man aged 61), or as an attraction
towards another that could grow, stay steady or wane (“I lost my desire for him”; female aged
35). And finally, it was described as an affective state - a mood or feeling of comfort:
Woman aged 54: [the] problem with sex was [my] level of desire – how much you actually
felt in the mood for sex and sexuality, or felt comfortable with being a sexual being yourself.
Several participants differentiated desire from arousal using the criteria of motivation
and responsiveness. Three of these participants described arousal as motivational and
responsive and desire as neither. One man contrasted his view of desire as an appetite with
his view of arousal, which he saw as of the moment and relating to a specific encounter:
16
16
Man aged 39: I think if I could make a distinction, it would be the drive is the long
term… The arousal is the short-term case-by-case based situation you are in, on any
given night, and trying to build up these feelings. [ Pause]…
Another woman viewed arousal as a response to a person, but desire as an emotional state
that could arise in the absence of a partner:
Woman aged 46: When I’m with somebody and they kiss and cuddle you, then you
get sexually aroused, but you can desire sex without that. You can be on your own
and feel desire.
Arousal as not motivational and not responsive
Arousal was talked about as not motivational and not responsive far less often than
desire. Occasionally, arousal was described as detached from a specific person or context. For
instance, it was described by two participants as integral to the relationship; and also as a
feeling of satisfaction with a partner (woman aged 23). For some men, the experience of a
waking erection contributed towards a view of arousal as not motivational and not
responsive:
Man aged 52: I think if you’re aroused, or you get aroused, I think it’s a natural
thing, a thing that happens on its own. It needn’t necessarily be anything to do with
sex. You can be aroused when you wake up in the morning and you can be on your
own, it doesn’t necessarily mean that you want sex.
Discussion
We found that both men and women had difficulty making a distinction between
desire and arousal although they could usually do so after some thought. This is consistent
with the qualitative findings of Brotto et al. (2009), who reported that women had difficulty
distinguishing between desire and arousal, and achieved greater clarity in their definitions
after thoughtful reflection. Consistent with Janssen and colleagues (2008), we found that men
17
17
also struggled to disentangle the terms or gave definitions that differed from the expert view.
We identified three criteria that individuals used to define sexual desire and arousal or
make a distinction between the two: temporal sequence, mind/body engagement, and
motivation/responsiveness. Participants commonly felt that desire preceded arousal; some felt
that desire was ‘mind’ and arousal ‘body’; and many felt that both desire and arousal were
responsive and motivational. However, for each of the criteria, we identified numerous
instances of participants reversing distinctions or blurring differences between the two. The
overall picture was one of variable definitions and overlap between terms.
Several qualitative studies have supported the idea that women often do not
differentiate between sexual desire and arousal (Beck et al., 1991; Brotto et al., 2009;
Carvalheira et al., 2010; Ellison, 2000; Giles & McCabe, 2009; Graham et al., 2004;
Goldhammer & McCabe, 2011). One study found that women diagnosed with Female
Sexual Arousal Disorder (FSAD) were more likely to distinguish between desire and arousal
(Brotto et al. 2009), and women without a diagnosis were more likely to conflate the two
constructs. But a second study found the opposite: an overlap between sexual desire and
arousal was more evident among women reporting lower levels of sexual functioning (Giles
& McCabe, 2009). We did not detect any clear differences in the way that these terms were
defined across the three groups of participants, according to experience of sexual function
problems (see Table one). It is not possible to say whether this lack of difference was due to
the small and purposive sample, or whether in fact, the experience of sexual difficulties does
not change the way that individuals define these terms. Further quantitative work would be
required to explore this in detail.
Consistent with previous studies (Giles & McCabe, 2009; Sand & Fisher, 2007), we
found that a linear model of sexual response – in which desire precedes arousal – was
endorsed by some, but not all participants. We also found some support for the idea of a
18
18
circular relationship between desire and arousal (desire feeding into arousal feeding into
desire, etc.). Several studies have indicated a lack of temporal pattern in women’s accounts.
In a focus group study involving women aged 18-84 years, women sometimes reported desire
as preceding arousal, at other times following it, and also sometimes perceived sexual arousal
without any experience of sexual interest/desire (Graham et al., 2004). These latter occasions
were often linked to physical triggers, such as ovulation. In an online survey of 3,000 women,
16% of respondents reported only engaging in sexual activity if they felt sexual desire at the
outset, but almost a third of women typically or always only recognized sexual desire after
they perceived that they were aroused (Carvalheira et al. 2010).
Although it is often assumed that the distinction between sexual desire and arousal is
more clear-cut in men than in women, there have been remarkably few studies on men’s
experiences of sexual desire (Kleinplatz, 2011). Apart from studies of hypogonadal men,
problems relating to desire in men have been also been under-researched (Brotto, 2010b;
Maurice, 2007). In one of the few qualitative studies focused on male experiences of sexual
desire and arousal (Janssen et al., 2008), men aged 18-70 years participating in focus group
discussions did not consistently or easily distinguish between sexual desire and arousal. Men
also described genital arousal (i.e. an erection) as sometimes occurring independently of
(subjective) sexual arousal and, conversely, feelings of sexual excitement were reported in
situations where they did not have an erection. These findings challenge some of the
stereotypes regarding men’s sexuality (Kleinplatz, 2011) as being more “straightforward” and
genital-focused than women’s sexuality.
Further evidence for overlap between these constructs comes from studies that have
investigated the psychometric properties of self-report measures of sexual functioning among
clinical and non-clinical samples of women. These studies have documented significant
overlap between the sexual desire and arousal domains (Dennerstein & Lehert, 2004;
19
19
Graziottin et al., 2009; Leiblum et al., 2006; Taylor et al., 1994; Sidi et al., 2008). Brotto et
al. (2010) reviewed studies reporting correlations between the FSFI Desire and Arousal
domains; across six studies, correlations between .52 and .85 were observed. High
correlations between desire and arousal have also been reported in studies employing other
questionnaire measures, such as the Profile of Female Sexual Function (Leiblum et al., 2006).
In a study of Australian women that utilized the Personal Experiences Questionnaire
(Dennerstein et al., 1997), Dennerstein and Lehert (2004) found that items related to sexual
“responsiveness” could not be separated from the construct of sexual interest or desire.
Overall, our findings support the view that there is diversity in women’s and men’s
experiences of sexual response (Carvalheira et al., 2010; Janssen et al., 2008; Tiefer et al.,
2002) and that no one model fully captures this diversity. These findings are supported by a
range of studies suggesting that sexual desire and arousal are not discrete categories and are
not always experienced in a uniform, temporal sequence, as traditional linear models of
sexual response assumed.
The methodological limitations of our study relate to qualitative approaches more
broadly. Qualitative methods are suited to exploring phenomena from the perspectives of
others. Semi-structured interviews provide rich and detailed descriptions but partly because
the data generated are cumbersome, the sample sizes are generally small. Our sampling was
purposive rather than probabilistic, with the aim of generating ideas and concepts that are
transferable to other contexts, rather than results that are statistically generalizable. A further
caveat is our focus on definitions rather than experience. An individual’s definition of sexual
desire and arousal may not accurately reflect how they experience the concepts (as
professionally defined). This may be particularly true where the terms are not often used in
everyday conversation. That said, participants often defined the terms by describing their
own experiences. And, for the purpose of achieving accuracy and reliability in clinical
20
20
diagnosis and research, an understanding of how individuals define these terms is important
in its own right.
So what can we conclude from these findings? Our results have implications for
clinical consultations. They imply that a shared meaning of desire and arousal between
professional and patient cannot be assumed. Lack of shared understanding may unwittingly
hamper the accuracy of history taking and diagnosis. This is most likely to be an issue during
brief consultations where time does not permit discussion of meanings. Our findings are also
important for epidemiological research, particularly because of the reliance on self-report to
measure sexual dysfunction. There is good evidence that bias arises where participants
misunderstand terms, or understand them differently to the meaning intended by researchers
(Catania, 1996; Tourangeau, 1996). Localized usage of terms may be particularly important
here. For instance, a term such as “turned on” which is often employed in questionnaires as a
vernacular term for arousal, may have a different meaning among U.S. and British English
speakers.
Finally, these findings have implications for diagnostic classification of desire and arousal
disorders. We believe that the blurred distinction between these concepts among many lay
people provides further support to recent proposals to merge the two diagnostic categories of
Female Sexual Arousal Disorder and Hyposexual Desire Disorder into a new diagnostic
category (“Sexual Interest/Arousal Disorder”) for the upcoming revision of the DSM (DSM-
5) (Brotto, 2010a and b; Brotto et al., 2010; Graham, 2010). Because of the limited literature
on men’s sexual desire, the proposed DSM-5 criteria for diagnosing low desire in men are
largely preserved from the previous edition of the DSM (DSM-IV-TR; APA, 2000). We
found that men as well as women found difficulty distinguishing between desire and arousal.
We hope that in future, the literature on men’s desire will develop sufficiently to permit a full
re-appraisal of the diagnostic categories for men. Certainly, our data suggests that such an
21
21
appraisal is warranted.
22
22
References
Ågmo, A. (2011). On the intricate relationship between sexual motivation and arousal.
Hormones and Behavior, 59, 681-688.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
Bancroft, J. (2005). The endocrinology of sexual arousal. Journal of Endocrinology, 186,
411-427.
Bancroft, J., Loftus, J., & Long, J. S. (2003). Distress about sex: A national survey of women
in heterosexual relationships. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32, 193-208.
Basson, R. (2000). The female sexual response: A different model. Journal of Sex and
Marital Therapy, 26, 51-64.
Basson, R. (2001). Human sex response cycles. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy, 27, 33–
43.
Beck, J. G., Bozman, A. W., & Qualtrough, T. (1991). The experience of sexual desire:
Psychological correlates in a college sample. Journal of Sex Research, 28, 443-456.
Brotto, L. A., Heiman, J. R., & Tolman, D. L. (2009). Narratives of desire in mid-age women
with and without arousal difficulties. Journal of Sex Research, 46, 387-398.
Brotto, L. A. (2010a). The DSM diagnostic criteria for hypoactive sexual desire disorder in
women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 221-239.
Brotto, L. (2010b). The DSM diagnostic criteria for hypoactive sexual desire disorder in men.
Journal of Sexual Medicine, 7, 2015-2030.
23
23
Brotto, L. A., Graham, C. A., Binik, Y. M., & Segraves, R. T. (2011). Should sexual desire
and arousal disorders in women be merged? A response to DeRogatis, Clayton,
Rosen, Sand, and Pyke (2010). Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40, 221-225.
Binik, Y. M., Brotto, L. A., Graham, C. A., & Segraves, R. T. (2010). Response of the DSM-
5 Sexual Dysfunctions Subworkgroup to commentaries published in the Journal of
Sexual Medicine. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 7, 2382-2387.
Carvalheira, A., Brotto, L. A., & Maroco, J. (2010). Portuguese version of Cues for Sexual
Desire Scale: The influence of relationship duration. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 8,
123-131.
Charmaz, K. (2006) Constructing grounded theory: A practical guide through qualitative
analysis. London: SAGE Publications Ltd.
Catania, J., Binson, D., Canchola, J., Pollack, L., Huack, W. and Coates, T. (1996). Effects of
interviewer gender, interviewer choice and item wording on responses to questions
concerning sexual behaviour. Public Opinion Quarterly, 60, 345-375.
Dennerstein, L., & Lehert, P. (2004). Modeling mid-aged women’s sexual functioning: A
prospective population-based study. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy, 30, 173-183.
Dennerstein, L., Dudley, E., Hopper, J., & Burger, H. (1997). Sexuality, hormones, and the
menopausal transition. Maturitas, 26, 83-93.
Ellison, C. (2000). Women’s sexualities. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
Furnham, A. (1988). Lay theories: everyday understanding of problems in the social
sciences. Elmsford, N. Y.: Pergamon Press.
Giles, K.R., & McCabe, M.P. (2009). Conceptualizing women’s sexual function: linear vs.
circular models of sexual response. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 6, 2761-2771.
24
24
Goldhammer, D. & McCabe, M. (2011) A qualitative exploration of the meaning and
experience of sexual desire among partnered women. Canadian Journal of Human
Sexuality, 20 (1-2), 19-29.
Graham, C. A., Sanders, S. A., Milhausen, R., & McBride, K. (2004). Turning on and turning
off: A focus group study of the factors that affect women’s sexual arousal. Archives
of Sexual Behavior, 33, 527-538.
Graham, C. A. (2010). Review of the DSM diagnostic criteria for Female Sexual Arousal
Disorder. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 240-255.
Graziottin, A., Koochaki, P. E., Rodenberg, C. A., & Dennerstein, L. (2009). The prevalence
of hypoactive sexual desire disorder in surgically menopausal women: An
epidemiological study of women in four European countries. Journal of Sexual
Medicine, 6, 2143-2153.
Hayes, R. D. (2011). Circular and linear modelling of female sexual desire and arousal.
Journal of Sex Research, 48, 130-141.
Janssen, E., McBride, K., Yarber, W., Hill, B., & Butler, S. (2008). Factors that influence
sexual arousal in men: A focus group study. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 252-65.
Janssen, E. (2011). Sexual arousal in men: A review and conceptual analysis. Hormones and
Behavior, 59, 708-716.
Kaplan, H. S. (1979). Disorders of sexual desire. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
King, M., Holt, V., & Nazareth, I. (2007). Women’s views of their sexual difficulties:
Agreement and disagreement with clinical diagnoses. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
36, 281-288.
Kleinplatz, P. (2011). Arousal and desire problems: conceptual, research and clinical
25
25
considerations or the more things change the more they stay the same. Sexual and
Relationship Therapy, 26, 3-15.
Laan, E., & Everaerd, W. (1995). Determinants of female sexual arousal:
Psychophysiological theory and data. Annual Review of Sex Research, 6, 32-76.
Laan, E., & Jansssen, E. (2007). How do men and women feel? Determinants of subjective
experience of sexual arousal. In E. Janssen (Ed.), The psychophysiology of sex (pp.
278-290). Bloomington, Indiana: Indiana University Press.
Laan, E., & Both, S. (2008). What makes women experience desire? Feminism &
Psychology, 18, 505–514.
Leiblum, S. R., Koochaki, P. E., Rodenberg, C.A., Barton, I. P., & Rosen, R. C. (2006).
Hypoactive sexual desire disorder in postmenopausal women: US results from the
Women’s International Study of Health and Sexuality (WISHeS). Menopause, 13,
46–56.
Levin, R.J. (2008). Critically revisiting aspects of the human sexual response cycle of
Masters and Johnson: Correcting errors and suggesting modifications. Sex and
Marital Therapy, 23, 393-9.
Levine, S. B. (2003). The nature of sexual desire: A clinician’s perspective. Archives of
Sexual Behavior, 32, 279-285.
Lief, H. (1977). Inhibited sexual desire. Medical Aspects of Human Sexuality, 7, 94–95.
Maurice, W. L. (2007). Sexual desire disorders in men. In S. R. Leiblum (Ed.), Principles
and practice of sex therapy (4th ed., pp. 181–211). New York: Guilford.
Meana, M. (2010). Elucidating women’s (hetero)sexual desire: Definitional challenges and
26
26
content expansion. Journal of Sex Research, 47, 104-122.
Mitchell, K., & Graham, C. A. (2008). Two challenges for the classification of sexual
dysfunction. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 5, 1552–1558.
Mitchell, K., Wellings, K., King, M., Nazareth, I., Mercer, C. & Johnson, A. (2011).
Scripting sexual function. Sociology of Health and Illness, 33(4), 540-553
Regan, P. C., & Berscheid, E. (1999). Lust: What we know about human sexual desire.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Ritchie, J. & Lewis, J. (2003). Qualitative research practice: A guide for social science
students and researchers. Thousand Oaks, C. A.: Sage Publications.
Sand, M., & Fisher, W. A. (2007). Women’s endorsement of models of female sexual
response: The nurses’ sexuality study. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 4, 708-719.
Sidi, H., Naing, L., Midin, M., & Nik Jaafar, N. R. (2008). The female response cycle: Do
Malaysian women conform to the circular model? Journal of Sexual Medicine, 5,
2359–2366.
Taylor, J. F., Rosen, R. C., & Leiblum, S. R. (1994). Self-report assessment of female sexual
function: Psychometric evaluation of the Brief Index of Sexual Function for Women.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 23, 627-643.
Tiefer, L. (1991). Historical, scientific, clinical and feminist criticisms of “The Human
Sexual Response Cycle” model. Annual Review of Sex Research, 2, 1-24.
Tiefer, L., Hall, M., & Travis, C. (2002). Beyond dysfunction: A new view of women’s
sexual problems. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy, 28, 225-232.
Toates, F. (2009). An integrative theoretical framework for understanding sexual arousal,
27
27
motivation, and behavior. Journal of Sex Research, 46, 168-193.
Tourangeau, R. and Smith, T. (1996). Asking sensitive questions: The impact of data
collection mode, question format and question context. Public Opinion Quarterly, 60,
275-304.
28
28
... [1] Eff ect of an edu cat ion al ... [2] Soc ioe con om ic im pac t ... [3] Hy per ten sio n [4] Hy per ten sio n is a ... [5] Blu m in hyp ert ens ion ... [6] Ma in Res ult s of ... [7] Sys tem atic rev iew ... [8] Sel f-rep ort ed pre val enc e ... [9] Soc ial dis par itie s ... [10 ] Pre val enc e and ... [11 ] Th e rel atio nsh ip ... [12 ] Beh avi our al ris k ... [ [27 ] Ob esi ty, hyp ert ens ion , and ... [28 ] Rel atio nsh ip bet we en ... [29 ] Th e rel atio nsh ip bet we en ... [30 ] Fac tor s ass oci ate d ... [31 ] Inc ide nce of hyp ert ens ion ... [32 ] Sal t con sum pti on ... [33 ] Ris k fac tor s rel ate d ... [34 ] Cor rel atio n of foo d ... [35 ] Th e rel atio nsh ip bet we en ... [36 ] Fac tor s affe ctin g ... [37 ] Do mi nan t fac tor s in ... [38 ] Th e rel atio nsh ip bet we en ... ...
... Ke yw ord s SAR S-C oV-2; Cen tra l Ner vou s Sys tem Dis ord ers ; Cer ebr al Hy pox ia; Hy pox ia; Mo rta lity [1] Pos t-C OV ID pai n ... [2] Th e mo der n org ani zat ion al ... [3] Mo lec ula r and clin ica l ... [4] Min i-per cut ane ous nep hro lith oto my ... [5] Mo dul ar poi ntofcar e ... [6] Th e mo difi ed pro ced ure ... [7] Tox ic me tab olic ... [8] En han cin g nut riti onrel ate d ... [9] Me tab olic enc eph alo pat hie s ... [10 ] Con ser vat ive ver sus ... [11 ] Lib era l or con ser vat ive ... [12 ] ...
... In addition to sexual function, sexual desire can also be affected by pandemic conditions. The starting point of sexual function and the necessity of sexual arousal is sexual desire, which is a set of forces and desires that lead a person to or away from sexual behaviors [9] . One of the factors affecting sexual desire is psychological factors [10] . ...
Article
Full-text available
... Only a few studies have investigated the association between mood and sexual desire. For this reason, we include in the overview below studies on both sexual desire and subjective arousal, as these concepts often overlap (e.g., Beck et al., 1991;Graham, 2010;Mitchell et al., 2014;Prause et al., 2008). Research regarding mood and sexual desire, often set in laboratory settings, suggests that people experience increased sexual desire when they are in a positive mood, but mostly found no association with negative mood (Mitchell et al., 1998;Peterson & Janssen, 2007;Ter Kuile et al., 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
The association between mood and sexual desire has been the object of significant scientific and public interest. How mood shapes and is shaped by sexual desire is typically studied within one and the same individual, yet sexual desire is often experienced in the context of a romantic relationship. To obtain a more complete picture of the relation between mood and sexual desire, we examined the temporal interplay between mood and sexual desire both within and between partners in a romantic relationship. Using data from an experience sampling study involving both partners of mixed-gender romantic couples (N = 188; Mage = 26.34, SDage = 5.33), we investigated how each partner’s mood (in terms of positive and negative affect) predicted their own sexual desire as well as that of their partner and vice versa. Results of both concurrent and temporal analyses confirmed bidirectional associations between mood and sexual desire both within and between partners, such that (1) both a person’s own and their partner’s positive mood predicted an increase in sexual desire, and a person’s own and their partner’s negative mood predicted a decrease in sexual desire. In addition, (2) both a person’s own and their partner’s sexual desire predicted an increase in positive mood, and a person’s own and their partner’s sexual desire predicted a decrease in negative mood. Only few gender differences were found. The results underscore how sexual desire can predict and be predicted by both romantic partners’ mood, highlighting the need for interactional models of sexual desire.
... Besides, emotional symptoms can either strengthen or weaken a person's feelings of sexual arousal and desire, 62,63 which are closely related to sexual functioning. 64 The evidence for the effects of body image on sexual functioning is relatively clear. Just as the findings of a qualitative study, poor self-image hampered the development of intimate relationships among CCSs. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Childhood Cancer Survivors (CCSs) are more likely to report sexual dysfunction than people without cancer history. Sexual functioning encompasses more than just sexual dysfunction. The scarcity of information regarding the status and influencing factors of sexual functioning in CCSs, hampers to devise suitable screening or interventions. This review aims to summarize research progress on sexual functioning and associated factors among CCSs.
... The diagnosis of SIAD replaces previous diagnostic categories: hypoactive sexual desire disorder (HSDD) and female sexual arousal disorder (FSAD; APA 2000). Of note, there is also emerging evidence that desire and arousal concerns in men also share some overlap (Mitchell et al. 2014). ...
... There also appears to be greater engagement in noncoital sexual relations [3]. Some authors concluded that sexual desire could be described as a motivational state [7,8], and pregnant women may avoid sexual intercourse or inevitably suffer from many problems in their sexual lives [1]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives: Pregnancy is a stage in which different physical and psychological changes take place that can affect the sexuality of the couple. The aim of the study is to identify how the physical and psychological changes derived from pregnancy affect the sexual desire of women and men. Methods: A systematic review of the literature was carried out in five databases, from which a total of 16,126 documents were obtained. After applying the PRISMA selection criteria, a total of 19 documents were selected. Results: Levels of sexual desire fluctuate during pregnancy, being the second trimester of gestation the period in which desire is at its highest and in which physical limitations and emotional changes decrease. Women have lower levels of sexual desire in the first trimester, while men have the lowest levels of desire in the third trimester. Conclusion: Pregnancy is a stage marked by physiological and psychological changes that modify several areas, including sexuality. Healthcare professionals should promote a healthy sexuality, avoiding the appearance of fears or sexual dysfunctions caused by the changes that occur during pregnancy.
... Each item was scored on a 5-point Likert scale (from always = 0 to never = 4). Questions of 1, 3,5,7,8,9,12,13,17,18,19,20 were scored in reverse. The minimum possible score was 0 and the maximum was 100, and a higher score indicated higher sensory cohesion (sexual desire). ...
Article
Full-text available
Aims: The Covid-19 pandemic has adversely affected various dimensions of life and lifestyles, and sexual life is no exception.The present study aimed to determine the effect of multimedia sexual index promotion packages on the sexual function, desire, and satisfaction of couples during the outbreak of Covid-19. Methods: In this randomized clinical trial study, 60 married people in Qazvin, Iran participated, in 2022. Participants were randomly assigned to two groups control and intervention. This was a single-blind study. A demographic questionnaire, Larson sexual satisfaction, Hurlbert Index of Sexual Desire, International Index of Erectile Function (IIEF), and Female Sexual Function Index (FSFI) was used to collect data. The Chi-square, independent t-test, Mann-Whitney, paired t-test, and Wilcoxon test were used for data analysis. Findings: The results showed that in the intervention group, during the pre-test and post-test stages, the mean and standard deviation of sexual function, desire, and satisfaction through the intragroup test were significantly different (p <0.05). Only in sexual desire, this difference was also observed in the control group (p <0.05). Conclusion: The results indicated a positive and significant effect of the multimedia sexual index promotion package on the sexual function, desire, and satisfaction of couples. Now that the Covid-19 pandemic has adversely affected people's sexual lives, such targeted interventions seem necessary.
... The diagnosis of SIAD replaces previous diagnostic categories: hypoactive sexual desire disorder (HSDD) and female sexual arousal disorder (FSAD; APA 2000). Of note, there is also emerging evidence that desire and arousal concerns in men also share some overlap (Mitchell, Wellings, & Graham, 2014). ...
... When repeatedly shown the same sexual stimuli, women and men showed a similar pattern of decline in their subjective arousal responses (Dawson et al., 2013). Subjective arousal is related to desire in that both can refer to sexual interest (Mitchell et al., 2014). These results suggest that short-term changes in arousal (and perhaps desire) in response to sexual stimuli may be similar for women and men. ...
Article
Full-text available
There is a lay assumption that women’s sexual desire varies substantially over time, whereas men’s is stable. This assumption is mirrored in prominent theories of desire, which posit that women are more variable than men in the extent to which they desire sex, and that women’s sexual desire is more contextually sensitive than men’s. We tested this assumption across three longitudinal studies. Study 1 assessed desire at 3 time points spanning 13 years ( N observations = 5562), and Studies 2 and 3 ( N observations = 11,282) assessed desire moment-to-moment over 7 days. When desire was measured over years, women were more variable in their sexual desire than men (Study 1). However, we found a different pattern of results when desire was measured over the short term. In Studies 2 and 3, we found no significant differences in women’s and men’s desire variability. The extent to which desire varied as a function of affective states (e.g., happiness) and relationship-oriented states (e.g., partner closeness) was similar for women and men, with some exceptions; women’s desire was more negatively associated with tiredness and anger in Study 2. These data qualify existing assumptions about sex differences in sexual desire variability.
Article
Full-text available
This qualitative study explored the meaning and experience of sexual desire for women. Data were gathered through semi-structured interviews with 40 partnered heterosexual women aged 20 to 61 years drawn from the general population. Thematic analysis of the interview transcripts indicated that the participants understood and experienced their sexual desire primarily within the context of their partner relationships and most frequently reported responsive rather than autonomous experiences of sexual desire. The implications of the study findings are discussed in relation to the definition, classification, and treatment of sexual desire disorders in women.
Article
Full-text available
We examined factors influencing responses to questions on sexual behavior among adult respondents 18-49 years old (unweighted N = 2,030) obtained through a random-digit dialing survey. Based on self-disclosure and perceived control theory, we hypothesized that giving people a choice in selecting the gender of their interviewer rather than being assigned an interviewer, and using questions that are ''supportive'' of what may be perceived of as nonnormative behavior (enhanced items), would increase data quality relative to, respectively, matched- or opposite-gender interviewer conditions and standard worded items, The enhanced items facilitated responding to a number of sensitive topics, However, the effects of item wording on item response are often mediated by interviewer conditions. The ''choice'' results suggest that giving respondents greater control decreases question threat. However, the overall findings argue for matching respondents and interviewers on gender over opposite-gender interviewers or allowing respondents to select their interviewer's gender. Wording and interviewer manipulations reduced the discrepancies between men's and women's self-reports of sexual behavior, but they did not eliminate them, and in some cases they had no effect. The present findings suggest that males tend to be influenced by variations in item wording, interviewer gender, and respondent control across a somewhat wider range of sexual topics. In general, the findings recommend matching respondents and interviewers on gender and the use of more supportive wording in sexual behavior questions. However, for assessment of some topics (e.g., sexual violence) in particular segments of the population (e.g., men), other procedures, such as increasing respondent control, may be a better choice. Overall, the data support the view that in terms of preferred procedures, not all sexual topics are created equal.
Article
Full-text available
This study compared three methods of collecting survey data about sexual behaviors and other sensitive topics: computer-assisted personal interviewing (CAPI), computer-assisted self-administered interviewing (CASI), and audio computer-assisted self-administered interviewing (ACASI). Interviews were conducted with an area probability sample of more than 300 adults in Cook County, Illinois. The experiment also compared open and closed questions about the number of sex partners and varied the context in which the sex partner items were embedded. The three mode groups did not differ in response rates, but the mode of data collection did affect the level of reporting of sensitive behaviors: both forms of self-administration tended to reduce the disparity between men and women in the number of sex partners reported. Self-admimstration, especially via ACASI, also increased the proportion of respondents admitting that they had used illicit drugs. In addition, when the closed answer options emphasized the low end of the distribution, fewer sex partners were reported than when the options emphasized the high end of the distribution; responses to the open-ended versions of the sex partner items generally fell between responses to the two closed versions.
Article
Full-text available
What makes women experience sexual desire? According to Kaplan, normal sexual response starts with desire, progresses through excitement or arousal, and ends with orgasm (Kaplan, 1974). This model implies that sexual desire is something you either have or don't have, and, if you don't have it, there may be no sex in your future. This drive model of sexual desire, a biomedical model, assumes that lack of desire represents a deviation from a physiological norm. In contrast, the biopsychosocial model promoted by the New View emphasizes that a woman's sexuality is largely produced by her social context and rejects the idea of desire norms rooted in physiology. The incentive motivation model of sexual desire that we will present here is based on new findings suggesting that the experience of desire may follow rather than precede sexual excitement, and suggests that desire emerges following sexual arousal initiated by a sexually meaningful stimulus.
Article
Article
This study provides an example of how feminist psychology can bridge qualitative and quantitative methods while keeping lived experience at the center of an inquiry. The goal of the study was to begin to understand adolescent girls' experiences of sexual desire. We describe three separate and synergistically related analyses of interviews with 30 adolescent girls. We begin with a qualitative analysis of their voiced experiences of sexual desire; follow with a quantitative analysis of the differences in how urban and suburban girls describe these experiences, assessing the role of reported sexual violation; and conclude with a second qualitative analysis exploring the interaction between social location and reported sexual violation. These three analyses enabled us to understand qualitatively and to quantify interrelated dimensions of desire as described by adolescent girls.
Article
It seems surprising that there should be so much controversy surrounding sexual arousal and desire. How much is sufficient? How much is too much? What is the relationship between arousal and desire? Is there a difference between sexual arousal and/or desire in men versus women? Some of the conceptual, research and clinical considerations for understanding our evolving views of sexual arousal and desire problems are discussed below. This article will begin with a brief resumé of the history of classification of arousal and desire problems, will continue by grappling with the intermingling of arousal and desire and will conclude by discussing changes in the conceptual context surrounding arousal and desire over the last 25 years. Please note that, unfortunately, there is a pronounced tendency in the literature to focus on low desire in women and overlook low desire in men, let alone gay men, except as curiosities and anomalies (Maurice, 2007). This may stem from broader social norms that define "sex" as penis-in-vagina intercourse, the implications of which are explored below. It is regrettable that this article is therefore limited by the heterosexist bias in the literature
Article
Despite the publication of various conceptual criticisms and suggested modifications in response to the human sexual response cycle described in the Masters and Johnson 196618. Masters , W. H. and Johnson , V. E. 1966 . Human sexual response , 1st ed. , Boston : Little, Brown & Company . View all references book, Human Sexual Response, incorrect or unexplained aspects of their reports on specific mechanisms involved in the sexual arousal process have been left uncorrected or undiscussed. The present article examines a number of these and proposes corrections or modifications to rectify the faults and omissions.