ArticlePDF Available

Distinguishing Healthy Adults From People With Social Anxiety Disorder: Evidence for the Value of Experiential Avoidance and Positive Emotions in Everyday Social Interactions

Authors:

Abstract

Despite the increased attention that researchers have paid to social anxiety disorder (SAD), compared with other anxiety and mood disorders, relatively little is known about the emotional and social factors that distinguish individuals who meet diagnostic criteria from those who do not. In this study, participants with and without a diagnosis of SAD (generalized subtype) described their daily face-to-face social interactions for 2 weeks using handheld computers. We hypothesized that, compared with healthy controls, individuals diagnosed with SAD would experience fewer positive emotions, rely more on experiential avoidance (of anxiety), and have greater self-control depletion (feeling mentally and physically exhausted after socializing), after accounting for social anxiety, negative emotions, and feelings of belonging during social interactions. We found that compared with healthy controls, individuals with SAD experienced weaker positive emotions and greater experiential avoidance, but there were no differences in self-control depletion between groups. Moreover, the differences we found could not be attributed to comorbid anxiety or depressive disorders. Our results suggest that negative emotions alone do not fully distinguish normal from pathological social anxiety, and that assessing social anxiety disorder should include impairments in positive emotional experiences and dysfunctional emotion regulation (in the form of experiential avoidance) in social situations. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2013 APA, all rights reserved).
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
Distinguishing Healthy Adults From People With Social
Anxiety Disorder: Evidence for the Value of Experiential
Avoidance and Positive Emotions in Everyday Social
Interactions
Todd B. Kashdan, Antonina S. Farmer, Leah M. Adams, Patty Ferssizidis, Patrick E. McKnight,
and John B. Nezlek
Online First Publication, July 1, 2013. doi: 10.1037/a0032733
CITATION
Kashdan, T. B., Farmer, A. S., Adams, L. M., Ferssizidis, P., McKnight, P. E., & Nezlek, J. B.
(2013, July 1). Distinguishing Healthy Adults From People With Social Anxiety Disorder:
Evidence for the Value of Experiential Avoidance and Positive Emotions in Everyday Social
Interactions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Advance online publication. doi:
10.1037/a0032733
Distinguishing Healthy Adults From People With Social Anxiety Disorder:
Evidence for the Value of Experiential Avoidance and Positive Emotions
in Everyday Social Interactions
Todd B. Kashdan, Antonina S. Farmer,
Leah M. Adams, Patty Ferssizidis,
and Patrick E. McKnight
George Mason University
John B. Nezlek
College of William and Mary, University of Social Sciences and
Humanities, Faculty in Pozna
´
n, Poland
Despite the increased attention that researchers have paid to social anxiety disorder (SAD), compared
with other anxiety and mood disorders, relatively little is known about the emotional and social factors
that distinguish individuals who meet diagnostic criteria from those who do not. In this study, participants
with and without a diagnosis of SAD (generalized subtype) described their daily face-to-face social
interactions for 2 weeks using handheld computers. We hypothesized that, compared with healthy
controls, individuals diagnosed with SAD would experience fewer positive emotions, rely more on
experiential avoidance (of anxiety), and have greater self-control depletion (feeling mentally and
physically exhausted after socializing), after accounting for social anxiety, negative emotions, and
feelings of belonging during social interactions. We found that compared with healthy controls, indi-
viduals with SAD experienced weaker positive emotions and greater experiential avoidance, but there
were no differences in self-control depletion between groups. Moreover, the differences we found could
not be attributed to comorbid anxiety or depressive disorders. Our results suggest that negative emotions
alone do not fully distinguish normal from pathological social anxiety, and that assessing social anxiety
disorder should include impairments in positive emotional experiences and dysfunctional emotion
regulation (in the form of experiential avoidance) in social situations.
Keywords: social anxiety disorder, experiential avoidance, positive emotions, experience sampling
Social anxiety disorder (SAD) is characterized by a persistent
fear of scrutiny by others due to the belief that such scrutiny will
lead to negative evaluation and rejection (Clark & Wells, 1995;
Heimberg, Brozovich, & Rapee, 2010). Researchers have com-
pared people with SAD and those without a diagnosis based on
self-reported negative emotions (e.g., Watson, Clark, & Carey,
1988), physiological correlates of fear (e.g., Beidel, Turner, &
Dancu, 1985; Hofmann, Newman, Ehlers, & Roth, 1995), and
avoidant behavior during laboratory social interactions (e.g., Chen,
Ehlers, Clark, & Mansell, 2002; Heuer, Rinck, & Becker, 2007).
This research, along with expert judgment and clinical observa-
tion, led to the existing diagnostic criteria for SAD (e.g., American
Psychiatric Association, 2000).
Most researchers distinguish the socially relevant anxious states
of those with SAD from those without a diagnosis based upon the
intensity, frequency, and duration of anxiety symptoms (Heim-
berg, Mueller, Holt, Hope, & Liebowitz, 1992; Mennin et al.,
2002). Compared with those without SAD, people with SAD are
anxious more often, and these feelings are stronger and last longer
when they occur. Nevertheless, these criteria do not take into
account recent research about SAD, and clinicians who rely on the
existing criteria may not identify some individuals with the disor-
der. Accurate diagnostic criteria are essential to identifying, clas-
sifying, and treating individuals with SAD. In this article we
present evidence that suggests that the current criteria need to be
expanded.
Positive Emotions
Although anxiety disorders have been linked with high negative
affect, SAD is the only anxiety disorder associated with low levels
of positive affect (Brown, 2007; Watson et al., 1988). Compared
with their nonanxious peers, people with SAD expect positive
events to be less likely to occur (Gilboa-Schechtman, Franklin, &
Foa, 2000), and, in fact, studies with nonclinical samples in ev-
eryday life have shown that excessive social anxiety is associated
with a lower frequency of positive life events (Farmer & Kashdan,
2012; Kashdan et al., 2011; Kashdan & Steger, 2006). A recent
meta-analysis found that the inverse relationship between SAD
and positive emotions could not be explained by the comorbidity
Todd B. Kashdan, Antonina S. Farmer, Leah M. Adams, Patty Ferssiz-
idis, and Patrick E. McKnight, Department of Psychology, George Mason
University; John B. Nezlek, Department of Psychology, College of Wil-
liam and Mary, University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Faculty in
Pozna
´
n, Poland.
This research was supported by the NIMH R21-MH073937 and the
Center for Consciousness and Transformation at George Mason University.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Todd B.
Kashdan, Department of Psychology, MS 3F5, George Mason University,
Fairfax, VA 22030. E-mail: tkashdan@gmu.edu
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology © 2013 American Psychological Association
2013, Vol. 122, No. 3, 000 0021-843X/13/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0032733
1
between depression and SAD (Kashdan, 2007). Despite evidence
that low levels of positive emotions characterize the experiences of
people with SAD, researchers have yet to examine whether posi-
tive emotions during the course of everyday life offer incremental
validity in distinguishing individuals with and without diagnoses
of SAD above and beyond more traditional constructs.
Experiential Avoidance
Distinct from the experience of social anxiety is the regulation
of this emotional experience (Gross, Sheppes, & Urry, 2011).
Emotion regulation refers to attempts to alter the onset or experi-
ence of an emotion, or to modify the outward expression an
emotion (Cisler, Olatunji, Feldner, & Forsyth, 2010). When avoid-
ance of an anxiety provoking situation is impossible, people with
SAD may engage in experiential avoidance, as defined by efforts
to escape, avoid, alter, or conceal undesirable emotions and
thoughts (Hayes, Wilson, Gifford, Follette, & Strosahl, 1996).
Despite evidence that experiential avoidance may be an important
change mechanism in the treatment of SAD (Dalrymple & Herbert,
2007), few studies have investigated the role of experiential avoid-
ance in SAD.
Research on constructs related to experiential avoidance sup-
ports the contribution of this emotion regulation strategy to im-
paired well-being. For example, experience-sampling studies have
found people with elevated social anxiety to be more likely to
make attempts to conceal emotions (Farmer & Kashdan, 2012) and
to experience the fewest positive experiences on days when they
engage in this emotion regulation strategy (Kashdan & Steger,
2006). Furthermore, some theorists view the concealment of anx-
iety as a safety behavior that aims to minimize contact with
unpleasant experiences during social interactions (Voncken, Al-
den, & Bögels, 2006). When people with SAD rely on such safety
behaviors, they inadvertently contribute to the maintenance of their
social anxiety symptoms (Wells et al., 1995). Research is mixed as
to whether experiential avoidance decreases (Sloan, 2004) or in-
creases (Feldner, Zvolensky, Eifert, & Spira, 2003) physiological
arousal. Nevertheless, attempts to avoid or conceal anxious
thoughts and feelings in social situations hinder emotional pro-
cessing (particularly of disconfirmatory evidence), and, conse-
quently, prevent habituation to unpleasant situations (Butler et al.,
2003). Theorists have suggested that the transition from normative
to pathological social anxiety in the form of SAD occurs when
people are unwilling to experience naturally occurring anxious
feelings, and their efforts to manage anxiety are ineffective or
counterproductive (Herbert & Cardaciotto, 2005; Kashdan, Weeks,
& Savostyanova, 2011).
Expanding the Conceptualization of SAD: Experiential
Avoidance, Self-Control Depletion, and Positive
Emotions
Experimental studies have shown that experiential avoidance
increases cognitive load during stressful tasks (Hayes et al., 1996).
Rigid attempts to avoid anxious thoughts and feelings reduce
attention to competing reward cues in one’s immediate environ-
ment (Gross & John, 2003). There is reason to expect these
problems to be particularly pronounced in people with SAD,
because they are more likely to view themselves as undesirable
and inferior to interaction partners (Kashdan & Savostyanova,
2011). Indeed, the prospect of having undesirable characteristics
exposed for scrutiny by other people may initiate anxiety and
self-conscious thoughts and feelings (Moscovitch, 2009). In the
hope of protecting themselves from evaluation and reducing any
additional unpleasant thoughts and feelings, people with SAD
attempt to conceal any perceived deficiencies (Moscovitch &
Huyder, 2011) and avoid the expression of intense emotions that
might draw public exposure (Heimberg et al., 2010; Hofmann,
2007).
Engaging in cognitively demanding situations may lead to self-
control depletion, as defined by the exhaustion of a person’s
capacity to regulate attention, energy, and tolerance of distress
(Finkel et al., 2006; Vohs, Baumeister, & Ciarocco, 2005). People
have a limited amount of energy, attention, and self-control at any
given time (Baumeister, 2002), and the effort exerted by people
with SAD to manage the impressions they make on other people
during social interactions (Clark & Wells, 1995; Heimberg et al.,
2010) may drain these resources (Vohs et al., 2005). In turn,
people who have exhausted their self-regulatory resources are
likely to make decisions based on immediate rewards, such as the
temporary relief of escaping from an anxiety-provoking situation
(Baumeister & Vohs, 2007). In such a state, people with SAD may
be less able to respond to potentially rewarding social cues and less
likely to make progress toward desired goals, such as satisfying
their need to belong and achieving a sense of meaning in life
(Mallott, Maner, DeWall, & Schmidt, 2009; Stillman et al., 2009).
In effect, people with SAD not only view social situations as
particularly stressful but may also suffer from the additional stress
and exhaustion that result from relying on the counterproductive
strategy of experiential avoidance.
A growing body of research indicates that experiential avoid-
ance is associated with excessive social anxiety and that this
emotion regulation strategy might account for the deficits in pos-
itive emotions and rewarding social experiences linked to exces-
sive social anxiety (Kashdan, Barrios, Forsyth, & Steger, 2006;
Kashdan & Breen, 2008; Kashdan & Steger, 2006; Rodebaugh &
Heimberg, 2008; Rodebaugh & Shumaker, 2012). Furthermore,
devoting finite effort and energy to alleviate momentary discom-
fort through experiential avoidance might not only interfere with
enjoyment of social interactions but also contribute to self-control
depletion, and consequently steal time and effort from other plea-
surable and meaningful life pursuits. We incorporated this per-
spective on pathological social anxiety, spanning experiential
avoidance, self-control depletion, and positive emotions into a
study of how to identify individuals with SAD using a dynamic,
ecologically valid methodology.
The Present Study
The primary goal of the present study was to determine if
positive emotions, experiential avoidance, and/or self-control de-
pletion aid in classifying individuals as meeting diagnostic criteria
for the generalized subtype of SAD. Nongeneralized SAD (when
feared situations are limited to public speaking or performing in
front of other people) fails to encompass the interpersonal dynam-
ics of interest that generate positive emotions (Hughes et al.,
2006). We collected data from adults diagnosed as having the
generalized subtype of SAD and a healthy comparison group,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
2
KASHDAN ET AL.
matched on several demographic characteristics. With these two
groups, we assessed the types of events expected to be problematic
for people with SAD—social interactions during the course of
daily life. Instead of relying on retrospective reporting in surveys
or interviews, we collected event-contingent data with an
experience-sampling approach. Participants recorded face-to-face
social interactions that lasted at least 10 minutes using hand-held
electronic diaries over the course of 2 weeks. Experience sampling
provided a tool to examine social events and experiences in their
natural, spontaneous context by minimizing the amount of time
between the actual event and the recall of the event to lessen the
influence of information processing biases (Reis & Gable, 2000).
This study aimed to empirically test recent theoretical models of
SAD (Cisler et al., 2010; Heimberg et al., 2010; Kashdan et al.,
2011; Moscovitch, 2009), and to improve our understanding of the
factors responsible for the shift from normative anxiety in social
situations to the experiences of people with a SAD diagnosis. Our
primary hypothesis was that low positive emotions, an overreli-
ance on experiential avoidance, and self-control depletion in the
context of everyday social interactions, would distinguish individ-
uals with SAD from a healthy comparison group above and be-
yond the more widely studied constructs of negative emotions,
social anxiety, and feelings of belonging. To examine the speci-
ficity of these relationships, we controlled for the presence of
depressive disorders or other comorbid anxiety conditions.
Method
Participants
Our initial sample consisted of 84 participants (52 women) from
Northern Virginia. Forty-one participants were diagnosed with
Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD), generalized subtype, and 43
(51%) were a healthy control group with no psychiatric disorders.
We matched groups on gender, race, marital status, and age (see
Procedures). Participants with SAD were not selected if they only
met criteria for the nongeneralized subtype, had a psychotic dis-
order, or qualified for a substance use disorder, but other comor-
bidities were allowed. All participants were native English speak-
ers. Nine participants were excluded from analyses because they
did not provide daily social interaction data. This led to 38 par-
ticipants with generalized SAD diagnoses (25 women) and 38
healthy controls (24 women). Excluded participants did not sig-
nificantly differ from included participants on study variables.
Sample details are reported in Table 1. Notably, 15 of the 38 SAD
participants had no comorbidities, and eight were receiving treat-
ment, but these variables did not predict experience-sampling,
trait, or compliance measures (ps .10).
Procedure
We recruited participants via flyers and online community ad-
vertisements. Following this, we conducted a phone screen with
potential participants following an informed consent procedure.
Trained research assistants used a structured questionnaire to as-
sess for social anxiety, generalized anxiety disorder, depression,
suicidality, and psychotic symptoms. We provided referrals as
needed following lethality assessments. Potential participants with
evidence of generalized social anxiety fears were retained for
further assessment.
Participants provided informed consent and completed demo-
graphic and personality questionnaires and then clinical psychol-
ogy doctoral students administered the Structured Clinical Inter-
view for DSM–IV Axis I Disorders (SCID; First, Spitzer, Gibbon,
& Williams, 2002) to assess for anxiety, mood, substance use,
eating, and psychotic disorders. The SCID was supplemented with
the SAD module of the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for
DSM–IV: Lifetime Version (Di Nardo, Brown, & Barlow, 1994).
To be eligible for the generalized SAD group, this condition had to
be the principal or most severe diagnosis. Interrater reliability for
SAD diagnoses was calculated, resulting in excellent agreement
(Cohen’s ␬⫽.87). Upon recruiting a participant with SAD, we
used demographic information (race/ethnicity, gender, and age
within 8 years) to find a comparable healthy control match. We did
this by using targeted advertisements as necessary. This matching
system was successful for 82% of our sample, and the final groups
did not differ significantly on these variables (see Table 1).
To provide experience-sampling data, we gave participants
hand-held computers (Palm Pilot Z22), programmed using the
Purdue Momentary Assessment Tool (PMAT; Weiss, Beal, Lucy,
& MacDermid, 2004). Participants received a 1.5-hr introductory
session, including practice with self-initiated recording of every
face-to-face social interaction lasting at least 10 minutes. We
defined a social interaction as “any situation involving you and one
or more other people in which the behavior of each person is
affected by the behaviors of the others.” We instructed participants
to complete the records as soon as possible after each interaction.
Research assistants described what did (e.g., conversation) and did
not (e.g., quietly watching a movie) qualify as social interactions,
and discussed survey items until participants felt comfortable with
the procedure. Definitions and procedures were based on 35 years
of daily diary research (Nezlek, 2012).
Two days into data collection, we contacted participants to
troubleshoot. Following this, we sent multiple reminder e-mails
each week to emphasize compliance, confidentiality, and data
coding details (i.e., time-and-date stamped entries), and we de-
briefed participants at the end of the 2-week assessment period. To
maximize compliance, participants received a minimum payment
of $165 and could earn up to $50 in bonus money. The entire study
consisted of three parts—social interaction records, end of day
records, and random prompts—with only the first being relevant to
this study. Participants received 50¢ for each completed random
prompt record and end-of-day record, plus $10 for each uninter-
rupted calendar week. Additionally, each end-of-day assessment
included a reminder about social interaction entries. Moreover, we
kept all measures brief to maintain participant motivation and
maximize responses—a strategy that does not sacrifice reliability
or validity (e.g., Nezlek, 2012).
Trait Measures
Social Interaction Anxiety Scale. This 20-item scale (SIAS;
Mattick & Clarke, 1998) measured fear and avoidance of social
interactions due to concerns about being scrutinized by other
people. Participants responded to items using a 5-point Likert scale
ranging from 0 (not at all characteristic of me)to4(extremely
characteristic of me), with higher scores representing greater so-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
3
DISTINGUISHING DISORDER
cial anxiety. This scale demonstrated strong reliability and validity
across clinical and community samples (Brown et al., 1997; Heim-
berg et al., 1992). We removed three reverse scored items, which
have been shown to slightly reduce reliability and validity (Rode-
baugh et al., 2011; Rodebaugh, Woods, & Heimberg, 2007). In our
sample, reliability was identical using the 17- and 20-item versions
(␣⫽.94), with a .997 (p .001) correlation between the two
versions. We used the 17-item SIAS-Straightforward (SIAS-S)
scores for subsequent analyses; however, results were virtually
identical when including reverse-score items.
The SAD group had greater social anxiety (M 43.44, SD
8.91) than healthy controls (M 8.70, SD 6.31), t(74) 19.60,
p .001, d 4.56. The means for our SAD group were com-
mensurate with clients in treatment for SAD (M 43.93, SD
11.84; Rodebaugh et al., 2011). We also compared our SAD
sample with established norms on the 20-item SIAS. Of our SAD
sample, 97.4% of participants scored above the optimal cut-off
score of 34 (in terms of sensitivity and specificity) for distinguish-
ing people with SAD from a healthy community comparison group
(Brown et al., 1997).
Beck Depression Inventory-II. Severity of depressive symp-
toms was assessed using this 21-item questionnaire (BDI-II; Beck,
Steer, & Brown, 1996). Participants responded to items on a scale
from 0 to 3 to reflect their experiences over a 2-week period, with
higher scores representing greater depressive symptoms. Our sam-
ple had acceptable internal reliability (␣⫽.94). The SAD group
had significantly greater BDI-II scores than healthy controls, M
16.71, SD 10.59 vs. M 2.87, SD 3.06; t(74) 7.74, p
.001, d 1.80.
Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II. This 7-item scale
(AAQ-II; Bond et al., 2011) assessed experiential avoidance, or the
inability to accept aversive internal experiences and pursue impor-
tant life goals despite the presence of these experiences. Partici-
pants used a 7-point scale from 1 (never true)to7(always true)
with higher scores reflecting a greater tendency to avoid unpleas-
ant experiences. This scale has strong reliability and validity across
clinical and community samples (Bond et al., 2011). Prior work
has shown that experiential avoidance as measured by the AAQ-II
is related to but distinct from anxiety sensitivity, contributing
unique variance to the prediction of anticipatory anxiety and
Table 1
Demographic and Diagnostic Information by Participant Group
Variable
Group
Difference testControl
a
SAD
a
Diagnosis 0.0%
Social anxiety disorder 0.0% 100%
Major depressive disorder 0.0% 21.1%
Dysthymia 0.0% 13.2%
Panic disorder 2.6% 5.3%
Post traumatic stress disorder 0.0% 13.2%
Generalized anxiety disorder 0.0% 5.3%
Obsessive-compulsive disorder 5.3% 5.3%
Specific phobia 28.9%
Age (M, SD) 29.39 (10.29) 27.29 (6.03) t(74) 1.09, p .280
Gender (% women) 65.8% 63.2%
2
(1) 0.06, p .811
Race-Ethnicity
2
(5) 2.89, p .717
African American 28.9% 18.4%
Asian/Asian American 2.6% 5.3%
Caucasian/White 52.6% 50.0%
Hispanic/Latino 7.9% 13.2%
Middle Eastern 0.0% 2.6%
Other 7.9% 10.5%
Education
2
(4) 3.65, p .455
High school or less 2.6% 13.2%
Some college 36.8% 28.9%
Associate’s degree 5.3% 7.9%
Bachelor’s degree 36.8% 28.9%
At least some graduate level 18.4% 21.1%
Family income
2
(3) 4.38, p .223
$15,000 10.5% 15.8%
$15,000–$49,999 26.3% 44.7%
$50,000–$79,999 26.3% 18.4%
$80,000 36.8% 21.1%
Relationship status
2
(4) 4.05, p .400
Single 65.8% 63.2%
Cohabitating 21.1% 13.2%
Married 10.5% 10.5%
Divorced/Separated 0.0% 7.9%
Other 2.6% 5.3%
On psychoactive medications 2.6% 21.1%
2
(1) 6.18, p .013
Note. SAD Social Anxiety Disorder.
a
n 38.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
4
KASHDAN ET AL.
functional impairment in clients with anxiety disorders (Kämpfe et
al., 2012). Our sample had acceptable internal reliability (␣⫽.92).
The SAD group had significantly greater scores on the AAQ-II
than healthy controls (M 24.38, SD 5.29 vs. M 11.76, SD
3.33; t(73) 12.39, p .001, d 2.90).
1
Experience-Sampling Social Interaction Measures
Social anxiety. To assess social anxiety during interactions,
participants answered three items (from Kashdan & Steger, 2006):
“I worried about what other people thought of me,” “I was afraid
that others did not approve of me,” and “I was worried that I would
say or do the wrong things.” Participants used a 5-point response
scale ranging from 1 (very slightly or not at all) to 5 (extremely).
All social interaction measures used this response scale.
Experiential avoidance. Experiential avoidance during inter-
actions were measured with four items: “How upset and bothered
were you about anxiety-related feelings or thoughts?”, “How much
did you try to hide and/or conceal your anxiety from others?”,
“How much did you try to control your anxiety-related feelings or
thoughts?”, and “To what degree did you give up saying or doing
what you like (or mattered to you) in order to control and manage
anxiety?”
Emotions. Participants rated how much eight emotional ad-
jectives described them “right now” during social interactions.
Negative emotions were anxious/nervous, angry, sad, and slug-
gish. Positive emotions were content, relaxed, enthusiastic, and
joyful.
Feelings of belonging. How much people felt their need to
belong was satisfied during interactions was measured with two
items: “I felt close and connected to others during the interaction”
and “I felt accepted by others during the interaction.”
Self-control depletion. Mental and physical resource deple-
tion during interactions were measured with two items: “I feel
mentally exhausted” and “Right now, it would take a lot of effort
for me to concentrate on something” (from Ciarocco, Twenge,
Muraven, & Tice, 2010).
Participant Compliance
Participants described an average of 9.83 social interactions
(SD 4.63), over 11.45 days (SD 4.80). There were no
significant differences between the SAD and control groups on
number of social interactions (M 10.11, SD 5.41 vs. M
9.55, SD 3.79; t(74) 0.52, p .606, d 0.12) and days the
diary was maintained (M 11.45, SD 4.83 vs. M 11.45, SD
4.84; t(74) 0.00, p .99, d 0.00). Furthermore, we used
time stamps of social interaction entries to ensure that participants
did not enter multiple social interactions in one sitting. All but two
entries (99.7%) were more than 25 minutes after the prior entry.
2
Results
The social interaction diary data comprised a hierarchically
nested data set, with observations (interactions) nested within
persons. Accordingly, the data were analyzed with a series of
multilevel models using HLM 6.08 (Raudenbush, Bryk, Cheong,
& Congdon, 2000). Our analyses followed guidelines and proce-
dures described by Nezlek (2011, pp. 9 –34).
Daily Measures: Descriptive Data, Reliability, and
Validity
We examined the reliability of experience-sampling measures
with three-level unconditional models with items nested within
interactions, and interactions nested within people. In the model
equations below, there were i items nested within j interactions
nested with k participants. In such an analysis, the reliability of the
Level 1 intercept is the functional equivalent of an interaction level
Cronbach’s alpha, adjusted for differences among interactions and
among people.
Item Level 1 : Y
ijk
⫽␲
0jk
c
ijk
Interaction Level 2 :
0jk
⫽␤
00k
r
0jk
Person Level 3 :
00k
⫽␥
000
u
00k
Each of the social interaction measures had acceptable reliabil-
ity (see Table 2). We computed social interaction measures as the
mean response to the items for each scale. All HLM analyses
presented below had interactions nested (n 742) within persons
(n 76). Table 2 also contains the basic multilevel descriptive
statistics of each measure: mean, standard error, and two variance
estimates: between- and within-person.
To evaluate the validity of our two primary experience-sampling
measures (social anxiety and experiential avoidance), we exam-
ined relationships between the participants’ interaction scores and
scores on corresponding trait measures (Nezlek, 2007). For exam-
ple, we modeled social anxiety during social interactions as a
function of the SIAS-S:
0j
⫽␥
0
⫹␥
1
SIAS-S
u
j
.
We estimated validity by comparing the between-person vari-
ance of social anxiety in the unconditional model (0.65) with the
residual between-person variance from the model above (0.29).
This 55% reduction in variance corresponds to a correlation of .74
(square root of .55), B .03, SE .004, t(74) 8.20, p .001.
As evidence of divergent validity, when we included the SIAS-S
and the BDI-II as predictors in the same model, the experience-
sampling measure of social anxiety was positively related to the
SIAS-S, B .02, SE .01, t(73) 4.36, p .001, but not the
BDI-II, B .02, SE .01, t(73) 1.51, p .135.
For our interaction level measure of experiential avoidance, the
AAQ-II was used as a trait predictor. The between-person variance
of experiential avoidance from the unconditional model was 0.46.
The residual variance after including the AAQ-II was 0.21—a
reduction of 56%, corresponding to a correlation of .75, B .07,
SE .01, t(73) 8.77, p .001. When we included the AAQ-II
and SAD diagnostic status as person level predictors in the same
model, the AAQ-II remained significantly related to the
experience-sampling measure of experiential avoidance, B .04,
SE .01, t(72) 2.66, p .010, after controlling for the effect
of SAD diagnosis, B .25, SE .11, t(72) 2.17, p .033.
1
One participant in the SAD group had missing data for the AAQ-II due
to a technical malfunction.
2
These two social interaction entries were completed within 10 minutes
of the prior entry and represented a change in activity during an elongated
social interaction (i.e., within instruction parameters).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
5
DISTINGUISHING DISORDER
These data provide support for the construct specificity for our
experience-sampling measure of experiential avoidance.
3
Notably,
our measure is specific to experiential avoidance during social
interactions, although the trait measure is not restricted to a par-
ticular context.
Differences Between SAD and Control Group in Social
Interaction Experiences
We examined relationships between SAD diagnostic status and
our social interaction measures with two-level models. Social
interaction measures were the dependent measures at Level 1 (in
separate models), and SAD diagnostic status was a predictor at
Level 2. SAD was contrast-coded (1 SAD, 1 Control) and
entered uncentered in the following model:
Interaction Level 1 : Y
ij
⫽␤
0j
r
ij
.
Person Level 2 :
0j
⫽␥
00
⫹␥
01
SAD
u
0j
Initially, we included the number of social interactions a person
described as a Level 2 covariate. Including this variable did not
influence the findings, which was not surprising, given that SAD
and number of interactions were unrelated. Thus, this covariate
was removed.
As summarized in Table 3, the SAD group reported more social
anxiety, negative emotion, experiential avoidance, and self-control
depletion in social interactions compared with the control group.
The SAD group also reported less positive emotion and feelings of
belonging.
To address construct specificity, we conducted additional anal-
yses controlling for the presence of comorbid mood and anxiety
disorders. We created contrast-coded variables to reflect whether
participants met diagnostic criteria for any other anxiety disorder
or mood disorder. We then entered these two variables, uncen-
tered, as additional Level 2 predictors.
For all outcomes, the SAD effects controlling for comorbid
conditions were similar to the effects found without these covari-
ates—as before, all SAD effects were significant at p .001.
Controlling for mood and anxiety disorder comorbidity, SAD
remained directly related to our experience-sampling measures of
social anxiety (B .51, SE .10, t(72) 5.03, p .001),
negative emotions (B .21, SE .05, t(72) 3.76, p .001),
experiential avoidance (B .49, SE .08, t(72) 5.94, p
.001), and self-control depletion (B .37, SE .10, t(72) 3.74,
p .001), and inversely related to positive emotions (B ⫽⫺.50,
SE .08, t(72) ⫽⫺6.61, p .001) and feelings of belonging
(B ⫽⫺.38, SE .08, t(72) ⫽⫺4.76, p .001). In contrast,
anxiety disorder comorbidity was not significantly related to any
social interaction measure (ps .25), and comorbid depressive
disorders were significant only in the prediction of negative emo-
tions (B .41, SE .13, t(72) 3.11, p .003), positive
emotions (B ⫽⫺.59, SE .17, t(72) ⫽⫺3.51, p .001), and
feelings of belonging (B ⫽⫺.58, SE .23, t(72) ⫽⫺2.58, p
.012). These results support the specificity of SAD in relation to
social interaction measures of social anxiety and experiential
avoidance.
Added Value of Experiential Avoidance, Self-Control
Depletion, and Positive Emotions in Predicting SAD
Diagnosis
Multilevel analyses. Following preliminary analyses, we ex-
amined our primary question about the added value of experiential
avoidance, self-control depletion, and positive emotions during
social interactions to understand SAD diagnoses. Would relation-
ships between these variables and SAD diagnosis remain after
controlling for variance attributable to more commonly used mea-
sures of social anxiety, negative emotions, and feelings of belong-
ing? To answer this question, we first conducted a series of
multilevel analyses with experiential avoidance, self-control de-
pletion, and positive emotions as dependent measures. We in-
cluded SAD diagnosis as a Level 2 predictor and the following as
simultaneous Level 1 predictors: social anxiety, negative emo-
tions, and feelings of belonging. Social anxiety, negative emotions,
and feelings of belonging were grand-mean centered to control for
individual differences (Nezlek, 2011, pp. 13–18).
Controlling for social anxiety, negative emotions, and feelings
of belonging in social interactions, the relationship between SAD
diagnosis and experiential avoidance remained significant (B
.11, SE .04, t(74) 3.19, p .003), as did the relationship
between SAD and positive emotions (B ⫽⫺.26, SE .05, t(74)
3
Supporting the validity of the experience-sampling measure of expe-
riential avoidance, the AAQ-II was a significant predictor, B .04, t
2.71, p .009, even after accounting for the variance attributable to the
SIAS-S, B .02, t 2.56, p .013.
Table 2
Multilevel Descriptive Statistics for Social Interaction Measures
Measure M (SE)
Variance
ICC ReliabilityWithin Between
Social anxiety 1.81 (.10) .63 .65 .50 .84
Negative emotions 1.55 (.05) .25 .17 .41 .85
Positive emotions 2.87 (.09) .62 .55 .31 .87
Belonging 3.56 (.08) .95 .42 .47 .78
Experiential avoidance 1.72 (.08) .55 .46 .45 .87
Self-control depletion 1.70 (.08) .67 .43 .40 .83
Note. Intraclass correlation coefficients (ICC) represent the proportion of
between-person variance to total variance.
Table 3
Differences Between Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD) and Healthy
Control Groups for Social Interaction Measures
Measure bSE t
Estimated means (SE)
Control SAD
Social anxiety .51 .08 6.71 1.30 (.11) 2.32 (.11)
Negative emotions .24 .04 5.76 1.30 (.06) 1.79 (.06)
Positive emotions .53 .07 8.09 3.41 (.09) 2.34 (.09)
Belonging .41 .07 5.95 3.97 (.10) 3.15 (.10)
Experiential avoidance .48 .06 8.11 1.23 (.09) 2.19 (.09)
Self-control depletion .37 .07 5.32 1.32 (.10) 2.06 (.10)
Note. Differences and estimated means were computed with two-level
HLM models, where SAD predicted each outcome measure separately. All
t-ratios have 74 df and were significantly different from 0 at p .001.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
6
KASHDAN ET AL.
4.84, p .001), but the relationship between SAD and self-
control depletion was no longer statistically significant (B .05,
SE .03, t(74) 1.41, p .16). These results suggest that the
association of SAD with greater experiential avoidance and less
positive emotions in everyday social interactions could not be
explained by variance attributable to social anxiety, negative emo-
tions, and feelings of belonging in social interactions.
Classification analysis. We supplemented the prior multi-
level analyses with a classification analysis. Following previous
work on diagnostic classification (e.g., Nock & Kessler, 2006), we
used hierarchical logistical regression to determine if experiential
avoidance, positive emotions, and self-control depletion improve
the accuracy of predicting SAD diagnoses beyond the contribu-
tions of social anxiety, negative emotions, and feelings of belong-
ing. To conduct these analyses, we aggregated within-person so-
cial interaction measures into between-person variables. At Step
One, we included social anxiety, negative emotions, and feelings
of belonging during social interactions as predictors of SAD di-
agnosis. At Step Two, we added experiential avoidance, positive
emotions, and self-control depletion during social interactions as
predictors.
As reported in Table 4, at Step One, we found evidence that both
social anxiety and feelings of belongingness during daily social
interactions were significant predictors of SAD diagnostic cate-
gory (R
2
.61). Specifically, greater social anxiety and less
belonging during daily social interactions helped to distinguish
participants with SAD from our healthy comparison group. This
finding provides additional support for the validity of our
experience-sampling measure of social anxiety. At Step One of the
model, with experience-sampling measures of social anxiety, neg-
ative emotions, and belonging as predictors, 81.6% of participants
with SAD were correctly classified (sensitivity) and 84.2% of
participants without SAD were correctly classified (specificity).
At Step Two, experiential avoidance and positive emotions
during social interactions predicted SAD diagnostic status above
and beyond the constructs in the prior step, with a 17% incremental
improvement in variance explained (R
2
.77). As shown in Table
4, greater experiential avoidance and less positive emotions during
social interactions added to the prediction of SAD diagnoses
beyond the contributions of the more routinely assessed constructs.
Self-control depletion did not significantly add to the prediction of
SAD diagnosis and neither social anxiety nor feeling of belonging
remained significant. Negative emotions emerged as a significant
predictor in the final model, though in the opposite direction.
Providing additional support, upon inclusion of comorbid depres-
sive and anxiety conditions as additional predictors, experiential
avoidance (B 7.63, SE 3.00, Wald 6.45, p .01, OR
2057) and positive emotions (B ⫽⫺3.68, SE 1.69, Wald
4.77, p .029, OR 0.03) continued to predict SAD diagnostic
status.
4
In sum, the odds of having SAD increased more than 163 times
with every one-point increase in the average level of experiential
avoidance reported in social interactions. The odds increased more
than 33 times with every one-point decrease in the average positive
emotions reported. As for the predictive value of our model, we
found evidence of high sensitivity (classifying 92.1% of individ-
uals with SAD) and specificity (classifying 86.8% of individuals
from the healthy control group). We found evidence for a strong
classification model where 89.5% of our cases were correctly
classified (68 out of 76). This represents an 11% improvement in
sensitivity and a 3% increase in specificity over a model with only
social anxiety, negative emotions, and belonging as predictors.
Discussion
The results of this experience-sampling study support theories
suggesting that, in addition to elevated anxiety in social interac-
tions, other characteristics distinguish the social experiences of
individuals with SAD. These characteristics include diminished
positive emotions (Kashdan, 2007; Watson et al., 1988) and ex-
periential avoidance, involving attempts to alter, avoid, or conceal
4
We ran separate logistic regression models for each discrete negative
and positive emotion, respectively. The only negative emotion item that
was statistically significant after including the Step Two predictors was
anger, 〉⫽⫺3.19, SE 1.32, Wald 5.86, p .015, OR .041. For
positive emotions, significant predictors included relaxed, 〉⫽⫺1.75,
SE .83, Wald 4.51, p .034, OR .174, content, 〉⫽⫺1.78, SE
.82, Wald 4.78, p .029, OR .168, and enthusiasm, 〉⫽⫺1.88,
SE .66, Wald 8.07, p .005, OR .153, with joy showing a trend
effect, 〉⫽⫺1.11, SE .61, Wald 3.32, p .069, OR .330.
Table 4
Hierarchical Logistic Regression Analyses Predicting Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD) Diagnostic Status From Social
Interaction Measures
Predictor BSEWald POR 95% CI R
2
Step One .605
Social anxiety 1.61 0.73 4.82 .028 5.00 [1.19, 21.04]
Negative emotions 0.81 1.11 0.53 .466 2.24 [0.26, 19.70]
Belonging 1.86 0.62 9.03 .003 0.16 [0.05, 0.52]
Step Two .772
Social anxiety 0.41 0.96 0.19 .666 1.51 [0.23, 9.82]
Negative emotions 4.62 2.24 4.23 .040 0.01 [0.00, 0.80]
Belonging 0.60 0.79 0.57 .451 0.55 [0.12, 2.60]
Self-control depletion 0.63 1.07 0.35 .556 0.53 [0.07, 4.36]
Experiential avoidance 5.10 1.92 7.07 .008 163.85 [3.82, 7036.34]
Positive emotions 3.51 1.27 7.69 .006 0.03 [0.003, 0.36]
Note. SE standard error; OR odds ratio; CI confidence interval. Bolded terms reflect a significant contribution to the prediction model. For the
full model, 92.1% of participants with SAD were correctly classified (sensitivity) and 86.8% of participants without SAD were correctly classified
(specificity).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
7
DISTINGUISHING DISORDER
anxious thoughts and feelings (Herbert & Cardaciotto, 2005; Kash-
dan et al., 2006). We found that individuals with SAD, compared
with healthy controls, could be differentiated by their attenuated
positive emotions and increased reliance on experiential avoidance
during social interactions in daily life. These relationships re-
mained after controlling for the variance attributable to more
routinely studied constructs of negative emotions, social anxiety,
and feelings of belonging and after accounting for the presence of
depressive disorders and comorbid anxiety conditions.
In prior attempts to distinguish individuals with and without
SAD, researchers relied on a narrow band of predictors. Nearly all
published studies on the classification of SAD have focused on
how well trait measures of social anxiety are able to identify SAD
when present (e.g., Mennin et al., 2002). In this study, we extended
recent research linking social anxiety to emotion regulation diffi-
culties and diminished/impaired positive experiences (for reviews,
see Kashdan et al., 2011; Watson & Naragon-Gainey, 2010). By
using a community sample of healthy adults and adults who met
diagnostic criteria for SAD, we provided evidence that experiential
avoidance and a dearth of positive emotions during social encoun-
ters offer improved predictive validity for SAD diagnosis beyond
anxiety during naturalistic social interactions.
To our knowledge, this was the first study to examine if expe-
riences during naturally occurring social interactions can distin-
guish people with and without SAD. Prior work on social anxiety
and both experiential avoidance and positive emotions has been
primarily limited to cross-sectional (Brown, Chorpita, & Barlow,
1998; Kashdan et al., 2006; Watson et al., 1988) or two-wave
(Kashdan & Breen, 2008) designs in which people aggregated
experiences across time and context using traditional global ques-
tionnaires. A small number of studies supplemented these ap-
proaches by examining how people with SAD respond in a labo-
ratory setting (Mallott et al., 2009; Taylor & Amir, 2012; Wallace
& Alden, 1997). Using event-contingent experience-sampling
methods with a carefully diagnosed sample, we were able to show
that the types of feelings, beliefs, and behaviors people with SAD
report on questionnaires, during interviews, or in laboratory ob-
servations also occur in real-world settings.
By asking participants to rate experiences as soon as possible
following each social interaction lasting over 10 minutes, our
approach circumvents some of the recall bias and distortion asso-
ciated with retrospective reports, allowing for greater ecological
validity over methods that rely on aggregated remembered expe-
riences or artificial contexts (Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1987;
Scollon, Kim-Prieto, & Diener, 2003). For a dynamic, contextu-
alized approach, we asked participants to report on their experi-
ences over the course of several weeks with electronic diaries, and
all entries were time-and-date stamped. This procedure ensured
that participants reported on their social interactions and associated
experiences over a range of social encounters, not merely recalling
a number of interactions on a single assessment occasion.
Within-person effects (with experience-sampling approaches)
and between-person effects (with item-correlations among trait
measures) are conceptually distinct (e.g., Affleck, Zautra, Tennen,
& Armeli, 1999). As evidence of this, we found that social anxiety
in the context of everyday social interactions significantly distin-
guished individuals with and without SAD diagnoses, but this
effect did not hold after the inclusion of positive emotions, expe-
riential avoidance, and self-control depletion into analytic models.
One explanation for this result is that the intensity/frequency of
social anxiety is less critical to pathology than the unwillingness to
remain in contact with unwanted anxious thoughts and emotions
(i.e., experiential avoidance). These results are consistent with
laboratory studies showing that shy participants without SAD
experience levels of social anxiety during conversations with
strangers as people with SAD (Heiser, Turner, Beidel, &
Roberson-Nay, 2009). Other researchers have shown that positiv-
ity impairments in daily life only occur when people with high trait
social anxiety both experience high social anxiety and tend to
suppress these anxious reactions on the same day (Kashdan &
Steger, 2006), suggesting that functional impairment hinges on the
co-occurrence of anxiety and experiential avoidance.
Our finding that experiential avoidance outperformed social
anxiety in distinguishing people with SAD demonstrates that so-
cial anxiety is more than just a collection of self-reported distress.
Notably, the very large odds ratio for experiential avoidance
(OR 163.85) as a predictor of diagnostic status is a function of
the near absence of experiential avoidance in the healthy control
group. This is likely due to the healthy control group generating
and regulating emotions differently in everyday social interactions
compared with the SAD group. Further research might examine
the use of different emotion regulation strategies during social
interactions to better understand this effect.
Prior research has shown that people with SAD tend to suppress
the expression of both negative emotions (Erwin, Heimberg, Sch-
neier, & Liebowitz, 2003) and positive emotions (Turk, Heimberg,
Luterek, Mennin, & Fresco, 2005). Other studies suggest that
people with SAD are more likely to act in an unassertive, submis-
sive manner (Hopko, McNeil, Zvolensky, & Eifert, 2002). Based
on these findings, we suspect that healthy adults in the community
may differ from people with SAD in their willingness to have
emotionally charged conversations. This would explain our seem-
ingly unusual finding that after accounting for experiential avoid-
ance, people with SAD could be distinguished from healthy con-
trols by the presence of less negative emotions in their everyday
social interactions. Given that people with SAD fear the possibility
of having their perceived deficiencies scrutinized and believe that
visible anxiety is a sign of weakness, then experiential avoidance
(of negative thoughts and feelings) appears to be a useful self-
protection strategy. Concealing the expression of perceived defi-
ciencies becomes a safeguard against the feared consequences
linked to SAD (Alden & Taylor, 2011). The unfortunate by-
product of experiential avoidance is that this biased allocation of
attention limits access to rewards, as shown by the deficient
generation of positive emotions in the present study (extending
prior work on positivity deficits; Kashdan et al., 2011).
Separating the generation of emotions from emotion regulation
is important when contemplating the design of future studies on
moment-to-moment dynamic processes that contribute to the de-
velopment and maintenance of SAD. We chose to use experience
sampling to best capture naturalistic experiences while maintain-
ing some level of measurement control. Despite these efforts, our
experience-sampling procedure might have missed crucial infor-
mation about how people with SAD navigate their social world.
First, although we assessed experiential avoidance, we did not
inquire about specific safety behaviors to minimize contact with
experiences (e.g., fidgeting, averting eye contact). These and other
microlevel processes in everyday contexts may provide additional
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
8
KASHDAN ET AL.
insights into the phenomenology of SAD. Second, people with
SAD tend to avoid the very situations that invoke anxiety for them.
We did not gather data about social opportunities that were
avoided or interactions that ended quickly. The broader tendency
to avoid discomfort may explain in part why we found such a large
effect for experiential avoidance within social interactions. Simi-
larly, the intentional avoidance of particularly anxiety-provoking
situations that would be mentally exhausting might explain why
we did not find a significant effect of self-control depletion in
classifying people with SAD.
Refined methodologies can evaluate the extent to which people
with SAD avoid/escape social activities and unwanted distress in
everyday life as it naturally unfolds. Future studies can explicitly
ask about briefer interactions (e.g., where someone escapes within
seconds of contact with another person), use of e-mail and texting
to avoid face-to-face encounters, and other tactics to prevent feared
interactions (e.g., taking different routes to a destination to avoid
people). Nevertheless, people with emotional disorders such as
SAD are often unaware of the extent of their behavioral avoidance
and monitoring may change behavior patterns.
Although our naturalistic data collection process had many
benefits, there are limitations worthy of future correction. First,
social interaction episodes involve at least two people, but we only
collected data from a single person. Future studies should consider
dyadic data collection for behavioral assessment reports (e.g.,
capturing a lack of responsiveness to another person’s self-
disclosure). Second, our participants sometimes recorded social
interactions after they occurred, leading to a degree of retrospec-
tive interpretation. The screening of time and date stamps ad-
dressed some of these concerns, but future research ought to ensure
that participants enter data as close as possible to the triggering
events (e.g., by using participants’ own mobile phones for ease of
data entry). Third, we did not test other relevant theoretical models
(e.g., estimates of the probability and cost of social blunders,
rumination, and fear of positive evaluation) due to response burden
concerns. Future work can expand the current findings with inno-
vative experimental, longitudinal, and intervention designs.
Our findings support an expanding conceptualization of SAD
where both negative and positive emotional reactions to social
events, and an overreliance on attempts to avoid anxious thoughts
and feelings during social events help differentiate individuals
with SAD from healthy adults in the community. Clinicians might
consider expanding the explicit target of interventions for SAD to
include the reduction of experiential avoidance and increase of
positive experiences (Alden & Taylor, 2011; Dalrymple & Her-
bert, 2007).
References
Affleck, G., Zautra, A., Tennen, H., & Armeli, S. (1999). Multilevel daily
process designs for consulting and clinical psychology: A preface for the
perplexed. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 746 –754.
doi:10.1037/0022-006X.67.5.746
Alden, L. E., & Taylor, C. T. (2011). Relational treatment strategies
increase social approach behaviors in patients with Generalized Social
Anxiety Disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 25, 309 –318. doi:
10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.10.003
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical man-
ual of mental disorders: DSM–IV–TR. Washington, DC: American Psy-
chiatric Publishing, Inc.
Baumeister, R. F. (2002). Ego depletion and self-control failure: An energy
model of the self’s executive function. Self and Identity, 1, 129–136.
doi:10.1080/152988602317319302
Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2007). Self-regulation, ego depletion,
and motivation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 1, 115–
128. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00001.x
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck
Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Beidel, D. C., Turner, S. M., & Dancu, C. V. (1985). Physiological,
cognitive and behavioral aspects of social anxiety. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 23, 109 –117. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(85)90019-1
Bond, F. W., Hayes, S. C., Baer, R. A., Carpenter, K. C., Guenole, N.,
Orcutt, H. K.,...Zettle, R. D. (2011). Preliminary psychometric
properties of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire–II: A revised
measure of psychological flexibility and acceptance. Behavior Therapy,
42, 676 688. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2011.03.007
Brown, E. J., Turovsky, J., Heimberg, R. G., Juster, H. R., Brown, T. A.,
& Barlow, D. H. (1997). Validation of the Social Interaction Anxiety
Scale and the Social Phobia Scale across the anxiety disorders. Psycho-
logical Assessment, 9, 21–27. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.9.1.21
Brown, T. A. (2007). Temporal course and structural relationships among
dimensions of temperament and DSM–IV anxiety and mood disorder
constructs. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 313–328. doi:
10.1037/0021-843X.116.2.313
Brown, T. A., Chorpita, B. F., & Barlow, D. H. (1998). Structural rela-
tionships among dimensions of the DSM–IV anxiety and mood disorders
and dimensions of negative affect, positive affect, and autonomic arous-
al. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 179 –192. doi:10.1037/0021-
843X.107.2.179
Butler, E. A., Egloff, B., Wilhelm, F. H., Smith, N. C., Erickson, E. A., &
Gross, J. J. (2003). The social consequences of expressive suppression.
Emotion, 3, 48 67. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.3.1.48
Chen, Y. P., Ehlers, A., Clark, D. M., & Mansell, W. (2002). Patients with
generalized social phobia direct their attention away from faces. Behav-
iour Research and Therapy, 40, 677– 687. doi:10.1016/S0005-
7967(01)00086-9
Ciarocco, N., Twenge, J. M., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (2010). The
State Self-Control Capacity Scale: Reliability, validity, and correlations
with physical and psychological stress. Manuscript under revision.
Cisler, J. M., Olatunji, B. O., Feldner, M. T., & Forsyth, J. P. (2010).
Emotion regulation and the anxiety disorders: An integrative review.
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 32, 68 82.
doi:10.1007/s10862-009-9161-1
Clark, D. M., & Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social phobia. In
R. G. Heimberg, M. Liebowitz, D. Hope, & F. Scheier (Eds.), Social
phobia: Diagnosis, assessment, and treatment. (pp. 69 –93). New York,
NY: Guilford Press.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1987). Validity and reliability of the
experience-sampling method. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease,
175, 526 –536. doi:10.1097/00005053-198709000-00004
Dalrymple, K. L., & Herbert, J. D. (2007). Acceptance and commitment
therapy for generalized social anxiety disorder: A pilot study. Behavior
Modification, 31, 543–568. doi:10.1177/0145445507302037
Di Nardo, P. A., Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H. (1994). Anxiety disorders
interview schedule for DSM–IV: Lifetime version (ADIS-IV-L). San
Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Erwin, B. A., Heimberg, R. G., Schneier, F. R., & Liebowitz, M. R. (2003).
Anger experience and expression in social anxiety disorder: Pretreat-
ment profile and predictors of attrition and response to cognitive-
behavioral treatment. Behavior Therapy, 34, 331–350. doi:10.1016/
S0005-7894(03)80004-7
Farmer, A. S., & Kashdan, T. B. (2012). Social anxiety and emotion
regulation in daily life: Spillover effects on positive and negative social
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
9
DISTINGUISHING DISORDER
events. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 41, 152–162. doi:10.1080/
16506073.2012.666561
Feldner, M. T., Zvolensky, M. J., Eifert, G. H., & Spira, A. P. (2003).
Emotional avoidance: An experimental test of individual differences and
response suppression using biological challenge. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 41, 403– 411. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00020-7
Finkel, E. J., Campbell, W. K., Brunell, A. B., Dalton, A. N., Scarbeck,
S. J., & Chartrand, T. L. (2006). High-maintenance interaction: Ineffi-
cient social coordination impairs self-regulation. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 91, 456 475. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.456
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (2002).
Structured clinical interview for DSM–IV–TR axis I disorders, research
version, non-patient edition (SCID-I/NP). New York, NY: Biometrics
Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute.
Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Franklin, M. E., & Foa, E. B. (2000). Anticipated
reactions to social events: Differences among individuals with general-
ized social phobia, obsessive compulsive disorder, and nonanxious con-
trols. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24, 731–746. doi:10.1023/A:
1005595513315
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion
regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-
being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348–362.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348
Gross, J. J., Sheppes, G., & Urry, H. L. (2011). Emotion generation and
emotion regulation: A distinction we should make (carefully). Cognition
& Emotion, 25, 765–781. doi:10.1080/02699931.2011.555753
Hayes, S. C., Wilson, K. G., Gifford, E. V., Follette, V. M., & Strosahl, K.
(1996). Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: A functional
dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 64, 1152–1168. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.64.6
.1152
Heimberg, R. G., Brozovich, F. A., & Rapee, R. M. (2010). A cognitive
behavioral model of social anxiety disorder: Update and extension. In
S. G. Hofmann & P. M. DiBartolo (Eds.), Social anxiety: Clinical,
developmental, and social perspective (2nd ed., pp. 395–422). New
York, NY: Elsevier.
Heimberg, R. G., Mueller, G. P., Holt, C. S., Hope, D. A., & Liebowitz,
M. R. (1992). Assessment of anxiety in social interaction and being
observed by others: The social interaction anxiety scale and the Social
Phobia Scale. Behavior Therapy, 23, 53–73. doi:10.1016/S0005-
7894(05)80308-9
Heiser, N. A., Turner, S. M., Beidel, D. C., & Roberson-Nay, R. (2009).
Differentiating social phobia from shyness. Journal of Anxiety Disor-
ders, 23, 469 476. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.10.002
Herbert, J. D., & Cardaciotto, L. A. (2005). An acceptance and
mindfulness-based perspective on social anxiety disorder. In S. M.
Orsillo & L. Roemer (Eds.), Acceptance and mindfulness-based ap-
proaches to anxiety (pp. 189 –212). Boston, MA: Springer. doi:10.1007/
0-387-25989-9_8
Heuer, K., Rinck, M., & Becker, E. S. (2007). Avoidance of emotional
facial expressions in social anxiety: The Approach-Avoidance Task.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 2990–3001. doi:10.1016/j.brat
.2007.08.010
Hofmann, S. G. (2007). Cognitive factors that maintain social anxiety
disorder: A comprehensive model and its treatment implications. Cog-
nitive Behaviour Therapy, 36, 193–209. doi:10.1080/
16506070701421313
Hofmann, S. G., Newman, M. G., Ehlers, A., & Roth, W. T. (1995).
Psychophysiological differences between subgroups of social phobia.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 224 –231. doi:10.1037/0021-
843X.104.1.224
Hopko, D. R., McNeil, D. W., Zvolensky, M. J., & Eifert, G. H. (2001).
The relation between anxiety and skill in performance-based anxiety
disorders: A behavioral formulation of social phobia. Behavior Therapy,
32, 185–207. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(01)80052-6
Hughes, A. A., Heimberg, R. G., Coles, M. E., Gibb, B. E., Liebowitz,
M. R., & Schneier, F. R. (2006). Relations of the factors of the tripartite
model of anxiety and depression to types of social anxiety. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 44, 1629 –1641. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.015
Kämpfe, C. K., Gloster, A. T., Wittchen, H. U., Helbig-Lang, S., Lang, T.,
Gerlach, A. L.,...Kircher, T. (2012). Experiential avoidance and
anxiety sensitivity in patients with panic disorder and agoraphobia: Do
both constructs measure the same? International Journal of Clinical and
Health Psychology, 12, 5–22.
Kashdan, T. B. (2007). Social anxiety spectrum and diminished positive
experiences: Theoretical synthesis and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychol-
ogy Review, 27, 348 –365. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2006.12.003
Kashdan, T. B., Adams, L., Savostyanova, A., Ferssizidis, P., McKnight,
P. E., & Nezlek, J. B. (2011). Effects of social anxiety and depressive
symptoms on the frequency and quality of sexual activity: A daily
process approach. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49, 352–360. doi:
10.1016/j.brat.2011.03.004
Kashdan, T. B., Barrios, V., Forsyth, J. P., & Steger, M. F. (2006).
Experiential avoidance as a generalized psychological vulnerability:
Comparisons with coping and emotion regulation strategies. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 44, 1301–1320. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.003
Kashdan, T. B., & Breen, W. E. (2008). Social anxiety and positive
emotions: A prospective examination of a self-regulatory model with
tendencies to suppress or express emotions as a moderating variable.
Behavior Therapy, 39, 1–12. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2007.02.003
Kashdan, T. B., & Savostyanova, A. A. (2011). Capturing the biases of
socially anxious people by addressing partner effects and situational
parameters. Behavior Therapy, 42, 211–223. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2010.07
.004
Kashdan, T. B., & Steger, M. F. (2006). Expanding the topography of
social anxiety: An experience-sampling assessment of positive emo-
tions, positive events, and emotion suppression. Psychological Science,
17, 120 –128. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01674.x
Kashdan, T. B., Weeks, J. W., & Savostyanova, A. A. (2011). Whether,
how, and when social anxiety shapes positive experiences and events: A
self-regulatory framework and treatment implications. Clinical Psychol-
ogy Review, 31, 786 –799. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2011.03.012
Mallott, M. A., Maner, J. K., DeWall, N., & Schmidt, N. B. (2009).
Compensatory deficits following rejection: The role of social anxiety in
disrupting affiliative behavior. Depression and Anxiety, 26, 438 446.
doi:10.1002/da.20555
Mattick, R. P., & Clarke, J. C. (1998). Development and validation of
measures of social phobia scrutiny fear and social interaction anxiety.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 455–470. doi:10.1016/S0005-
7967(97)10031-6
Mennin, D. S., Fresco, D. M., Heimberg, R. G., Schneier, F. R., Davies,
S. O., & Liebowitz, M. R. (2002). Screening for social anxiety disorder
in the clinical setting: Using the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale. Journal
of Anxiety Disorders, 16, 661– 673. doi:10.1016/S0887-
6185(02)00134-2
Moscovitch, D. A. (2009). What is the core fear in social phobia? A new
model to facilitate individualized case conceptualization and treatment.
Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 16, 123–134. doi:10.1016/j.cbpra
.2008.04.002
Moscovitch, D. A., & Huyder, V. (2011). The negative self-portrayal scale:
Development, validation, and application to social anxiety. Behavior
Therapy, 42, 183–196. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2010.04.007
Nezlek, J. B. (2007). A multilevel framework for understanding relation-
ships among traits, states, situations, and behaviors. European Journal of
Personality, 21, 789 810.
Nezlek, J. B. (2011). Multilevel modeling for social and personality psy-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
10
KASHDAN ET AL.
chology. In J. B. Nezlek (Ed.), The SAGE library in social and person-
ality psychology methods (pp. 133–138). London, UK: Sage.
Nezlek, J. B. (2012). Diary methods for social and personality psychology.
In J. B. Nezlek (Ed.), The SAGE library in social and personality
psychology methods (pp. 29–32). London, UK: Sage.
Nock, M. K., & Kessler, R. C. (2006). Prevalence of and risk factors for
suicide attempts versus suicide gestures: Analysis of the National Co-
morbidity Survey. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 616 623.
doi:10.1037/0021-843X.115.3.616
Raudenbush, S. W., Bryk, A. S., Cheong, Y. F., & Congdon, R. T. (2000).
HLM. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International.
Reis, H. T., & Gable, S. L. (2000). Event-sampling methods. In H. T. Reis
& C. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social psychology
(pp. 190 –222). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Rodebaugh, T. L., & Heimberg, R. G. (2008). Measurement of ambivalent
and purposeful engagement after aversive social experiences. Journal of
Anxiety Disorders, 22, 693–706. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.07.003
Rodebaugh, T. L., Heimberg, R. G., Brown, P. J., Fernandez, K. C.,
Blanco, C., Schneier, F. R., & Liebowitz, M. R. (2011). More reasons to
be straightforward: Findings and norms for two scales relevant to social
anxiety. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 25, 623–630. doi:10.1016/j
.janxdis.2011.02.002
Rodebaugh, T. L., & Shumaker, E. (2012). Avoidance goals for a specific
social situation influence activated negative and positive affect. Cogni-
tive Therapy and Research, 36, 36 46. doi:10.1007/s10608-010-9313-3
Rodebaugh, T. L., Woods, C. M., & Heimberg, R. G. (2007). The reverse
of social anxiety is not always the opposite: The reverse-scored items of
the Social Interaction Anxiety Scale do not belong. Behavior Therapy,
38, 192–206. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2006.08.001
Scollon, C. N., Kim-Prieto, C., & Diener, E. (2003). Experience sampling:
Promises and pitfalls, strengths and weaknesses. Journal of Happiness
Studies, 4, 5–34. doi:10.1023/A:1023605205115
Sloan, D. M. (2004). Emotion regulation in action: Emotional reactivity in
experiential avoidance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 1257–
1270. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2003.08.006
Stillman, T. F., Baumeister, R. F., Lambert, N. M., Will, A., Nathan, C., &
Fincham, F. D. (2009). Alone and without purpose: Life loses meaning
following social exclusion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
45, 686 694. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.03.007
Taylor, C. T., & Amir, N. (2012). Modifying automatic approach action
tendencies in individuals with elevated social anxiety symptoms. Behav-
iour Research and Therapy, 50, 529 –536. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2012.05
.004
Turk, C. L., Heimberg, R. G., Luterek, J. A., Mennin, D. S., & Fresco,
D. M. (2005). Emotion dysregulation in generalized anxiety disorder: A
comparison with social anxiety disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Re-
search, 29, 89 –106. doi:10.1007/s10608-005-1651-1
Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., & Ciarocco, N. J. (2005). Self-regulation
and self-presentation: Regulatory resource depletion impairs impression
management and effortful self-presentation depletes regulatory re-
sources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 632– 657.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.4.632
Voncken, M. J., Alden, L. E., & Bögels, S. M. (2006). Hiding anxiety
versus acknowledgment of anxiety in social interaction: Relationship
with social anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1673–1679.
doi:10.1016/j.brat.2005.11.005
Wallace, S. T., & Alden, L. E. (1997). Social phobia and positive social
events: The price of success. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106,
416 424. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.106.3.416
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Carey, G. (1988). Positive and negative
affectivity and their relation to anxiety and depressive disorders. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 346 –353. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.97.3
.346
Watson, D., & Naragon-Gainey, K. (2010). On the specificity of positive
emotional dysfunction in psychopathology: Evidence from the mood and
anxiety disorders and schizophrenia/schizotypy. Clinical Psychology
Review, 30, 839 848. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.002
Weiss, H. M., Beal, D. J., Lucy, S. L., & MacDermid, S. M. (2004).
Constructing EMA studies with PMAT: The Purdue Momentary Assess-
ment Tool user’s manual. West Lafayette, IN: Military Research Insti-
tute at Purdue University.
Wells, A., Clark, D. M., Salkovskis, P., Ludgate, J., Hackmann, A., &
Gelder, M. (1995). Social phobia: The role of in-situation safety behav-
iors in maintaining anxiety and negative beliefs. Behavior Therapy, 26,
153–161. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(05)80088-7
Received October 12, 2012
Revision received March 20, 2013
Accepted March 25, 2013
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
11
DISTINGUISHING DISORDER
... S urvivors of early childhood emotional and psychological neglect often find adult experiences of shared positive emotional states aversive due to their being unfamiliar-and thus confusing and anxiety-provoking (Kashdan et al., 2013). These individuals lack access to early adaptive memory networks (Solomon & Shapiro, 2008) of shared positive affect linked to a positive model of self and thus they cannot predict a positive outcome from such interactions (Cotraccia, 2022). ...
... Survivors of childhood emotional neglect with adult SAD compared with healthy controls show "less neurological activity, slower reaction times, and poorer memory for accepting happy faces" (Kashdan, 2007, p. 359). Therefore, Kashdan et al. (2013) suggest "Clinicians might consider expanding the explicit target of interventions for SAD to include the reduction of experiential avoidance and increase of positive experiences." (p. ...
Article
Research has highlighted the negative effects of early neglect and abuse on the development of emotional self-regulation and attachment. Attention has focused more on negative affect states related to past adverse and traumatic experiences than on the deactivating effects of the absence of early shared positive affect states from parent-infant play, warmth, and affection. Treatment strategies for survivors of early neglect and abuse have focused on methods to metabolize memories of traumatic events. Skills development models have emphasized the benefits of distress tolerance, acceptance, and mindfulness training for patients with persistent negative affect states. Research on the benefits of therapeutic interventions intended to increase positive affect indicates they promote prosocial behaviors and creativity, broaden the scope of attention, reduce emotional symptoms and behavioral problems, and improve physical health. Within the field of eye movement desensitization and reprocessing (EMDR) therapy, procedures to increase positive affect have generally focused on resource development and installation procedures intended to increase access to positive affect-related memories and images. A neglected clinical issue is the inability of survivors of early neglect to tolerate and integrate actual positive interpersonal experience into positive emotional states, interpersonal scripts, or self-concepts. These deficiencies, characteristic of those with dismissing insecure attachment, help produce and maintain profound psychological, medical, and social problems that limit the ability of patients with these histories from progressing in psychotherapy. This article explores research supporting the potential benefits of an EMDR-related procedure, the positive affect tolerance (PAT) and integration protocol, along with possible mechanisms by which the PAT protocol in general, and its bilateral stimulation procedures, might produce clinical benefits for survivors of early neglect. Case vignettes illustrate the potential challenges of implementing and the potential impact of the PAT protocol. Research designs and assessment tools are described that could clarify the potential benefits of the PAT protocol compared to other treatments designed to increase positive affect.
... Momentary social anxiety was assessed with the three items suggested in Kashdan and Steger 67 (e.g., 'I worried that I would say or do something wrong right now'). It has been used in previous ESM studies 68,69 . In the current study, the within-and between-person reliabilities were 0.84 and 0.99 respectively. ...
Article
Full-text available
Social anxiety and paranoia often co-occur and exacerbate each other. While loneliness and negative schemas contribute to the development of social anxiety and paranoia separately, their role in the development of the two symptoms co-occurring is rarely considered longitudinally. This study examined the moment-to-moment relationship between social anxiety and paranoia, as well as the effects of loneliness and negative schemas on both experiences individually and coincidingly. A total of 134 non-clinical young adults completed experience sampling assessments of momentary social anxiety, paranoia, and loneliness ten times per day for six consecutive days. Participants’ negative-self and -other schemas were assessed with the Brief Core Schema Scale. Dynamic structural equation modelling revealed a bidirectional relationship between social anxiety and paranoia across moments. Loneliness preceded increases in both symptoms in the next moment. Higher negative-self schema was associated with a stronger link from paranoia to social anxiety; whereas higher negative-other schema was associated with a stronger link from social anxiety to paranoia. Our findings support the reciprocal relationship between social anxiety and paranoia. While loneliness contributes to the development of social anxiety and paranoia, negative self and other schemas appear to modify the relationships between the two symptoms.
... It has been found that EA readily provokes paradoxical effects, increasing the intensity and occurrence of the internal events a person is trying to prevent [13]. Numerous studies have linked EA with social anxiety, reporting significant positive associations [e.g., [14][15][16][17]. These findings have contributed to important adjustments in the treatment of SAD, resulting in an increased implementation of mindfulness-and acceptancebased interventions [e.g., 14]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Both anxiety sensitivity (AS) and experiential avoidance (EA) have been linked to social anxiety disorder (SAD). However, previous studies did not consider their joint variance and the heterogeneity of SAD. In this mixed methods cross-sectional survey, we examined 121 online participants (age range: 16–70 years) who self-reported as socially anxious. We compared AS and EA levels in individuals with a primary fear of noticeable anxiety symptoms vs. behaving ineptly. AS and EA were highly prevalent across the sample. Surprisingly, the noticeable symptoms subtype showed slightly lower AS and EA levels than the inept behavior subtype. The noticeable symptoms subtype scored notably lower on social anxiety measures (mean = 69.8) than the inept behavior subtype (mean = 89.3). EA was uniquely associated with social anxiety in both subtypes, while AS was uniquely associated with social anxiety only in the inept behavior subtype. The joint variance explained substantially more of the noticeable symptoms subtype’s social anxiety (32.5%) compared to the inept behavior subtype’s (9.4%). Qualitative themes aligned with these findings, indicating a self-reinforcing dynamic between high AS, high EA, and social anxiety symptoms. Potential clinical implications are discussed. Future research should examine causality in the AS-EA-SAD dynamic, considering the heterogeneity of SAD.
... Interestingly, curiosity is associated with being more comfortable with anxiety (Kashdan et al., 2013), allowing risk-taking behavior, which has long been acknowledged as integral to creativity (Dewett, 2007;Eisenman, 2013;Runco, 2015;Simpson Steele, 2015;Sternberg & Lubart, 1992). Taking sensible risks was postulated to be a prerequisite for creativity, as it promotes breakthroughs and innovativeness (Baas et al., 2015;Sternberg & Lubart, 1992), further linking uncertainty with exploration and novelty in creativity. ...
Article
Curiosity and creativity are central pillars of human growth and invention. While they have been studied extensively in isolation, the relationship between them has not yet been established. We propose that curiosity and creativity both emanate from the same mechanism of novelty-seeking. We first present a synthesis showing that curiosity and creativity are affected similarly by a number of key cognitive faculties such as memory, cognitive control, attention, and reward. We then review empirical evidence from neuroscience research, indicating that the same brain regions are involved in both curiosity and creativity, focusing on the interplay between three major brain networks: the default-mode network, the salience network, and the executive control network. After substantiating the link between curiosity and creativity, we propose a novelty-seeking model (NSM) that underlies them both and suggest that the manifestation of the NSM is governed by one's state of mind (SoM).
... Despite reporting findings that were at odds with our initial predictions, our paper adds to the previously literature suggesting "experiential avoidance" 18,19 , or lack of tolerance, to be present both in OCD and in SAD 20,21 . Our results expand the findings linking decreased tolerance to severity of anxiety 6 , showing it to be particularly relevant in anxiety disorders that, according to the model by Gray and McNaughton 22 , are more clearly characterized by avoidable (i.e. ...
Article
Background: Although research has shown that mood and anxiety disorders manifest disturbed emotion regulation, it is unclear whether anxiety disorders differ between each other in terms of their emotion regulation strategies. In the present study, we investigated whether patients with anxiety disorders present different affective styles. Methods: We assessed affective styles of 32 obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) patients, 29 social anxiety disorder (SAD) patients, 29 panic disorder (PD) patients, and 20 healthy controls through the Affective Style Questionnaire (ASQ). A multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) was performed to compare the affective styles across groups (OCD, SAD, PD and control), while controlling for depression, anxiety symptoms and age. Results: The MANCOVA revealed a significant, small-medium, main effect of diagnostic group on affective styles. The planned contrasts revealed that OCD and SAD patients reported significantly lower scores for "tolerance" (ASQ-T) compared to healthy controls group. There were no differences between PD group and healthy controls. Conclusions: Our findings provide evidence that OCD and SAD have difficulty tolerating strong emotions existing in the present moment in an open and non-defensive way.
... -Social presence questions are inspired by Nowak and Biocca [16] and Bailenson, Blascovich, Beall and Loomis [2] (but see also Swidrak, Pochwatko and Matejuk [21]); social interaction by Nezlek, Imbrie and Shean [15]. -The measures related to the mental state recorded after each meeting were based on Kashdan et al. [11] and Ciarocco, Twenge, Muraven, and Tice [6]. ...
Preprint
Alpha-XR Mission conducted by XR Lab PJAIT focused on research related to individual and crew well-being and participatory team collaboration in ICE (isolated, confined and extreme) conditions. In this two-week mission within an analog space habitat, collaboration, objective execution and leisure was facilitated and studied by virtual reality (VR) tools. The mission commander and first officer, both experienced with virtual reality, took part in daily briefings with mission control. In the first week the briefings were voice-only conducted via a channel on Discord. During the following week last briefings were conducted in VR, using Horizon Workrooms. This qualitative pilot study employing participatory observation revealed that VR facilitates communication, especially on complex problems and experiences, providing the sense of emotional connection and shared understanding, that may be lacking in audio calls. The study points to the need to further explore VR-facilitated communication in high-stake environments as it may improve relationships, well-being, and communication outcomes.
... There is limited research on PVS functioning within the spectrum of anxiety disorders, with most studies focusing on patients with specific anxiety disorders such as SAD and GAD, e.g., (14)(15)(16). These studies suggest that individuals with SAD and GAD tend to have reduced positive experiences and use experiential avoidance as a coping mechanism. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Anxiety and depressive disorders share common features of mood dysfunctions. This has stimulated interest in transdiagnostic dimensional research as proposed by the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) approach by the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) aiming to improve the understanding of underlying disease mechanisms. The purpose of this study was to investigate the processing of RDoC domains in relation to disease severity in order to identify latent disorder-specific as well as transdiagnostic indicators of disease severity in patients with anxiety and depressive disorders. Methods: Within the German research network for mental disorders, 895 participants (n = 476 female, n = 602 anxiety disorder, n = 257 depressive disorder) were recruited for the Phenotypic, Diagnostic and Clinical Domain Assessment Network Germany (PD-CAN) and included in this cross-sectional study. We performed incremental regression models to investigate the association of four RDoC domains on disease severity in patients with affective disorders: Positive (PVS) and Negative Valance System (NVS), Cognitive Systems (CS) and Social Processes (SP). Results: The results confirmed a transdiagnostic relationship for all four domains, as we found significant main effects on disease severity within domain-specific models (PVS: β = −0.35; NVS: β = 0.39; CS: β = −0.12; SP: β = −0.32). We also found three significant interaction effects with main diagnosis showing a disease-specific association. Limitations: The cross-sectional study design prevents causal conclusions. Further limitations include possible outliers and heteroskedasticity in all regression models which we appropriately controlled for. Conclusion: Our key results show that symptom burden in anxiety and depressive disorders is associated with latent RDoC indicators in transdiagnostic and disease-specific ways.
Article
Previous research has found associations between mental health difficulties and interpretation biases, including heightened interpretation of threat from neutral or ambiguous stimuli. Building on this research, we explored associations between interpretation biases (positive and negative) and three constructs that have been linked to migrant experience: mental health symptoms (GSI), post-migration living difficulties (PMLD) and perceived ethnic discrimination (PEDQ). Two hundred and thirty students who identified as first- (n = 94) or second-generation ethnic minority migrants (n = 68), and first-generation White migrants (n = 68) completed measures of GSI, PEDQ, and PMLD. They also performed an interpretation bias task using Point-Light Walkers (PLW), dynamic stimuli with reduced visual input that are easily perceived as humans performing an action. Five categories of PLW were used: four that clearly depicted human forms undertaking positive, neutral, negative, or ambiguous actions, and a fifth that involved scrambled animations with no clear action or form. Participants were asked to imagine their interaction with the stimuli and rate their friendliness (positive interpretation bias) and aggressiveness (interpretation bias for threat). We found that the three groups differed on PEDQ and PMLD, with no significant differences in GSI, and the three measured were positively correlated. Poorer mental health and increased PMLD were associated with a heightened interpretation for threat of scrambled animations only. These findings have implications for understanding of the role of threat biases in mental health and the migrant experience.
Article
Full-text available
Background Impaired processing of emotional expression recognition (EER) has been theorized to be a maintaining factor of social anxiety disorder (SAD). However, the evidence suggesting a decreased ability to recognize emotional facial expressions in this population is mixed. Methods We performed a systematic review and meta-analysis investigating the relationship between SAD and EER. We included English peer-reviewed full-text articles with (1) a primary diagnosis of SAD, (2) an EER task using a validated face database, and (3) accuracy and/or reaction time as outcome measures. Risk of Bias was assessed using the RoBANS, and results were synthesized using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis. The literature search took place between April 17, 2020 and January 26, 2022. Sixteen articles (N = 788) were selected from a search of six databases. EER was assessed through accuracy and reaction time, with stimulus duration and comorbidity considered as moderators.ResultsOur results indicate that individuals with SAD may have difficulty recognizing emotional expressions overall, and when presenting with a comorbidity demonstrate poorer performance at recognizing happy facial expressions. Individuals with SAD also take a greater amount of time to recognize happy facial expressions.Conclusion Our results suggest that individuals with SAD demonstrate poorer performance in accurately recognizing various expressions, such as happy facial expressions. Given the narrow aim of this study, some results are underpowered.
Article
Full-text available
The present research describes the development and psychometric evaluation of a second version of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (AAQ-II), which assesses the construct referred to as, variously, acceptance, experiential avoidance, and psychological inflexibility. Results from 2,816 participants across six samples indicate the satisfactory structure, reliability, and validity of this measure. For example, the mean alpha coefficient is .84 (.78–.88), and the 3-and 12-month test–retest reliability is .81 and .79, respectively. Results indicate that AAQ-II scores concurrently, longitudinally, and incrementally predict a range of outcomes, from mental health to work absence rates, that are consistent with its underlying theory. The AAQ-II also demonstrates appropriate discriminant validity. The AAQ-II appears to measure the same concept as the AAQ-I (r = .97) but with better psychometric consistency.
Article
Joint effects of daily events and dispositional sensitivities to cues of reward and punishment on daily positive affect (PA) and negative affect (NA) were examined in 3 diary studies. Study 1 showed that positive events were strongly related to PA but not NA, whereas negative events were strongly related to NA but not PA. Studies 2 and 3 examined how the dispositional sensitivities of independent appetitive and aversive motivational systems, the Behavioral Activation System (BAS) and the Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS), moderated these relationships. Participants in Study 2 with higher BAS sensitivity reported more PA on average; those with more sensitive BIS reported more NA. Also, BIS moderated reactions to negative events, such that higher BIS sensitivity magnified reactions to negative events. Study 3 replicated these findings and showed that BAS predisposed people to experience more positive events. Results demonstrate the value of distinguishing within-person and between-person effects to clarify the functionally independent processes by which dispositional sensitivities influence affect.
Chapter
Social anxiety disorder (SAD) is one of the most common and impairing psychological disorders. To advance our understanding of SAD, several researchers have put forth explanatory models over the years, including one which we originally published almost two decades ago (Rapee & Heimberg, 1997), which delineated the processes by which socially anxious individuals are affected by their fear of evaluation in social situations. Our model, as revised in the 2010 edition of this text, is summarized and further updated based on recent research on the multiple processes involved in the maintenance of SAD.
Article
This volume provides an overview of research methods in contemporary social psychology. Coverage includes conceptual issues in research design, methods of research, and statistical approaches. Because the range of research methods available for social psychology have expanded extensively in the past decade, both traditional and innovative methods are presented. The goal is to introduce new and established researchers alike to new methodological developments in the field.
Article
Anxiety disorder research may benefit from examining motivational factors, such as the difference between approach and avoidance goals. Self-regulation theories in particular suggest that affect serves as feedback for goal pursuit, with anxiety primarily providing feedback regarding avoidance. However, little information is available on participant goals for a specific task that generates social anxiety. In Study 1, we demonstrate that confidence regarding avoidance goals, importance ascribed to these goals, and the interaction between confidence and importance showed the most robust relationships with activated negative affect and positive affect ratings. In Study 2, we partially replicated the interactive effect but fully replicated the general finding of the importance of confidence in avoidance goals for both types of affect. Against hypothesis, ratings regarding avoidance goals were also more strongly related to positive affect. It appears likely that the various findings regarding cost and probability in the study of social anxiety disorder are related to these findings.
Article
Social phobia has become a focus of increased research since its inclusion in DSM-III. However, assessment of social phobia has remained an underdeveloped area, especially self-report assessment. Clinical researchers have relied on measures that were developed on college populations, and these measures may not provide sufficient coverage of the range of situations feared by social phobic individuals. There is a need for additional instruments that consider differences in the types of situations (social interaction vs. situations involving observation by others) that may be feared by social phobics and between subgroups of social phobic patients. This study provides validational data on two instruments developed by Mattick and Clarke (1989): the Social Interaction Anxiety Scale (SIAS), a measure of anxiety in social interactional situations, and the Social Phobia Scale (SPS), a measure of anxiety in situations involving observation by others. These data support the use of the SIAS and SPS in the assessment of individuals with social phobia.
Article
The development and validation of the Social Phobia Scale (SPS) and the Social Interaction Anxiety Scale (SIAS) two companion measures for assessing social phobia fears is described. The SPS assesses fears of being scrutinised during routine activities (eating, drinking, writing, etc.), while the SIAS assesses fears of more general social interaction, the scales corresponding to the DSM-III-R descriptions of Social Phobia—Circumscribed and Generalised types, respectively. Both scales were shown to possess high levels of internal consistency and test–retest reliability. They discriminated between social phobia, agoraphobia and simple phobia samples, and between social phobia and normal samples. The scales correlated well with established measures of social anxiety, but were found to have low or non-significant (partial) correlations with established measures of depression, state and trait anxiety, locus of control, and social desirability. The scales were found to change with treatment and to remain stable in the face of no-treatment. It appears that these scales are valid, useful, and easily scored measures for clinical and research applications, and that they represent an improvement over existing measures of social phobia.