ArticlePDF Available

Metacognitive strategies in student learning: Do students practise retrieval when they study on their own?

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Basic research on human learning and memory has shown that practising retrieval of information (by testing the information) has powerful effects on learning and long-term retention. Repeated testing enhances learning more than repeated reading, which often confers limited benefit beyond that gained from the initial reading of the material. Laboratory research also suggests that students lack metacognitive awareness of the mnemonic benefits of testing. The implication is that in real-world educational settings students may not engage in retrieval practise to enhance learning. To investigate students' real-world study behaviours, we surveyed 177 college students and asked them (1) to list strategies they used when studying (an open-ended free report question) and (2) to choose whether they would reread or practise recall after studying a textbook chapter (a forced report question). The results of both questions point to the same conclusion: A majority of students repeatedly read their notes or textbook (despite the limited benefits of this strategy), but relatively few engage in self-testing or retrieval practise while studying. We propose that many students experience illusions of competence while studying and that these illusions have significant consequences for the strategies students select when they monitor and regulate their own learning.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Metacognitive strategies in student learning:
Do students practise retrieval when they study
on their own?
Jeffrey D. Karpicke
Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, USA
Andrew C. Butler and Henry L. Roediger III
Washington University in St. Louis, MO, USA
Basic research on human learning and memory has shown that practising retrieval of information (by
testing the information) has powerful effects on learning and long-term retention. Repeated testing
enhances learning more than repeated reading, which often confers limited benefit beyond that gained
from the initial reading of the material. Laboratory research also suggests that students lack
metacognitive awareness of the mnemonic benefits of testing. The implication is that in real-world
educational settings students may not engage in retrieval practise to enhance learning. To investigate
students’ real-world study behaviours, we surveyed 177 college students and asked them (1) to list
strategies they used when studying (an open-ended, free report question) and (2) to choose whether they
would reread or practise recall after studying a textbook chapter (a forced report question). The results
of both questions point to the same conclusion: A majority of students repeatedly read their notes or
textbook (despite the limited benefits of this strategy), but relatively few engage in self-testing or
retrieval practise while studying. We propose that many students experience illusions of competence
while studying and that these illusions have significant consequences for the strategies students select
when they monitor and regulate their own learning.
Keywords: Testing effect; Retrieval; Metacognition; Strategies.
A powerful way to enhance student learning is by
testing information. When students have been
tested on material they remember more in the
long term than if they had repeatedly studied it.
This phenomenon is known as the testing effect
and shows that the act of retrieving information
from memory has a potent effect on learning,
enhancing long-term retention of the tested
information (for review, see Roediger & Kar-
picke, 2006a). The testing effect is especially
striking in light of current findings showing
limited benefits of repeated reading for student
learning (see Callender & McDaniel, 2009;
McDaniel & Callender, 2008). Our recent re-
search has generalised the testing effect to
educational materials (Butler & Roediger, 2007;
Karpicke & Roediger, 2007, 2008; Roediger &
Karpicke, 2006b) and real-world classroom en-
vironments (see McDaniel, Roediger, & McDer-
mott, 2007). Testing enhances learning not only if
instructors give tests and quizzes in the classroom
but also if students practise recall while they study
#2009 Psychology Press, an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an Informa business
Address correspondence to: Jeffrey D. Karpicke, Department of Psychological Sciences, Purdue University, 703 Third Street,
West Lafayette, IN 47907-2081, USA. E-mail: karpicke@purdue.edu
We thank Stephanie Karpicke and Julie Evans for collecting and scoring the questionnaire data. This research was supported by a
Collaborative Activity Grant from the James S. McDonnell Foundation and a grant from the Institute of Education Science.
MEMORY, 2009, 17 (4), 471!479
http://www.psypress.com/memory DOI:10.1080/09658210802647009
Downloaded By: [Washington University School] At: 14:05 5 May 2009
on their own. If students were to practise retrieval
of information while studying this strategy would
have the potential to greatly improve academic
performance. However, we do not know the
extent to which students practise recall while
they study in real-world educational settings
(relative to other less-effective strategies like
repeated reading) or whether students who prac-
tise recall do so because they are aware of the
mnemonic benefits. These are important and
practically relevant research questions but few
studies have been aimed at answering them (see,
e.g., Kornell & Bjork, 2007).
The objective of this research was to determine
the extent to which students practise recall
relative to other study strategies in real-world
educational settings. In addition we wanted to
examine whether students who choose to engage
in retrieval practice do so because they know that
testing promotes long-term retention. Another
reason students may use testing during studying is
to determine what information is known and what
is not known so that future study time can be
allocated to the unknown material (see Dunlosky,
Hertzog, Kennedy, & Thiede, 2005; Dunlosky,
Rawson, & McDonald, 2002). This is a fine
justification for testing but it differs from using
testing as a learning device in its own right. To
accomplish these goals we created a new study
strategies questionnaire and surveyed a large
sample of undergraduate students. Although
there are a variety of study strategy inventories
in the education literature (see Entwistle &
McCune, 2004; Pintrich, Smith, Garcia, &
McKeachie, 1993; Weinstein, Schulte, & Palmer,
1987) these and other inventories do not specifi-
cally assess whether students practise retrieval
while studying. Our survey included a free report
question asking students to list the strategies they
use while studying and a forced report question
that asked them to choose between repeated
reading or repeated testing. The purpose of
including both forced and free report question
formats was to gain converging evidence aimed at
the target issue and to circumvent possible
response biases created by using either format
alone (see Schuman & Presser, 1996; Schwarz,
1999). We predicted that relatively few students
would report self-testing as a study strategy and
that the majority of students would report choos-
ing to reread or engage in some other non-testing
activity when forced to choose a study strategy.
We also predicted that most students who
selected self-testing would be unaware of the
mnemonic benefits of testing.
In the first section of this paper we provide a
brief overview of relevant research on repeated
reading, repeated testing, and students’ metacog-
nitive awareness of the testing effect. Next we
present the results of our survey of study strate-
gies. In the final section we interpret the survey
results in light of current theories of metacogni-
tion and self-regulated learning and then discuss
the practical and educational implications of our
findings.
MOTIVATION FOR THE SURVEY:
PRIOR RESEARCH ON REPEATED
READING VS REPEATED TESTING
The testing effect refers to the finding that taking
a test enhances long-term retention more than
spending an equivalent amount of time repeat-
edly studying. There are clear and direct implica-
tions of the testing effect for student learning.
One way for students to enhance their learning
would be to practise recalling information while
studying. However, research on the testing effect
has also shown that when students are asked to
assess their own learning they sometimes fail to
predict that testing enhances learning more than
repeated reading (e.g., Karpicke & Roediger,
2008). In short, there is a rapidly growing body
of research (briefly reviewed below) indicating
that testing has powerful effects on learning but
students lack metacognitive awareness of the
testing effect.
Students often report that they repeatedly read
their notes or textbook while studying (Carrier,
2003; Pressley, Van Etten, Yokoi, Freebern, &
Van Meter, 1998; Van Etten, Freebern, & Press-
ley, 1997). Yet there are several reasons to
question the effectiveness of repetitive reading
beyond reading a single time. Basic research on
memory has shown that spending extra time
maintaining or holding items in memory does
not by itself promote learning (Craik & Watkins,
1973) and students may spend large amounts of
additional time studying despite no gain in later
memory for the items, a phenomenon called
‘‘labour-in-vain’’ during learning (Nelson & Leo-
nesio, 1988). Recent research with educationally
relevant materials has shown that repeatedly
reading prose passages produces limited benefits
beyond a single reading (Amlund, Kardash, &
Kulhavy, 1986; Callender & McDaniel, 2009).
472 KARPICKE, BUTLER, ROEDIGER
Downloaded By: [Washington University School] At: 14:05 5 May 2009
This is especially true when repeated readings are
massed together in a single learning session,
although spaced rereading tends to produce
positive effects (Rawson & Kintsch, 2005). In
short, memory research has shown many times
that repetitive reading by itself is not an effective
strategy for promoting learning and long-term
retention (for review, see McDaniel & Callender,
2008).
In contrast, several studies have shown that
repeated testing is a potent method for producing
robust learning. In one of our studies (Karpicke &
Roediger, 2008) we had students learn a set of
Swahili vocabulary words across alternating study
and test periods. In study periods students studied
a Swahili word and its English translation (ma-
shua !boat) and in test periods they saw the
Swahili words as cues to recall the English words
(mashua !?). The students learned the words in
one of four conditions and students in all condi-
tions took a final test 1 week after initial learning.
In two learning conditions, once a word was
correctly recalled it was dropped from further
test periods. The students who recalled each word
only once in these two conditions recalled just
35% of the items on the final test a week later. In
the other two conditions students continued to
repeatedly recall words even after they had
recalled them once. Students who repeatedly
recalled the words during learning recalled about
80% of the items on the final test. Repeated
retrieval practice*even after students were able
to successfully recall items in the learning phase*
produced large positive effects on long-term
retention.
Were students aware of the effect of repeated
testing on long-term retention? At the end of the
initial learning phase we asked students to predict
how many pairs they would recall on the final test
a week later. There was no difference in average
predictions across the four conditions: All groups
predicted they would recall about 50% of the
items. Despite the large effect of repeated retrie-
val on retention, students were not aware of the
mnemonic benefit of testing. Similar findings
have occurred in other experiments examining
the testing effect and students’ judgements of
learning (e.g., Agarwal, Karpicke, Kang, Roedi-
ger, & McDermott, 2008; Karpicke, McCabe, &
Roediger, 2006; Roediger & Karpicke, 2006b).
In sum, basic laboratory research on human
learning and memory has shown that (1) repeated
reading by itself is a questionable and often
ineffective study strategy, (2) repeated retrieval
practice produces robust learning and long-term
retention, but (3) students appear to lack meta-
cognitive awareness of the testing effect. The
implication of this basic research is that students
may not practise retrieval when they study in real-
world educational settings. Instead they may
spend their time repeatedly reading material
when they study. The objective of our survey
was to examine the prevalence of retrieval
practice, relative to other study strategies, in
students’ real-world study behaviours and stu-
dents’ metacognitive awareness of the benefits of
self-testing.
A SURVEY OF STUDENTS’ LEARNING
STRATEGIES
One reaction we have encountered when we
present our research on the testing effect goes
something like this: ‘‘This is completely obvious.
Of course testing enhances learning. We already
knew this. None of this is new or surprising.’’
Perhaps the testing effect is obvious to some
instructors*but is it obvious to students? If so
we would expect students to report that they
frequently practise recall while studying. But our
basic laboratory research has consistently shown
that students lack metacognitive awareness of the
testing effect. In fact students sometimes predict
that repeated reading will produce better long-
term retention than repeated testing (Roediger &
Karpicke, 2006b). The intent of our survey was to
determine whether students’ self-reported study
behaviours would converge with our laboratory
findings.
Method
We surveyed 177 undergraduate students at
Washington University in St. Louis about the
strategies they use to study for exams. The
students were participants in various learning
and memory experiments in our laboratory and
they completed the survey at the very end of their
experimental session. Washington University stu-
dents are a select group with average SAT scores
greater than 1400 (Verbal"Quantitative). Our
survey included two questions aimed at identify-
ing how often the students practised recalling
information while studying. Question 1 was
an open-ended free report question in which
students listed the strategies they used when
METACOGNITIVE STRATEGIES IN LEARNING 473
Downloaded By: [Washington University School] At: 14:05 5 May 2009
studying and then rank ordered the strategies in
terms of how frequently they used them. All 177
students answered Question 1. Question 2 was a
forced report question that asked students to
imagine they were studying a textbook chapter
for an exam and to choose one of three alter-
natives: (1) repeated reading of the chapter, (2)
practising recall of material from the chapter
(with or without the opportunity to reread the
chapter, in different versions of this question), or
(3) engaging in some other study activity. A total
of 101 students answered Version 1 of Question 2
(testing without restudy) and the other 76 stu-
dents answered Version 2 (testing with restudy).
Students completed the entire questionnaire in
about 5 to 10 minutes. Our goals were to identify
students’ typical study strategies and to assess
how frequently they repeatedly read material or
engaged in retrieval practice, and our analysis
focused on the frequency with which students
reported these particular strategies.
Results
Question 1: Students’ free report of study
strategies. The first question on the survey asked:
‘‘What kind of strategies do you use when you are
studying? List as many strategies as you use and
rank-order them from strategies you use most
often to strategies you use least often.’’ We
initially reviewed all responses from all students.
Based on our initial assessment we identified 11
strategies that occurred relatively frequently
(more than once across all student responses).
Two independent raters then categorised all
responses. There was close to 100% agreement
between the two raters and the first author
resolved any scoring discrepancies.
Figure 1 shows the frequency distribution of
the number of strategies listed by students in
response to Question 1. The figure shows that
most students listed and described three strate-
gies (M#2.9). Table 1 shows the 11 strategies and
the percent of students who listed each strategy.
The table also shows the percent of students who
ranked each strategy as their number one strategy
and the mean rank of each strategy. Repeated
reading was by far the most frequently listed
strategy with 84% of students reporting it. Not
only did students indicate that they repeatedly
read while studying but they also indicated that
rereading was a favoured strategy*55% of stu-
dents reported that rereading was the number one
strategy they used when studying. Table 1 also
shows another key finding: Only 11% of students
(19 of 177) reported that they practised retrieval
while studying. These students unambiguously
indicated in their list of strategies that they
practised testing themselves by recalling informa-
tion while they studied. Only 1% (2 of 177
students) identified practising recall as their
number one strategy. The results in Table 1
clearly show that a large majority of students
repeatedly read their textbook or notes but
relatively few students engage in self-testing by
practicing recall while studying.
Table 1 also shows that students reported other
strategies that could be interpreted as forms of
self-testing. For example, 43% of students indi-
cated that they answer practice problems while
studying and 40% reported using flashcards. Each
activity could be interpreted as a type of self-
testing, but of course there are ways students
might use these study methods without engaging
in retrieval practice. For example, students may
read practice questions and then look up and
copy answers from the text. This would qualify as
answering practice problems but students who do
this would not be practising or even attempting
recall of the answers. Likewise, students may
write facts on flashcards and repeatedly read
them rather than practising recall. A clear limita-
tion of the free response question is that our
procedure did not prompt each student to
Figure 1. Frequency distribution showing the number of
strategies listed by students.
474 KARPICKE, BUTLER, ROEDIGER
Downloaded By: [Washington University School] At: 14:05 5 May 2009
elaborate on potentially ambiguous answers (cf.
the ethnographic interviewing technique of Press-
ley and colleagues; Pressley et al., 1998; Van
Etten et al., 1997). Nevertheless, even if we
considered the 40% of students who use flash-
cards or the 43% who answer practice problems
as students engaging in forms of self-testing, these
percentages are dwarfed by the 84% of students
who repeatedly read while studying.
The results of Question 1 indicate that re-
peated reading is the most popular study strategy
among college students (see too Carrier, 2003),
far more popular than practising retrieval, even
though retrieval practice is a more effective study
strategy. Students listed a variety of study strate-
gies but indicated that they use these alternative
study strategies far less frequently than repeated
reading. Question 2 asked students to choose
repeated reading or self-testing and prompted
them to explain the reasoning behind their choice.
By including a second question in forced report
format we hoped to find converging evidence and
to resolve ambiguities inherent in our first open-
ended free report question.
Question 2: Forced report questions about
repeated studying vs testing. Question 2 was a
forced report question about repeated studying
versus repeated testing. There were two versions
of the question. Version 1 asked students to
consider testing without going back and re-
studying, and Version 2 involved testing followed
by restudying (to get feedback after attempting
recall). The first version was given to 101 students
and the second version was given to 76 students.
Version 1 of Question 2 was as follows:
Imagine you are reading a textbook chapter for
an upcoming exam. After you have read the
chapter one time, would you rather:
A. Go back and restudy either the entire
chapter or certain parts of the chapter.
B. Try to recall material from the chapter
(without the possibility of restudying the
material).
C. Use some other study technique.
The students were asked to select one alter-
native and write a brief explanation for their
choice. The scenario described in the question
was based directly on our research showing that
taking a recall test, even without feedback,
enhances long-term retention more than spending
the same amount of time restudying (Roediger &
Karpicke, 2006b).
Table 2 shows the percentage of students who
chose to restudy, self-test, or do something else
after reading a textbook chapter. Most students
unambiguously selected an alternative and ex-
plained their choice, but four students gave
ambiguous responses that could not be scored.
The table shows that 57% of students chose to
restudy (option A) and 21% indicated that they
would use some other study technique (option C).
Thus 78% of students indicated they would not
want to test themselves after reading a textbook
chapter. Only 18% of the students indicated that
they would self-test after studying (option B). To
examine students’ metacognitive awareness of the
TABLE 1
Results of Question 1
Strategy Percent who list strategy Percent who rank as #1 strategy Mean rank
1. Rereading notes or textbook 83.6 (148) 54.8 (97) 1.5
2. Do practice problems 42.9 (76) 12.4 (22) 2.1
3. Flashcards 40.1 (71) 6.2 (11) 2.6
4. Rewrite notes 29.9 (53) 12.4 (22) 1.8
5. Study with a group of students 26.5 (47) 0.5 (1) 2.9
6. ‘‘Memorise’’ 18.6 (33) 5.6 (10) 2.0
7. Mnemonics (acronyms, rhymes, etc) 13.5 (24) 2.8 (5) 2.4
8. Make outlines or review sheets 12.9 (23) 3.9 (7) 2.1
9. Practise recall (self-testing) 10.7 (19) 1.1 (2) 2.5
10. Highlight (in notes or book) 6.2 (11) 1.6 (3) 2.3
11. Think of real life examples 4.5 (8) 0.5 (1) 2.8
Percent of students listing different learning strategies, percent who ranked strategies as their #1 strategy, and mean rankings of
strategies. Raw numbers of students are in parentheses.
Mean number of strategies listed was 2.9 (SD#0.96). Percentages of students indicating their #1 strategy do not add to 100%
because some students merged multiple strategies when reporting their #1 strategy (e.g., indicating that rereading and rewriting
notes were their #1 strategy).
METACOGNITIVE STRATEGIES IN LEARNING 475
Downloaded By: [Washington University School] At: 14:05 5 May 2009
mnemonic benefits of testing we separated stu-
dents’ responses based on their explanation for
why they chose self-testing. This analysis showed
that 10% of all students (or more than half of
those who chose self-testing) reported they would
self-test to generate feedback and guide their
future studying (even though Version 1 of this
question stated that students could not restudy
after testing). Only 8% of all students indicated
that they would test themselves because practis-
ing retrieval would help them do well on the
upcoming exam. This pattern of responding
suggests that most students were unaware of the
mnemonic benefits of self-testing. The results of
Version 1 of this forced report question provide
converging evidence with our first free report
question. Relatively few students reported that
they would test themselves after studying a text-
book chapter and even fewer indicated they
would test themselves because they knew the
act of practising recall was valuable for learning.
In Version 2 of Question 2 the scenario and
alternatives were identical to Version 1 except
that option B read ‘‘Try to recall material from
the chapter (with the possibility of restudying
afterward).’’ We imagined this would increase the
number of students choosing testing perhaps to
levels near ceiling if students recognised that
testing followed by rereading would produce far
superior learning to rereading without testing.
Table 3 shows the percent of students who chose
each option. The percentage of students choosing
self-testing increased when students could reread
after the test (42% in Question 2 vs 18% in
Question 1) and the percentage was about equal
to the percentage of students choosing repeated
reading (42% vs 41%). Students’ explanations of
their choices indicated that the increased like-
lihood of choosing testing was due to the possi-
bility of restudying after the test. Of the 32
students who chose self-testing, 25 provided
unambiguous explanations that we categorised
as testing for feedback or testing to practise
recall. A total of 23 students (30%) indicated
that they would test themselves to generate
feedback they could use when restudying
whereas only two students (3%) chose testing
because they believed the act of practising recall
would help them remember in the future. The
results of Version 2 of Question 2 expand on the
results of Version 1 by showing that students were
more likely to select self-testing when they could
restudy after testing but that very few students
are aware that the act of practising recall itself
enhances learning. What is perhaps most striking
about the data in Table 3 is that even when
students had the option of rereading after self-
testing, the majority of students (58%) continued
to indicate that they would not test themselves.
DISCUSSION
The objective of this research was to collect
benchmark data on college students’ real-world
study behaviours to assess how often students use
retrieval practice relative to other strategies and
whether they know about the mnemonic benefits
of self-testing. Our basic laboratory studies sug-
gested that students are not aware of the testing
effect, leading us to predict that they may not
practise retrieval while studying in real-world
settings. The results of our survey support this
prediction. The majority of students indicated
that they repeatedly read their notes or textbook
while studying. Relatively few reported that they
TABLE 2
Version 1 of Question 2
Imagine you are reading a
textbook chapter for an upcoming
exam. After you have read the
chapter one time, would you rather: Overall
Test for
feedback
Test to
practise
recall
A. Go back and restudy either the entire
chapter or certain parts of the chapter
57.4 (58)
B. Try to recall material from the chapter
(without the possibility of restudying the material) 17.8 (18) 9.9 (10) 7.9 (8)
C. Use some other study technique 20.7 (21)
Percent of students who chose to restudy, self-test (without restudying), or do something else after reading a textbook chapter.
Raw numbers of students are in parentheses (N#101).
We were unable to score ambiguous responses given by four students.
476 KARPICKE, BUTLER, ROEDIGER
Downloaded By: [Washington University School] At: 14:05 5 May 2009
tested themselves and of those who engaged in
self-testing only a handful reported doing so
because they believed the act of practising
retrieval would improve their learning. Our
survey results point to the conclusion that many
students do not view retrieval practice as a
strategy that promotes learning. If students do
practise recall or test themselves while studying
they do it to generate feedback or knowledge
about the status of their own learning, not
because they believe practising recall itself en-
hances learning.
Our results agree with laboratory experiments
showing that students lack metacognitive aware-
ness of the testing effect when they monitor their
own learning. A growing body of research has
shown that students sometimes predict that
practising retrieval will produce no effect on
retention (Karpicke & Roediger, 2008) or that
they will remember more in the long term if they
repeatedly study material rather than test it
(Agarwal et al., 2008; Karpicke et al., 2006;
Roediger & Karpicke, 2006b). If we assume that
metacognitive monitoring processes guide stu-
dents’ decisions to choose different learning
strategies*an assumption at the core of the
influential monitoring-and-control framework of
metacognition (Nelson & Narens, 1990)*then
the implication of these laboratory results is that
students may not choose to test themselves when
they regulate their own learning in real-world
educational settings. Our survey data confirm
that this lack of awareness of the testing effect
has consequences for students’ real-world study
behaviours.
In addition to agreeing with basic laboratory
findings our survey results also agree to some
extent with a recent survey by Kornell and Bjork
(2007). They surveyed college students about
their study behaviours and asked the students,
‘‘If you quiz yourself while you study ...why do
you do so?’’ The students selected one of four
alternatives: 18% selected ‘‘I learn more that way
than I would through rereading’’; 68% selected
‘‘To figure out how well I have learned the
information I’m studying’’; 4% indicated ‘‘I find
quizzing more enjoyable than rereading’’; and 9%
said ‘‘I usually do not quiz myself.’’ Kornell and
Bjork’s data indicate that the majority of students
(91%) do quiz themselves while studying but few
do so because they view the act of quizzing itself
as a method of enhancing learning (Kornell and
Bjork reasoned that the 18% of students who
selected ‘‘I learn more that way than I would
through rereading’’ believed that quizzing pro-
duced a direct mnemonic benefit; cf. Roediger &
Karpicke, 2006a). Likewise, our survey data
indicate that few students view practising recall
as an activity that enhances learning. However,
far more students indicated that they tested
themselves in the Kornell and Bjork survey than
in our study, and this may be due to a difference
in survey procedures. Whereas we used a combi-
nation of free and forced report questions to
gauge how often students practise retrieval,
Kornell and Bjork used one question focused on
why students might quiz themselves and the
framing of this question may have influenced
students’ responses (see Schuman & Presser,
1996; Schwarz, 1999). It is well known that a
single question can be framed in different ways
and alter the choices and decisions people make
(Tversky & Kahneman, 1981). Nevertheless our
results generally agree with those of Kornell and
Bjork in showing that few students view retrieval
practice as a method of enhancing learning.
Further, the differences between the two sets of
results highlight potentially important differences
between free and forced report methods of
questioning.
TABLE 3
Version 2 of Question 2
Imagine you are reading a textbook
chapter for an upcoming exam. After
you have read the chapter one time,
would you rather: Overall Test for feedback Test to practise recall
A. Go back and restudy either the entire
chapter or certain parts of the chapter
40.8 (31)
B. Try to recall material from the chapter
(with the possibility of restudying afterward) 42.1 (32) 30.3 (23) 2.6 (2)
C. Use some other study technique 17.1 (13)
Percent of students who chose to restudy, self-test followed by restudying, or do something else after reading a textbook chapter.
Raw numbers of students are in parentheses (N#76).
METACOGNITIVE STRATEGIES IN LEARNING 477
Downloaded By: [Washington University School] At: 14:05 5 May 2009
Our results fit with the broad theoretical
notion that students experience illusions of com-
petence when monitoring their own learning
(Bjork, 1999; Jacoby, Bjork, & Kelley, 1994;
Koriat & Bjork, 2005). Koriat and Bjork (2005)
argued that illusions of competence tend to occur
when students’ judgements of learning are biased
by information available during study but not
available during testing (see also Jacoby et al.,
1994). Several experimental findings are consis-
tent with this view. For example, students’ judge-
ments of learning are less accurate when made in
study trials than in test trials (Dunlosky &
Nelson, 1992). Students are less accurate at
judging the difficulty of anagrams when the
solution is present than when it is not (Kelley &
Jacoby, 1996). We believe repeated reading pro-
duces a similar illusion of competence. Specifi-
cally, repeatedly reading material like text
passages increases the fluency or ease with which
students process the text. Students may base their
assessments of their learning and comprehension
on fluency even though their current processing
fluency with the text right in front of them, is
not diagnostic of their future retention. Our
survey results show that the illusions students
experience during learning may have important
consequences and implications for the decisions
they make and the strategies they choose when
studying on their own.
Students generally exhibit little awareness of
the fact that practising retrieval enhances learn-
ing. A clear practical implication is that instruc-
tors should inform students about the benefits of
self-testing and explain why testing enhances
learning. When students rely purely on their
subjective experience while they study (e.g., their
fluency of processing during rereading) they may
fall prey to illusions of competence and believe
they know the material better than they actually
do. A challenge for instructional practice is to
encourage students to base their study strategies
on theories about why a particular strategy*like
practising repeated retrieval*promotes learning
and long-term retention.
Manuscript received 5 October 2008
Manuscript accepted 20 November 2008
REFERENCES
Agarwal, P. K., Karpicke, J. D., Kang, S. H. K.,
Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (2008).
Examining the testing effect with open- and
closed-book tests. Applied Cognitive Psychology,
22, 861!876.
Amlund, J. T., Kardash, C. A. M., & Kulhavy, R. W.
(1986). Repetitive reading and recall of expository
text. Reading Research Quarterly,21, 49!58.
Bjork, R. A. (1999). Assessing our own competence:
Heuristics and illusions. In D. Gopher & A. Koriat
(Eds.), Attention and peformance XVII. Cognitive
regulation of performance: Interaction of theory and
application (pp. 435!459). Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Butler, A. C., & Roediger, H. L. (2007). Testing
improves long-term retention in a simulated class-
room setting. European Journal of Cognitive Psy-
chology,19, 514!527.
Callender, A. A., & McDaniel, M. A. (2009). The
limited benefits of rereading educational texts.
Contemporary Educational Psychology,34, 30!41.
Carrier, L. M. (2003). College students’ choices of study
strategies. Perceptual and Motor Skills,96, 54!56.
Craik, F. I. M., & Watkins, M. J. (1973). The role of
rehearsal in short-term memory. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior,12, 599!607.
Dunlosky, J., Hertzog, C., Kennedy, M. R. F., & Thiede,
K. W. (2005). The self-monitoring approach for
effective learning. Cognitive Technology,10,4!11.
Dunlosky, J., & Nelson, T. O. (1992). Importance of the
kind of cue for judgments of learning (JOL) and the
delayed-JOL effect. Memory & Cognition,20, 374!
380.
Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., & McDonald, S. L. (2002).
Influence of practice test on the accuracy of
predicting memory performance for paired associ-
ates, sentences, and text material. In T. J. Perfect &
B. L. Schwartz (Eds.), Applied metacognition (pp.
68!92). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
Press.
Entwistle, N., & McCune, V. (2004). The conceptual
bases of study strategy inventories. Educational
Psychology Review,16, 325!345.
Jacoby, L. L., Bjork, R. A., & Kelley, C. M. (1994).
Illusions of comprehension, competence, and re-
membering. In D. Druckman & R. A. Bjork (Eds.),
Learning, remembering, believing: Enhancing hu-
man performance (pp. 57!80). Washington, DC:
National Academy Press.
Karpicke, J. D., McCabe, D. P., & Roediger, H. L.
(2006). Testing enhances recollection: Process dis-
sociation estimates and metamemory judgments.
Poster presented at the 47th Annual Meeting of
the Psychonomic Society, Houston, TX.
Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L. (2007). Repeated
retrieval during learning is the key to long-term
retention. Journal of Memory and Language,57,
151!162.
Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L. (2008). The critical
importance of retrieval for learning. Science,319,
966!968.
Kelley, C. M., & Jacoby, L. L. (1996). Adult egocentr-
ism: Subjective experience versus analytic bases for
judgment. Journal of Memory and Language,35,
157!175.
Koriat, A., & Bjork, R. A. (2005). Illusions of compe-
tence in monitoring one’s knowledge during study.
478 KARPICKE, BUTLER, ROEDIGER
Downloaded By: [Washington University School] At: 14:05 5 May 2009
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, and Cognition,31, 187!194.
Kornell, N., & Bjork, R. A. (2007). The promise and
perils of self-regulated study. Psychonomic Bulletin
& Review,14, 219!224.
McDaniel, M. A., & Callender, A. A. (2008). Cogni-
tion, memory, and education. In H. L. Roediger
(Ed.), Cognitive psychology of memory, Vol. 2 of
Learning and memory: A comprehensive reference
(pp. 819!843). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.
McDaniel, M. A., Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B.
(2007). Generalizing test-enhanced learning from
the laboratory to the classroom. Psychonomic Bul-
letin & Review,14, 200!206.
Nelson, T. O., & Leonesio, R. J. (1988). Allocation of
self-paced study time and the ‘‘labor-in-vain effect’’.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, and Cognition,14, 676!686.
Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A
theoretical framework and new findings. In G. H.
Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and
motivation (Vol. 26, (pp. 125!141)). New York:
Academic Press.
Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A. F., Garcia, T., & McKea-
chie, W. J. (1993). Reliability and predictive validity
of the Motivated Strategies for Learning Question-
naire (MSLQ). Educational and Psychological Mea-
surement,53, 801!813.
Pressley, M., Van Etten, S., Yokoi, L., Freebern, G., &
Van Meter, P. (1998). The metacognition of college
studentship: A grounded theory approach. In D. J.
Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.),
Metacognition in educational theory and practice
(pp. 347!366). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates Inc.
Rawson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (2005). Rereading effects
depend on time of test. Journal of Educational
Psychology,97, 70!80.
Roediger, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006a). The power
of testing memory: Basic research and implications
for educational practice. Perspectives on Psycholo-
gical Science,1, 181!210.
Roediger, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006b). Test
enhanced learning: Taking memory tests improves
long-term retention. Psychological Science,17, 249!
255.
Schuman, H., & Presser, S. (1996). Questions and
answers in attitude surveys. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Schwarz, N. (1999). Self-reports: How the questions
shape the answers. American Psychologist,54, 93!
105.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of
decisions and the psychology of choice. Science,211,
453!458.
Van Etten, S., Freebern, G., & Pressley, M. (1997).
College students’ beliefs about exam preparation.
Contemporary Educational Psychology,22, 192!212.
Weinstein, C. E., Schulte, A. C., & Palmer, D. R.
(1987). Learning and Study Strategies Inventory
(LASSI). Clearwater, FL: H&H Publishing.
METACOGNITIVE STRATEGIES IN LEARNING 479
Downloaded By: [Washington University School] At: 14:05 5 May 2009
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE
This article was downloaded by:
[Washington University School]
On:
5 May 2009
Access details:
Access Details: [subscription number 908307764]
Publisher
Psychology Press
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK
Memory
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t713683358
Metacognitive strategies in student learning: Do students practise retrieval
when they study on their own?
Jeffrey D. Karpicke a; Andrew C. Butler b; Henry L. Roediger III b
a Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, USA b Washington University in St. Louis, MO, USA
Online Publication Date: 01 May 2009
To cite this Article Karpicke, Jeffrey D., Butler, Andrew C. and Roediger III, Henry L.(2009)'Metacognitive strategies in student learning:
Do students practise retrieval when they study on their own?',Memory,17:4,471 — 479
To link to this Article: DOI: 10.1080/09658210802647009
URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658210802647009
Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf
This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or
systematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or
distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.
The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents
will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses
should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss,
actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly
or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.
... In a very important series of research studies investigating the effect of testing on retention, Karpicke et al. [24] and Roediger III and Karpicke [34] conducted several experiments on students studying passages of prose articles and then measured the forgetting and recall of the material as a function of testing and restudying. They found that testing improves longterm retention compared to restudying. ...
Article
This is a descriptive article illustrating a multi-year research study in mathematics education of college students in the U.S. It focuses on the first two years in the undergraduate mathematics curriculum. The study presents the theory, practice, and results of an innovative instructional program (Keystone model) applied to algebra classes at the college. The research investigated two areas of student learning outcomes: performance in mathematics and improvements in students’ concentration skills. Under the application of this program, students not only achieved significant outcomes in mathematics, as evidenced by formative and summative assessments but also improved their concentration skills, as demonstrated in the gains in national reading comprehension tests. In the following, we present, in extensive detail, the elements of the Keystone approach and its implementation in a learning environment, such as a college mathematics classroom.
... For example, some other strategies reported in the SRL literature [18], such as time management strategies (i.e., keeping track of time), specific goal-setting (i.e., setting clear goals of what they are expected to achieve), environmental restructuring (i.e., using a computer to write the abstract, or preparing writing materials), self-monitoring (through self-questioning), or self-consequences (i.e., promising rewards), were not mentioned at all when students were confronted with the learning task. On the one hand, these findings appear similar to past evidence suggesting that students deploy few strategies when studying [88]. On the other hand, this might indicate that students were very focused on completing the task and had automated the specific task strategies. ...
Article
Full-text available
A lack of qualitative studies examining adolescent students' voices regarding the contextual, relational, and self-regulatory factors that drive their language achievement has been observed. Therefore , the present study aimed to address this issue. Sixteen face-to-face semi-structured interviews were conducted with secondary school students in Greece to document and analyse their perceptions of the factors that influence language achievement. The study was guided by social-cognitive theory and models of self-regulated learning. Computer-assisted thematic analysis was performed using abductive open coding followed by refinement of codes. Three overarching global themes were identified in line with theoretical expectations, namely personal factors (motivation and self-regulatory strategies), relational factors (teachers' practices, parental achievement expectations, peer influences), and structural factors (the educational system). Afterwards, an abductive thematic network analysis was conducted to explore and theorise about potential relationships between the emergent themes in the data. It was found that parental expectations for higher performance and the teachers' behaviours were associated with students' language achievement. Task-related characteristics and peers influenced students' regulation of effort in learning. The students believed that they were not objectively graded and frequent summative assessments created test anxiety. The findings are discussed in light of existing empirical evidence and in terms of educational implications.
Article
Purpose This tutorial guides speech-language pathologists (SLPs) through the research base for expository intervention and evidence-based decision making for clinical implementation. Method In the first half of the tutorial, the nature and development of expository discourse is described, and then attention is turned to the research base that informs SLP expository intervention. The many educational domains in which relevant research can be found and the diverse terminology used are explained. From reading comprehension and compositional writing, two areas with strong research evidence that are suitable to SLP use—text structure and learning strategies—are identified. The second half of the tutorial focuses on crafting SLP interventions from the educational evidence. An expository intervention designed specifically for SLPs from this research base, called Sketch and Speak, is presented. In this intervention, visual and written note-taking strategies are combined with oral formulation and practice strategies and taught through individualized oral interactions around informational texts. SLPs are guided on how to adapt this and other expository treatments to their own priorities and service delivery situations while maintaining quality features or “active ingredients” of researched treatments. Conclusions There is considerable educational research on expository intervention. From this literature, SLPs can strategically adopt and adapt to implement evidence-based interventions to help students understand, speak, and write in this important discourse form.
Preprint
Full-text available
Background Students employ a variety of study strategies to learn and master content in their courses. Strategies vary widely in their effectiveness for promoting deep, long-term learning, yet most students use ineffective strategies frequently. Efforts to educate students about effective study strategies have revealed that knowledge about effective strategies is by itself insufficient for encouraging widespread and lasting changes. An important next step is to uncover factors that influence the decisions students make about study strategy use. We explored the association between beliefs about intelligence (mindset, universality, and brilliance) and study strategies. The most effective study strategies are error-prone, and beliefs about intelligence carry implications for whether errors are a normal and even beneficial part of the learning process (e.g., growth mindset) or signs of insufficient intelligence (e.g., fixed mindset). Therefore, we hypothesized that beliefs about and reactions to errors would mediate a relationship between beliefs about intelligence and study strategies. We tested this hypothesis by surveying 345 undergraduates enrolled in an introductory biology class at a public, research-active university in northwestern United States. Results Confirmatory factor analysis indicated that the internal structure of all measures functioned as expected in our sample. We fit a structural equation model to evaluate our hypothesized model. We found that mindset, but not universality nor brilliance, predicts variance in both beliefs about errors and reactions to errors. In turn, adaptive reactions to errors (but not beliefs about errors) are associated with the use of highly effective study strategies and spacing study sessions. There was a significant indirect relationship between growth mindset and spacing of study sessions. Conclusions Our results provide evidence for a mechanism explaining the association between students’ mindset beliefs and academic outcomes: believing that intelligence is improvable is associated with more adaptive reactions to making errors, which correlates with choosing more error-prone and therefore more effective study strategies. Future interventions aimed at improving students’ study strategies may be more effective if they simultaneously target reacting adaptively to errors and emphasize that intelligence is improvable.
Article
Full-text available
Is self-assessment enough to keep physicians' cognitive skills-such as diagnosis, treatment, basic biological knowledge, and communicative skills-current? We review the cognitive strengths and weaknesses of self-assessment in the context of maintaining medical expertise. Cognitive science supports the importance of accurately self-assessing one's own skills and abilities, and we review several ways such accuracy can be quantified. However, our review also indicates a broad challenge in self-assessment is that individuals do not have direct access to the strength or quality of their knowledge and instead must infer this from heuristic strategies. These heuristics are reasonably accurate in many circumstances, but they also suffer from systematic biases. For example, information that feels easy to process in the moment can lead individuals to overconfidence in their ability to remember it in the future. Another notable phenomenon is the Dunning-Kruger effect: the poorest performers in a domain are also the least accurate in self-assessment. Further, explicit instruction is not always sufficient to remove these biases. We discuss what these findings imply about when physicians' self-assessment can be useful and when it may be valuable to supplement with outside sources.
Article
Retrieval practice typically benefits learning in children, although little is known about the benefits of retrieval practice for learning spelling. We investigated this issue in three experiments with fifth-grade children from a low-income area of Brazil. In the experiments, children first read a list of words (study), and after a short interval wrote down the studied words after hearing and rereading them (copy) or after only hearing them (retrieval practice). After an interval of 4 days, spelling performance was greater for words from the retrieval practice condition than for words from the copy condition, but only when immediate corrective feedback was provided (Experiment 3). The current findings, therefore, suggest that retrieval practice followed by corrective feedback is an effective strategy to improve spelling performance of fifth-grade children.
Article
When retrieving information from memory there is an interplay between memory and metamemory processes, and the prefrontal cortex has been implicated in both memory and metamemory. Previous work shown that High Definition transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (HD-tDCS) over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) can lead to improvements in memory and metamemory monitoring, but findings are mixed. Our original design targeted metamemory, but because the prefrontal cortex plays a role in both memory and metamemory, we tested for effects of HD-tDCS on multiple memory tasks (e.g., recall, cued recall, and recognition) and multiple aspects of metamemory (e.g., once-knew-it ratings, feeling-of-knowing ratings, metamemory accuracy, and metamemory control). There were HD-tDCS-related improvements in cued recall performance, but not other memory tasks. For metamemory, there were HD-tDCS-related increases in subjective once-knew-it ratings, but not other aspects of metamemory. These results highlight the need to consider the effects of HD-tDCS on memory and metamemory at different timepoints during retrieval, as well as specific conditions that show benefits from HD-tDCS.
Article
Full-text available
Although tests and assessments-such as those used to maintain a physician's Board certification-are often viewed merely as tools for decision-making about one's performance level, strong evidence now indicates that the experience of being tested is a powerful learning experience in its own right: The act of retrieving targeted information from memory strengthens the ability to use it again in the future, known as the testing effect. We review meta-analytic evidence for the learning benefits of testing, including in the domain of medicine, and discuss theoretical accounts of its mechanism(s). We also review key moderators-including the timing, frequency, order, and format of testing and the content of feedback-and what they indicate about how to most effectively use testing for learning. We also identify open questions for the optimal use of testing, such as the timing of feedback and the sequencing of complex knowledge domains. Lastly, we consider how to facilitate adoption of this powerful study strategy by physicians and other learners.
Article
Full-text available
This paper reports on a new self-report, Likert-scaled instrument that was designed to assess motivation and use of learning strategies by college students. The motivation scales tap into three broad areas: (1) value (intrinsic and extrinsic goal orientation, task value), (2) expectancy (control beliefs about learning, self-efficacy); and (3) affect (test anxiety). The learning strategies section is comprised of nine scales which can be distinguished as cognitive, metacognitive, and resource management strategies. The cognitive strategies scales include (a) rehearsal, (b) elaboration, (c) organization, and (d) critical thinking. Metacognitive strategies are assessed by one large scale that includes planning, monitoring, and regulating strategies. Resource management strategies include (a) managing time and study environment; (b) effort management, (c) peer learning, and (d) help-seeking. Scale reliabilities are robust, and confirmatory factor analyses demonstrated good factor structure. In addition, the instrument shows reasonable predictive validity to the actual course performance of students.
Chapter
There is a growing theoretical and practical interest in the topic of metacognition; how we monitor and control our mental processes. Applied Metacognition provides a coherent and up-to-date overview of the relation between theories in metacognition and their application in real-world situations. As well as a theoretical overview, there are substantive chapters covering metacognition in three areas of application: metacognition in education, metacognition in everyday life memory and metacognition in different populations. A diverse range of topics are covered such as how we judge our own learning, why we create false beliefs about our past, how children learn to monitor and control their memory, how well eyewitnesses can judge the accuracy of their own memories and how memory judgements change across the lifespan. The book has contributions from many of the leading researchers in metacognition from around the world.
Chapter
Based on findings in basic memory and comprehension research, we note implications for enhancing acquisition of factual information. For each implication, we identify techniques that could be implemented to assist students’ acquisition of material in fact-laden courses. We evaluate the potential effectiveness of those techniques for education practice by reviewing translational research that uses educationally relevant materials and test tasks. Specifically, extending basic memory research, we discuss implications from findings on rote repetition (rereading text), elaboration (elaborative interrogation, advanced organizers, and imagery mnemonics), and desirable difficulty (creating interference, spacing, and generation). We suggest a contextualistic framework for understanding when difficulty is and is not desirable, including considerations of transfer-appropriate processing, material-appropriate processing, and individual learner differences. Building on basic research in comprehensive, we identify study methods that are sensitive to individual differences, techniques to improve successful inferencing, and principled text revisions to enhance comprehension. The final section touches on the mnemonic benefits of retrieval and discusses recent research demonstrating the value of using repeated testing to enhance learning (i.e., to improve performance on final criterial tests). The research discussed in this chapter supports the efforts to apply basic findings in memory and cognition to enhance educational practice.
Chapter
This chapter focuses on research program, providing a description of a theoretical framework that has evolved out of metamemory research, followed by a few remarks about the methodology. Research in metamemory is initiated by the paradoxical findings that people can accurately predict their subsequent likelihood of recognizing nonrecallable items and that they can quickly and accurately decide-on the basis of no more than a cursory search through memory-that they will not retrieve particular sought after items. Those findings lead to develop a methodology based on psychophysical methods that are used to empirically investigate people's feeling of knowing. The results of the experiments convinced that for dealing with only a part of a complex metacognitive system and to account adequately for feeling-of-knowing phenomena, a larger perspective was needed. This eventuated in the present theoretical framework that emphasizes the role of control and monitoring processes. The embedding of the feeling of knowing in a richer framework helped to dissipate the paradoxical nature of the feeling of knowing. The chapter discusses that today there are many capable, active investigators and a wealth of solid empirical findings.
Article
Sixty graduate students instructed to read a 669-word expository passage one, two, or three times completed both free and cued recall measures on three test occasions. Effects of repeated reading of to-be-learned material on recall and retention, as well as persistence of errors over time, were examined. Both quantitative and qualitative differences in recall were found as a function of the number of times text was read. Subjects who read the passage twice prior to first recall remembered significantly more text information than subjects reading the passage one or three times. Subjects who read the passage three times exhibited disproportionate recall of details versus main ideas. Persistence of initial encoding errors was remarkably stable for all groups despite the fact that all subjects reread the passage after the first recall. As expected, errors on the first cued recall were more likely to be repeated on a delayed recall, once they had been repeated on an immediate retest. /// [French] Soixante étudiants gradués à qui on avait demandé de lire un passage narratif de 669 mots une fois, deux fois ou trois fois ont complété des mesures de rappel libre et dirigé selon trois possibilités de test. On a examiné les effets de lecture répétée du matériel à apprendre sur le rappel et le maintien aussi bien que la persistence des erreurs au cours du temps. On a trouvé des différences quantitative et qualitative en rappel comme fonction du nombre de fois que le texte était lu. Les sujets qui avaient lu le passage deux fois avant le premier rappel se sont souvenus de beaucoup plus d'information de texte que les sujets qui avaient lu le passage une ou trois fois. Les sujets qui avaient lu le passage trois fois ont montré un rappel disproportionné d'idées principales par rapport aux détails. La persistance d'erreurs de codification initiales était remarquablement stable pour tous les groupes en dépit du fait que tous les sujets ont relu le passage aprés le premier rappel. Comme prévu, les erreurs du premier rappel dirigé étaient plus susceptibles d'être répétées en rappel différé, une fois qu'elles avaient été répétées sur un test réadministré immédiatement. /// [Spanish] Se aplicaron tres tests de recuerdo libre y asociado a 60 estudiantes de post-grado despues de que estos leyeron un pasaje expositorio de 669 palabras una, dos, o tres veces. Los resultados fueron examinados para observar los efectos que la tarea de lectura repetida de un material por aprenderse tuvo sobre el recuerdo y la retención, así como sobre la persistencia de errores en el tiempo. Se encontraron diferencias cuantitativas y cualitativas en la capacidad de recuerdo de los sujetos como una función del número de veces que el texto fue leído. Los sujetos que leyeron el pasaje dos veces previo a la primera prueba recordaron significativamente más información sobre el texto que aquellos sujetos que leyeron el pasaje una o tres veces. Los sujetos que leyeron el pasaje tres veces mostraron un recuerdo desproporcionado de detalles comparado con su recuerdo de ideas principales. La persistencia de errores de codificación incurridos al principio fue marcadamente estable para todos los grupos sin importar el hecho de que todos los sujetos releyeron el pasaje después de la primera prueba. Como se esperaba, los errores en la primera prueba de recuerdo asociado aparecieron con mayor probabilidad en la tarea de recuerdo retardado, una vez que se habían repetido en un re-test inmediato. /// [German] Sechzig hochschulstudenten, die gebeten wurden, ein-, zwei-, oder dreimal einen 669 Wörter langen Sachtext zu lesen, erfüllten sowohl die freie wie auch direkte Abrufmessungen in drei durchgeführten Tests. Untersucht wurde die Wirkung wiederholten Lesens von zu lernendem Material auf sowohl Abrufungs- und Erinnerungskapazität als auch Fehlerkonsistenz über einen Zeitraum. Sowohl quantitative wie qualitative Unterschiede in der Abrufung erwiesen sich als Funktionen der Anzahl der Lesungen. Testpersonen, die den Text zweimal von der ersten Gedächtnisprüfung lasen, erinnerten sich an bedeutend mehr Informationen aus dem Text als Testpersonen, die den Text einmal oder dreimal lasen. Testpersonen, die den Text dreimal gelesen hatten, wiesen unverhältnismäßig gute Erinnerung von Einzelheiten gegenüber der Hauptgedanken auf. Die Hartnäckigkeit der beim ersten Lesen gemachten Fehler war erstaunlich gleichbleibend für alle Gruppen, trotz der Tatsache, daß alle Testpersonen den Text nach der ersten Gedächtnisprobe nochmal lasen. Wie zu erwarten, wurden Fehler, die bei der ersten direktiven Gedächtnisprüfung gemacht wurden, mit großer Wahrscheinlichkeit bei späterer Erinnerung wiederholt, nachdem sie sich einmal bei sofortiger nochmaliger Testdurchführung wiederholt hatten.