ArticlePDF Available

Reading Comprehension: Its Nature and Development

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Reading comprehension is the process by which we understand the texts we read. It is the purpose of reading, why we teach it, and why we care about it. It is also the prerequisite for meaningful learning from text. As I discuss in this entry, reading comprehension is complex, and we are still far from a complete understanding of it. But we have learned a great deal. My purpose in this paper is to review recent research and theory around three basic questions: (1) What is reading comprehension?, (2) What factors contribute to the development of reading comprehension?, and (3) Who are the “poor comprehenders”? This entry sketches answers to these questions and provides links to sources in which they are treated in more depth. Reading comprehension does not develop in a vacuum, in isolation from other language and literacy processes, so this entry should be read in conjunction with the others in this section on reading comprehension, and with those in other sections.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Kirby, J. R.
Page 1 of 8
http://www.literacyencyclopedia.ca
Reading Comprehension: Its Nature and Development
Written by: John R. Kirby, Ph.D., Faculty of Education, Queen’s University
Introduction and Key Research Questions
Reading comprehension is the process by which we understand the texts we read. It is
the purpose of reading, why we teach it, and why we care about it. It is also the
prerequisite for meaningful learning from text. As I discuss in this entry, reading
comprehension is complex, and we are still far from a complete understanding of it. But
we have learned a great deal. My purpose in this paper is to review recent research and
theory around three basic questions: (1) What is reading comprehension?, (2) What
factors contribute to the development of reading comprehension?, and (3) Who are the
“poor comprehenders”? This entry sketches answers to these questions and provides
links to sources in which they are treated in more depth. Reading comprehension does
not develop in a vacuum, in isolation from other language and literacy processes, so
this entry should be read in conjunction with the others in this section on reading
comprehension, and with those in other sections.
Recent Evidence
1. What is reading comprehension?
Reading comprehension is the application of a skill that evolved for other purposes
(listening or oral comprehension) to a new form of input (text). Unlike listening
comprehension, reading comprehension is not something for which our brains have
evolved. Whereas oral comprehension seems to develop “naturally” with minimal
deliberate intervention, reading comprehension is more challenging and requires
deliberate instruction. Humans have been accomplished in oral comprehension for
100,000 years or more (Donald, 1991), and virtually all humans do it; reading
comprehension has only been practiced for 5,000 years, and for most of that time the
majority of humans did not do it (Olson, 1994). It should not be surprising that reading
comprehension is difficult. The application of comprehension to text amplifies our
mental capacities. It is fundamental to full participation in society, now and for the
foreseeable future.
Levels of processing in reading
It is important to understand that reading occurs at several different levels, and how
these levels interact. One way of describing those levels is presented in Figure 1 (see
below). The lowest level shown there, Words, sits on top of many even lower levels of
processing that are beyond the scope of this paper (see Kirby, 1988, and Kirby &
Williams, 1991). Successful word recognition (either pronunciation, or, more rarely,
recognition of meaning without being able to pronounce) is a prerequisite for the higher
Kirby, J. R. (2007). Reading comprehension: Its nature and development. In Encyclopedia of Language and Literacy
Development. Canadian Language and Literacy Research Network,
.
.
Kirby, J. R.
Page 2 of 8
http://www.literacyencyclopedia.ca
levels of comprehension. If some words cannot be recognized, the higher levels can
compensate to some extent (shown as “top-down processing” in Figure 1). Unknown
words can be inferred in some cases; however, this is more difficult than it sounds, it
can only work for some kinds of words and only for a small number of words in any text,
and it is very processing intensive. Once words have been recognized, the question of
word meaning arises (see the entry on vocabulary in this section of the Encyclopedia,
Biemiller, 2007); it is possible to make sense of text when the meaning of some words is
lacking or hazy, but beyond a modest level of uncertainty, comprehension becomes
impossible.
Figure 1. Levels of processing in reading comprehension.
Two types of processing occur: “bottom-up” and “top-down”. In bottom-up processing,
words are formed into phrases, and phrases are formed into more abstract units called
propositions or ideas; these processes require knowledge of syntax (grammar). Just as
several words can be processed into one phrase, several phrases can be processed
into one idea. Comprehension at the phrase or idea level results in a relatively shallow
understanding of what the text stated directly, often termed literal comprehension.
Further processing of these ideas either selects particular ones as main ideas, or
constructs main ideas from them, and then thematic generalizations or abstractions out
of the main ideas. Top-down processing occurs when higher-level information, just as
knowledge of the general topic of the text, helps the reader identify lower-level
information. It is important to recognize that both bottom-up and top-down processing
often occurs in reading comprehension. This is called interactive processing. The higher
levels of processing require prior knowledge to help decide what is important, and
especially to see the deeper implications of the text.
Processing occurs in these levels to make best use of working memory resources.
Working memory contains information or thoughts that we are currently aware of; it is
generally agreed that working memory is limited to four or five units and that there is no
limit to the size of those units (e.g., Baddeley, 1986). Thus we might only be able to
retain four or five unrelated words, but with coherent text we can process hundreds or
Themes
Main ideas
Ideas/Detail
s
Phrases
Words
Bottom-up
processing
Top-down
processing
Kirby, J. R.
Page 3 of 8
http://www.literacyencyclopedia.ca
even thousands of words into a relatively small number of main ideas and even fewer
themes. With those higher-level units retained, we can often reconstruct (through top-
down processing) the important lower-level units. Efficient and automatic functioning of
the lower levels in Figure 1 is a prerequisite for the higher levels. If too much conscious
awareness has to be devoted to, for instance, sounding-out and recognizing words, it is
difficult for thinking to reach much above the Phrase level. In this way the lower levels
can act as bottlenecks.
The simple view of reading
Gough and Tunmer (1986) proposed the simple view of reading, in which reading
comprehension is seen as the product of decoding and listening comprehension.
Though simple, this approach does a remarkably good job of accounting for the data
(e.g., Johnston & Kirby, 2006; Joshi & Aaron, 2000), and it reminds us that the ability to
decode words is absolutely essential for skilled reading; those with either very low
decoding skills or very low oral comprehension skills will be poor reading
comprehenders. Decoding or word reading is often the bottleneck that prevents readers
from attaining higher or adequate text comprehension (see Figure 1). However, listening
comprehension, which represents verbal ability, is also essential. Verbal ability is a key
component of intelligence, and may be very difficult to improve through instruction; it
includes knowledge of vocabulary, grammar, the ability to make inferences, and so on.
Decoding provides a more promising and fruitful target for instruction.
Two important factors beyond decoding and listening comprehension can be added:
fluency and strategies. Fluency (speed and expression; e.g., Kuhn & Stahl, 2003) is not
an issue in listening as the speaker controls the pace, but it is needed for reading
comprehension because of working memory limitations. If word recognition is slow, then
previous words will have faded from working memory before later words are recognized,
and their joint meaning will not be able to be processed. Strategies (e.g., Dole et al.,
1991; National Reading Panel, 2000, chapter 4) are important in reading, and more
useful than in listening, because the text stays present and allows re-inspections.
Strategies are particularly useful when the text is long and/or complex, and the reader
has many options about where to attend. We expect skilled readers to extract more
from text than they would from speech, and some of that comes from strategic, goal-
directed, deliberate processing. Strategies are conscious, goal-oriented plans that call
on tactics which can vary from underline long words to create a mental simulation to see
if the author is right (Kirby, 1988). Strategies depend on prior knowledge (of content,
and of strategies) and on the learners’ intentions; intentions can be characterized as
combinations of deep and surface processing (Biggs, 1993), or depth and breadth of
processing (Kirby & Woodhouse, 1994).
Comprehension involves the relating of two or more pieces of information (e.g., Kintsch,
1999). Those pieces of information can come from long-term memory (prior knowledge),
but in reading comprehension at least one piece must come from the text. The pieces of
information can be simple or quite complex ideas, ranging from the word cat to the
concept democracy. The relating can also be of many sorts, such as is an example of,
is the same as, causes, or acts on in a specified way. The information to be integrated
is held in working memory (Baddeley, 1986), and the relating operation takes up space
Kirby, J. R.
Page 4 of 8
http://www.literacyencyclopedia.ca
there too. As we read, we update our mental representation of the text’s meaning; these
mental representations are known as mental models (Johnson-Laird, 1983) or situation
models (Kintsch, 1999).
These pieces of information are the different types of content shown in Figure 1, for
instance, words, ideas, main ideas, or themes. As information is processed, the lower-
level units are integrated into higher-level units; long-term memory stores some low-
level information, but comprehension relies critically upon long-term memory’s higher-
level, more abstract or schematic information. The abstract information is stored in the
form of schemas, which function like generalized mental or situation models. For
readers with rich knowledge, a word such as democracy evokes and brings to life many
ideas without taking up additional working memory space; for readers with less relevant
knowledge, the word itself may take up one or more spaces, with no additional
information brought along “for free”. Comprehension is enhanced when the contents of
working memory are higher-level units; children struggling to identify words are unlikely
to be able to attain even modest levels of comprehension. When lower-level units are
recognized automatically, there is a greater chance of higher levels being attained. It is
critical to build up the automaticity of the lower-level units (e.g., words). It is equally
important to remember that the processing of lower-level units does not guarantee the
comprehension of higher level units.
2. What factors contribute to the development of reading comprehension?
The previous section mentioned a number of factors involved in reading
comprehension; these and their antecedents can be seen as the causes or sources of
reading comprehension (see Figure 2). As shown in Figure 2, vocabulary knowledge
(Wagner et al., 2007) and prior knowledge contribute to listening comprehension,
though both and many other factors shown are also related to verbal intelligence. It is
difficult to see how readers can understand a text if there are many unknown words or
concepts.
Figure 2. Sources of reading comprehension
Reading
Comprehension
Listening
comprehension
Fluency
Strategies
Phonological awareness
Naming speed
Orthographic knowledge
Morphological
awareness
Phonics knowledge
Cognitive speed
Orientation
Purpose
Verbal Intelligence
Vocabulary
Prior knowledge
Kirby, J. R.
Page 5 of 8
http://www.literacyencyclopedia.ca
Over the last 25 years or so, we have learned a great deal about how the brain
accomplishes the lower-level aspects of reading, especially decoding (e.g., Adams,
1990; Rayner et al., 2001). We know that a number of factors contribute to word
reading, including phonological awareness (Stanovich, 2000), naming speed (Wolf &
Bowers, 1999), orthographic knowledge (Levy et al., 2006), morphological awareness
(Deacon & Kirby, 2004; Nunes & Bryant, 2006), and phonics knowledge (Adams, 1990).
Fluency is less well understood (Kuhn & Stahl, 2003), but clearly depends upon
decoding efficiency, and cognitive and naming speed (Wolf & Bowers, 1999). As fluency
drops, it becomes less and less likely that the needed information is still active in
working memory, making comprehension less and less likely.
Reading comprehension strategies have been studied extensively (National Reading
Panel, 2000, chapter 4). Dole et al. (1991) listed 5 major strategies, each of which is
associated with greater reading comprehension: determining importance, summarizing
information, drawing inferences, generating questions, and monitoring comprehension.
None of these factors has much influence in the absence of motivation and interest.
Most children are interested in reading when they begin school, but some can lose
interest/motivation if their skills are not adequate or if the text content does not suit
them.
3. Who are the “poor comprehenders”?
The factors listed in the previous section indicate the characteristics that will lead to
poor reading comprehension. Children with lows levels of skill in the various contributing
factors will struggle with reading comprehension, children with more areas of low skill
will struggle more, and the more they struggle the more their interest will suffer, creating
a vicious cycle. It is no secret that many children prefer other activities to reading, and
that uninteresting text content can turn a capable reader into an unenthusiastic reader
very quickly (Pressley, 2002, chapter 8).
Two subgroups of children who show poor reading comprehension deserve mention.
First, it should be no surprise that children with reading disabilities have difficulties in
reading comprehension (Cornoldi & Oakhill, 1996): these children’s most obvious and
primary problem is with word reading, which is critical for reading comprehension.
However it is important to recognize that some reading disabled children can develop
adequate or even good levels of reading comprehension, especially if time constraints
are not imposed (Lefly & Pennington, 1991). It is not yet clear how they do this, but it
almost certainly involves a great deal of practice, re-reading, and strategy use.
Secondly, some authors refer to a group of children termed poor comprehenders (e.g.,
Cain, Oakhill & Bryant, 2004; Nation, 2005). These children have normal word level
processing, but also have some language comprehension difficulties that interfere with
reading comprehension. These language difficulties involve drawing inferences,
understanding figurative language, and monitoring their own comprehension. Most
current assessment approaches to reading disability will overlook these children who
may need intensive language-oriented intervention. Some of these children may have
Kirby, J. R.
Page 6 of 8
http://www.literacyencyclopedia.ca
low general ability, hence, it may not be surprising that they have low reading
comprehension. However, this is no excuse not to teach them. Other children, though
this is not yet clear, may have relatively specific difficulties in comprehension processes,
difficulties that targeted instruction may help to overcome; this remains a question for
future research.
Future Directions
There is continuing need for research that explores the factors shown in Figure 2, their
inter-relationships, and how they affect reading at both the word and comprehension
levels. Some relatively new factors, such as naming speed and morphology, may allow
us to better understand reading comprehension and its failures.
One of the challenges in this area concerns the ways in which reading comprehension
is measured (see Paris, 2007, this section). Formal, standardised tests are very
expensive to develop, and often fail to include the highest levels of reading
comprehension (those related to the thematic or situation model level). Reading
comprehension measures are also often given without word reading measures, making
it difficult to diagnose the source of any observed problems. There is need for
continuing development of theory-based tests and testing policies. All of this work points
towards instructional research that explores how best to combine the factors shown in
Figure 2 to design effective classroom and remedial instruction (see Phillips, 2007, this
section).
Conclusions
Reading comprehension is a complex process in itself, but it also depends upon other
important lower-level processes. It is a critical foundation for later academic learning,
many employment skills, and life satisfaction. It is an important skill to target, but we
should not forget about the skills on which it depends. To improve the reading
comprehension skills of poor performers, we need to understand that there is no “magic
wand” hidden in Figure 2, and no secret weapon that will quickly improve reading
competencies for all poor readers. Careful assessment is required to determine
individual children’s strengths and weaknesses, and programs need to be tailored
accordingly; most poor readers will need continued support in many areas. The roots of
most reading comprehension problems lie in the early elementary years. Waiting to
address them in later elementary or even secondary school is a high-risk strategy.
Date Posted Online: 2007-09-25 11:13:06
Kirby, J. R.
Page 7 of 8
http://www.literacyencyclopedia.ca
References
Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Biggs, J. B. (1993). What do inventories of students’ learning processes really
measure? A theoretical review and clarification. British Journal of Educational
Psychology, 63, 1-17.
Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Children's reading comprehension ability:
Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 31-42.
Cornoldi, C., & Oakhill, J. (Eds.) (1996). Reading comprehension difficulties: Processes
and intervention. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Deacon, S. H., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological: Is it more than phonological?
Evaluating the roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading
development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 223-238.
Dole, J. A., Duffy, G. G., Roehler, L. R., & Pearson, P. D. (1991). Moving from the old to
the new: Research on reading comprehension. Review of Educational Research,
61, 239-264.
Donald, M. (1991). The origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of
culture and cognition. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.
Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability.
Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6-10.
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Johnston, T. C., & Kirby, J. R. (2006). The contribution of naming speed to the simple
view of reading. Reading and Writing, 19, 339-361.
Joshi, R. M., & Aaron, P. G. (2000). The component model of reading: Simple view of
reading made a little more complex. Reading Psychology, 21, 85-97.
Kintsch, W. (1999). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Kirby, J. R. (1988). Style, strategy, and skill in reading. In R.R. Schmeck (Ed.) Learning
styles and learning strategies. New York: Plenum Press.
Kirby, J. R., Parrila, R., & Pfeiffer, S. (2003). Naming speed and phonological
processing as predictors of reading development. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 95, 453-464.
Kirby, J. R., & Woodhouse, R. A. (1994). Measuring and predicting depth of processing
in learning. Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 40, 147-161.
Kuhn, M. R., & Stahl, S. A. (2003). Fluency: A review of developmental and remedial
practices. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 3-21.
Lefly, D. L., & Pennington, B. F. (1991). Spelling errors and reading fluency in
compensated dyslexics. Annals of Dyslexia, 41, 143162.
Levy, B. A., Gong, Z., Hessels, S., Evans, M. A., & Jared, D. (2006). Understanding
print: Early reading development and the contributions of home literacy
experiences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 63-93.
Nation, K. (2005). Children’s reading comprehension difficulties. In M. J. Snowling and
C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp 248-265). Oxford UK:
Blackwell Publishing.
Kirby, J. R.
Page 8 of 8
http://www.literacyencyclopedia.ca
National Reading Panel (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence-based
assessment of the scientific literature on reading and its implications for reading
instruction. Bethesda MD: National Institute of Child Health and Human
Development.
Nunes, T., & Bryant, P. (2006). Improving literacy by teaching morphemes. Routlege.
Olson, D. R. (1994). The world on paper: The conceptual and cognitive implications of
writing and reading. Cambridge UK: Cambridge University Press.
Paris, S. G., & Stahl, S. A. (Eds.) (2005). Children’s reading comprehension and
assessment. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Parrila, R. K., Aunola, K., Leskinen, E., Nurmi, J.-E., & Kirby, J. R. (2005). Development
of individual differences in reading: Results from longitudinal studies in English and
Finnish. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 299-319.
Pressley, M. (2002). Reading instruction that works. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press.
RAND Reading Study Group. (2002). Reading for understanding: Toward and R & D
program in reading comprehension. Washington, DC: RAND Education.
Rayner, K., Foorman, B. R., Perfetti, C. A., Pesetsky, D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2001).
How psychological science informs the teaching of reading. Psychological Science
in the Public Interest, 2(2), 31-74.
Stanovich, K. E. (2000). Progress in understanding reading: Scientific foundations and
new frontiers. New York: Guilford.
Sweet, A. P. (2005). Assessment of reading comprehension: The RAND Reading Study
Group vision. In S. G. Paris and S. A. Stahl (Eds.), Children’s reading
comprehension and assessment (pp 3-12). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Wagner, R. K., Muse, A. E., & Tannenbaum, K. R. (Eds.) (2007). Vocabulary instruction:
Implications for reading comprehension. New York: Guilford Press.
Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental
dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415-438.
To cite this document:
Kirby, J. R. (2007). Reading comprehension: Its nature and development.
Encyclopedia of Language and Literacy Development (pp. 1-8). London,
ON: Canadian Language and Literacy Research Network. Retrieved from
http://www.literacyencyclopedia.ca/pdfs/topic.php?topId=227
View publication statsView publication stats
... Reading is one of the four main language skills which plays a pivotal role in learning process. Reading comprehension is defined as a process of understanding the text being read (John R. Kirby, 2007). Eristiana Sri Augustin (2014) cites Langan (2002) who says that reading comprehension is the ability of understanding the text and it starts with the general feeling of what the text means to a deeper understanding of the text. ...
... Eristiana Sri Augustin (2014) cites Langan (2002) who says that reading comprehension is the ability of understanding the text and it starts with the general feeling of what the text means to a deeper understanding of the text. John R. Kirby (2007) further elaborates the process of multilevel, lowest level is recognition of words which is prerequisite for the understanding of other levels but in some cases when word are not recognized well the upper levels can compensate for an overall comprehension of the text. Meneghetti, Carretti & De Beni (2006) say that reading comprehension is a very complex phenomenon, it requires the cognitive ability to integrate information of the text with that of reader"s knowledge which results in mental representation of the in an elaborative manner. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed at finding the impact of using short stories in enhancing the reading comprehension skills of EFL learners. It was a mixed-method approach study, both qualitative and quantitative data was collected. It was quasi-experimental research and two groups, experimental (consisting of 25 males and 23 females) and controlled group (consisting of 24 males and 21females) were participants for the study. Means of multiple-choice pretest-posttest scores were used to determine the level of improvement. Independent t-test was used to evaluate the significance of the difference in the outcome of controlled and experimental groups. Furthermore, One-way ANCOVA was used to eliminate the initial achievement difference. For qualitative data 7 Lecturers were interviewed and those lecturers were selected through purposive sampling. Result of the study suggested that short stories lay a positive impact on reading comprehension of the EFL learners.
... Reading is not merely a process of exact identification of letters, words, and ultimately sentences leading to comprehension built from letter to word to phrase to sentence. Also cited from Kirby (2007), reading comprehension is the process by which we understand the texts we read. It is the purpose of reading, why we teach it, and why we care about it. ...
Article
p>Tujuan dari penelitian ini berupa kemampuan membaca siswa terhadap pemakaian media membaca bersumber internet sebagai media pembelajaran bahasa Inggris. Fokus utama dari penelitian ini berupa peningkatan kemampuan membaca murid melalui media membaca bersumber internet sebagai media pembelajaran bahasa Inggris. Penelitian ini bertujuan untuk melihat apakah media membaca bersumber internet dapat meningkatkan kemampuan membaca siswa selama pengajaran daring COVID-19 2020. Pengumpulan data pada penelitian ini melalui pre-test post-test model untuk data kuantitatif, dan wawancara beserta observasi untuk data kualitatif. Peneliti melakukan pertemuan dengan murid sejumlah 7 kali. Metode penelitian berupa penelitian kuasi-eksperimental dengan model pre-test dan post-test. Subjek dari penelitian ini berupa 20 murid dari kelas 8B dan 8C SMP Gunungjati 1 Purwokerto. Subjek penelitian dibagi menjadi 2 kelas, kelas eksperimental dan kelas kontrol. Kelas eksperimental Hasil dari penelitian ini menunjukan bahwa material bacaan bersumber internet mampu meningkatkan kemampuan membaca siswa secara signifikan., Hasil penelitian menunjukan bahwa kemampuan membaca siswa meningkat secara signifikan, dibuktikan dengan hasil uji Mann-Whitney . Hasil uji Mann-Whtiney (Nilai Asymp.Sig 0,046, jika nilai Asymp.sig lebih kecil dari 0,05 maka H2 diterima) menunjukan bahwa murid dari kelas eksperimental mendapatkan peningkatan kemampuan membaca secara signfikan jika dibandingkan dengan kelas kontrol. Hasil ini juga didukung wawancara, serta observasi. Untuk menerapkan hasil penelitian ini, peneliti menyarankan bahwa guru untuk faham betul mengenai kemampuan membaca bahasa Inggris dari siswa. Menngetahui material membaca yang benar untuk siswa yang benar sangatlah penting untuk guru dapat menerapkan media mengajar ini. Kata Kunci: Membaca, Kemampuan Membaca, Material Bacaan Bersumber Internet </p
... It was determined that one of the most important, consistent and developing themes in this field is related to "reading comprehension". Reading comprehension is the understanding and interpretation of written language and the ability to derive meaning from text (Kirby, 2007). Meta-analyses indicate that strategy instruction (metacognitive strategies), which helps students understand the purpose of reading, adapt to the demands of the task, and monitor comprehension processes, is effective in improving students' reading comprehension skills (Corso et al. 2023). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
This study aims to examine the evolutionary development of meta-analysis studies in the field of education and to determine the impact of these studies on scientific knowledge. For this purpose, performance analysis (e.g. total number of publications, number of contributing authors, total number of citations, etc.) was used to determine the contribution of research components to the field of education and science mapping (e.g. co-citation analysis, co-word analysis, co-authorship analysis) techniques were used to examine the relationships between research components. A total of 1979 documents from 455 education-related sources between 1980-2022 from the Web of Science (WOS) database were examined. The result indicated that although the number of meta-analysis studies published between 1980 and 2004 was relatively close to each other, there was a high increase in meta-analysis studies after 2004. Author productivity was found to be distributed according to Lotka Law and 15 journals were core journals that published meta-analyzed studies in educational sciences. Co-word analysis was performed to determine the basic concepts, themes, and trends, co-citation analysis to determine the relationships between the publications that cited the same publication together, and co-authorship analysis to determine the social relationships between the authors. According to the results obtained as a result of bibliometric analysis, suggestions were made for future meta-analysis studies in the field of educational sciences.
... Reading is the structuring of information with the help of prior knowledge for a specific purpose (Meneghetti, Carretti, & De Beni, 2006). Kirby (2007) emphasized the complexity of this skill, whereas Snow (2002) highlighted that understanding what one reads involves factors in three different areas (reader, text, and reading activity). Nation and Norbury (2005) noted that the process of extracting meaning from a text incorporates numerous cognitive processes, ranging between the identification of letters/words and the interpretation of the message based on an individual's worldview. ...
... Reading skill closely related with the reading comprehension. According to Kirby (2007), one of the applications of a talent gained for any other purposes-it can be listening or oral comprehension-to a new kind of input (text) is reading comprehension. Fahmi, Friatin, and Irianti (2020) said that in order to understand the meaning from the text, reading comprehension is a complex cognitive process that involves many different brain functions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Reading as one of the crucial skill in English language usually hard to comprehend by the students. To achieve the reading comprehension, teacher has to provide the equipment to measure the students understanding. Worksheet as the appropriate equipment to measure students understanding, generally using the LOTS level of questions rather than HOTS level of questions. In fact, the LOTS level of questions cannot increase the students’ comprehension critically. Worksheet should be selected by the appropriate level of reading questions. It can be analyzed by using the Revised Bloom’s Taxonomy. This study aims to find out; (1) the level of reading questions taxonomy in English student’s worksheet based on Revised Bloom’s Taxonomy, (2) the distribution level of question’s taxonomy in English students worksheet based on Revised Bloom’s Taxonomy. The descriptive qualitative research design with research instruments in the form of document analysis and checklist was employed in this research. The research implied the dominant level of questions is remembering level with 35 items out of 58 questions (60.34%). Followed by understanding (20.68%), creating (10.34%), analyzing (3.44%), evaluating (3.44%) and applying (1.72%)
... Reading comprehension is seen as a process where readers acquire an understanding of a reading text (Kirby, 2007). Within the context of reading comprehension, a certain number of components are regarded as vital aspects in this area while given that one correlates with the other, which in return allows readers to grasp text more thoroughly. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aims to explore the use of the EDSAW acronym, a reading strategy in locating contextual clues systematically among tertiary-level students. It takes a quantitative research design to answer the research question, ‘How effective is the use of the EDSAW acronym in comprehending a reading text?’ The sample was selected based on a purposive sampling technique that comprised 10 students from a public university who scored band 4 in the Malaysian University English Test (MUET). The data collection technique involves a pre-test and a post-test. A reading text was given during the pre-test where students were tasked to locate contextual clues without using the EDSAW acronym. Then, the same participants were taught to use the acronym in finding contextual clues in a reading text. The mean scores of the pre-test and post-test were compared. The findings revealed that there was a significant increase of 2.3 points in the mean score of the post-test. This study highlights the need for teachers and students to use the reading strategy EDSAW acronym during the teaching and learning of reading in ESL classrooms and fills a void in the area of reading comprehension by enabling students to read more independently.
... Reading is not merely a process of exact identification of letters, words, and ultimately sentences leading to comprehension built from letter to word to phrase to sentence. Also cited from Kirby (2007), reading comprehension is the process by which we understand the texts we read. It is the purpose of reading, why we teach it, and why we care about it. ...
Article
Full-text available
p>The main concern of this research is improving students’ reading comprehension through internet-sourced reading materials as English teaching media. This research was intended to find out whether internet-sourced reading materials as English teaching media could improve students’ reading comprehension significantly in online teaching during COVID-19 Pandemic of 2020. The data were collected through pre-test and post-test results (quantitative) and Interview as well as observation (qualitative). The researcher conducts 7 meetings for pre-test, treatment and then post-test for both of experimental and controlled class. The research methodology was quasi-experimental research design with pre-test and post-test experimental. The subjects of this research were 20 students of 8B and 8C at SMP Gunungjati I Purwokerto. The subject of this research then divided into two classes, experimental and controlled class. The results of this research showed that internet-sourced reading materials for online English teaching could improve students’ reading comprehension, and improve students reading comprehension significantly. It is further proved with the Wilcoxon test result. Wilcoxon test result on experimental class ( Asymp.Sig value 0.016, if the Asymp.Sig value was lower than 0.05 then H1 was accepted) which showed that experimental class students’ reading comprehension were improved. Furthermore, the research showed that students’ reading comprehension was improved significantly. It is proved through Mann-Whitney test result of both experimental and controlled class. The result of Mann-Whitney test ( Asymp.Sig value 0.046 if the Asymp.Sig value was lower than 0.05 then H2 was accepted) which showed that students from experimental class test has significant improvement of reading comprehension when compared with controlled class. This result also supported by interview with the teacher, as well as through observation of students’ activity. Keywords: Reading, Reading Comprehension, English Teaching Media, Internet-sourced Reading Materials </p
Article
Full-text available
The pupose of this study was to understand students' perceptions about potential and benefits of improving Extending Concept Through Language Activities (ECOLA) method as a tool for improving students' reading comprehension skill. This study used a descriptive qualitative with the subjects of 19 students of the 10th grade of Vocational High School of SMK Islam Gondanglegi. Data collection was carried out through questionnaire based on likert scale and interview. In collecting data, questionnaire were made to collect information on respondents’ perceptions of the ECOLA method and its impact on their reading comprehension skills, while the interview were made to obtain further information regarding students perceptions about using the ECOLA method. Based on the analysis of the results, researcher concluded that the ECOLA method generally has positive acceptance for improving reading comprehension skills. This is evident from the results of the questionnaire which shows the high motivation and enthusiasm of the participants. 65% of respondents agreed that this method had improved their reading comprehension skills. Additionally, 80% of respondents agreed that ECOLA helped them understand reading material better. Likewise, in interviews, participants expressed appreciation and satisfaction with ECOLA's approach, which includes interactive discussions and enhanced learning experiences.
Article
La presente investigación tuvo como objetivo fundamental el diseño de estrategias metodológicas para el uso efectivo de libros interactivos empleados en el mejoramiento de la comprensión lectora de estudiantes de octavo año de la Unidad Educativa Enrique Ponce Luque. Basándose en análisis de encuestas y correlaciones estadísticas significativas, se desarrollaron actividades relacionadas con las estrategias metodológicas enfocadas en el uso efectivo de libros interactivos, que incluyeron grupos de lectura, uso de videos y animaciones, actividades interactivas y sistemas de retroalimentación en tiempo real. Validadas mediante la metodología del criterio de expertos, las estrategias fueron altamente valoradas, destacando su innovación y potencial impacto educativo, lo cual resaltó la importancia de la aplicación de la tecnología en la educación moderna.
Article
This study explored the effect of metacognitive reading strategy intervention in English on helping Moroccan learners reversely transfer metacognitive reading strategies from English (L3) into French (L2). In particular, it investigated whether an improvement in metacognitive reading strategy in English (L3) would lead to an improvement in reading comprehension achievements in French (L2). 67 eleventh-grade students were purposively selected and randomly assigned to an experimental group (n = 35) and a control group (n = 32). All participants were administered the Metacognitive Reading Strategies Questionnaire (Abu-Rabia, 2019) and a reading comprehension pre-test and post-test in English and French. Only the experimental group received training in metacognitive reading strategies (MCRS) in English. The difference between metacognitive strategy awareness levels and reading comprehension scores before and after the intervention was examined using one-way-MANOVA. Results indicated a significant improvement in the experimental group's metacognitive strategy awareness and reading comprehension scores in both English and French. Based on these results, we suggest that learners of English as a foreign language should be explicitly trained in the use of MCRS, and a link between the different languages taught at school should be established. Thus, metacognitive reading strategies and skills acquired in one language can be positively transferred to another.
Article
Full-text available
The authors review theory and research relating to fluency instruction and development. They surveyed the range of definitions for fluency, primary features of fluent reading, and studies that have attempted to improve the fluency of struggling readers. They found that (a) fluency instruction is generally effective, although it is unclear whether this is because of specific instructional features or because it involves children in reading increased amounts of text; (b) assisted approaches seem to be more effective than unassisted approaches; (c) repetitive approaches do not seem to hold a clear advantage over nonrepetitive approaches; and (d) effective fluency instruction moves beyond automatic word recognition to include rhythm and expression, or what linguists refer to as the prosodic features of language.
Article
This book proposes a theory of human cognitive evolution, drawing from paleontology, linguistics, anthropology, cognitive science, and especially neuropsychology. The properties of humankind's brain, culture, and cognition have coevolved in a tight iterative loop; the main event in human evolution has occurred at the cognitive level, however, mediating change at the anatomical and cultural levels. During the past two million years humans have passed through three major cognitive transitions, each of which has left the human mind with a new way of representing reality and a new form of culture. Modern humans consequently have three systems of memory representation that were not available to our closest primate relatives: mimetic skill, language, and external symbols. These three systems are supported by new types of ''hard'' storage devices, two of which (mimetic and linguistic) are biological, one technological. Full symbolic literacy consists of a complex of skills for interacting with the external memory system. The independence of these three uniquely human ways of representing knowledge is suggested in the way the mind breaks down after brain injury and confirmed by various other lines of evidence. Each of the three systems is based on an inventive capacity, and the products of those capacities - such as languages, symbols, gestures, social rituals, and images - continue to be invented and vetted in the social arena. Cognitive evolution is not yet complete: the externalization of memory has altered the actual memory architecture within which humans think. This is changing the role of biological memory and the way in which the human brain deploys its resources; it is also changing the form of modern culture.
Article
With reports from several studies showing the benefits of teaching young children about morphemes, this book is essential reading for anyone concerned with helping children to read and write. By breaking words down into chunks of meaning that can be analyzed as complete units rather than as strings of individual letters, children are better able to make sense of the often contradictory spelling and reading rules of English. As a result, their enjoyment of learning about words increases, and their literacy skills improve. Written by leading researchers for trainee teachers, practising teachers and interested parents, this highly accessible and innovative book provides sound, evidence-based advice and materials that can be used to help teach children about morphemes, and highlights the beneficial effects of this approach. © 2006 editorial matter and selection, Terezinha Nunes and Peter Bryant. All rights reserved.
Article
Given the morphophonemic nature of the English orthography, surprisingly few studies have examined the roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading. This 4-year longitudinal study (Grades 2-5) compared these two factors in three aspects of reading development: pseudoword reading, reading comprehension. and single word reading. Morphological awareness contributed significantly to pseudoword reading and reading comprehension, after controlling prior measures of reading ability. verbal and nonverbal intelligence, and phonological awareness. This contribution was comparable to that of phonological awareness and remained 3 years after morphological awareness was assessed. In contrast, morphological awareness rarely contributed significantly to single word reading. We argue that these results provide evidence that morphological awareness hits a wide-ranging role in reading development, one that extends beyond phonological awareness.
Chapter
This chapter is divided into two parts. The first describes the effect of Pat Rabbitt's influence in encouraging the first author to use the increasingly sophisticated methods of ageing research to answer questions about the fundamental characteristics of working memory, together with reflections on why so little of this work reached publication. The second part presents a brief review of the literature on working memory and ageing, followed by an account of more recent work attempting to apply the traditional method of experimental dissociation to research on normal ageing and Alzheimer's disease. The discussion suggests that even such simple methods can throw light on both the processes of ageing and the understanding of working memory.
Article
This article investigates how well kindergarten phonological awareness (PA) and naming speed (NS) account for reading development to Grade 5. The authors use regression analyses to predict reading development, with mental ability and prior achievement controlled, and follow the reading development of children having combinations of adequate or inadequate PA and NS. PA was most strongly related to reading in the first 2 years of school, and NS's initially weaker relationship increased with grade level. Children with weak PA and slow NS were most likely to develop reading difficulties by Grade 5, followed by children with slow NS alone. The authors discuss the roles of NS and PA in reading development and the need to clarify the constructs underlying NS.