Content uploaded by George Knox
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by George Knox on Feb 24, 2014
Content may be subject to copyright.
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 2010
Report No. 10-109
Unplanned Buying on Shopping Trips
David R. Bell, Daniel Corsten, and George Knox
“Unplanned Buying on Shopping Trips” © 2010 David R. Bell, Daniel Corsten, and George
Knox; Report Summary © 2010 Marketing Science Institute
MSI working papers are distributed for the benefit of MSI corporate and academic members
and the general public. Reports are not be to reproduced or published, in any form or by
any means, electronic or mechanical, without written permission.
Report Summary
Motivated by the common wisdom that up to 70% of consumers’ purchases are decided upon in
the store, marketers have expended considerable research energy on what in-store factors
influence shoppers’ purchase decisions. But out-of-store factors influence unplanned purchases
too, and if retailers understand those factors, they may be able to leverage them.
Here, David Bell, Daniel Corsten, and George Knox examine diary panel data from 441
households in a Western European country to uncover how several out-of-store factors influence
unplanned purchases.
One major factor is the nature of the shopping goal: Is the consumer shopping to take advantage
of a particular promotion or to buy a particular item—both concrete goals—or is the consumer
shopping to take care of weekly needs, which is a more abstract goal? Reasons for choosing a
particular store are also important. Did the shopper choose the store for its prices? Its selection?
Its service? To avoid crowds? Convenience is a third factor: Is the consumer aiming to do one-
stop shopping, or is he or she visiting a given store as part of a multiple-store shopping trip? The
researchers also looked at the interaction between out-of-store and in-store advertising.
For each shopping trip participating households made during the two-week observation period,
the shoppers recorded their reasons for the shopping trip and picked why they had chosen the
stores they did from a list of possible reasons relating to the factors under investigation. They
also completed a questionnaire in which they noted which of their purchases were planned and
which were unplanned.
The researchers found that as shoppers’ overall shopping goals became more abstract, the
shoppers made more unplanned purchases. Similarly, unplanned buying increased when
shoppers chose a store for its low prices or its attractive promotions. A store’s assortment and
service had no effect on unplanned purchases, however. When shoppers chose a store for the
convenience of one-stop shopping, unplanned buying went up; when a store was one in a series
of stores to be visited, by contrast, unplanned buying decreased. Finally, although out-of-store
marketing had no significant direct effect on trip-level unplanned buying, there was an
interaction between out-of-store marketing and in-store marketing that did boost unplanned
buying.
These findings have immediate relevance for retailers. They show that ad campaigns such as
Wal-Mart’s “Save Money. Live Better,” which focus on abstract shopping benefits, are likely to
generate increased unplanned spending. In the current study, when shoppers’ goals were at their
most abstract, their unplanned purchases went up 60%. Similarly, the fact that there is an
interaction between out-of-store and in-store marketing that boosts unplanned spending suggests
that retailers should reevaluate the importance of out-of-store advertising. The current study also
validates focusing on the characteristics of the shopping trip rather than the shopper. That is,
rather than focusing only on attracting particular types of customer, marketers and retailers may
also fruitfully work on promoting a certain type of shopping trip (one with abstract goals, for
example).
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
1
The research confirms that shopping trips to hard discounters, which offer low prices, a large
selection, and the convenience of one-stop shopping, are more likely to have the most abstract
goals (in the case of the two big-box discounters in the study, 53% and 44% of the visits were
motivated by the most abstract goals). Interestingly, however, when shoppers visit supermarkets
with an abstract goal in mind, there is an interaction between the supermarket format and the
shopping goal that results in a boost in sales over and above what can be attributed to the abstract
goal alone. The bottom line is that all retail formats benefit when the shoppers’ goals are
abstract.
The current study was conducted in a single European country, and the researchers urge further
research in other parts of the world to see how generalizable the results are and how well they
apply to countries at other stages in the evolution of retail markets.
David R. Bell is Professor, Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania. Daniel Corsten is
Professor, Instituto de Empresa Business School, Madrid. George Knox is Assistant Professor,
Tilburg University, The Netherlands.
Acknowledgments
We thank Andre Bonfrer, Marnik Dekimpe, Els Gijsbrechts, Christophe Van den Bulte, seminar
participants at the 2007 Wharton Marketing Conference, Ross Rizley, Susan Keane, and two
anonymous MSI reviewers for their comments. We are very grateful to Sjoerd Schaafsma of
Aecasis and Gilles Martin of Unilever for sharing knowledge and insights about shoppers and
shopping, and for access to data, and also to Olga Liberzon of Deloitte Consulting LLP for
valuable insights. Any errors, of course, are our own. Authorship is alphabetical; all authors
contributed equally.
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
2
“Supermarkets are places of high impulse buying … – fully 60 to 70 percent of purchases there
were unplanned, grocery industry studies have shown us.”
Paco Underhill
1
Managers, acting in accordance with this widespread belief, invest considerable resources
inside the store to influence shoppers. Recently, the Grocery Marketing Association forecasted a
compound annual growth rate of over 20 percent for in-store marketing budgets; furthermore,
Advertising Age reported “… the oft-quoted statistic that consumers make 70% of brand
decisions in the store boosted shopper marketing and made other advertising seem almost
pointless.” Unplanned buying clearly results from exposure to in-store stimuli; we argue that it
also depends on conditions established before the shopper enters the store, some of which are
under the retailer’s control. We take the retailer’s perspective and focus on these largely ignored
out-of-store factors, including the overall trip goal and other shopping trip antecedents, while
controlling for known in-store drivers. Retailers can benefit by generating additional unplanned
buying from their existing shopper base.
Unplanned buying is essential to retailers yet academic research is sparse and what
constitutes “unplanned buying” differs by study. We examine unplanned category purchases,
since a majority of items on shopping lists are at the category, rather than brand or stock-
keeping-unit, level (Block and Morwitz 1999); our dependent variable, the total number of
unplanned category purchases per trip, allows us to assess the basket-level impact of our out-of-
store factors. Classic (e.g., Kollat and Willett 1967) and recent (e.g., Inman, Winer, and Ferraro
2009) articles study category characteristics and shopper activities inside the store that have
implications for consumer welfare, e.g., ways in which consumers can safeguard themselves
from “too much” unplanned buying. In contrast, we explore the role of consumer pre-shopping
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
3
strategies and show how a retailer can use this “shopping trip antecedent” perspective to
stimulate unplanned buying.
2
In sum, we study how what a shopper “brings to the store” affects
how she behaves once “inside the store.” We focus on actionable trip-level drivers, such as the
abstractness of the overall shopping goal (Lee and Ariely 2006) and specific goals associated
with store choice (e.g., those related to anticipated prices and assortments, as in Bell and Lattin
1998 and Briesch, Chintagunta, and Fox 2009). Controlling for the main effects of in-store
stimuli, we examine the interaction between out-of-store and in-store promotions (Kahn and
Schmittlein 1989; 1992). We build on studies linking shopping trip antecedents to in-store
choices (e.g., Briesch et al. 2009; Hansen and Singh 2009; Kahn and Schmittlein 1989; 1992),
and show how they affect unplanned buying.
In contrast to most published research, we use diary panel data to investigate unplanned
buying. Panel data are critical to our substantive objective; a positive relationship between, for
example, shopping goal abstractness and unplanned buying in cross-sectional data cannot
distinguish two rival explanations: (1) “abstract-goal shoppers (a specific shopper segment) do
more unplanned buying”, and (2) “the same shopper does more unplanned buying on trips when
her shopping goal is abstract”. If (1) is true, retailers may only be able to augment unplanned
buying by attracting certain types of shopper; if (2) is true, more unplanned buying can be
generated from the existing customer base. This distinction is crucial, since it will be more costly
for the retailer to pursue (1) than (2).
3
We contribute three new findings to the collective knowledge on unplanned buying. (1)
Unplanned buying increases monotonically with the abstractness of the overall shopping goal
held by the shopper before entering the store. (2) Store-linked goals held prior to shopping
produce trip-specific changes in unplanned buying. On trips where the household chooses the
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
4
store for good pricing and shopping convenience there is more unplanned buying; on trips where
the store is chosen as part of a multi-store shopping strategy there is less (more than one store-
specific goal can be activated on a trip). (3) Out-of-store marketing has no direct effect on
unplanned buying; however, exposure to out-of-store marketing reinforces the lift in unplanned
buying that is triggered by in-store marketing. We show that the collective revenue impact of
these effects is significant and we offer some preliminary evidence that the “abstract goal” effect
differs across retail formats for the same shopper. While hard discounters see a larger share of
shoppers’ abstract trips, a shopper visiting a full service supermarket with an abstract shopping
trip goal does even more unplanned buying (over and above that due to the abstract goal alone).
The paper is organized as follows. We first summarize prior findings, introduce our
shopping trip antecedent perspective, and develop our hypotheses. Next, we describe the unique
diary panel data (over 18,000 purchases in 58 categories, from more than 3,000 trips, 400
households, and 23 stores) and measures. We then specify Poisson and Tobit models and report
the findings. The final section offers implications for managers and researchers.
Literature Review and Conceptual Development
Our objective is to understand how the goals held by shoppers and the marketing they are
exposed to before they enter a store shape their unplanned buying decisions once inside the
store. We begin with a brief summary of previous findings and then introduce our conceptual
framework and hypotheses.
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
5
Prior research
Kollat and Willett (1967), in a classic and widely cited study, find unplanned buying is
positively related to transaction size, and negatively related to shopping lists and the number of
years married. By examining the frequency of past customer experience with the chosen
unplanned items, they surmise that “in-store stimuli usually reminds shoppers of present or
future needs rather than evoking new needs” (p. 30). Granbois (1968) finds that unplanned
buying increases with time spent in the store, number of aisles shopped, and the number of
people in the shopping party. Park, Iyer, and Smith (1989) find that shoppers do the most
unplanned buying when they are in unfamiliar stores and under no time pressure. Beatty and
Ferrell (1998) focus on individual differences and find the “propensity for impulsiveness” trait is
a significant driver of unplanned buying. Rook and Fisher (1995) study individual differences as
well; they show that normative evaluations moderate the acceptability of impulse buying—
purchasing a gift on the spur of the moment is a good thing, but splurging on oneself is not.
Based on the self-control literature, Inman, Winer, and Ferraro (2009) predict and find that
certain category characteristics, like hedonicity, and consumer in-store activities, such as the
number of aisles shopped, increase unplanned buying across individuals.
More information on prior findings and methods is summarized in Table 1 (following
References). A common theme across these articles is the focus on in-store drivers of unplanned
buying and the effects of individual difference variables (i.e., demographics and shopping
habits). Our study complements these by examining out-of-store factors and trip-level
antecedents of unplanned buying. Studies that focus on pre-shopping factors from which the
motivation and context for a shopping trip emerge are rare (“Marketing actions that influence
shopper behaviour” is a focus of MSI’s 2010 “Shopper Marketing” research initiative).
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
6
This research: Out-of-store factors and hypotheses
We develop our conceptual framework similar to Chandon et al (2009), who study the
effectiveness of in-store marketing. As shown in Figure 1 (following References), we isolate out-
of-store factors, controlling for in-store factors, and allow for the possibility that time spent
shopping is endogenous.
4
In our model, we focus on the pre-shopping process of a household,
which includes establishing an overall shopping goal, developing store-specific shopping goals,
and possible exposure to out-of-store marketing (e.g., store fliers in the mail, word-of-mouth
from family and friends, television advertising). Each of these three shopping trip elements is
shown in Figure 1. The overall shopping trip goal ranges from concrete to abstract, whereas
store-specific goals cover pricing, assortment, service, location convenience, and crowding
(more than one store-specific goal can be activated on any particular trip); out-of-store marketing
encompasses a variety of factors. Note that each of these shopping trip elements are (in principle
at least) within the sphere of influence of the retailer.
Overall shopping trip goal (H
1
). Shoppers may enter a store with an overall goal ranging
from the very precise and concrete (e.g., to take advantage of a specific promotion) to the
relatively abstract (e.g., to fill up on weekly needs). Construal level and mind-set theories also
distinguish between abstract and precise goals (e.g., Gollwitzer 1999; Trope, Liberman and
Wakslak 2007); decision makers in “abstract” states are more flexible and receptive to their
environments whereas those in more precise states are “closed-off” to their surroundings. More
recent applied research also emphasizes the importance of goal abstraction: “The success of
marketing actions, such as promotions, depends on the goals (emphasis ours) consumers have
when they are exposed to such promotions” (Lee and Ariely 2006, p. 60). Related evidence
shows that the “type of trip”—a proxy for shopping goal abstractness—affects in-store
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
7
behaviors, conditional on store choice (e.g., Seetharaman, Ainslie, and Chintagunta 1999;
Walters and Jamil 2003).
H
1
: Unplanned buying increases monotonically with the abstractness of the individual
consumer’s overall shopping trip goal.
To test H
1
we use a continuum of abstraction ranging from concrete goals (“shopping for
special offers and promotions”, “shopping for immediate consumption”, “shopping for a meal on
the same day”), to the relatively abstract (“fill-in trip: daily essentials and top-up shopping”) and
the most abstract (“major trip: shopping for the whole week or more”).
5
We also need to rule out
plausible alternative explanations. On trips where shoppers have an abstract goal they peruse
more items and visit more aisles; as a result, they make more unplanned purchases. We rule these
explanations out by adjusting for the number of planned purchases (a proxy for items perused),
and the amount of time spent in store (a proxy for the number of aisles visited).
Store-specific goals (H
2
). Store choices depend on price image perceptions (Hansen and
Singh 2009), breadth and depth of assortment (Briesch, Chintagunta, and Fox 2009), location
convenience (Huff 1964), the ability to do one-stop shopping (Messenger and Narasimhan 1997),
and store service—an important element in store positioning (Lal and Rao 1997). Any reason for
choosing a store, by definition, affects store choice (positively). What is not known is whether
these store-specific goals determined ex ante before the visit, also affect unplanned buying in the
store.
6
Prior research implies that shoppers will do more unplanned buying in stores with low
prices, because they feel more normatively justified (Rook and Fisher 1995). On trips where
shoppers take advantage of one promoted product, they will likely be aware of and buy other
promoted products (Lichtenstein, Netemeyer, and Burton 1995). Similarly, wider assortments
tempt shoppers to deviate from plans and also encourage those with poorly defined preferences
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
8
to do more unplanned buying. Stores with good service also engender confidence and pleasure in
shopping, which is positively related to unplanned buying (Donovan et al 1994; Sherman and
Smith 1987). Hence
H
2A
(Pricing): Unplanned buying increases on trips where the shopper chooses the
store for low prices and attractive promotions.
H
2B
(Assortment): Unplanned buying increases on trips where the shopper chooses
the store because it has a wide assortment.
H
2C
(Service): Unplanned buying increases on trips where the shopper chooses the
store because it has good service.
To test H
2A
-H
2C
we elicit these store choice goals directly from shoppers. The store choice
reasons “Low prices”, “Large assortment”, and “Good service” are hypothesized to increase
unplanned buying on the trip; however, other reasons for store choice need not increase
unplanned buying. Location convenience from one-stop shopping in the chosen store
(“Everything I need in one place”), and one-stop shopping for the trip in general should have
opposite effects. A store chosen for “one-stop shopping” should see more unplanned buying—by
by committing to only one store the shopper may be signaling that she has insufficient time
(Zeithaml 1985) or cognitive resources (Bettman 1979) to create a detailed plan on a given trip.
Conversely, trips where the shopper plans to visit multiple stores may indicate more complex
planning, as she may spread her category purchases across stores. On these trips she may also
cherry-pick from co-located stores (Fox and Hoch 2005). These behaviors imply less unplanned
buying in individual stores on a multi-store trip.
H
2D
(Specific Convenience): Unplanned buying increases on trips where the shopper
chooses the store for “one stop shopping”.
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
9
H
2E
(General Convenience): Unplanned buying decreases on trips where the shopper
chooses the store because “I can visit other stores at the same time”.
Finally, on some trips the shopper might choose a store to avoid crowds. One consequence
of a less crowded store is less time waiting in line, and less exposure to the product choices of
other shoppers. Moreover, according to the self-control literature, exposure to environmental
cues like noise and crowding decreases self-control (Evans 1979). Recently, Levav and Zhu
(2009) found that shoppers react against confinement by expressing a need for more variety.
Therefore, we expect a negative relationship between choosing a store to avoid crowds and
unplanned buying.
H
2F
(Crowding): Unplanned buying decreases on trips when the shopper chooses the
store to avoid in-store crowding and long queues.
Out-of-store and in-store marketing (H
3
). Shopper responsiveness to marketing stimuli is
the sine qua non of research in retailing. Shoppers redeem coupons when benefits exceed the
cost of sorting and clipping (e.g., Chiang 1995; Neslin 1990); they stockpile when savings
exceed the storage and holding costs (e.g., Blattberg, Eppen, and Leiberman 1981; Bell and
Hilber 2006). They respond to monetary and non-monetary promotions (e.g., Chandon, Wansink,
and Laurent 2000), are induced to buy more by signs and displays (Dhar and Hoch 1996; Inman
and McAlister 1993; Inman, McAlister, and Hoyer 1990) and their overall responsiveness is
predicted by their psychographics (Ailawadi, Neslin, and Gedenk 2001).
It is well known that in-store marketing activities capture a shopper’s attention and
therefore drive up unplanned buying (Inman, Winer and Ferraro 2009). On trips where the
shopper takes note of marketing information outside the store environment, they are likely
engaged in planning (Bettman 1979) so this should not affect unplanned buying. Prior research
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
10
suggests, but does not test, the idea that when a shopper is exposed to out-of-store marketing on
a trip, in-store stimuli can trigger forgotten needs (Inman, Winer and Ferraro 2009; Kollat and
Willett 1967), suggesting a positive interaction between out-of-store and in-store marketing.
H
3
: Unplanned buying increases on trips when the shopper who has been exposed to
out-of-store marketing also encounters in-store stimuli.
To test H
3
we measure exposure to marketing stimuli outside the store environment
(newspaper inserts, store leaflets in the mail, and other sources, such as television
advertising) and interact this with exposure to in-store marketing.
Control variables
We control for the direct effects of in-store marketing and other out-of-store contextual
factors that are not of substantive interest per se (see Figure 1, following References). These
factors include travel time to the store and whether the store was visited second or later in a
multi-store trip; both are proxies for “fixed costs” of shopping (e.g., Tang, Bell, and Ho 2001).
We also control for the mode of travel (walking, cycling, or driving) which affects the capacity
to transport goods, shopping periodicity (e.g., Helsen and Schmittlein 1993), weekend shopping
patterns (Kahn and Schmittlein 1989), and trip-level variation in shopper gender (e.g., Kollat and
Willett 1967). The number of planned purchases, i.e., those determined prior to the shopping
trip, controls for the ex ante trip-level basket size.
More time in the store on a trip leads to more unplanned buying (Park, Iyer, and Smith
1989). We have no substantive interest in the effect of time, but we do need to control for it
appropriately. One approach is to argue that, conditional upon the other variables, time spent in
store can be included as a direct covariate (see Inman, Winer, and Ferraro 2009 for this
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
11
approach); however, like Inman, Winer, and Ferraro (2009), we are concerned that time in the
store is possibly endogenous. In our data there is a relatively small trip-level positive correlation
between time in the store and the number of categories bought (r = .24). We use two model-
based solutions to address this endogeneity problem—instrumental variables Poisson regression
and Tobit regression—and discuss both in the Model and Findings section. Our goal is to show
that the estimates of interest, i.e., those that test H
1
-H
3
, are robust to alternative methods of
controlling for the effect of time spent in the store.
Data and Measures
The diary panel contains over 18,000 category purchases from 58 product categories
(listed in the Appendix). Participating households were screened to be representative of the
market for the country in question and were paid €20 for their cooperation. For each trip,
households completed a short questionnaire and checked off whether each category purchase was
“planned in advance of the store visit and purchased” or “decided in store and purchased.” The
questionnaire included several other questions; respondents did not know that we were studying
unplanned buying per se. Households filled in a new questionnaire directly after each trip and
attached their receipts (we asked this to ensure accurate reporting and subsequently cross-
checked receipts with questionnaires). After two weeks, the research firm visited each household
to collect the questionnaires.
Recall that a key methodological difference between this study and prior research is that
ours uses panel diary data, rather than one-shot experiments or shopper intercept data (see Table
1, following References). These data allow us to disentangle whether changes in unplanned
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
12
buying are truly driven by factors that vary across shopping trips for the same customer and not
simply by differences across customers (unobserved heterogeneity). This is important because if
the retailer can generate more unplanned buying from the existing shopper base, it obviates the
need to attract a certain type of shopper who is especially susceptible to unplanned buying. We
estimate panel fixed effects models to ensure that parameters are estimated on within-customer,
rather than between-customer, differences, but this requires at least two observations per
household.
7
This leaves 441 households who take 3,014 supermarket trips during the two-week
observation period in June-July 2006. Households take between 2 and 23 trips (the mode is 6)
and trips occur at 23 distinct retail chains. Sample statistics for the variables in our study are
given in Table 2, following References.
“Fill-in trip for daily essentials and top up shopping” is the most prevalent shopping trip
goal (43% of all trips), followed by “major trips that occur weekly or less frequently” (26%),
“shopping for meals on the same day” (15%), “shopping for immediate consumption” (11%),
and “shopping for special offers and promotions” (3%). “Leaflets delivered to the home” are the
most commonly observed out-of-store marketing device (seen for 19% of all trips). Shoppers can
report multiple goals for choosing a particular store on a trip (the average number of store goals
per trip is 1.7), so the percentages sum to more than one. The most common is “able to visit other
stores at the same time” (37% of all trips). The average number of unplanned category purchases
per trip is 1.39 (the range is 0 to 10) and the average total number of planned categories is 5.00.
For both variables, we report the log totals; descriptive statistics are shown in Table 2 (following
References).
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
13
Models and Findings
We now motivate and estimate several models of unplanned buying to ensure the
conclusions we draw are valid. To account for the possibly endogenous relationship between
unplanned buying and time spent in store, we use an instrumental variables Poisson model for
the count (of unplanned category purchases per trip) and two different Tobit specifications that
explicitly model the rate of unplanned buying per unit time spent in store.
A Poisson model of unplanned buying
Consider h = 1, 2, … H households taking t = 1, 2, … T
h
shopping trips. The total number
of unplanned purchases on each trip t for each household h, is UP
ht
and we assume that UP
ht
follows a Poisson distribution. First, the number of unplanned category purchases is an integer
with no a priori upper bound.
8
Second, as shown in Ross (1996), the Poisson distribution can be
derived as an approximation to the sum of independent Bernoulli random variables (X
1
, X
2
, ... X
n
)
with heterogeneous parameters. To see this, let X
i
= 1 if an unplanned purchase is made in
category i = 1, 2, … N, and 0 otherwise. N is the total number of categories. Dropping subscripts,
let
1
N
i
i
UP X
=
=
∑
and allow unplanned purchase incidence probabilities to vary across categories,
X
i
|
θ
i
~ Bernoulli(
θ
i
). If we assume that
θ
i
follows a Beta distribution B(a, b) across categories,
the marginal distribution of X
i
is Bernoulli with probability
a
p
a b
=
+
.
9
If p is small, then UP ~
Poisson (Np) which leads to equation (1) with Np =
µ
.
Although a multivariate probit model could be applied to the category-level data,
10
modeling the total number of unplanned category purchases is better suited to our trip-level
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
14
research objectives. The categories themselves (listed in the Appendix) are defined at a level that
makes cross-category substitution less relevant; furthermore, we have no information on
category-level marketing. The large number of categories (58) would also make this approach
tough to implement.
UP
ht
follows a fixed effects Poisson model (Winkelmann 2008, p. 222):
( )
( )( )
exp
,
!
ht
UP
h ht h ht
ht ht h
ht
P UP x
UP
α µ α µ
α
−
= where exp ( )
ht ht ht
x
µ τ β
′
= . (1)
The Poisson-distributed variable is the product of an exponential mean function,
µ
ht
and a
multiplicative household-specific effect, , that is estimated jointly with . The mean
µ
ht
in
equation (1) is a combination of the non-negative rate, , adjusted for the length of
exposure
τ
ht,
i.e., the amount of time spent in the store. Explanatory variables ( ) are the out-
of-store factors of interest as well as the set of controls and store fixed effects. The expected
number of unplanned purchases is:
( )
,
ht ht h h ht
E UP x
α α µ
=
. (2)
There is a closed-form analytical expression for that can be inserted back into the
likelihood. Because this obviates the need to estimate H separate household-level fixed effect
parameters, the estimates of are neither biased nor inconsistent (Winkelmann 2008). The first-
order condition for uses only within-shopper variation and is a product of the residuals, scaled
by the within-household average ratio of observed unplanned buying ( ) to expected
unplanned buying ( ), and the explanatory variables:
1 1
0
h
H T
h
ht ht ht
h t
h
UP
UP x
µ
µ
= =
− =
∑ ∑
(3)
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
15
Household effects
h
α
are estimated non-parametrically; hence we completely control for
any characteristics that vary across, but not within, households (e.g., household size, income,
deal-proneness, shopping enjoyment). In contrast, studies without this design can only control
for household characteristics they observe; they must assume that all the other unobserved
characteristics are randomly distributed and independent of the explanatory variables. The latter
assumption is especially hard to justify, considering the large set of characteristics potentially
correlated with shopping goals and unobserved in typical datasets. In summary, our fixed-effects
model is estimated on shopping trip-level (within-household) differences and avoids biased
estimates of
β
that arise from: (1) misspecification of the distribution of random effects, and (2)
correlation between the shopper-level baseline
h
α
and the explanatory variables x
ht
. A Hausman
test comparing our fixed effects model with a random effects model (which assumes that the
distribution of
h
α
is independent of x
ht
), rejects the random effects specification (p < .001).
Finally, because the first-order conditions in equation (3) are identical to method of moments
estimation, “one does not need to worry about over-dispersion, or other expressions of non-
Poisson-ness” (Winkelmann 2008, p. 227).
An additional issue in our application is that exposure time, i.e., time in store, is
potentially endogenous (see Figure 1 and the earlier discussion of this point). We account for this
by: (1) using instrumental variables for exposure time in the Poisson model described above, and
(2) estimating two separate Tobit specifications that directly model the rate of unplanned
buying.
11
We use day, hour and location dummy variables as instruments that have significant
effects on exposure time (first stage regression R
2
=.47), but not on unplanned buying directly
(
χ
2
(16)
= 14.09, p = .59), and thereby satisfy the instrumental variables exclusion restriction.
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
16
A Tobit model of unplanned buying
To further account for the possibly endogenous relationship between unplanned buying
and exposure time (time spent shopping), we model the rate of unplanned buying as the
dependent variable, i.e., the total number of unplanned purchases on a trip divided by the time
spent in the store,
. Since this new variable is continuous and censored at zero, we can use a
Tobit model to relate it to explanatory variables,
( )
{ }
max , 0
log
ht
ht h ht ht
ht
UP
y x
α β ε
τ
′
= = + + . (4)
We estimate two versions of the Tobit model, because, unlike the Poisson, there is no
analytical trick that lets us circumvent estimating all H household-level fixed effects. The first
uses standard maximum likelihood estimation, but it is well-known that this procedure generates
inconsistent estimates due to the incidental parameters problem (Baltagi 2008). The second uses
a semi-parametric approach, trimmed least absolute deviations (LAD) to estimate the fixed
effects, which overcomes this problem (Honoré 1992).
Findings: Hypotheses H
1
-H
3
Table 3 (following References) reports the estimates for the fixed effect Poisson IV, Tobit,
and Trimmed LAD Tobit models. The signs and levels of significance for the focal variables and
the control variables are remarkably consistent across all three specifications and this provides us
with some assurance as to the robustness and validity of the estimates. Henceforth, we focus on
the Poisson results. Column four shows the marginal effects (from the Poisson IV model); for
continuous covariates these are computed at one standard deviation above and below the mean.
Overall shopping trip goal (H
1
). As the overall trip goal becomes more abstract (holding
the shopper and all else constant), there is more unplanned buying. The relevant coefficients
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
17
increase from
β
1
= -.278 to
β
5
= .462 (the same monotonically increasing sequence is seen in the
coefficients from the Tobit and trimmed Tobit models). A joint test of a monotonic ordering
from the concrete goals to the relatively abstract (“fill-in”) to the most abstract (“major”) is
highly significant (
χ
2
(4)
= 34.14, p < .001). Pair-wise tests are consistent with H
1
: the “major-
trip” effect is larger than the “fill-in trip” effect (
χ
2
(1)
= 8.37, p = .004); fill-in trip effects are
about the same as effects for shopping for meals on the same day (
χ
2
(1)
= 2.39, p = .123). The
effect of “same day” is about the same as the effect of shopping for “immediate consumption”
(
χ
2
(1)
= 1.18, p = .277) but the effect of immediate consumption is larger than that for the goal of
shopping for specific promotions (
χ
2
(1)
= 4.89, p < .027). Thus, H
1
is largely supported.
Store-specific goals (H
2
). While any goal that leads to store choice on a trip is positive for
the retailer concerned (from a traffic perspective), it remains to be seen whether specific reasons
translate into incremental (unplanned) category purchases. Our test of H
2
is stringent; the model
includes seventeen additional controls and store fixed effects to account for baseline store
differences apparent for all customers. On a trip when the shopper chooses a store for its “low
prices” (
β
6
= .111, t-statistic = 1.82) or “attractive promotions” (
β
7
= .120, t-statistic = 2.41)
there is a 12-13% increase in unplanned buying; however, assortment and service goals have no
effect. Thus, we see modest support for H
2A
but not for H
2B
or H
2C
.
On trips when a store is chosen for store-specific convenience (“one stop shopping”) there
is 12% more unplanned buying (
β
10
= .111, t-statistic = 2.36). On trips when it is chosen for
general convenience in the context of a larger plan, which may involve cherry picking and
basket splitting across stores (“I can visit other stores at the same time”), there is less unplanned
buying (
β
11
= -.119, t-statistic = -2.59). Hence, H
2D
and H
2E
are supported. The control variable
that measures whether a shop was visited second, or later in a multi-store trip, is not significant
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
18
(
γ
6
= -.022, t-statistic = -.46). Combining this finding with support for H
2D
and H
2E
implies that
on multi-store trips a shopper does less unplanned buying overall, and not just at stores she visits
later in the shopping sequence. Finally, on trips where the chosen store is selected because it is
“less crowded” the shopper does less unplanned buying (
β
12
= -.119, t-statistic = -1.97).
Consistent with H
2F
store congestion (and more exposure to the category choices of other
shoppers) increases unplanned buying for the focal shopper on that trip.
Out-of-store and in-store marketing (H
3
). Trip-level exposure to out-of-store marketing
activity has no significant direct effect on a household’s unplanned buying (
γ
11
through
γ
13
are
insignificant in all three model specifications). As predicted, there are however positive
interaction effects with in-store marketing. On trips when a household is aware of leaflets prior
to shopping and also reads leaflets seen in-store while shopping, there is 36% more unplanned
buying (
β
14
= .305, t-statistic = 2.70). Similarly, on trips where a household has prior exposure to
store marketing through advertising seen on TV, or delivered through coupons, or friends and
family and also reads leaflets while shopping, there is 68% more unplanned buying (
β
15
=.518, t-
statistic = 2.14). This is strong evidence for the interplay between out-of-store marketing and in-
store marketing. Thus, in H
3
we find strong support for the untested conjecture of Kollat and
Willett (1967) that in-store marketing can trigger forgotten needs.
Control variables and robustness checks
Control variables. Our model includes an extensive set of trip-level controls, in addition to
store and household fixed effects. We have no substantive interest in the signs and significance
of the control variables per se; however it is important that they are either consistent with well-
established results or plausible (for new variables). The pattern of effects is consistent across all
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
19
three model specifications—we comment briefly on a few notable effects. Exposure to in-store
marketing stimulates unplanned buying. Coefficients on shelf features and displays seen on a trip
are highly significant (
γ
8
= .345, t-statistic = 4.03 and
γ
9
= .468, t-statistic = 4.65, respectively).
This finding has been reported in the literature; however, our panel data models allow us to claim
that this is unambiguously a trip-level effect for an individual shopper. Cross-sectional analysis
cannot rule out the following alternative explanation—only promotion sensitive shoppers scan
features and displays—and only these kinds of shoppers do unplanned buying.
12
(We also find
larger in-store marketing coefficients in a model without fixed effects. This suggests that the
findings for in-store marketing effects reported in the literature may be overstated.)
Unplanned buying increases on trips when the shopper travels by bicycle or car (relative
to a base case of walking). Trips to more distant stores involve less unplanned buying (
γ
1
= -.121,
t-statistic = -3.65). This suggests that when the fixed travel cost is high, the shopper may be more
inclined to plan category purchases. Consistent with Inman, Winer, and Ferraro (2009),
unplanned purchasing increases when the shopping trip is taken by a female member of the
household (
γ
6
= .345, t-statistic = 4.16). Kahn and Schmittlein (1989; 1992) speculate that the
overall shopping trip goal interacts with response to in-store promotions. We find negative
interaction effects (
γ
15
and
γ
17
) only in the Poisson model, because they are artifacts of the log-
linear model specification, which defines interactions as proportional to the main effects, rather
than a substantive finding per se (there are no significant effects found in the Tobit models).
Robustness checks. The main findings are robust to Poisson and Tobit specifications. The
fit of the Poisson model is acceptable—the squared correlation between predicted and actual
values is .49. The R
2
KL
metric for non-linear models proposed by Cameron and Windmeijer
(1997) and based on the Kullback-Leibler divergence, is .46. If we replace the number of
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
20
planned purchases with a set of dummy variables the results are largely unchanged. Since time is
possibly endogenous we use an IV estimation strategy; however, qualitatively similar effects for
the parameters of interest obtain with non-parametric controls for time, i.e., if we use dummy
variables to capture shopping trips occurring in discrete intervals of time. We also quantify the
additional variation in unplanned buying explained by a trip-level perspective. The R
2
KL
is .29 in
a household-effects-only model; hence, adding trip-to-trip variation increases R
2
KL
by over 50%.
Thus, our substantive trip-level perspective is also justified on statistical grounds.
Discussion and Conclusion
In contrast to prior literature, we explain trip-level unplanned buying that originates from
decisions made by the shopper before she steps into the store, while controlling for previously
found in-store effects. This is a key point of differentiation; most studies focus on differences
across shoppers and categories, and on the effects of stimuli found inside the store. Furthermore,
our panel data models allow a true trip-level interpretation of the model coefficients and our
findings are not confounded by heterogeneity across shoppers. .
Key findings
Figure 2 (following References) shows the average expected percentage change in
unplanned category purchases as a function of the overall shopping trip goal, all other factors
held constant. Consistent with H
1
and recent experimental work (e.g., Lee and Ariely 2006) as
well as goal-setting (Gollwitzer 1999) and construal level (Trope, Liberman and Wakslak 2007)
theories, the more abstract the shopping goal, the more unplanned buying. Major trips have the
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
21
greatest “scope” for unplanned buying because the shopping mission involves satisfying a range
of household needs. One need could relate to a meal (e.g., dinner) but not the precise category
(e.g., chicken). These trips show the greatest percentage lift in unplanned buying—almost 60%.
Fill-in trips which are used for “daily essentials” and “topping up” follow with a 27% increase in
unplanned buying. Using the trip receipt data, we know the average trip is €21.45, with on
average 5.0 planned category purchases and 1.4 unplanned category purchases. This means that
unplanned purchases contribute about €4.70 to an average receipt and planned purchases
contribute about €16.75. Unplanned buying on trips where the shopper activates her most
abstract overall trip goal contributes, on average, an additional €2.77, a 10% increase in the total
amount spent.
Over forty years ago, Kollat and Willett (1967, p. 29) reasoned that: “During major trips
… the shopper’s needs are not well defined, thus the shopper is more receptive to in-store
stimuli.” Since we control for trip-level exposure to many other factors, the effect of “concrete
versus abstract goals” shown in Figure 2 is over and above that due to marketing stimuli seen by
the shopper, overall basket size, time spent in store, and the other variables we control for (see
Table 3, following References). Our fixed effects models estimate household-level intercepts and
therefore also rule out explanations such as “certain types of households are more likely to have
abstract goals.”
Any store-specific goal that brings a shopper to a store on a trip has a positive effect on
traffic; our study, however, shows that these goals also affect unplanned buying on the trip once
the shopper is inside the store. On trips where the store is selected for “low prices” and
“attractive promotions” there is more unplanned buying because the shopper may feel more
normatively justified when she engages in incremental purchases (Rook and Fisher 1995). We
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
22
find moderate increases in unplanned buying of 12-13%. Again, the overall category and Euro
value of this lift is about 2% for the average shopper on the average trip.
Store-specific convenience (“one-stop shopping”) leads to 12% more unplanned buying
whereas general convenience with respect to a larger shopping plan (“I can visit other stores at
the same time”) reduces unplanned buying by a similar amount. We also show that on multi-
store trips, the shopper does less unplanned buying overall and not just in stores visited second,
third, or later. To the extent that multi-store trips are an increasing reality in the evolving retail
landscape (Gijsbrechts, Campo, and Nisol 2008), there may be a corresponding decline in
unplanned buying. In summary, we find that the specific goal attached to a specific store not only
affects the shopper’s initial store choice but also her unplanned buying inside the store. Note that
our test for incremental buying based on store-specific trip goals is very stringent because the
model includes fixed effects for stores and households as well as a large set of controls.
It is well known that exposure to out-of-store marketing activity facilitates planning and
that exposure to in-store marketing stimuli generates unplanned buying. Hence, these marketing
instruments appear to work in opposing directions. Nevertheless, we hypothesized (H
3
) and
found that when it comes to unplanned buying, in-store and out-of-store marketing can be
mutually reinforcing. This implies that marketing activities should be assessed from the
perspective of their collective, rather than individual, weight.
Implications for managers and researchers
The findings summarized above offer new implications for managers and researchers. We
comment on two—the overall value of the trip-based view of shopping behavior and the efficacy
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
23
of alternative retail formats. Both issues are drawing increasing attention, yet neither has been
linked to unplanned buying.
The shopping trip view. Sophisticated retailers, including Walmart, collect detailed data on
shopping patterns over time and segment shoppers according to the purpose of their shopping
trip (Fox and Sethuraman 2006). In support of this approach, we find that trip-level factors
greatly improve our ability to understand unplanned buying (adding trip-to-trip variation
increased R
2
KL
by over 50%). This has implications for retail competition as it provides support
for moving beyond the more common practice of targeting customers (share-of-customer
competition) to targeting shopping trips (share-of-shopping-trip competition). Many retailers
believe that most purchase decisions are made inside the store (Advertising Age, July 28, 2008)
and allocate funds to in-store marketing to stimulate unplanned buying with in-store displays,
promotions, and technological innovations (Albert and Winer 2008). We endorse the importance
of these factors, but our results also point to the critical role of largely overlooked out-of-store
factors, such as the overall shopping trip goal and idiosyncratic store-specific goals. Convincing
shoppers to keep their goals abstract to generate more unplanned buying, can be achieved, for
example, by using time-dependent coupons that capitalize on the regularity of shopping patterns
(Fox, Metters and Semple, 2003), or by advertising more abstract shopping benefits (Walmart
exhorts customers to “Save Money. Live Better.”). More needs to be done to understand the
overall trip goal, store-specific goals and prior marketing exposures that shoppers bring with
them before they enter the store.
A parallel implication for researchers is that the shopping trip goal needs to be construed
in detail and that this will require more than receipt data alone can reveal. Moreover, in-store and
out-of-store marketing stimuli interact; it is not simply the case that out-of-store facilitates
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
24
planning and in-store stimulates opportunistic behavior. Finally, trip-level store-specific choice
goals affect more than just traffic—they can have positive or negative effects on incremental
buying within the store. Individually, the percentage lifts and decrements are modest (10-14%);
however, taken over many trips they have an economically meaningful impact.
Unplanned buying across retailers and retail formats. Hard discounters have dramatically
altered the retail landscape of Western Europe and North America (Cleeren et al. 2010; Van
Heerde, Gijsbrechts and Pauwels 2008). This not only raises the possibility for similar change in
other regions of the world, but also raises a need for new research on how shoppers behave in
this format. Traditional supermarkets and hard discounters vary in observable and substantial
ways on pricing, assortment, location, and store environment, but it is unclear how in-store
decisions vary across these formats, and, in particular, how this translates to unplanned buying.
Our research, which controls for differences across households, and allows the same shopper to
visit different stores and formats, is well suited to address this issue.
A key within-person finding in this research is H
1
—when the shopper activates an abstract
goal before shopping she does more unplanned buying in the store. But are shoppers more likely
to choose a particular format when they have abstract goals; moreover, does this phenomenon
interact with the format of the visited store (controlling for other variables and store-level fixed
effects)?
13
First, the data reveal that shoppers are more likely to visit hard discounter formats
when the overall trip goal is most abstract (“major trip”). The two hard discounters in our data
have 53% and 44% abstract trip visits; the traditional supermarkets only have 10-20%. To
address the second issue, we re-estimate our model but add two interaction terms. We find that
the coefficient on the interaction between the most abstract overall trip goal (“major trip”) and
choice of a traditional supermarket format is positive and significant (
β
trad
= .488, t-statistic =
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
25
2.79); the same coefficient for the hard discounter interaction is negative and not significant.
Stores of all types benefit when the shopper enters with an abstract goal; the positive interaction
implies that traditional supermarkets see an additional lift, over and above that from the abstract
goal alone. This translates into an additional €2.10 per trip for the traditional full service
supermarket and again highlights the impact of unplanned buying on store revenues.
Limitations
We investigate the effects of out-of-store factors on unplanned buying in one western
European market. Retail markets are in different stages of evolution—a cross-country
comparison of how what “shoppers bring to the store” affects unplanned buying is an important
area for future research. We use panel data to show how trip-to-trip variation drives unplanned
buying; however, our observation window is relatively short. (Unplanned buying is measurement
intensive so one must also consider possible sample attrition as time windows are lengthened.)
Longitudinal analysis of steady-state shopping habits around unplanned buying is another
important area for future research.
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
26
Appendix
List of Product Categories Used in the Analysis (in alphabetical order)
Baby and toddler food
Baking and dessert products
Bath and shower products
Beer
Books, CD’s, CD-roms
Bread (incl. crackers/toast/biscuit rusk) and bread rolls
Butter/margarine
Cake/biscuits/chocolate/ sweets
Cereals (corn flakes, cruesli, etc.)
Cheese
Chilled meals/pizzas
Chilled soup
Cleaning products
Clothes (incl. shoes, jewelry, clocks etc.)
Coffee and tea
Crisps/salted snacks/nuts
Deodorant
Dishwasher/washing up liquid/powder
Dry groceries (/salt/spices/herbs)
Eggs
Fabric conditioner
Fish (incl. crustacean and shellfish)
Flowers and plants
Fresh dairy products (drinks and desserts)
Fresh vegetables/fruit/potatoes
Frozen ice cream
Frozen meals/pizzas/snacks
Frozen vegetables/ potato products/fish/meat
Household goods (dishcloths, brushes, candles,
crockery, matches, light bulbs, etc.)
Long-life dairy products
Magazines
Mayonnaise and other cold sauces
Meals in a tin/jar/packet/box (incl. dinner kit)
Meat/chicken (incl. meat products)
Medicine/pills/supplements
Mixes for meals/packet mixes/ cooking sauces
Moisturising cream and body lotion
Nappies/other babyand toddler products
Office articles (incl. Computers/printers)
Olive oil/vinegar
Other articles
Other products in a jar/tin (meat, fish, olives, gherkins,
etc.)
Pasta/ rice
Pastries and confectionary
Pet food and? pet care
Sandwich filling (non chilled)
Sanitary products/panty liners
Shampoo and conditioner
Shaving products
Smoking materials
Soft drinks/juices/ice tea/sport drinks/diluting juice
Soups and bouillon (tinned/packet)
Sugar and condensed milk/creamer
Toilet paper/kitchen rolls/tissues
Toothbrushes/toothpaste/ oral care
Vegetables in a tin/jar
Washing powder/liquid
Wine and other alcoholic beverages
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
27
Notes
1
From the popular book, Why We Buy:The Science of Shopping by Paco Underhill.
2
Some major retailers (including Walmart) increasingly target customers according to the
purpose of their shopping trip (Fox and Sethuraman 2006); we validate this orientation as an
approach to understanding unplanned buying (see Discussion and Conclusion section).
3
We thank an anonymous reviewer for suggesting this important clarification.
4
We provide more details in the Model and Findings section and thank an anonymous reviewer
for suggesting the approaches we take. See equations (1) to (4) and the related discussion.
5
Some research (e.g., Kahn and Schmittlein 1989; 1992) distinguishes “major” and “fill-in” trips
ex post from grocery receipts. Our measures, developed from direct consumer self-reports (e.g.,
Walters and Jamil 2003), are more comprehensive, mutually exclusive and collectively
exhaustive, and were pre-tested by a professional marketing research company hired by our data
provider, a large multinational CPG company.
6
We test H
2A
-H
2F
after controlling for baseline unplanned purchasing in each store (though store
fixed effects); coefficients for the hypotheses are identified on household level trip-to-trip
variation only.
7
We also estimate random effects models using all the data; however, a Hausman test shows that
the key random effects modelling assumption—that the regressors are uncorrelated with the
random effect is rejected. We provide more details in the next section.
8
Technically, the 58 categories in the consumer survey is an upper bound, but this is far greater
from the observed maximum number of unplanned category purchase decisions on a single trip
(10).
9
See Knorr-Held and Besag (1998, p. 2050) and Ross (1996). This Poisson approximation also
allows unplanned purchase incidence probabilities to be weakly positively correlated across
categories. Ross (1996, p. 465) provides the error bound for the Poisson approximation when
correlations are present.
10
We thank an anonymous reviewer for this suggestion.
11
We are very grateful to an anonymous reviewer for suggesting the Tobit specification.
12
We thank an anonymous reviewer for drawing our attention to this point. A fixed effects panel
data model rules out these kinds of across-shopper differences.
13
We thank an anonymous reviewer for drawing our attention to this point.
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
28
References
Advertising Age (2008), “Pick a Product: 40% of Public Decide in Store.” July 28, 2008.
Ailawadi, Kusum L., Scott A. Neslin, and Karen Gedenk (2001), “Pursuing the Value Conscious
Consumer: Store Brands Versus National Brand Promotions.” Journal of Marketing 65 (1),
71-89.
Albert, Terri C. and Russell S. Winer (2008), “Brother, Can You Spare a Dime? The Spare Change
Effect at Point-of-Purchase.” Storrs?, Conn.: University of Connecticut, Working Paper.
Baltagi, Badi H. (2008), Econometric Analysis of Panel Data. Chichester, U.K.: Wiley.
Beatty, Sharon E., and M. Elizabeth Ferrell (1998), “Impulse Buying: Modeling Its Precursors.”
Journal of Retailing 74 (2), 169-91.
Bell, David R., and Christian A. L. Hilber (2006), “An Empirical Test of the Theory of Sales: Do
Household Storage Constraints Influence Consumer and Store Behavior?” Quantitative
Marketing and Economics 4 (2), 87-117.
———, and James M. Lattin (1998), “Consumer Shopping Behavior and Preference for Store Price
Format: Why ‘Large Basket’ Shoppers Prefer EDLP.” Marketing Science 17 (1), 66-88.
Bettman, James R. (1979), An Information Processing Theory of Consumer Choice. Reading, Mass.:
Addison-Wesley.
Blattberg, Robert C., Gary D. Eppen, and J. Lieberman (1981), “A Theoretical and Empirical
Evaluation of Price Deals for Consumer Non-Durables.” Journal of Marketing 45 (1), 116-29.
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
29
Block, Lauren G., and Vicki G. Morwitz (1999), “Shopping Lists as an External Memory Aid for
Grocery Shopping: Influences on List Writing and List Fulfillment.” Journal of Consumer
Psychology 8 (4), 343-75.
Briesch, Richard , Pradeep Chintagunta, and Edward J. Fox (2009), “How Does Assortment Affect
Grocery Store Choice?” Journal of Marketing Research 46 (2), 176-89.
Bucklin, Randolph E., and James M. Lattin (1991), “A Two-State Model of Purchase Incidence and
Brand Choice.” Marketing Science 10 (1), 24-39.
Cameron, A. Colin, and Frank A. G. Windmeijer (1997), “An R-squared Measure of Goodness of Fit
for Some Common Non-Linear Regression Models.” Journal of Econometrics 77, 329-42.
Chandon, Pierre, J. Wesley Hutchinson, Eric T. Bradlow, and Scott H. Young (2009), “Does In-
Store Marketing Work? Effects of the Number and Position of Shelf Facings on Brand
Attention and Evaluation at the Point of Purchase.” Journal of Marketing 73 (6), 1-17.
———, Brian Wansink, and Gilles Laurent (2000), “A Benefit Congruency Framework of Sales
Promotion Effectiveness.” Journal of Marketing 64 (4), 65-81.
Chiang, Jeongwen (1995), “Competing Coupon Promotions and Category Sales.” Marketing
Science 14 (1), 105-22.
Cleeren, Kathleen, Frank Verboven, Marnik G. Dekimpe, and Katrijn Gielens (2010), “Intra- and
Inter-Format Competition among Discounters and Supermarkets.” Marketing Science,
forthcoming.
Dhar, Sanjay K., and Stephen J. Hoch (1996), “Price Discrimination Using In-Store
Merchandising.” Journal of Marketing 60 (1), 17-30.
Donovan, Robert J., John R. Rossiter, Gilian Marcoolyn, and Andrew Nesdale (1994), “Store
Atmosphere and Purchasing Behavior.” Journal of Retailing 70 (3), 283-94.
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
30
Evans, Gary W. (1979), “Behavioral and Physiological Consequences of Crowding in Humans.”
Journal of Applied Social Psychology 9 (1), 27-46.
Fox, Edward J., and Raj Sethuraman (2006), “Retail Competition” in Retailing in the 21st
Century: Current and Emerging Trends, eds. Manfred Krafft and Murali K. Mantrala, 193-
210. Berlin, Germany: Springer.
———, and Stephen J. Hoch (2005), “Cherry-Picking.” Journal of Marketing 69 (1), 46-62.
———, Richard D. Metters, and John Semple (2003), “Every House a Warehouse: An Inventory
Model of Shopping Behavior.” Place: Southern Methodist Univ. Working Paper.
Gijsbrechts, Els, Katia Campo, and Patricia Nisol (2008), “Beyond Promotion-Based Store
Switching: Antecedents and Patterns of Systematic Multiple-Store Shopping.” International
Journal of Research in Marketing 25, 5-21.
Gollwitzer, Peter M. (1999), “Implementation Intentions: Strong Effects of Simple Plans.”
American Psychologist 54 (7), 493-503.
Granbois, Donald H. (1968), “Improving the Study of Customer In-Store Behavior.” Journal of
Marketing 32 (4), 28-33.
Grocery Marketing Association (2007), “Shopper Marketing: Capturing a Shopper’s Mind, Heart,
and Wallet.” Prepared by Deloitte LLP.
Hansen, Karsten, and Vishal Singh (2009), “Market Structure Across Retail Formats.” Marketing
Science 28 (4), 656-73.
Helsen, Kristiaan, and David C. Schmittlein (1993), “Analyzing Duration Times in Marketing:
Evidence for the Effectiveness of Hazard Rate Models.” Marketing Science 12 (4), 395-414.
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
31
Honoré, Bo E. (1992), “Trimmed Lad and Least Squares Estimation of Truncated and Censored
Regression Models with Fixed Effects.” Econometrica 60 (3), 533-65.
Huff, David (1964), “Defining and Estimating a Trading Area.” Journal of Marketing 28 (3), 34-8.
Inman, J. Jeffrey, Russell S. Winer, and Rosellina Ferraro (2009), “The Interplay Between
Category Characteristics, Customer Characteristics, and Customer Activities on In-Store
Decision Making.” Journal of Marketing 73 (5), 19-29.
———, and Leigh McAlister (1993), “A Retailer Promotion Policy Model Considering
Promotion Signal Sensitivity.” Marketing Science 12 (3), 339-56.
———, ———, and Wayne D. Hoyer (1990), “Promotion Signal: Proxy for a Price Cut?” Journal
of Consumer Research 17 (1), 74-81.
Iyer, Easwar S. (1989), “Unplanned Purchasing: Knowledge of Shopping Environment and Time
Pressure.” Journal of Retailing 65 (1), 40-57.
Kahn, Barbara K., and David C. Schmittlein (1992), “The Relationship Between Purchases Made on
Promotion and Shopping Trip Behavior.” Journal of Retailing 63 (3), 294-315.
———, and ——— (1989), “Shopping Trip Behavior: An Empirical Investigation.” Marketing
Letters 1 (1), 55-69.
Kollat, David T., and Ronald P. Willett (1969), “Is Impulse Purchasing Really a Useful Concept for
Marketing Decisions?” Journal of Marketing 33 (1), 79-83.
———, and ——— (1967), “Customer Impulse Purchasing Behavior.” Journal of Marketing
Research 4 (2), 21-31.
Knorr-Held, Leonhard, and Julian Besag (1998), “Modeling Risk from a Disease in Time and
Space.” Statistics in Medicine 17 (18), 2045-60.
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
32
Lal, Rajiv, and Ram Rao (1997), “Supermarket Competition: The Case of Every Day Low Pricing.”
Marketing Science 16 (1), 60-80.
Lee, Leonard, and Dan Ariely (2006), “Shopping Goals, Goal Concreteness, and Conditional
Promotions.” Journal of Consumer Research 33 (2), 60-70.
Levav, Jonathan, and Rui (Juliet) Zhu (2009), “Seeking Freedom Through Variety.” Journal of
Consumer Research 36 (4), 600-10.
Lichtenstein, Donald R., Richard G. Netemeyer, and Scott Burton (1995), “Assessing the Domain
Specificity of Deal Proneness: A Field Study.” Journal of Consumer Research 22 (4), 314-26.
Messinger, Paul R., and Chavravarthi Narasimhan (1997), “A Model of Retail Formats Based on
Consumers’ Economizing on Shopping Time.” Marketing Science 16 (1), 1-23.
Neslin, Scott A. (1990), “A Market Response Model for Coupon Promotions.” Marketing Science 9
(2), 125-45.
Park, C. Whan, Easwar S. Iyer, and Daniel C. Smith (1989), “The Effects of Situational Factors on
In-Store Grocery Shopping Behavior: The Role of Store Environment and Time Available for
Shopping.” Journal of Consumer Research 15 (4), 422-33.
Rook, Dennis W., and Robert J. Fisher (1995), “Normative Influences on Impulsive Buying
Behavior.” Journal of Consumer Research 22 (3), 305-13.
Ross, Sheldon M. (1996), Stochastic Processes, 2
nd
ed. New York, N.Y.: Wiley.
Seetharaman, P.B., Andrew Ainslie, and Pradeep K. Chintagunta (1999), “Investigating State
Dependence Effects Across Categories.” Journal of Marketing Research 36 (4), 488-500.
Sherman, Elaine, and Ruth B. Smith (1987), “Mood States of Shoppers and Store Image: Promising
Interactions and Possible Behavioral Effects.” In Advances in Consumers Research, vol. 14, eds.
M. Wallendorf and P. Anderson, 251-4. Provo, Utah: Association for Consumer Research.
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
33
Tang, Christopher S., David R. Bell and Teck-Hua Ho (2001), “Store Choice and Shopping
Behavior: How Price Format Works.” California Management Review 43 (2), 42-9.
Trope, Yaacov, Nira Liberman, and Cheryl Wakslak (2007), “Construal Levels and Psychological
Distance: Effects on Representation, Prediction, Evaluation and Behavior,” Journal of Consumer
Psychology 17 (2), 83-95.
Underhill, Paco (2000), Why We Buy: The Science of Shopping. New York, N.Y.: Simon and
Schuster.
Van Heerde, Harald, Els Gijsbrechts, and Koen Pauwels (2008), “Winners and Losers of a Major
Price War.” Journal of Marketing Research 25 (1), 5-21.
Vohs, Kathleen D., and Ronald J. Faber (2007), “Spent Resources: Self-Regulatory Resource
Availability Affects Impulse Buying.” Journal of Consumer Research 33 (1), 537-47.
Walters, Rockney G., and Maqbul Jamil (2003), “Exploring the Relationship Between Shopping
Trip Type, Purchases of Products on Promotion, and Shopping Basket Profit.” Journal of
Business Research 56 (1), 17-29.
Winkelmann, Rainer (2008), Econometric Analysis of Count Data. Berlin, Germany: Springer.
Zeithaml, Valarie A. (1985), “The New Demographics and Market Fragmentation.” Journal of
Marketing 49 (Summer), 64-75.
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
34
Table 1
Summary of Selected Literature on Unplanned Buying
Research Study Variables Research Methods and Data
Key Finding
Kollat and Willett (1967)
“Customer Impulse Purchasing
Behavior”
Granbois (1968)
“Improving the Study of Customer
In-Store Behavior”
Park, Iyer, and Smith (1989)
“The Effects of Situational Factors
on In-Store Grocery Shopping
Behavior: The Role of Store
Environment and Time Available
for Shopping”
Beatty and Ferrell (1989)
“Impulse Buying: Modeling Its
Precursors”
Main dependent variable: Number of
different products purchased
Independent variables: Shopper traits, i.e.,
demographics, and Shopping trip factors,
e.g., transaction size, major trip, purchase
frequency, use of shopping list
Main dependent variable: Number of
different products purchased
Independent variables: Shopper traits, e.g.,
demographics, and Shopping trip factors,
e.g., time in store, number in shopping
party
Dependent variable: Purchase of products to
satisfy needs that were? unrecognized
Independent variables: Shopping trip factors,
e.g., store knowledge, and time available
for shopping
Main dependent variable: Likelihood of an
impulse purchase
Independent variables: Shopper traits, i.e.,
demographics, “impulse buying
tendency”, Shopping trip factors, e.g.,
time, budget, enjoying
Collection method: Shopper interviews on store
entry and exit
Amount and type of data:
5
96 shoppers, 64
categories, cross-sectional data
Collection method: Shopper interviews on store
entry and exit, observation of shoppers while
shopping
Amount and type of data: 388 “shopping parties”,
84 categories, cross-sectional data
Collection method: Shoppers interviewed as in
Kollat and Willett (1967)
Amount and type of data: 68 shopping parties in
four experimental conditions (high or low
knowledge; no time pressure or time pressure),
cross-sectional data
Collection method: Shoppers interviewed as in
Kollat and Willett (1967)
Amount and type of data: 533 shoppers, 153 who
made “impulsive” purchases, cross-sectional
data
“Most unplanned
purchases are a response
to forgotten needs and
out-of-stock”
“Study of unplanned
purchasing can be
improved by combining
survey with observational
methods”
“Most unplanned
purchasing done in the
low store knowledge / no
time pressure condition”
“Individual differences in
propensity for
impulsiveness is a
significant driver of
unplanned buying”
Bucklin and Lattin (1991)
“A Two-State Model of Purchase
Incidence and Brand Choice”
Rook and Fisher (1995)
“Normative Influences on
Impulsive Buying Behavior”
Inman, Winer, and Ferraro (2009)
“The Interplay Between Category
Factors, Customer Characteristics,
and Customer Activities on In-
Store Decision Making”
Our Study (2010)
“Unplanned Buying on Shopping
Trips”
Main dependent variable: Probability of
category purchase incidence; latent
shopping state (planned or opportunistic)
Main independent variables: Shopper
“traits”, i.e., deal loyalty, Shopping trip
factors, e.g., inventory, store loyalty,
marketing mix variables
Main dependent variable: alternative
purchase scenarios that vary in level of
“impulsiveness”
Main independent variables: Shopper
“traits”, i.e., buying impulsiveness,
normative evaluations of impulsiveness as
moderator
Main dependent variable: Decision type
classified as planned, generally planned,
or completely unplanned, for each product
category
Main independent variables: Shopper traits,
i.e., demographics, Shopping trip factors,
e.g., time, use of shopping list, etc.,
Category factors, e.g., display, coupon
availability, category hedonicity
Main dependent variable: Number of
unplanned category purchases per trip
Main independent variables: Pre-visit, out-
store-factors (overall shopping trip goal,
store-specific goals, out-of-store
marketing)
Collection method: Purchase data collected from
supermarket scanners
Amount and type of data: 152 shoppers, 52 weeks
of purchases, 2 categories, panel data structure
Collection method: Respondent evaluation of
hypothetical buying scenarios (study 1), actual
buying behavior (study 2)
Amount and type of data: 212 undergraduate
students (study 1), 104 mall shoppers (study 2),
cross-sectional data
Collection method: Shoppers interviewed as in
Kollat and Willett (1967)
Amount and type of data: 2,300 shoppers, 14 US
cities, over 40,000 purchases, cross-sectional data
Collection method: Shoppers interviews and self-
reports
Amount and type of data: 441 shoppers, 3,014
shopping trips, 58 product categories, over 18,000
purchases, panel data
“Probability of unplanned
state is higher in low
loyalty stores, and for
households who buy on
deal”
“Impulsive buyers (trait)
do more impulsive buying
but this is moderated by
normative evaluation of
acceptability of impulsive
purchase”
“Stable category factors
and customer-self control
factors exert the most
influence on unplanned
buying”
“Unplanned buying
increases monotonically
with the abstractness of
the shopping goal held by
the shopper before
entering the store.”
Table 2
Model Variables and Summary Statistics
Model Variables
1
Mean
Proportion
Standard
Deviation
Min Max
H
1
: Shopping Trip Goal
Shopping for Special Offers and Promotions .031 .174 0 1
Immediate Consumption; To Use Straight Away .112 .315 0 1
Same Day; Shopping for Meals on the Same Day .149 .346 0 1
Fill-in Trip; Daily Essentials, Top-up Shopping .431 .495 0 1
Major Trip; Weekly or Less Often .256 .424 0 1
H
2
: Store Choice Goals
A: “Low Prices” .243 .429 0 1
A: “Attractive Promotions and Special Offers” .298 .458 0 1
B: “Large Assortment” .217 .412 0 1
C: “Friendly Store, Good Service” .149 .356 0 1
D: “Store Offers One Stop Shopping” .312 .463 0 1
E: “I Can Visit Other Stores at the Same Time” .365 .482 0 1
F: “No Crowds in the Store” .116 .320 0 1
H
3
: Out-of-Store Marketing
Special Offers Seen in the Newspaper .013 .112 0 1
Special Offers Seen in the Leaflet Delivered to Home .189 .392 0 1
Special Offers Seen on TV, Radio, in Coupons, or
Communicated by Friends and Family
.025 .157 0 1
Control Variables
Travel Time to Store (minutes) 7.874 6.640 0 70
Travel to Store by Bicycle or Scooter .325 .469 0 1
Travel to Store by Car or Taxi .479 .500 0 1
Trip on Friday or Saturday (Stores closed Sunday) .379 .485 0 1
Primary Shopper Female on Current Trip .814 .389 0 1
Multi-Store Shopping Trip (At Least One Other Store
Visited on this Trip Prior to Current Store)
.179 .384 0 1
Notes: Proprietary survey panel data collected from 441 shoppers, taking 3,014 shopping trips at supermarkets in a
Western European country. The data were collected in conjunction with a major multinational packaged goods
manufacturer who wishes to remain anonymous. The data cover the period June 12 to July 10, 2006.
1
All variables aside from times and category counts are dummy variables.
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
37
Table 2 (Continued)
Variables
Mean
Proportion
Standard
Deviation
Min Max
Control Variables (cont’d)
Total Number of Planned Category Purchases 4.997 4.147 1 28
Special Offers Seen at the Shelf .271 .446 0 1
Special Offers Seen on Display Away from Shelf .165 .371 0 1
Stay Informed about Special Offers From Store Leaflet in
the Shop
.257 .357 0 1
I Wanted the Shopping Trip to be Fast and Efficient .679 .467 0 1
Exposure Variable
Time Spent Shopping (minutes) 17.821 11.484 1 85
Dependent Variable
Total Number of Unplanned Category Purchases 1.39 1.93 0 10
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
38
Table 3
Parameter Estimates from Fixed Effect Poisson and Tobit Models
1
Dependent Variable: UP
ht
(Number of Unplanned Category Purchases)
FE
Poisson
(IV)
2
FE
Tobit
3
FE
Trimmed
Tobit
4
Marginal
Effect
(%)
5
H
1
: Shopping Trip Goal Abstractness
β
1
, Shopping for Special Offers and Promotions
-.278
+
-.174 -.210 -24%
β
2
, Immediate Consumption; To Use Straight Away
.023 -.070 -.072 -
β
3
, Same Day; Shopping for Meals on the Same Day
.119 .138 .132 -
β
4
, Fill-in Trip; Daily Essentials, Top-up Shopping
.241
**
.222
*
.233
*
27%
β
5
, Major Trip; Weekly or Less Often
.462
***
.522
***
.569
***
59%
H
2
: Store Choice Goals
β
6
, A: “Low Prices”
.111
+
.148
**
.156
*
12%
β
7
, A: “Attractive Promotions and Special Offers”
.120
*
.085
+
.075 13%
β
8
, B: “Large Assortment”
.064 .064 .074 -
β
9
, C: “Friendly Store; Good service
.088 .112
+
.094 -
β
10
, D: “Store Offers One Stop Shopping”
.111
*
.131
**
.121
*
12%
β
11
, E: “I Can Visit Other Stores at the Same Time” -.119
**
-.033 -.064 -11%
β
12
, F: “No Crowds in the Store”
-.129
*
-.067 -.119
+
-12%
H
3
: Out-of-Store Marketing
β
13
, Special Offers Seen in the Newspaper x
Stay Informed Through Leaflet About Offers
.209 -.095 .360 -
β
14
, Special Offers Seen in the Leaflet Delivered to Home x
Stay Informed Through Leaflet About Offers
.305
**
.252
*
.210 36%
β
15
, Special Offers Seen on TV, Radio, in Coupons, or
Communicated by Friends and Family x
Stay Informed Through Leaflet About Offers
.518
*
.571
*
.505
*
68%
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
39
Table 3 (Continued)
Dependent Variable: UP
ht
(Number of Unplanned Category Purchases)
FE
Poisson (IV)
2
FE
Tobit
3
FE
Trimmed
Tobit
4
Control Variables
γ
1
, Travel Time to Store (log minutes)
-.121
**
-.054 -.039
γ
2
, Travel to Store by Bicycle or Scooter
.180
*
.147
*
.172
*
γ
3
, Travel to Store by Car or Taxi
.385
***
.377
***
.414
***
γ
4
,Trip on Friday or Saturday (Stores closed Sunday)
-.107
**
-.066
+
-.059
γ
5
, Primary Shopper Female on Current Trip
.345
***
.332
**
.279
**
γ
6
, Multi-Store Shopping Trip (At Least One Other
Store Visited on this Trip Prior to Current Store)
-.022 -.058 -.040
γ
7
, Total Number of Planned Category Purchases (log units)
-.613
***
-.330
***
-.326
***
γ
8
, Special Offers Seen at the Shelf
.345
***
.377
***
.280
**
γ
9
, Special Offers Seen on Display Away from Shelf
.468
***
.456
***
.419
***
γ
10
, I Wanted the Shopping Trip to be Fast and Efficient
-.479
***
-.671
***
-.597
***
γ
11
, Special Offers Seen in the Newspaper
-.221 .174 -.028
γ
12
, Special Offers Seen in the Leaflet Delivered to Home
-.052 -.015 -.015
γ
13
, Special Offers Seen on TV, radio, in Coupons, or
Communicated by Friends and Family
-.046 -.051 -.113
γ
14
, Special Offers Seen at the Shelf x Major Trip
-.074 .046 .044
γ
15
, Special Offers Seen on Display Away from Shelf x Major Trip
-.259
*
-.084 -.142
γ
16
, Special Offers Seen at the Shelf x Fill-in Trip
-.170 -.047 -.068
γ
17
, Special Offers Seen on Display Away from Shelf x Fill-in Trip
-.212
+
-.015 -.065
Log Likelihood
-2,985 -2,298 -
Notes: Total number of households = 441; shopping trips = 3,014.
***
p < .001;
**
p < .01;
*
p < .05;
+
p < .10
1
Household and store fixed effects for all models suppressed to save space (available upon request).
2
The R
2
in the first stage regression (with instruments for time) is .47.
3
We also estimated a random effects Tobit model; the fixed effects model is preferred under the Hausman test.
4
The Trimmed Tobit least absolute deviations (LAD estimator) estimates fixed effects semi-parametrically (Honoré
1992). We estimate this model as a robustness check.
5
Marginal effects for continuous covariates calculated at one standard deviation above and below the mean.
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
40
Figure 1
Conceptual Framework
H
1
: Overall Shopping Trip Goal
(From concrete to abstract)
H
2A-F
: Store-Specific Goals
(Low prices, wide assortment, etc)
Unplanned Buying
H
3
: Out-of-Store Marketing
(Interaction with in-store)
Out-of-Store Control Variables
(Travel mode, day of week, etc)
In-Store Control Variables
(In-store marketing, etc)
Time Spent in the Store
(Instrumented)
Out
-
of
-
Store Factors
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
41
Figure 2
Expected Percentage Change in Unplanned Buying as a Function of the Overall Shopping
Trip Goal (All Other Variables Constant)
Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series
42