ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

There is growing interest in understanding how emotion regulation affects adaptation. The present study examined expressive suppression (which involves inhibiting the overt expression of emotion) and how it affects a critical domain of adaptation, social functioning. This investigation focused on the transition to college, a time that presents a variety of emotional and social challenges. Analyses focused on 2 components of suppression: a stable component, representing individual differences expressed both before and after the transition, and a dynamic component, representing variance specific to the new college context. Both components of suppression predicted lower social support, less closeness to others, and lower social satisfaction. These findings were robustly corroborated across weekly experience reports, self-reports, and peer reports and are consistent with a theoretical framework that defines emotion regulation as a dynamic process shaped by both stable person factors and environmental demands.
Content may be subject to copyright.
The Social Costs of Emotional Suppression: A Prospective Study of the
Transition to College
Sanjay Srivastava
University of Oregon
Maya Tamir
Boston College
Kelly M. McGonigal
Stanford University
Oliver P. John
University of California, Berkeley
James J. Gross
Stanford University
There is growing interest in understanding how emotion regulation affects adaptation. The present study
examined expressive suppression (which involves inhibiting the overt expression of emotion) and how
it affects a critical domain of adaptation, social functioning. This investigation focused on the transition
to college, a time that presents a variety of emotional and social challenges. Analyses focused on 2
components of suppression: a stable component, representing individual differences expressed both
before and after the transition, and a dynamic component, representing variance specific to the new
college context. Both components of suppression predicted lower social support, less closeness to others,
and lower social satisfaction. These findings were robustly corroborated across weekly experience
reports, self-reports, and peer reports and are consistent with a theoretical framework that defines emotion
regulation as a dynamic process shaped by both stable person factors and environmental demands.
Keywords: emotion regulation, social support, social adjustment, relationship satisfaction, life transitions
The hundreds of new faces that await freshmen as they begin
their first year of college can be overwhelming. Without the
comforts of home and the familiarity of high school, many students
experience a mix of excitement and dread as they prepare to enter
the unknown. During this emotionally intense and disorienting
time, meeting new people, making new friends, and creating a new
social support system are of paramount importance in having a
successful first year in college (Christie & Dinham, 1991). What
factors influence how students navigate the social challenges in
this crucial transition?
Given the intense emotions that are part of this transition, one
critical factor should be the way individuals regulate their emo-
tions. Recent work has shown that one common way that individ-
uals regulate emotions is through expressive suppression—that is,
by attempting to inhibit their emotion-expressive behavior. Draw-
ing from theory and research on emotions and their social func-
tions, we hypothesized that expressive suppression should have
substantial and important consequences for social functioning. The
goal of the present research is to investigate those social conse-
quences. The focal context of our study, the college transition, is
one where we expected those consequences to be particularly
pronounced and important.
Expressive Suppression: A Process Model
and Individual Differences
According to contemporary theories of emotion, emotions begin
with an evaluation of internal or external cues that have particular
relevance to an individual. When attended to and evaluated in
certain ways, these emotion cues give rise to a coordinated set of
response tendencies that involve experiential, behavioral, and
physiological systems (Mauss, Levenson, McCarter, Wilhelm, &
Gross, 2005). The central construct in our investigation, expressive
suppression, may be viewed through the lens of a process model of
emotion regulation (Gross, 2001). Within this model, different
forms of emotion regulation can be distinguished in terms of when
they have their primary impact along the timeline of the unfolding
emotional response. A broad distinction in the process model is
between antecedent-focused and response-focused emotion regu-
lation. Antecedent-focused regulation refers to things we do, either
consciously or automatically, before emotion-response tendencies
have become fully activated. Response-focused regulation refers to
things we do once an emotion is underway and response tenden-
cies have already been generated.
Sanjay Srivastava, Department of Psychology, University of Oregon;
Maya Tamir, Department of Psychology, Boston College; Kelly M.
McGonigal and James J. Gross, Department of Psychology, Stanford
University; Oliver P. John, Department of Psychology, University of
California, Berkeley.
This research was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant R01
MH58147 to James J. Gross and a Faculty Research Grant from the
University of California to Oliver P. John.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Sanjay
Srivastava, Department of Psychology, 1227 University of Oregon, Eu-
gene, OR 97403-1227. E-mail: sanjay@uoregon.edu
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology © 2009 American Psychological Association
2009, Vol. 96, No. 4, 883–897 0022-3514/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0014755
883
One prominent form of response-focused emotion regulation is
expressive suppression, which may be defined as a form of re-
sponse modulation that involves inhibiting ongoing emotion-
expressive behavior (Gross, 1998). For example, one might try to
look composed while feeling devastated inside, or one might try to
look calm while feeling angry or resentful. From a theoretical
perspective, several things are noteworthy about suppression.
1
First, it occurs relatively late in the emotion process, potentially
after affective experience has already been generated. Second, it
targets a component of emotion— expressive behavior—that is
potentially visible to others and that serves a communicative
function. Thus, the consequences of suppression may extend be-
yond internal experience and into the social world.
Experimental studies have demonstrated that people can suc-
cessfully use suppression to reduce expressive behavior. In labo-
ratory experiments, when participants have been instructed to use
suppression to regulate emotion, they show decreased emotion-
expressive behavior (e.g., Gross & Levenson, 1993, 1997). In
addition to altering expressive behavior, suppression has some
potential costs for affective experience. Several studies have found
that suppression leaves intact the subjective experience of negative
emotion but decreases the experience of positive emotions (Gross
& Levenson, 1997; Stepper & Strack, 1993; Strack, Martin, &
Stepper, 1988).
Research on individual differences in suppression has used the
Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; Gross & John, 2003).
This scale includes items referring to suppression of both positive
and negative emotions, as well as general emotion items that do
not reference valence; all of the items load on a common, general
factor. Findings with the scale are consonant with the experimental
research and add further information about people who use sup-
pression outside of the laboratory. Individuals who make frequent
use of suppression deal with stressful situations by masking their
inner feelings and clamping down on their outward displays of
emotion. Their efforts at suppression leave them with less positive
emotion and with more negative emotions, including feelings of
inauthenticity, than in individuals who use suppression less fre-
quently. Such efforts are partially successful, in that individuals
express less negative emotion than they actually experience; how-
ever, in absolute terms, they still express as much as individuals
who suppress less frequently. Individual differences in suppression
are distinct from other forms of emotion regulation and are not
correlated with cognitive reappraisal, an antecedent-focused form
of emotion regulation. In terms of the global Big Five traits (John
& Srivastava, 1999), suppression is somewhat associated with low
levels of extraversion but not at all with neuroticism or low
agreeableness (Gross & John, 2003; John & Gross, 2007).
The Social Context of Suppression:
New Research Questions
Suppression is a way of regulating emotions that targets expres-
sive behavior—a component of emotion with a social-
communicative function. An important topic for research, there-
fore, is how the use of suppression affects social functioning. In
one of the few experimental studies of the interpersonal conse-
quences of suppression, Butler et al. (2003) had unacquainted pairs
of participants watch a documentary about the bombing of Hiro-
shima and Nagasaki together and then discuss their reactions. In
some dyads, one partner was instructed (unbeknownst to the other)
to suppress their emotional expressions during the discussion.
Interacting with a partner who suppressed was more stressful than
interacting with a partner who acted naturally, as indexed by
increases in blood pressure. These findings suggest that by dis-
rupting the give and take of emotional communication, suppres-
sion has the potential to undermine social functioning to a signif-
icant degree.
Although studies such as these are an important first step, they
are limited in several ways: They involve an explicit and blunt
experimental manipulation; they feature a single context in which
emotionally expressive behavior was clearly normative and so-
cially desirable; and they involve the random assignment of par-
ticipants to a suppression group, rather than naturally occurring
within- or between-person variation in suppression. In the present
study, we sought to overcome these limitations.
Stability and Dynamics in Suppression
Most prior research on emotion regulation has mirrored the “two
disciplines” of correlational and experimental research identified
by Cronbach (1957). Correlational designs typically compare sta-
ble attributes of individuals but do not emphasize how individuals
adapt to their circumstances. Experimental designs focus on emo-
tion regulation cued by an experimenter’s instructions, but they
ignore both stable individual differences and spontaneously en-
acted emotion regulation. Cronbach argued that to understand all
the forces that constitute the whole person, which he called “the
organism at present,” one must consider both stable and dynamic
influences. Individual differences and dynamic enactment of emo-
tion regulation are part of the same larger picture (Bonanno, Papa,
Lalande, Westphal, & Coifman, 2004; Carver & Scheier, 1998;
Hoeksma, Oosterlaan, & Schipper, 2004), but neither correlational
nor experimental designs capture this totality very well. The
present investigation was designed to incorporate stable and dy-
namic components of emotion regulation in a longitudinal design,
allowing us to test simultaneously for both effects.
An Ecologically Meaningful Context and Outcomes
Does suppression have measurable and meaningful ramifica-
tions in important life contexts? Previous correlational studies
have focused on global, decontextualized outcomes, whereas ex-
perimental studies have measured outcomes in controlled labora-
tory settings. In the present investigation, we studied an emotion-
ally intense and personally meaningful time of life, when emotion
regulation resources are heavily demanded and have important
consequences—namely, the transition to college.
The college transition is often a stressful and demanding period,
during which many students confront new personal challenges and
learn to cope with multiple demands (Cantor, Norem, Niedenthal,
Langston, & Brower, 1987). Common sources of stress include
changes in relations with friends and family members, new social
activities, academic demands, financial responsibilities, and new
temptations (Petruzzello & Motl, 2006). Social challenges are
1
From this point forward, we use the more concise term suppression to
refer to the central construct of this investigation, suppression of expressive
behavior.
884 SRIVASTAVA, TAMIR, MCGONIGAL, JOHN, AND GROSS
among the most prominent in this transition (Shaver, Furman, &
Buhrmester, 1985), perhaps nearly twice as common as challenges
in the academic domain (Arthur & Hiebert, 1996). The transition
to college disrupts existing social support networks, separating
individuals from high school friends and family and forcing them
to form new relationships (Shaver et al., 1985). The largest impact
of the transition to college is often experienced upon entry, during
the first term, with reports of distress attenuating over time as
students adjust to their new environment (Compas, Wagner,
Slavin, & Vannatta, 1986; Gall, Evans, & Bellerose, 2000).
Multiple Indicators of Social Functioning
Humans desire to maintain close and supportive relationships
with others, and our psychological and physical well-being de-
pends on how well we are able to do so (Baumeister & Leary,
1995). Functionalist theories of emotion indicate that emotionally
expressive behavior is central to maintaining social bonds. Expres-
sive behavior communicates information to others about an indi-
vidual’s emotional state and associated needs and intentions. This
communicative function of emotion is central to a number of
adaptive interpersonal processes that rely on knowing others’
mental states (such as distress-sympathy cycles, social sharing, and
mutual interest; Keltner & Haidt, 1999). Drawing from this func-
tionalist perspective, we hypothesized that relatively greater use of
suppression by individuals in their everyday lives would disrupt
the normal flow of emotion-based communication and impede
social functioning as a result.
Specifically, we examined three indicators of adaptive social
functioning that we expected to be disrupted by suppression, as
well as a fourth that we did not expect to be disrupted. Social
support is an important predictor of health and may be particularly
critical during life transitions, such as the transition to college
(Brissette, Scheier, & Carver, 2002). Individuals who suppress the
expression of their emotions may fail to elicit social support
(Eisenberg, Fabes, Schaller, & Miller, 1989; Labott, Martin,
Eason, & Berkey, 1991). Closeness to others has important impli-
cations for general adaptation (for reviews, see Rholes & Simpson,
2004). Emotion-expressive behavior facilitates closeness by sig-
naling approachability (Simpson, Gangestad, & Nations, 1996),
the desire to affiliate (Harker & Keltner, 2001), the establishment
of rapport (Tickle-Degnan & Rosenthal, 1990), and the willingness
of the expresser to provide care and support (Lin, 1986). We thus
expected that individuals who suppress would miss opportunities
to establish close relationships with others. A subjective sense of
social satisfaction is one of the dominant predictors of life satis-
faction (Myers & Diener, 1995). We expected that individuals who
suppress would have less satisfying social lives as a consequence.
In contrast with these other outcomes, we did not expect that
suppression would be strongly related to likability (Gross & John,
2003). Likability is an important dimension in sociometric re-
search, where it serves as an indicator of how much others prefer
to interact with an individual (Newcomb, Bukowski, & Pattee,
1993). Closeness and likability are distinct constructs: Closeness
often builds over time, but liking is a quick and automatic evalu-
ation that can be made with no meaningful social interaction
whatsoever (Albright, Kenny, & Malloy, 1988). Our hypotheses
about suppression were based on its impact on interpersonal pro-
cesses; because liking depends less on interpersonal interactions,
we expected it to be less affected by suppression. Analyses of
likability could thus help establish a boundary for the adverse
social consequences of suppression. Because of concerns about the
validity of self-reported likability, we only assessed it with peer
reports.
The Present Study
The present study employed a prospective longitudinal design to
test the real-life social consequences of suppression during the
challenging transition to college (see Figure 1). We assessed
suppression on two occasions: a pretransition assessment during
the summer while participants were still at home (approximately 2
months before the beginning of college) and an early transition
assessment on campus right at the beginning of the fall term. This
enabled us to distinguish between two components of suppression:
stable individual differences and dynamic changes across the tran-
sition to college. We measured social outcomes using a
multimethod approach. First, we assessed ongoing social experi-
ences in weekly experience reports throughout the first term of
college. Second, we assessed social functioning with global reports
at the end of the term. Third, to gain a more objective assessment
of individuals’ social functioning, we analyzed reports completed
by knowledgeable peers at the end of the term.
We report the results in two parts. In Part 1, we report the
mean-level changes in suppression across the transition. We then
focus on the weekly experience reports, presenting descriptive
S1S
2
W1
E
Students
arrive on
campus
Fall term
(10 weeks)
End of
instruction
2 months
prior to the
transition
Predictors
Outcomes
At colle
g
eAt home
Context
W2 W
3 W
4 W
5 W
6 W
7 W
8 W
9 W
10
B
Figure 1. Study design timeline. Letters are positioned to indicate the timing of different assessments: S
suppression; B baseline reports of social functioning; W weekly experience reports of social functioning
outcomes; E end-of-term self-reports and peer reports of social functioning outcomes.
885
SOCIAL COSTS OF EMOTIONAL SUPPRESSION
analyses of the “typical” trajectory for a variety of social function-
ing indicators across the first 10 weeks of college, as well as
effects of suppression on these indicators. In Part 2, we focus on
the end-of-term assessments, as reflected in self-reports and peer
reports.
Part 1: Change in Suppression Across the Transition and
Social Experiences During the First Term
In Part 1, we focus on the assessment of suppression before and
after the transition to college and on the weekly experience reports
during the first term. We address two questions. First, as individ-
uals make the transition to a new college environment, in what
ways, if any, do their levels of suppression change? Second, how
do stable and dynamic components of suppression relate to social
functioning during the first term at college?
Consistency and Change in Suppression
Individual differences in emotion regulation reflect well-
established patterns of self-regulation (Gross & John, 2003; John
& Gross, 2004). For this reason, we expected suppression to show
moderate rank-order consistency, even across a transition as sig-
nificant as the one from high school to college. At the same time,
we recognize that different situations call for different types and
levels of emotion regulation. Individuals who suddenly find them-
selves in a new environment, surrounded by as yet unfamiliar
people, may well respond by being more guarded about expressing
their emotions. Although previous research suggests that suppres-
sion generally decreases from early to middle adulthood (John &
Gross, 2004), we expected mean levels of suppression to increase
across the transition.
Prospective Analyses of Stable and Dynamic Suppression
We expected suppression to lead to adverse outcomes in three
domains of social functioning: social support, closeness to others,
and social satisfaction. For each outcome variable, we estimated
three models that were constructed to narrow down the set of
plausible explanatory models through carefully selected controls.
Causation is established by satisfying three criteria: association
between a proposed cause and effect, direction (i.e., the cause
precedes the effect), and isolation from “third variables” (Bollen,
1989). Although this was not a randomized experiment and we
could not control for all possible third variables, the models were
designed to test for association and direction and to at least
partially address isolation.
Model 1 tested for associations between the dependent variables
and the stable and dynamic components of suppression, with no
controls. In Model 2, we added variables representing baseline
levels of the social functioning outcomes; these analyses help
establish the direction of effects (i.e., from suppression to social
functioning). Many of the outcome measures of social functioning
referred specifically to the college context, and thus, identical
measures would not have made sense in the summer before col-
lege. However, wherever possible, we employed conceptually
relevant baseline variables as controls. In Model 3, we introduced
social activity and positive and negative emotional experience into
the models. Previous research has shown that individual differ-
ences in suppression are correlated with extraversion, so we in-
cluded social activity (an index of extraversion expressed in the
college environment) to isolate the effects of suppression from
extraversion. In addition, some previous experimental and corre-
lational research suggested that the use of suppression may lead to
less positive or more negative emotional experience. To test
whether the findings could be attributed centrally to suppression,
and not just a side effect of its impact on emotional experience, we
included positive and negative emotions in Model 3 as controls.
Our longitudinal design allowed us to address another important
issue, unresolved in the previous experimental and correlational
research: Were the social outcomes of suppression merely tran-
sient, or did they persist throughout the first term? Our prediction
for the stable suppression component was fairly straightforward:
We expected it to consistently predict outcomes over time. The
prediction for the dynamic component of suppression was less
obvious, however: Would it represent a brief disruption in social
functioning, or would it set a longer lasting precedent? Block
(1982) proposed that an individual’s responses immediately after a
major transition have an enduring impact: Early responses set the
tone for newly formed relationships and establish enduring pat-
terns and precedents for social interactions. Thus, we tentatively
hypothesized that the suppression effects would not diminish in
magnitude throughout the term (i.e., the effects would persist and,
thus, not interact with week).
Method
Participants
Data were available for 278 students (58% female; Mage 18
years) who had completed a measure of suppression twice, once
during the summer prior to their arrival on campus and a second
time within 2 weeks of the start of fall term. Participation in the
summer survey was voluntary; subjects who completed further
assessments were compensated for their time. The sample was
ethnically diverse: 15% identified themselves as Hispanic, 31% as
Asian, 7% as African-American, 60% as Caucasian, and 4% as
Native-American (participants were allowed to mark more than
one category). Various subsets of this sample participated in the
two major parts of this longitudinal investigation, as described
below.
Measures
To facilitate interpretation, we converted scores of all other
continuous measures to Percent of Maximum Possible (POMP)
scores (P. Cohen, Cohen, Aiken, & West, 1999). POMP scores are
computed by a linear transformation that rescales a variable to
have a theoretical range from 0 to 100. Transformations to POMP
scoring put unstandardized statistics (like coefficients from regres-
sions or multilevel models) into a more interpretable metric, be-
cause all measures have the same raw scale. POMP transforma-
tions do not affect inferential statistics like tor Ftests. Means and
standard deviations of the main variables are reported in Table 1.
Suppression. Suppression was measured using the suppression
scale of the ERQ (Gross & John, 2003). The 4-item suppression
scale was designed to be brief but has shown good reliability,
consistent evidence of unifactorial structure, and convergent as
886 SRIVASTAVA, TAMIR, MCGONIGAL, JOHN, AND GROSS
well as discriminant validity, including substantial correlations
with peer-reported suppression (Gross & John, 2003; John &
Gross, 2004). The instructions made no reference to any time
frame (i.e., participants were not constrained to interpret items as
trait or state statements). Participants rated their agreement or
disagreement with each item on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree)
to7(strongly agree). The ERQ suppression items are as follows:
“I control my emotions by not expressing them,” “When I am
feeling positive emotions (e.g., joy, amusement), I make sure not
to express them,” “I keep my emotions to myself,” and “When I
am feeling negative emotions (e.g., anxiety, sadness), I make sure
not to express them.” In this sample, as in the previous studies, all
four suppression items had substantial loadings (.55) on a com-
mon factor, and this was true at both time-points.
2
The first
administration took place during the summer prior to partici-
pants’ freshman year of college; the second administration took
place within the first 2 weeks of fall term (see Figure 1).
Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficients were similar to previ-
ous research (.71 in the precollege assessment and .77 in the
on-campus assessment).
Social support from parents and from friends. To assess how
much social support participants received from others, participants
rated social support on a scale from 1 (no support)to7(great
support). Participants separately rated support from new college
friends and from parents (or guardians). Averaged across weeks,
support from these two sources correlated only .37, indicating that
these two sources of support were distinct.
To control for baseline levels of support, we used the 12-item
Interpersonal Support Evaluation List (S. Cohen & Hoberman,
1983), which was administered in the summer assessment. This
inventory measures the perceived availability of social support; it
includes items like, “When I need suggestions on how to deal with
a personal problem, I know someone I can turn to.” The scale had
a mean of 77.7 (POMP scored) and standard deviation of 15.7, and
it was internally consistent (␣⫽.87). Its correlation with average
weekly support from friends was r.35 ( p.05), with support
from parents r.08 (ns).
Closeness to others. Participants rated the closeness of their
relationships from two perspectives, as agent and as recipient of
closeness. One item asked about the degree to which participants
felt “affectionate, loving, caring/warm toward others,” and the
other item asked about the degree to which they felt “cared about,
loved, connected to others.” Ratings were made on a scale from 0
(not at all)to4(extremely). We combined these items into a
closeness composite (␣⫽.79).
To control for baseline levels of closeness, we used comparable
items from the summer assessment. Participants were asked to rate
the extent to which they typically experience affection and love on
a scale from 1 (not at all)to5(extremely). These two ratings were
POMP scored and combined to form a composite with M68.2
(SD 21.6, ␣⫽.68). The correlation with average weekly
closeness was r.39 ( p.05).
Social and academic satisfaction. Social satisfaction was as-
sessed with the item “How satisfied did you feel with your social
life?” Academic satisfaction was assessed using the item “How
satisfied did you feel with your academic life?” The items were
rated from 1 (not at all)to7(extremely). Mean weekly ratings of
academic and social support were correlated .41, indicating that
subjects made meaningful distinctions between the two domains.
To control for baseline levels of social satisfaction, we used the
item “I am satisfied with my social life,” which was administered
in the summer assessment. Participants rated this item on a scale
from 1 (strongly disagree)to5(strongly agree). The mean (after
POMP transformation) was 61.4 (SD 30.2). The item correlated
with average weekly social satisfaction r.45 ( p.05).
Additional control variables for Model 3. Social activity was
assessed using the item “Did you go to any parties or social
events? If so, how many?” Each week, participants responded with
one of four choices: 0, 1, 2, or 3(scored as 3). Positive and
negative emotions experienced during the week were assessed
with multi-item scales, with each emotion item rated on a scale
from 0 (not at all)to4(extremely). The positive emotion scale
consisted of 3 items: “happy, pleased, contented”; “proud, a sense
of accomplishment, successful”; and “interested, intellectually en-
gaged/stimulated” (␣⫽.61). The negative emotion scale consisted
of 4 items: “anxious, nervous”; “sad, depressed, down”; “tired,
fatigued”; and “angry, irritated, pissed off” (␣⫽.68).
2
Psychometric analyses in the current data and previous studies (e.g.,
Gross & John, 2003; John & Gross, 2004) indicate that the ERQ measures
a general suppression factor. However, to check against the possibility that
the findings might be driven by suppression of just positive or just negative
emotions, we reran the analyses in this article using the single items
referring explicitly to positive or negative emotions. The pattern of effects
with these more specific indicators was consistent with the reported results,
further supporting our interpretation of the present results as attributable to
a general suppression factor.
Table 1
Descriptive Statistics for Main Study Variables
Variable NMSD
Antecedent suppression (ERQ)
Summer before college 278 35.73 17.24
First 2 weeks of fall term 278 40.30 19.73
Weekly reports through first term
Support from parents 233 59.21 26.67
Support from new friends 233 57.92 22.06
Closeness 233 66.36 17.17
Social satisfaction 233 60.76 17.40
Academic satisfaction 233 54.64 15.63
End-of-term self-reports
Social support from friends 204 57.38 26.24
Closeness 204 66.75 28.36
Social satisfaction 204 63.43 23.26
Academic satisfaction 204 51.49 22.86
End-of-term peer reports
Closeness 143 74.57 17.89
Social satisfaction 143 68.82 16.67
Academic satisfaction 143 60.63 17.03
Likability 143 76.28 16.47
Note. The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; Gross & John,
2003) was completed during the summer before college and again during
the first 2 weeks of the first college term. Standard deviations for weekly
reports and peer reports are calculated to reflect between-subject variance.
887
SOCIAL COSTS OF EMOTIONAL SUPPRESSION
Participation and Attrition in the Weekly
Experience Reports
Weekly assessments were available for 233 (84%) of the 278
students who had completed both emotion-regulation measures.
On average, they completed six of the weekly reports. We com-
pared suppression scores in precollege and initial on-campus as-
sessments for the 233 who responded to at least one weekly
assessment versus the 45 who did not and found no differences (all
ps.28). The number of responses per week ranged from 90 (the
week that included Thanksgiving break, when many students went
home) to 183 (the last week of the term), and response rates did not
systematically increase or decrease over the course of the term.
Analyses
Operationalizing stable and dynamic components of suppres-
sion. The cross-context design allowed us to model two compo-
nents of suppression: a stable component representing variance shared
between the precollege and on-campus assessment and a dynamic
component representing variance unique to the on-campus arrival
assessment. To estimate the effects of stable and dynamic components
of emotion regulation, we entered baseline scores (assessed in the
summer prior to the transition) and change scores (the difference of
post-transition minus pretransition) simultaneously.
This analysis is a hybrid of static-score and change-score mod-
els for panel data (Finkel, 1995). Its logic can be connected to
Cronbach’s assertion about multiple influences on the “organism
at present.” Suppression measured at a single occasion (SUP
t
)
reflects two influences: variance shared across multiple time points
(STABLE) and variance that is specific to that time point (DY-
NAMIC
t
). Thus, two measurements reflect three sources of vari-
ance, STABLE, DYNAMIC
1
, and DYNAMIC
2
:
SUP1STABLE DYNAMIC1, (1)
SUP2STABLE DYNAMIC2, (2)
Because STABLE is the same at both time points, it is subtracted
out in the change score:
SUP SUP2SUP1, (3)
SUP STABLE DYNAMIC2STABLE
DYNAMIC1, (4)
SUP DYNAMIC2DYNAMIC1. (5)
This decomposition helps illustrate why entering baseline and
difference scores allowed us to estimate stable and dynamic ef-
fects. In our analyses, we entered the baseline score (SUP
1
) and the
difference score (SUP) simultaneously into a regression equation
as follows:
Yb0b1SUP1b2共⌬SUP. (6)
When Equations 1 and 4 are substituted, this equation is the same
as the following:
Yb0b1STABLE DYNAMIC1b2DYNAMIC2
DYNAMIC1. (7)
Because regression coefficients reflect the effect of each vari-
able while holding the other constant, the variance that is shared
across both terms in the regression—that is, DYNAMIC
1
, the
variance specific to Time 1— effectively “cancels out,” making b
1
the estimate of the effect of STABLE on the dependent variable
and b
2
the estimate of the effect of DYNAMIC
2
on the dependent
variable.
3
Multilevel regression models of weekly experience reports.
The weekly experience reports formed a nested data structure, with
up to 10 reports nested within each person. Therefore, we analyzed
the weekly experience reports using multilevel regression analyses
(also known as hierarchical linear models or linear mixed models)
with maximum-likelihood estimation. This approach allowed us to
use all available data, even from participants who did not complete
all 10 weekly reports. At Level 1 (within-person effects), the
outcome measure was modeled as a function of an intercept and a
linear slope of week. Week was centered in the middle of the fall
term, so that the intercept would represent “average” social func-
tioning during the fall term. The Level 1 covariance structure
included autoregressive effects; that is, error terms from adjacent
weeks could be correlated with each other. In the Level 2 equations
(between-person effects), we entered baseline and change scores of
suppression to estimate the effects of stable and dynamic suppres-
sion, as described above. Both Level 2 random effects (for the
intercept and the week slope) were estimated with an unrestricted
covariance structure.
The tests of stable and dynamic suppression built on this basic
model: Model 2 added Level 2 effects of the baseline social
functioning measures, and Model 3 further added effects of social
activity, positive affect, and negative affect at Level 1.
Results and Discussion
For illustrative purposes, means and standard deviations for core
variables are presented in Table 1, and zero-order correlations
among suppression and the outcome variables are presented in
Table 2. We note two observations about these correlations. First,
suppression measured at either of the antecedent time points was
correlated with all of the subsequent social outcome variables,
consistent with an effect of stable suppression. Second, for all but
one expected outcome (support from parents; see also below), the
correlation with the temporally closer fall assessment of suppres-
sion was stronger than the correlation with summer suppression, an
observation that is consistent with an effect of dynamic suppres-
sion. More rigorous, model-based tests of these hypotheses are
presented later in this section.
3
Even though the conceptual decomposition revolves around three
components (STABLE, DYNAMIC
1
, and DYNAMIC
2
), the analysis esti-
mates effects of only two of these components because of the cancellation.
When we ran the analyses using SUP
2
(instead of SUP
1
) and SUP as
predictors, the coefficients for SUP
2
(which, according to the decomposi-
tion, reflect the stable effect) were identical to the coefficients on SUP
1
in
the analyses reported. The coefficients for SUP then represented the
variance in suppression that was unique to Time 1 (i.e., DYNAMIC
1
). As
one would expect, these effects were not significant and were near zero.
888 SRIVASTAVA, TAMIR, MCGONIGAL, JOHN, AND GROSS
Consistency and Change in Suppression
Suppression showed moderate rank-order consistency between
the home environment and college (r.63, p.01). Although
significant, this correlation is far from unity, leaving substantial
room for individual-level changes across the initial transition pe-
riod. Therefore, we expected to be able to distinguish both stable
and dynamic components of suppression.
Did the participants, on average, increase in their use of sup-
pression across the transition? A ttest indicated that mean levels of
suppression increased significantly from the summer prior to col-
lege (M35.7) to the arrival on campus (M40.3), t(277)
4.36 ( p.01). In other words, as participants left their familiar
social networks and began exploring a novel social environment in
college, they increased the extent to which they regulated the overt
behavioral expression of their emotions.
Suppression and Social Experiences During the First
Term of College
Social support from parents. We begin with this social out-
come variable because we predicted divergent effects for the stable
and dynamic components of suppression, thus providing a direct
and strong test of our operationalization of the stable– dynamic
distinction. In the multilevel model, stable suppression signifi-
cantly predicted lower levels of social support from parents (i.e., a
lower Level 1 intercept, centered in the middle of the term) during
the first term in college (␥⫽– 0.28, p.05; see the first row in
Figure 2). That is, new college students who were stable suppres-
sors—whose high scores reflected consistently frequent use of
suppression in both their home environment and their new college
environment— elicited less subsequent support from their parents
than did low scorers. All of the variables were POMP scored (that
is, scaled from 0 to 100), so this coefficient indicated that an
increase of one point of stable suppression corresponded to a
reduction of just over one quarter of a point of parental support.
We expected that this effect would not hold for the dynamic
component of suppression, which reflects the student’s unique use
of suppression in the new college environment. That was what we
found: The dynamic suppression component was not related to
parental support, a source of support that preexisted the new
college environment (␥⫽– 0.03, ns; see Figure 2). This differen-
tial pattern of findings supports the validity of the stable– dynamic
distinction and the interpretation of the dynamic component as
variance that is time- and context-specific.
Social support from new college friends. In contrast with
parental support, we predicted that both stable and dynamic sup-
pression would predict less social support in the new relationships
students form at college, and this was indeed the case. Figure 2
shows the regression coefficients for stable and dynamic suppres-
sion for the three models across the top row. In Model 1, both
components of suppression had significant and negative associa-
tions with support from friends (␥⫽ 0.32 for stable suppression,
and ␥⫽ 0.25 for dynamic suppression; both ps.05). Because
the effects of the stable and dynamic components were assessed
simultaneously, this analysis indicates that each was a significant
predictor independent of the other.
In Model 2, we added a control for baseline levels of social
support prior to the transition. The effects of both stable and
dynamic suppression remained significant and negative in Model 2
(␥⫽ 0.23 for stable suppression, and ␥⫽– 0.23 for dynamic
suppression; both ps.05). This finding was consistent with a
directional interpretation that suppression was an antecedent of
low social support (the baseline control was also significant in this
analysis).
Finally, in Model 3, we added effects for social activity, positive
emotions, and negative emotions. In this model, the effects of both
Table 2
Zero-Order Correlations Among Antecedent Suppression and Subsequent Weekly Experience
Variable
Antecedent
suppression
(ERQ) Weekly experience reports
Summer
before
college
Early
fall
term
Support
from
parents
Support
from
friends Closeness
Social
satisfaction
Antecedent suppression (ERQ)
Summer before college
Early fall term .63
Mean of weekly experience
reports through first term
Support from parents .17 .07 —
Support from new friends .23 .33 .39
Closeness .23 .46 .25 .60
Social satisfaction .16 .39 .17 .51 .69
Academic satisfaction .10 .19 .21 .29 .29 .29
Note.N123. ERQ Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (Gross & John, 2003). To obtain reliable estimates,
we used only subjects with greater than a 50% response rate on the weekly reports to calculate these correlations;
cases with further missing data were deleted. In this subsample, absolute correlations greater than .18 are
significant at p.05. These correlations with partial data are for illustrative purposes only. The primary analyses
reported in the text used full-information methods on the fuller sample.
889
SOCIAL COSTS OF EMOTIONAL SUPPRESSION
Figure 2. Effects of antecedent stable and dynamic suppression on weekly experience report intercepts.
Dependent variables are listed along the vertical axis. Dots represent the coefficient estimates, and line segments
represent 95% confidence intervals; coefficients whose intervals do not cross zero are therefore significant at p
.05. Model 1 included the stable and dynamic suppression variables as well as the effect of week. Model 2
included all Model 1 effects plus a control for baseline social functioning. Model 3 included all Model 2 effects
plus controls for social activity and positive and negative emotion. Graphing technique was adapted from
Kastellac and Leoni (2007). Numerical values are available from Sanjay Srivastava upon request.
890 SRIVASTAVA, TAMIR, MCGONIGAL, JOHN, AND GROSS
suppression components remained significant (␥⫽– 0.21 for
stable suppression, and ␥⫽ 0.21 for dynamic suppression; both
ps.05). Thus, the effects of stable and dynamic suppression on
social support from friends were independent from each other and
could not be explained by differences in social activity or emo-
tional experience.
In a final set of control analyses, we reran the analysis of
emotional support from new friends, controlling for support from
parents. Again, the effects of both stable and dynamic suppression
remained significant and largely unchanged in magnitude (see
Figure 2). Like Model 2 (which controlled for pretransition social
support), this test further indicated that the findings for support
from friends are specific to the emerging social network at college
and cannot be attributed to global support patterns already in place
prior to the arrival on campus.
Closeness to others. As shown in Figure 2, both the stable
and the dynamic components of suppression were associated
with less closeness to others over the entire first academic term.
This was true even when we controlled for baseline closeness,
social activity, and positive and negative affect in Model 3,
none of which confounded or mediated the effects of stable and
dynamic suppression. The findings were consistent with previ-
ous experimental and correlational findings. Expanding on prior
studies, the present findings demonstrate that both stable and
dynamic suppression exert similar and independent influences
on relationship closeness, even when measured over an ex-
tended period of time.
Social and academic satisfaction. As shown in Figure 2, both
stable and dynamic suppression predicted lower social satisfaction
at college, even when we controlled for baseline levels of social
satisfaction, social activity, and positive and negative emotions
(i.e., in Model 3).
Suppression has been associated with lower life satisfaction in
general (see Gross & John, 2003). Indeed, in this investigation, the
stable component of suppression was associated with lower aca-
demic satisfaction. To test the specificity of the social satisfaction
findings, we conducted analyses of social satisfaction with aca-
demic satisfaction entered as a Level 1 control variable. The
results, summarized in Figure 2, were clear: Both effects of sup-
pression on social satisfaction remained significant and consistent
in magnitude, even after we controlled for academic satisfaction,
indicating that the effects were not merely a reflection of lower
global satisfaction.
Transience or persistence of outcomes: Interactions with week
in college. We expected the effects of stable suppression to be
evident throughout the term; we tentatively hypothesized that
dynamic suppression might show a similar pattern (as opposed to
predicting only a transient disruption in social functioning at the
start of the term). To examine these possibilities, we tested
whether the effects of stable and dynamic suppression in the above
analyses interacted with week. If the effects of suppression were
transient, we would expect a Suppression Week interaction such
that high versus low suppressors score quite differently on social
functioning indices in early weeks but score similarly to one
another in later weeks. However, none of the interactions were
significant: The statistical effects of suppression on social func-
tioning were of similar magnitude throughout the academic term.
Part 2: Social Functioning at the End of Term as
Assessed By Self- and Peer Reports
The experience-sampling approach employed in Part 1 asked
participants to report their ongoing experiences soon after they
occurred. In Part 2, we expanded our approach in three ways. First,
we asked participants at the end of their first term to provide global
summary reports of their lives since beginning college. Such
reports reflect the participants’ life assessments and evaluations
integrated over time, and they can therefore have important impli-
cations not captured by online reports, making them an important
and complementary source of information (Wirtz, Kruger, Scollon,
& Diener, 2003). Second, we supplemented self-reports with the
reports of peers who knew the participants well. Self-reports
provide a direct window into individuals’ perceptions of their
social world. Well-acquainted peers who have observed the indi-
vidual in a wide range of social settings provide an important
additional perspective on the individual’s social adjustment. Self-
and peer reports each provide valid but complementary informa-
tion (Vazire & Mehl, 2008). Third, the collection of peer reports
allowed us to assess another domain of social functioning, likabil-
ity, which is difficult to assess with self-reports.
Given that Part 1 showed suppression to be associated with less
social support, less closeness to others, and lower social satisfac-
tion, we expected to reproduce those patterns using summary
reports at the end of the term. We expected such patterns to be
evident in both self-reports and peer reports. In addition, we
examined likability, as rated by peers. Whereas social support,
closeness to others, and social satisfaction reflect direct relation-
ship outcomes, the extent to which a person is liked by others
reflects the individual’s effect on others. Individuals can elicit
positive reactions from others without necessarily forming close
interpersonal bonds. Prior research suggests that suppression is not
related to evaluative impressions (Gross & John, 2003), and al-
though we expected suppression to impair relationship outcomes,
we did not necessarily expect suppressors to be disliked by others.
Method
Participants and Attrition in the End-Quarter Assessments
The 278 participants from Part 1 were invited to complete an
end-quarter assessment. Because we anticipated competing time
demands on participants from final exams and other obligations,
we designed a very brief assessment with the goal of achieving a
respectable response rate. Two hundred four (73%) of the original
Part 1 participants agreed to participate and completed the self-
report section of the end-quarter assessment. To examine attrition
effects, we compared these end-quarter participants with the non-
participants on suppression at each of the two earlier assessments,
and we found no differences; the absolute correlations with par-
ticipation (vs. nonparticipation) were both below .05, and neither
was even close to significance ( ps.23).
To gain additional insight and corroboration of social conse-
quences, we also asked the participants who completed the end-
of-term self-reports to nominate up to three people who knew them
well and to provide names and addresses at which we could
contact these peers. We then mailed paper questionnaires with a
postage-paid return envelope to the peers. Peer nominations could
891
SOCIAL COSTS OF EMOTIONAL SUPPRESSION
be solicited from only those participants who agreed to participate
in the end-quarter self-report assessment; at least one peer report
was obtained for 143 of those participants (70%).
Measures
Means and standard deviations of core variables are reported in
Table 1.
Social support. Participants rated how often they went to
others to talk about personal problems and how often friends came
to them to talk about personal problems. We focused on self-
reports here because perceived support differs from actual support
received. The former is generally more strongly related to positive
outcomes (Wethington & Kessler, 1986), perhaps because re-
ceived support confounds the availability of a support network
with the adverse events that require its use. Ratings ranged from 1
(never)to4(often). We collapsed these items into a single com-
posite measure of (mutual) social support (␣⫽.73).
Closeness to others. Participants and their peers rated their
agreement with the statement, “I had close relationships with
others” (self) or “X has close relationships with others” (peer).
Ratings ranged from 1 (disagree)to7(agree).
Two aspects of life satisfaction in college: Social and academic.
Participants and peers rated “How satisfied were you with social
life at college?” (self-report) or “How satisfied does X seem with
his/her social life at college?” (peer report). Participants and peers
also rated academic satisfaction: “How satisfied were you with
your own academic performance at college?” (self) or “How
satisfied does X seem with his/her academic performance?” (peer).
As in the weekly reports, social and academic satisfaction were
moderately correlated (in self-reports, r.22, p.001; in peer
reports, r.38, p.001).
Likability. Peers rated the extent to which they agreed with
two statements concerning the target: “X is the kind of person
almost everyone likes” and “X is someone people really enjoy
spending time with.” These items required peers to evaluate the
participants’ social interactions more broadly, rather than reporting
solely on their own relationship with the participant. Both items
were rated from 1 (strongly disagree)to7(strongly agree). We
averaged the two items to create a single index of likability (␣⫽
.78).
Other controls. We used the same control variables as in Part
1. We averaged over the 10 weeks the weekly reports of social
activity, positive emotion, and negative emotion we had obtained
earlier to create overall person-level variables for these analyses.
Analyses
When self-reports and peer reports were available for the same
constructs, we treated the data as having a nested structure (one to
four reports nested within each participant). Therefore, when peer
reports were available, we ran multilevel models with multiple
reports (self plus 0 –3 peers) nested within each subject and with
the stable and dynamic suppression terms as Level 2 predictors.
We entered a contrast code for source (self vs. peer) as a Level 1
predictor to control for main effects of data source, and we
included terms for cross-level interactions between source and the
suppression variables to test whether the observed effects were
significantly different for the two data sources. Such combined
analyses benefit from aggregation, but are only appropriate if there
is measurement equivalence across data sources (i.e., self-reports
must be psychometrically comparable with peer reports). With
single items, measurement equivalence cannot be tested and must
be assumed. Therefore, we also report separate analyses using
regressions for the self-reports and multilevel models for the peer
reports. As in the previous section, we included in all analyses the
stable and dynamic terms entered simultaneously to test for their
independent contribution in predicting the outcomes.
Results and Discussion
For illustrative purposes, zero-order correlations among sup-
pression and the self- and peer-outcome variables are reported in
Table 3. We again note that suppression from both time points was
correlated with outcome variables, consistent with a stable sup-
pression effect, and that correlations of outcomes with fall sup-
pression were stronger than correlations with summer suppression,
consistent with a dynamic suppression effect. More rigorous tests
of these hypotheses follow in this section.
Social Support
As shown in the top row of Figure 3, both stable suppression and
dynamic suppression were significantly associated with lower lev-
els of self-reported social support in Model 1 (s⫽⫺0.35 and
0.33, respectively). The effect of stable suppression was reduced
after a control for baseline social support was introduced in Model
2 (stable suppression ␥⫽⫺0.21, p.07). After controls for
social activity and positive and negative emotions were introduced
in Model 3, the effect of stable suppression was not significant
(although the coefficient remained negative). However, the effect
of dynamic suppression was significant even in Model 3 with all
controls.
Closeness to Others
Consistent with the findings in Part 1, both stable suppression
and dynamic suppression had a negative impact on close relation-
ships at the end of the term. These effects remained significant in
Model 3 with all controls introduced (see the second row of Figure
3). There was a significant interaction with data source, indicating
that the effects of suppression were somewhat stronger in self-
reports, as compared with peer reports. When we examined the
effects for each data source separately, stable and dynamic sup-
pression had negative consequences for close relationships in both
self-reports and peer reports. Stable and dynamic suppression were
both significantly related to self-reported closeness even in Model
3. Dynamic suppression was marginally related to peer-rated
closeness after controls were introduced ( p.09 in Model 3).
Social Satisfaction
Consistent with the findings in Part 1, both stable and dynamic
suppression predicted lower social satisfaction at the end of the
term. In the combined analysis, stable and dynamic suppression
both had significant effects in Model 3 with all controls. Data
source did not interact with these effects, suggesting that overall
the effects for self-reports and peer reports were similar in mag-
nitude. Indeed, when examining data sources separately, we ob-
892 SRIVASTAVA, TAMIR, MCGONIGAL, JOHN, AND GROSS
served that the same basic pattern emerged in both self-reported
and peer-reported social satisfaction, although some effects were
no longer significant in these lower power analyses. When we
included self-reported academic satisfaction at the end of the term
as an additional control, the effects of both stable and dynamic
suppression on self-reported social satisfaction remained virtually
unchanged.
Likability
In prior research, suppression was not related to peer-rated
likability (Gross & John, 2003). Similarly, in the present study,
neither the stable nor the dynamic components of suppression were
related to peer-rated likability at the end of the first academic term.
In other words, although selves and peers both indicated that
suppressors had less social support and were less satisfied with
their social lives, suppressors were not necessarily disliked by
others. Indeed, likability may engage an overall evaluation of the
person as a social stimulus, rather than a specific judgment of the
person as an interaction partner. Suppression, in this respect, may
influence outcomes involving interpersonal relationships but may
be less directly relevant to the overall impression an individual
makes on others.
General Discussion
In this longitudinal investigation, suppression was predictive of
multiple adverse social outcomes following the transition to col-
lege. These findings held across three different domains of social
functioning (social support, closeness to others, and social satis-
faction) and three different assessment methods (weekly diaries,
end-of-term self-reports, and peer reports). It is important to note
that these effects were of similar direction and magnitude for both
stable individual differences in suppression and recent, dynami-
cally invoked changes in suppression.
Suppression: A Socially Important Emotion
Regulation Process
Suppression predicted several different indicators of social func-
tioning: social support, closeness, and social satisfaction. Although
this was not a randomized experiment, control analyses supported
a directional interpretation as well as isolation from several plau-
sible confounding variables, consistent with the conclusion that
suppression is an antecedent of poor social functioning in these
domains. Poorer social functioning was observed in self-reports
and peer reports 10 weeks after the transition to college, suggesting
that these outcomes may be relatively enduring. Consistent with
previous research (Gross & John, 2003), suppression was not
associated with likability: Although suppressors miss opportunities
to form close and meaningful relationships, they do not evoke
negative evaluations from others. The findings were corroborated
by peers, indicating that suppression alters behavior in ways that
are observable by others. In other words, suppression extends
beyond the individual into the social field.
Why was suppression associated with these adverse outcomes?
At the outset, we started with the general proposition that because
suppression targets a social-communicative channel of emotion, its
consequences ought to be prominent in the social domain. Our
measure reflected suppression of emotions in general, rather than
suppression of just positive or just negative emotions. Different
emotions can serve different social functions, but emotions also
have shared social functions, such as calling attention to what is
personally important and meaningful, communicating internal
states, etc. (Keltner & Haidt, 1999). Because the findings reported
here depend on a general suppression factor, they are probably
based on such shared mechanisms.
The present results are consistent with the proposition that
suppression has meaningful, diverse, and persistent social conse-
quences in an important real-world context. Drawing on these
results, we offer three possible mechanisms by which suppression
may disrupt social functioning. First, to the extent that suppression
Table 3
Zero-Order Correlations Among Suppression and the End-of-Term Self-Reports and Peer Reports
Variable
Antecedent
suppression (ERQ) End-of-term self-reports End-of-term peer reports
Summer before
college
Early fall
term
Social
support Closeness
Social
satisfaction
Academic
satisfaction Closeness
Social
satisfaction
Academic
satisfaction
End-of-term self-reports
Social support .10 .21 —
Closeness .17 .32 .44 —
Social satisfaction .13 .22 .30 .56
Academic satisfaction .06 .20 .04 .16 .13
Mean of end-of-term peer reports
Closeness .21 .29 .36 .36 .24 .08
Social satisfaction .17 .24 .04 .36 .41 .12 .48
Academic satisfaction .05 .17 .05 .13 .10 .54 .11 .33
Likability .06 .06 .25 .29 .19 .10 .64 .42 .00
Note. N 112. ERQ Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (Gross & John, 2003). To obtain reliable estimates, we used only subjects with at least 2 peer
respondents to calculate these correlations; cases with further missing data were deleted. In this subsample, absolute correlations greater than .19 are
significant at p.05. These correlations with partial data are for illustrative purposes only. The primary analyses reported in the text used full-information
methods on the fuller sample.
893
SOCIAL COSTS OF EMOTIONAL SUPPRESSION
Figure 3. Effects of antecedent stable and dynamic suppression on end-of-term self-reports and peer reports.
Dependent variables and data sources are listed along the vertical axis. Dots represent the coefficient estimates,
and line segments represent 95% confidence intervals; coefficients whose intervals do not cross zero are
therefore significant at p.05. Model 1 included the stable and dynamic suppression variables. Model 2
included all Model 1 effects plus a control for baseline social functioning. Model 3 included all Model 2 effects
plus controls for social activity and positive and negative emotion. Graphing technique was adapted from
Kastellac and Leoni (2007). Numerical values are available from Sanjay Srivastava upon request.
894 SRIVASTAVA, TAMIR, MCGONIGAL, JOHN, AND GROSS
is successful, it will dissociate an individual’s internal emotional
experience from the information available to that individual’s
social partners. A number of important social processes rely on
others knowing about an individual’s internal emotional states: For
example, displays of distress elicit sympathy from others (Eisen-
berg et al., 1989; Labott et al., 1991), and shared positive and
negative experiences can facilitate social bonding (Collins &
Miller, 1994; Kowalski, 1996). A second possibility, consistent
with the concept of emotional “leakage” (Ekman & Friesen, 1969),
is that individuals who attempt to suppress their expressive behav-
ior are only partially successful. If social partners correctly infer
that an individual is suppressing, they may perceive a suppressor
as being uninterested in intimacy or even inauthentic in a social
interaction. A third possibility involves the cognitive consequences
of suppression. Experimental studies have shown that suppression
imposes a cognitive load (Richards, Butler, & Gross, 2003; Rich-
ards & Gross, 2000). To the extent that certain relationship pro-
cesses demand cognitive resources like attention (Tickle-Degnan
& Rosenthal, 1990), individuals who are preoccupied with regu-
lating their emotions might have difficulty fully engaging and
responding to others in social interactions. The findings from this
study encourage the future exploration of these possible mecha-
nisms through designs that allow for more micro-analysis of be-
havior, such as lab studies of interactions or experience sampling.
Stable and Dynamic Suppression: Implications for
Understanding Regulatory Processes
In this study, suppression reflected both stable personal factors
and dynamic responses to the current situational context. These
findings suggest that neither a trait-like nor a situationist concep-
tion of suppression is sufficient on its own. As a practical matter
for researchers, the fact that we were able to meaningfully divide
variance in a questionnaire measure into stable and dynamic com-
ponents echoes warnings against too easily categorizing measures
and constructs as exclusively trait or state (Allen & Potkay, 1981;
Fleeson, 2004). When a researcher assesses emotion regulation at
a single point in time, it is likely that the observation reflects both
stable and dynamic factors, and this consideration must factor into
both research design and theorizing.
We have used the “dynamic” label in the accurate but somewhat
narrow sense of something characterized by change. Yet it is
suggestive of a broader set of ideas about regulatory processes, and
in particular dynamic systems, that can guide our interpretation of
the findings and provide additional theoretical context for the
findings of this study (Carver & Scheier, 1998). Theoretically, we
see emotion regulation as interacting dynamically with the envi-
ronment as the individual anticipates and responds to events (cf.
Hoeksma et al., 2004). Temperament and early learning form the
basis of stable tendencies but not in a reflexive or deterministic
way; responses to a given situation depend on the individual’s
perception and interpretation of the social context and the demands
that it brings.
The average long-term trend in adulthood is for suppression to
decrease (John & Gross, 2004); by contrast, we found that mean
levels of suppression increased across the transition. This likely
reflects the challenges of being in a new place, separated from
loved ones and surrounded by strangers. Variance in the dynamic
component of suppression may reflect differences in how the
transition was construed—as a traumatic separation by some and
as an exciting social opportunity by others. Because emotion-
regulation processes can be primed by subtle associations and cues
(Mauss, Cook, & Gross, 2007), such differences might be ex-
plained by individuals’ encoding systems that respond differently
to different contexts (Cervone, 1997; Fleeson, 2004; Funder, 2006;
Mischel & Shoda, 1998). An important topic for future studies will
be to further examine factors that lead to changes in suppression,
including both features of the environment that may affect all
individuals similarly and construal processes that may vary from
one individual to the next.
Did changes in suppression occasioned by the transition to
college endure? Although we did not reassess suppression at the
end of the fall term, indirect evidence suggests that changes in
suppression may have endured. Both the stable and dynamic
components of suppression had effects on social functioning that
were evident throughout the 10-week term and at the end-of-term
assessment, a pattern that suggests that dynamic changes in sup-
pression endured after the transition. When people go through
major transitions, they restructure their environments through se-
lective and evocative transactions that, in turn, reinforce the indi-
vidual differences that guided the restructuring (Block, 1982;
Caspi & Moffitt, 1993). This echoes a broader theme in dynamic
systems, that disruptions can shift a system to a new, enduring
state (Carver & Scheier, 1998). An individual who responds to the
college transition by becoming emotionally guarded in the first
few days at college will miss opportunities to make close friend-
ships; conversely, having fewer close friendships might afford
fewer opportunities to share feelings, which could create an envi-
ronmental feedback loop that serves to solidify and maintain the
individual’s initial tendency to suppress. In future studies, this
mutually reinforcing effect on suppression in social environments
could be more directly examined by tracking both suppression and
indicators of social functioning across many points in time.
Limitations and Future Directions
The present study represents our effort to integrate stable and
dynamic effects in the context of a major real-world life transition,
with a temporal scope covering several months. This design had a
number of advantages, but like any approach, it had limitations as
well.
One limitation was the temporal resolution and scope of the
design, especially with respect to suppression dynamics. Dynamics
can occur on many different timescales, often requiring different
designs to optimally study them. By analogy, a geologist interested
in the earth’s movement might take measurements on the order of
milliseconds (using a seismograph) to study earthquakes and on
the order of centuries (using the fossil record) to study continental
drift. In this study, we measured suppression 2 months apart to
capture change associated with a major life transition. However, it
is important to recognize that the underlying mechanisms and
consequences might have been different had we focused on a
scope of years (to investigate long-term development), hours (to
study mood-related fluctuations), or milliseconds (to study imme-
diate responses to specific stimuli). Major life transitions are a
worthy focus, but they are not the only worthy focus. Similarly, we
note that our interest in a major life transition led us to create a
before-and-after design with two measurements of suppression. A
895
SOCIAL COSTS OF EMOTIONAL SUPPRESSION
design with more measurement occasions would have allowed for
more complex data-analytic approaches, like trait-state models or
difference models, which would have enabled us to test whether
the stable and dynamic components of suppression are correlated
or interact with each other, or growth-mixture models, which
would have allowed us to search for subgroups of growth patterns
(such as stable-high suppressors, increasers, and decreasers).
Another limitation is that we focused on just one kind of
transition (to college) at just one university. The ways that we
believe suppression alters interpersonal interactions are quite gen-
eral; however, we acknowledge that other transitions might pro-
vide quite a different picture of the link between emotion regula-
tion and social functioning. The college transition is culturally
valued, it is encountered with a peer group, and it occurs for most
people at a developmental stage when social identity is still mal-
leable. Not all transitions share these features. In view of these
limitations, one important direction for future research is to apply
the present approach in other places and in the context of other
important life transitions, to help determine how suppression af-
fects social functioning across a variety of life domains. In addi-
tion, it will be important to examine periods of life that are not
marked by major transitions, to explore how the social conse-
quences of expressive suppression are manifested in everyday life.
References
Albright, L., Kenny, D. A., & Malloy, T. E. (1988). Consensus in person-
ality judgments at zero acquaintance. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 55, 387–395.
Allen, B. P., & Potkay, C. R. (1981). On the arbitrary distinction between
states and traits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41,
916 –928.
Arthur, N., & Hiebert, B. (1996). Coping with the transition to post-
secondary education. Canadian Journal of Counselling, 30, 93–103.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for
interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psycho-
logical Bulletin, 117, 497–529.
Block, J. (1982). Assimilation, accommodation, and the dynamics of
personality development. Child Development, 53, 281–295.
Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New
York: Wiley.
Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., Lalande, K., Westphal, M., & Coifman, K.
(2004). The importance of being flexible: The ability to enhance and
suppress emotional expression predicts long-term adjustment. Psycho-
logical Science, 157, 482– 487.
Brissette, I., Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (2002). The role of optimism
in social network development, coping, and psychological adjustment
during a life transition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
82, 102–111.
Butler, E. A., Egloff, B., Wilhelm, F. H., Smith, N. C., Erickson, E. A., &
Gross, J. J. (2003). The social consequences of expressive suppression.
Emotion, 3, 48 – 67.
Cantor, N., Norem, J. K., Niedenthal, P. M., Langston, C. A., & Brower,
A. M. (1987). Life tasks and cognitive strategies in a life transition.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1178 –1191.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior.
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (1993). When do individual differences matter?
A paradoxical theory of personality coherence. Psychological Inquiry, 4,
247–271.
Cervone, D. (1997). Social-cognitive mechanisms and personality coher-
ence: Self-knowledge, situational beliefs, and cross-situational coher-
ence in perceived self-efficacy. Psychological Science, 8, 43–50.
Christie, N. G., & Dinham, S. M. (1991). Institutional and external influ-
ences on social integration in the freshman year. Journal of Higher
Education, 62, 412– 436.
Cohen, P., Cohen, J., Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1999). The problem of
units and the circumstance for POMP. Multivariate Behavioral Re-
search, 34, 315–346.
Cohen, S., & Hoberman, H. (1983). Positive events and social supports as
buffers of life change stress. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 13,
99 –125.
Collins, N. L., & Miller, L. C. (1994). Self-disclosure and liking: A
meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 457– 475.
Compas, B. E., Wagner, B. M., Slavin, L. A., & Vannatta, K. (1986). A
prospective study of life events, social support, and psychological symp-
tomatology during the transition from high school to college. American
Journal of Community Psychology, 14, 241–257.
Cronbach, L. J. (1957). The two disciplines of scientific psychology.
American Psychologist, 12, 671– 684.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Schaller, M., & Miller, P. A. (1989). Sympa-
thy and personal distress: Development, gender differences, and inter-
relations of indexes. New Directions for Child Development, 44, 107–
126.
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). Nonverbal leakage and clues to
deception. Psychiatry, 32, 88 –105.
Finkel, S. E. (1995). Causal analysis with panel data. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Fleeson, W. (2004). Moving personality beyond the person-situation de-
bate: The challenge and the opportunity of within-person variability.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 83– 87.
Funder, D. C. (2006). Towards a resolution of the personality triad:
Persons, situations, and behaviors. Journal of Research in Personality,
40, 21–34.
Gall, T. L., Evans, D. R., & Bellerose, S. (2000). Transition to first-year
university: Patterns of change in adjustment across life domains. Journal
of Social and Clinical Psychology, 19, 544 –567.
Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integra-
tive review. Review of General Psychology, 2, 271–299.
Gross, J. J. (2001). Emotion regulation in adulthood: Timing is everything.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 214 –219.
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion
regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-
being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348 –362.
Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional suppression: Physiol-
ogy, self-report, and expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 64, 970 –986.
Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1997). Hiding feelings: The acute effects
of inhibiting negative and positive emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psy-
chology, 106, 95–103.
Harker, L. A., & Keltner, D. (2001). Expressions of positive emotion in
women’s college yearbook pictures and their relationship to personality
and life outcomes across adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 80, 112–124.
Hoeksma, J. B., Oosterlaan, J., & Schipper, E. M. (2004). Emotion regu-
lation and the dynamics of feelings: A conceptual and methodological
framework. Child Development, 75, 354 –360.
John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regu-
lation: Personality processes, individual differences, and lifespan devel-
opment. Journal of Personality, 72, 1301–1334.
John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Individual differences in emotion
regulation. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp.
351–372). New York: Guilford.
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: History,
measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John
(Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp.
102–138). New York: Guilford Press.
896 SRIVASTAVA, TAMIR, MCGONIGAL, JOHN, AND GROSS
Kastellac, J. P., & Leoni, E. L. (2007). Using graphs instead of tables in
political science. Perspectives on Politics, 5, 755–771.
Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (1999). Social functions of emotions at multiple
levels of analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 505–522.
Kowalski, R. M. (1996). Complaints and complaining: Functions, anteced-
ents, and consequences. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 179 –196.
Labott, S. M., Martin, R. B., Eason, P. S., & Berkey, E. Y. (1991). Social
reactions to the expression of emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 5,
397– 417.
Lin, N. (1986). Conceptualizing social support. In N. Lin, A. Dean, &
W. M. Ensel (Eds.), Social support, life events, and depression (pp.
17–30). New York: Academic Press.
Mauss, I. B., Cook, C. L., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Automatic emotion
regulation during anger provocation. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 43, 698 –711.
Mauss, I. B., Levenson, R. W., McCarter, L., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J.
(2005). The tie that binds? Coherence among emotion experience, be-
havior, and physiology. Emotion, 5, 175–190.
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1998). Reconciling processing dynamics and
personality dispositions. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 229 –258.
Myers, D. G., & Diener, E. (1995). Who is happy? Psychological Science,
6, 10 –19.
Newcomb, A. F., Bukowski, W. M., & Pattee, L. (1993). Children’s peer
relations: A meta-analytic review of popular, rejected, neglected, con-
troversial, and average sociometric status. Psychological Bulletin, 113,
99 –128.
Petruzzello, S. J., & Motl, R. W. (2006). Physical activity and mental
health in college students. In M. V. Landow (Ed.), College students:
Mental health and coping strategies (pp. 41–57). Hauppauge, NY: Nova
Science.
Rholes, W. S., & Simpson, J. A. (2004). Attachment theory and close
relationships. New York: Guilford Press.
Richards, J. M., Butler, E. A., & Gross, J. J. (2003). Emotion regulation in
romantic couples: The cognitive consequences of concealing feelings.
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 20, 599 – 620.
Richards, J. M., & Gross, J. J. (2000). Emotion regulation and memory:
The cognitive costs of keeping one’s cool. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 79, 410 – 424.
Shaver, P., Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1985). Aspects of a life
transition: Network changes, social skills and loneliness. In S. Duck &
D. Perlman (Eds.), The Sage series in personal relationships (Vol. 1, pp.
193–220). London: Sage.
Simpson, J. A., Gangestad, S. W., & Nations, C. (1996). Sociosexuality
and relationship initiation: An ethological perspective of nonverbal
behavior. In G. O. Fletcher & J. Fitness (Eds.), Knowledge structures in
close relationships (pp. 121–146). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Stepper, S., & Strack, F. (1993). Proprioceptive determinants of emotional
and nonemotional feelings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 64, 211–220.
Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating
conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feed-
back hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
768 –777.
Tickle-Degnan, L., & Rosenthal, R. (1990). The nature of rapport and its
nonverbal correlates. Psychological Inquiry, 1, 285–293.
Vazire, S., & Mehl, M. R. (2008). Knowing me, knowing you: The
accuracy and unique predictive validity of self and other ratings of daily
behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1202–1216.
Wethington, E., & Kessler, R. C. (1986). Perceived support, received
support, and adjustment to stressful life events. Journal of Health and
Social Behavior, 27, 78 – 89.
Wirtz, D., Kruger, J., Scollon, C. N., & Diener, E. (2003). What to do on
spring break? The role of predicted, on-line, and remembered experience
in future choice. Psychological Science, 14, 520 –524.
Received October 13, 2006
Revision received October 10, 2008
Accepted October 17, 2008
E-Mail Notification of Your Latest Issue Online!
Would you like to know when the next issue of your favorite APA journal will be available
online? This service is now available to you. Sign up at http://notify.apa.org/ and you will be
notified by e-mail when issues of interest to you become available!
897
SOCIAL COSTS OF EMOTIONAL SUPPRESSION
... The early stage of the first year is crucial for college students to transition from high school to university (Carragher & McGaughey, 2016;Shankland et al., 2010). Students undergo transformations in various aspects such as identity, learning methods, content, culture, and living environment (Hussey & Smith, 2010;Srivastava et al., 2009). A challenges of this transition phase, including personal (e.g., balancing work commitments with studies), socio-emotional (e.g., making new friends), and academic (e.g., completing assignments) aspects (Sanagavarapu et al., 2019). ...
... It has been argued that at this stage, students face multiple changes in their identity, learning methods, content, culture, and living environment. Therefore, their behaviors, habits, attitudes, and beliefs also need to be adjusted (Hussey & Smith, 2010;Srivastava et al., 2009). Some of these can be acquired through educational activities and learned in the classroom. ...
Article
Full-text available
The first year of college is critical for students’ growth and development. A better adaptation process helps students make a quick and successful transition from high school to college. Using a longitudinal tracking design to explore the dynamic complexity of the adaptation process, we conducted a tracking study of first-year students at a highly selective comprehensive research-oriented university in Beijing, China. The results showed that first-year students had high adaptation perceptions of college. Their adaptation remained stable in the short-term owing to the impact of orientation education, but declined significantly in the long term, falling into an adaptation “trap.” First-year students’ adaptation was not affected by demographic characteristics or family socioeconomic status variables other than sex. Students’ preparation behaviors only explained adaptation in the early stage. Positive orientation experiences and individual psychological traits had a significant positive effect on students’ adaptation and effectively counteracted the risk of decreasing adaptation. The study discussed why first-year students fall into the adaptation trap based on tacit knowledge perspective and suggested that identifying factors that influence first-year students’ adaptation may improve the relevance of educational interventions.
... Whereas, the effects of suppression on psychosocial outcomes are more contentious. Srivastava et al. (2009) suggested that suppression predicts lower social support, weaker social bonds, and lower social satisfaction. However, Ma et al. (2012) argued that suppression is more effective at down-regulating negative emotional experiences and physiological responses at the unconscious level, which may not be captured in self-report measures. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed to examine the relationship between pet attachment and social support, as well as the chain mediating role of emotion regulation and empathy in a sample of young adult cat owners. A total of 319 participants completed the Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS), Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ), The Chinese Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI-C) and The Perceived Social Support Scale (PSSS). The results indicated that (1) Pet attachment, reappraisal in emotion regulation, empathy and social support are positively correlated with one another; (2) Pet attachment not only has a direct effect on social support among young adults, but also influences it indirectly through three pathways: the mediating role of reappraisal, the mediating role of empathy, and a chain mediating role involving both reappraisal and empathy. These findings provide a theoretical foundation for the common phenomenon of young adults keeping pet cats, reveal the psychological mechanism by which pet attachment influences social support, extend attachment theory, and enrich research on pet attachment and social support.
... This study's strongest correlation was between suppression and social support, which could imply moderation. Anderson et al. (2000) state that a failure to manage emotions can hinder relationships across the transition, and Srivastava et al. (2009) reported that suppression negatively impacts social support during transitions. Pauw et al. (2022) found that emotional suppression use can decrease when social support is perceived as high, depending on the contextual variability of social support. ...
Article
This study profiled the association between social–emotional competencies, psychological well‐being (PWB), and secondary/middle school transition. Analysis drew from 233 sixth‐class/sixth‐grade students aged 11–13 years who completed measures of emotion regulation, perceived social support, self‐esteem, and PWB at baseline and 1‐month follow‐up in primary school, and at 6‐month follow‐up post‐secondary school transition. COVID‐19 school closures, school socioeconomic status and gender were examined as moderators. Repeated‐measures multi‐level models revealed a significant decline in boys' emotional suppression use, an increase in boys' self‐esteem, and a decline in girls' PWB across the transition. Further, perceived social support, self‐esteem, and gender were significant predictors of post‐transition PWB while controlling for baseline PWB. This highlights the importance of enhancing social support and self‐esteem across secondary school transition and considering gender differences in school transition effects. Policymakers should consider interventions that bolster these factors during this critical developmental phase.
... However, this explanation seems less probable given research indicating the reverse relationship. Specifically, Dyson and Renk (2006) reported higher levels of depression, anxiety, and stress among first-year postsecondary students, presumably due to the difficulties associated with transitioning from high school to postsecondary studies and from adolescence to adulthood (Srivastava et al., 2009). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study examined the mental health of postsecondary students who reported sexual violence and explored if mental health effects of depression, anxiety, stress as well as posttraumatic stress were related to the types and forms of sexual violence experienced. A sample of 924 culturally diverse female-identifying students in Ontario, Canada answered questionnaires about sexual violence experiences, mental health, and educational outcomes. The results of the study revealed that students reporting any form of sexual violence had higher scores for depression, anxiety, stress, and posttraumatic stress than students not reporting sexual violence. This study also found that the type of sexual violence experienced and the methods or tactics used were differentially associated with mental health symptoms. The results of this study highlight the importance of acknowledging the unique roles that types of sexual violence – particularly non-physical methods of coercion – have on the mental health of female postsecondary students.
... Our results suggest that the tendency to engage in expressive suppression combined with IPV victimization may also be associated with substance use across a variety of substances (not limited to alcohol use). Additionally, because expressive suppression is associated with other social consequences, like lower social support (Srivastava et al., 2009), those who engage in habitual expressive suppression may have difficulty seeking support and, instead, engage in substance use behavior. We did not find evidence that expressive suppression moderated the association between IPV victimization and general health or sickness behavior, however, potentially pointing to expressive suppression as particularly impactful for health behaviors of IPV victims instead of sickness behaviors or perceived health status. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Intimate partner violence (IPV) is related to many negative health outcomes for victims. Our aim was to determine whether two types of emotion regulation strategies—cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression—moderate the effect of IPV victimization on health over time. Method: We recruited 1,200 participants to complete an initial survey during the beginning of the COVID-19 pandemic that assessed IPV victimization, habitual emotion regulation strategy use, and three indexes of health (i.e., general health, sickness behavior, and substance use). These participants then completed a follow-up survey 10 months later to reassess their health. We evaluated how habitual cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression moderated the effect of IPV victimization on each of the measures of health at follow-up, controlling for the initial respective measure of health. Results: Accounting for initial health, IPV victimization at the beginning of the pandemic was associated with worse general health, more sickness behavior, and greater substance use 10 months later. At higher levels of habitual expressive suppression, IPV victimization was associated with greater substance use. Unexpectedly, at higher levels of habitual cognitive reappraisal, IPV victimization was associated with worse general health and greater substance use. Conclusions: Our work demonstrates the long-term health effects of IPV victimization and that both expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal can exacerbate these effects on general health and substance use. Findings highlight a specific context in which cognitive reappraisal may not be a health-protective emotion regulation strategy. This should be considered in practical and therapeutic settings that serve individuals who have experienced IPV.
... An example is a person holding back one's tears in front of colleagues when receiving bad news. Reducing emotional expression, however, does typically not reduce the actual emotional experience (Sorić et al., 2013), but is cognitively costly and has been associated with disadvantageous outcomes in daily life (e.g., Balzarotti et al., 2010;Jin-Kyoung et al., 2014;John & Gross, 2004;Srivastava et al., 2009). Therefore, suppression is generally considered a maladaptive emotion regulation strategy. ...
Article
Full-text available
As emotions may hinder goal pursuit, regulating them is important. Although the relationship between self-control and emotion regulation has mainly been investigated on a dispositional level, the engagement in self-control and emotion regulation fluctuates across situations. Research on how and to what extent self-control and emotion regulation impact one another within individuals over time is scarce. In the present study, we investigated the within-person reciprocal relationships between momentary self-control and the emotion regulation strategies reappraisal and suppression. Ecological momentary assessment was employed to collect diary data five times a day over four days using repeated, short measures of emotions, self-control, reappraisal, and suppression. Using Dynamic Structural Equation Modeling (DSEM) of data from 1874 assessments provided by 111 participants, results showed that the relationship between self-control and the emotion regulation strategies reappraisal and suppression is reciprocal within persons over time. Momentary suppression related to lower levels of momentary self-control later on, and self-control, in turn, was positively associated with the employment of subsequent reappraisal. Findings are discussed in light of the importance of self-control in dealing with emotions that impede goal pursuit, and the contextual importance of this relationship. Overall, this study adds to the question how emotion regulation and self-control are intertwined on a within-person level and provides insight into how people could deal with their emotional experiences to achieve their long-term goals.
Article
Suicide is a major cause of death among adolescents, with suicidal ideation (SI) being a common symptom in this group. SI arises from a complex mix of biological, environmental, and psychological factors, however, the specific relationships between them is not yet fully understood. Network theory has been proposed as a promising framework to analyze these relationships, with latent network models (LNM) offering a novel approach to capture their complex underlying dynamics. We examined a SI-based LNM in a sample of 1539 students from secondary public schools (M = 15.336; SD = 1.022; female = 52.39 %). The model included depressive and anxiety symptoms, feelings of hopelessness, emotion regulation strategies, and cognitive-behavioral and problem-solving skills. Strength and expected influence indices were calculated for each variable. Hopelessness and depressive symptoms showed the highest strength and expected influence values within the model, respectively. Our findings suggest that hopelessness might play a crucial mediating role linking common mental disorders and emotion regulation strategies with SI in adolescents. Expressive suppression had a direct and negative association with SI, showing its underlying regulatory role when other factors are controlled. Cognitive-behavioral and problem-solving skills showed weak links with SI. Primary care- and school-based interventions should center on hopelessness as a relevant direct predictor for SI, and potential mediator in the course of SI. A combination of research and intervention efforts directed at reducing hopelessness in youths may prove to be essential for reducing suicide-related behaviors altogether.
Article
Full-text available
Emotion regulation plays a central role in mental health and illness, but little is known about even the most basic forms of emotion regulation. To examine the acute effects of inhibiting negative and positive emotion, we asked 180 female participants to watch sad, neutral, and amusing films under 1 of 2 conditions. Suppression participants (N = 90) inhibited their expressive behavior while watching the films; no suppression participants (N = 90) simply watched the films. Suppression diminished expressive behavior in all 3 films and decreased amusement self-reports in sad and amusing films. Physiologically, suppression had no effect in the neutral film, but clear effects in both negative and positive emotional films, including increased sympathetic activation of the cardiovascular system. On the basis of these findings, we suggest several ways emotional inhibition may influence psychological functioning.
Article
Full-text available
This article reports 2 experiments that test whether both emotional and nonemotional feelings may be influenced by uninterpreted proprioceptive input. The logic of the procedure was adopted from studies by F. Strack, L. Martin, and S. Stepper (1988), who unobtrusively manipulated people's facial expressions. In the 1st experiment, a functionally equivalent technique was used to vary the posture of the body. Study 1 results revealed that success at an achievement task led to greater feelings of pride if the outcome was received in an upright position rather than in a slumped posture. Study 2 results revealed that nonemotional feelings of effort were influenced by contraction of the forehead muscle (corrugator), and Ss' self-ratings on a trait dimension reflected this experience when the facial contraction was maintained during the recall of behavioral episodes exemplifying this trait. To account for these results, a framework is proposed that draws on a distinction between noetic and experiential representations.
Article
Even a casual observer of human behavior can see that there are profound differences in how individuals regulate their actions. Some individuals doggedly pursue a single goal or ideal for many years, making many personal sacrifices and at great personal cost. Others seem to give in to their immediate impulses with barely a thought for the consequences. Some students earn their highest grades under severe stress and in the face of adversity. The same levels of stress and adversity may lead other students to drop out and abandon their academic goals altogether. Indeed, many students seem to perform best under more relaxed conditions. At the workplace, some employees demonstrate high levels of initiative and set their own agenda, regardless of what others may think. Others prefer to follow the instructions of their superiors, and are eager to learn what is expected of them. These and other individual differences in self-regulation are the central focus of the present chapter. The following sections offer some preliminary reflections on the neglect of individual differences in psychological research. Next, the chapter considers individual differences in motives and needs, and how global notions of self-regulation and the will can be decomposed into more specific psychological functions and mechanisms. Finally, this chapter shows how this functional analysis of the will can be used to understand a wide array of effects of individual differences in affect regulation (i.e., action vs. state orientation).