ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Purpose – If the ideal self is the emotional driver of intentional change, the purpose of this paper is to explore the components of a person's personal vision and how it comes from their ideal self. Design/methodology/approach – Based on the concept of the ideal self from intentional change theory, the paper examines a variety of theoretical foundations, from psychoanalytic to positive psychology. Each views the ideal self and its components as deficiencies needing therapeutic intervention or the heights of human experience and intrinsic motivation. Findings – The ideal self is a primary source of positive affect and psychophysiological arousal helping provide the drive for intentional change. Many current frameworks or theories examine only portions of this model and, therefore, leave major components unaddressed. The ideal self is composed of three major components: an image of a desired future; hope (and its constituents, self‐efficacy and optimism); and a comprehensive sense of one's core identity (past strengths, traits, and other enduring dispositions). Originality/value – Intentional change is hard work and often fails because of lack of sufficient drive and the proper intrinsic motivation for it. This model of the ideal self creates a comprehensive context within which a person (or at other fractals, a group or system) can formulate why they want to adapt, evolve, or maintain their current desired state.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Journal of Management Development
The ideal self as the driver of intentional change
Richard E. Boyatzis Kleio Akrivou
Article information:
To cite this document:
Richard E. Boyatzis Kleio Akrivou, (2006),"The ideal self as the driver of intentional change", Journal of
Management Development, Vol. 25 Iss 7 pp. 624 - 642
Permanent link to this document:
http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/02621710610678454
Downloaded on: 30 October 2015, At: 18:41 (PT)
References: this document contains references to 115 other documents.
To copy this document: permissions@emeraldinsight.com
The fulltext of this document has been downloaded 13536 times since 2006*
Users who downloaded this article also downloaded:
Richard E. Boyatzis, (2006),"An overview of intentional change from a complexity perspective", Journal of
Management Development, Vol. 25 Iss 7 pp. 607-623 http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/02621710610678445
Ellen B. Van Oosten, (2006),"Intentional change theory at the organizational level: a case study", Journal of
Management Development, Vol. 25 Iss 7 pp. 707-717 http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/02621710610678508
Anita Howard, (2006),"Positive and negative emotional attractors and intentional change", Journal of
Management Development, Vol. 25 Iss 7 pp. 657-670 http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/02621710610678472
Access to this document was granted through an Emerald subscription provided by emerald-srm:569802 []
For Authors
If you would like to write for this, or any other Emerald publication, then please use our Emerald for
Authors service information about how to choose which publication to write for and submission guidelines
are available for all. Please visit www.emeraldinsight.com/authors for more information.
About Emerald www.emeraldinsight.com
Emerald is a global publisher linking research and practice to the benefit of society. The company
manages a portfolio of more than 290 journals and over 2,350 books and book series volumes, as well as
providing an extensive range of online products and additional customer resources and services.
Emerald is both COUNTER 4 and TRANSFER compliant. The organization is a partner of the Committee
on Publication Ethics (COPE) and also works with Portico and the LOCKSS initiative for digital archive
preservation.
*Related content and download information correct at time of download.
Downloaded by Technická univerzita vo Zvolene At 18:41 30 October 2015 (PT)
The ideal self as the driver of
intentional change
Richard E. Boyatzis
Weatherhead School of Management, Case Western Reserve University,
Cleveland, Ohio, USA, and
Kleio Akrivou
Department of Organizational Behavior, Case Western Reserve University,
Cleveland, Ohio, USA
Abstract
Purpose If the ideal self is the emotional driver of intentional change, the purpose of this paper is to
explore the components of a person’s personal vision and how it comes from their ideal self.
Design/methodology/approach Based on the concept of the ideal self from intentional change
theory, the paper examines a variety of theoretical foundations, from psychoanalytic to positive
psychology. Each views the ideal self and its components as deficiencies needing therapeutic
intervention or the heights of human experience and intrinsic motivation.
Findings The ideal self is a primary source of positive affect and psychophysiological arousal
helping provide the drive for intentional change. Many current frameworks or theories examine only
portions of this model and, therefore, leave major components unaddressed. The ideal self is composed
of three major components: an image of a desired future; hope (and its constituents, self-efficacy and
optimism); and a comprehensive sense of one’s core identity (past strengths, traits, and other enduring
dispositions).
Originality/value Intentional change is hard work and often fails because of lack of sufficient
drive and the proper intrinsic motivation for it. This model of the ideal self creates a comprehensive
context within which a person (or at other fractals, a group or system) can formulate why they want to
adapt, evolve, or maintain their current desired state.
Keywords Self development, Change management, Individual psychology, Leadership development
Paper type Conceptual paper
Much has been written about the importance of our dreams or aspirations in
motivating change or development (Oettingen, 1996; Snyder, 2000a, b; McClelland,
1985; Lewin and Dembo, 1947). Some of this comes from the goal setting and goal
orientation literature (Locke and Latham, 1990; Van Der Walle et al., 2001), and
reaching as far back as Lewin and Dembo’s (1947) conceptualization of levels of
aspiration as contrasted to a person’s level of activation. In recent years, a person’s
“vision” and visualization of desired behavior (Taylor et al., 1998) have been described
as an element in sports performance (Snyder et al., 2002), academic performance (Curry
et al., 1997), psychotherapy (Schecter, 1974), and helping recovery from illness and
surgery (Moyers, 1993; Matthews et al., 2004). Related psychological concepts, such as
hope, efficacy, optimism and positive expectations, have helped to elaborate selected
processes by which the person may look to the future, and a hopeful and/or positive
future, and drive the popularity of positive psychology (Seligman and
Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). Yet, little theoretical work has been done to integrate these
ideas or research.
The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available at
www.emeraldinsight.com/0262-1711.htm
JMD
25,7
624
Journal of Management Development
Vol. 25 No. 7, 2006
pp. 624-642
q Emerald Group Publishing Limited
0262-1711
DOI 10.1108/02621710610678454
Downloaded by Technická univerzita vo Zvolene At 18:41 30 October 2015 (PT)
In this article, we offer a theoretical model of the ideal self. It is proposed that the
ideal self is the driver of intentional change in one’s behaviour, emotions, perceptions,
and attitudes. The ideal self is the first discovery of intentional change theory (i.e. ICT)
as described in the first article in this special issue (Boyatzis, earlier in this issue).
Because of misguided and incomplete models of how to stimulate desired, intentional
change, the ideal self is perhaps the least understood of all of the components of ICT.
The idea l self
The ideal self is a psychological component of the self (Baumeister, 1998, a, b; Higgins,
1989a) partially conscious and partially unconscious, varying from individual to
individual. It is both privately conceptualised and socially influenced (in Nasby, 1997;
Schecter, 1974). The traditional psychoanalytic therapeutic model sees idealisation as a
defensive function of the self and thus in need of therapeutic intervention (in Schecter,
1974). Within the perspective of positive psychology, the ideal self (IS) is not considered
a defensive function; it is the core mechanism for self-regulation and intrinsic
motivation. It is manifest as a personal vision, or an image of what kind of person one
wishes to be, what the person hopes to accomplish in life and work.
Although the capacity for cognitive-affective ideal self formation “is more strongly
rooted in some personalities than in others” (Schecter, 1974), the ideal self (IS) is an
evolving, motivational core within the self, focusing a person’s desires and hope,
aspirations and dreams, purpose and calling. Discrepancies or congruence between the
actual (i.e. real self) and the person’s ideal self result in unique emotional and
behavioral consequences (Boldero and Francis, 1999). The ideal self serves a
mechanism linked to self-regulation; it helps to organize the will to change and direct it,
with positive affect from within the person. Deep positive affect creates an affective
tone of the specific cognitive processes that take place in the formulation and
Nourishment of the ideal self. The result harnesses the will or drive for self direction,
intentional change, and desired future accomplishments, or in selected cases providing
the energy to maintain and sustain current ideal states in life and work.
In the model of the ideal self proposed, emotion, and more specifically positive
emotion, is seen to have a core role. Positive affect is defined as “a state of high energy,
full concentration, and pleasurable engagement” (Watson and Tellegen, 1985).
Although both cognitive and emotional processes are required for the person to
activate and articulate an ideal self, it is trait based positive emotion which becomes
the driver and the substance of the ideal self overall. Positive affect improves the
thoroughness, efficiency, and flexibility of complex decision making and influences
one’s sense of standards to evaluate your progress against a set of standards. Also, it
facilitates the quality and quantity of pathways of thought and seems to boost an
aspect of executive function, which is the ability to adjust efficiently to new
information and undertake new problem solving efforts in congruence with the new
information (Ashby et al., 1999; Aspinwall and Leaf, 2002).
We propose that once the force of the ideal self is activated, it plays an executive or
motivational function within the self. It monitors and guides all actions and decisions
in a direction which ensures deeper self-satisfaction through the articulation and
direction towards either: the emergence of a new state of being with self actualization
as a core quality evident in either an internal sense of the self in action, such as
knowing you are acting with character and consistent with one’s values, or as evident
The ideal self
625
Downloaded by Technická univerzita vo Zvolene At 18:41 30 October 2015 (PT)
to others through one’s accomplishments; or the maintenance of a current character (i.e.
way of being) or state or condition in life or work, with increased clarity and
mindfulness. The latter requires effort, intentional effort, to sustain the balances
achieved, so it must be a result of focused effort to alter the likely forces of dissonance
and entropy.
Like the concept of “approach motivation” of the 1960s and 1970s in personality
psychology (McClelland, 1985), it is believed the ideal self will show opposite effects
than fear or “avoidance motives” (Boyatzis, 1973; McAdams, 1980; Kelner, 1990). It will
show a longer lasting effect, but has a slower, more complex start up rate than fear or
“avoidance motives”.
The role of fear and avoidance motives are to arouse emotional and cognitive
processes within the person that have the opposite effect of the ideal self, as mentioned
in the Boyatzis article earlier in this special issue and explained in more detail in the
Dyck and Howard articles later in the issue. Arousal of a fear stimulates neural
circuitry starting in the amygdala and emanating with dominant activity in the right
versus the left prefrontal cortex. At the same time, it promotes activation of the
sympathetic nervous system, creating a set of neural and endocrine processes that
stimulate negative or defensive emotions, resulting in a likely shift in perceptions of the
environment as more threatening (or merely anticipating that future events will be
more threatening). This results in defensive or hostile actions that typically result in a
person’s withdrawing or inhibiting new thoughts and alternative ways to approach a
situation.
Instead of moving forward, toward a desired future or condition, the person moves
away from and protects himself/herself from threatening aspects of the present or
future. In this manner, arousal of the ideal self engages the positive emotional attractor
(Boyatzis, article earlier in this issue) and its impact on intentional change. Arousal of
fear or avoidance motives engage the negative emotional attractor with its impact on
the person defending himself/herself or being forced to contemplate adaptation not
previously considered (Boyatzis article earlier in this issue).
The ideal self activates the person’s “will,” and by association the possibility of
increased self-monitoring, especially in terms of progress toward or behaviour
consistent with the purpose reflected in the activation of the person’s will. This is the
teleos, or the expression of the person’s will; in James’ terms it is the person’s
“conscious volition” (James, 1897). As he suggested, once activated, the teleological
effect of the will provides the possibility of increased self-monitoring in terms of the
decisions and choices. This can be translated into decisions to sacrifice certain
immediate rewards for the sake of accomplishment of more important and often
longer-term goals.
Components of the ideal self
The overall model offered of the ideal self is shown in Figure 1. As can be seen, we
propose that there are three major components converging into the articulation of the
person’s ideal self, and the resulting personal vision.
The ideal self contains imagery of a desired future (a novel one, or one existing over
time, or one continually forming and revisited). This image is the articulation or
realization of the person’s dreams, aspirations, and fantasies. It is of cognitive nature
yet, fuelled by the affect resulting from one’s passion, dreams, and values. Specifically,
JMD
25,7
626
Downloaded by Technická univerzita vo Zvolene At 18:41 30 October 2015 (PT)
we believe the person’s dreams of the desired future/state are a function of his/her:
sense of calling or purpose in life; driven by their passion, values and operating
philosophy; and stage in life or one’s career.
Second, the ideal self is emotionally fuelled by hope. Although the psychological
processes related to hope are still under research, most researchers agree that hope is
caused by the degree of the person’s optimism. Also, it is the expression of their degree
of self-efficacy. Self-efficacy determines their perceptions of possibilities to
differentiate this component from “pie in the sky” or false hope (Groopman, 2004).
Some of the most prominent current literature on hope sees the concept as mainly
cognitive in nature (Snyder, 2000c). Yet we believe that hope is an experienced state
and, therefore, may be more accurately portrayed as an emotional state. This view is
supported by Aspinwall and Leaf (2002) and by Skinner (1996). Additionally, the hope
component of the ideal self model is defined by one’s ability to generate cognitive
processes that assess and judge the feasibility of that which is hoped. Even here,
though, the cognitions are less judgemental and more affective.
The third component of the ideal self is the person’s core identity. This is relatively
stable, and likely unconscious set of enduring individual characteristics, like his/her
unconscious motives and traits, as well as roles adopted consistently in social settings.
In this manner, the core identity is the personal context within which underlies the
historical and continuing aspects of a person’s ideal self and one’s deeply seated
autobiographical themes that make a vision coherent and intense.
A major confusion about the ideal self comes from the “ought self.” The ought self
as a concept is used in various labels in the literature (Baumeister, 1998; Higgins,
1989a; Markus and Nurius, 1986). It explains a version of the ideal self imposed by
others, or by a person’s internal desire to please others (Boyatzis, 1973). Reference
groups or social identity groups affect the individual by anticipatory socialization or
value induction. Groups that you wish to be a part of or identify with and feel that you
Figure 1.
Components of the
ideal self
The ideal self
627
Downloaded by Technická univerzita vo Zvolene At 18:41 30 October 2015 (PT)
belong to become sources of either a person’s ideal self or your ought self. Parents,
teachers, respected or feared authority figures, or those with whom you wish to be
admired, respected, or loved become sources of one’s ideal self or ought self.
The dilemma is that it is often confusing, in the moment, when these forces or social
pressures for role conformity are occurring. Are they things you really wish to be or
accomplish, or are you compromising your deeper dreams and values to be considered
a “good” member of a group? Quite simply, the ought self is someone else’s version of
what they think your ideal self should be. To the extent this becomes intentionally
integrated into a person’s ideal self, there appears to be no conflict among the various
selves. But if they are somewhat different and a person works toward the ought self, at
some point in the future, they will awake and feel betrayed, frustrated, and even angry
at the time and energy they wasted in pursuit of dreams and expectations that they
were never passionate about.
Ideal self leads to a personal vision
As described in the first article in this special issue, by Boyatzis, discovery, or more
accurately conscious realization of one’s ideal self may appear as a surprise or an
epiphany. This emergence of a new insight or awareness is a discontinuous break with
prior consciousness about one’s aspirations or future. In complexity theory terms, it is
a phase change. It is a small adjustment to a person’s awareness of their desires that
has a huge impact on their perceptions and choices. The ideal self provokes a phase
change in the person’s change or adaptation process. In this way, it provokes or
invokes intentional change.
To have this impact on the person’s behaviour, feelings, and perceptions, the
articulation of the ideal self can be a strong personal vision. This engages the positive
emotional attractor, which in turn enables an assessment of a person’s capability as it
may help or hinder movement toward the ideal self. We call this the personal balance
sheet (see the Taylor article and the Dyck and Howard articles in this issue). This
promotes the development of a person’s learning agenda and then a more articulated
learning plan, experimentation and practice with new behaviour, feelings, and
perceptions, and the eventual desired changes in either the person’s actual behaviour
(their real self) or their aspirations and dreams of the future (their ideal self). As shown
in the ICT, each of these discoveries is facilitated by the observations, interpretation,
feedback, and encouragement of others with whom the person has a trusting
relationship.
Hope: the affective driver
A major determinant of the ideal self is hope. Scholarly work on hope place its
antecedents in the motivational and cognitive literature of 1960s to 1980s, stressing the
desire to seek goals and the importance of cognition and the architecture of human
thought processes (Anderson, 1983; Ashby et al., 1999). Goals are seen to be the
cognitive component that is at the core of recent hope research. Accordingly, hope has
been conceptualised as a cognitive set which is built on the importance of goals
(Snyder, 2000c, 1998, 1996, 1994, 1991; Snyder, Ilardi, Michael and Cheavens, 2000;
Snyder et al., 1997; Lee et al., 1989; Pervin, 1989). Hope has often been seen as a
unidimensional construct related to a general perception that goals can and will be met
(Cantril, 1967; Erickson et al., 1975; Farber, 1968; Frank, 1973, 1968; French, 1952;
JMD
25,7
628
Downloaded by Technická univerzita vo Zvolene At 18:41 30 October 2015 (PT)
Gottschalk, 1974; Lewin, 1938; Menninger, 1959; Melges and Bowlby, 1969; Mowrer,
1960; Stotland, 1969).
Ludema (1996) traced the roots of the concept of hope. He reported in his review on
the concept of hope in Western tradition, that in Greco-Roman times, there was an
ambivalence about hope. It was both a human projection of desire, with human failings
and limitations. Judeo-Christian tradition saw hope as a divine gift with practical
implications here on Earth. But it was the Christian theologians of the Middle Ages
that brought hope into an ethereal level. Augustine called hope a basic human virtue
and a path to God. Meanwhile, Aquinas claimed hope gave direction toward action.
From in-depth interviews, Ludema (1996) concluded that hope has four enduring
qualities:
(1) hope brought people together and built relationships;
(2) hope assumes an openness to the future and imagination;
(3) hope is an “ultimate concern” of human nature; and
(4) hope feeds creativity.
Snyder referred to the myth of Pandora box in ancient Greece, as an early
conceptualisation of the concept of hope (Snyder, 2000a, b).
Among the recent research on hope as a psychological construct, Menninger
wrote an academic lecture on hope in 1959, and Erickson defined hope as a
psychiatric variable in his 1975 publication (Erickson et al., 1975; Menninger,
1959). The most extensive research on hope as a psychological construct has been
done by C.R. Snyder (Snyder, Rand and Sigmon, 2002; Snyder, Rand, King,
Feldman and Woodward, 2002; Snyder, 1996, 1994, 1991). He has outlined a
three-dimensional construct. In his terms, hope is “a positive motivational state
that is based on an interactively derived sense of successful a. agency (goal
directed) energy and b. pathways (planning to meet goals), as well as c. goals”
(Snyder, 2000c). As noted earlier, goals provide targets of thought processes. They
may be verbal descriptions or visual images. They vary in terms of temporal
frame and degree of specificity (Snyder, 2000c). They may reflect positive, or
approach goals or negative goal outcomes (Snyder et al., 2002). “Pathways
thinking” involves the perception that a path to the hoped future is feasible
(Snyder et al., 2002).
In Snyder’s model, his concept of “pathways thinking” is similar to our notion that
self-efficacy affects the person’s experience of hope by creating a belief in the
feasibility, or possibility that the desired future or state might occur. In this way, the
hope is genuine and not foolish (Groopman, 2004; Snyder, 2000c), therefore that it is
realistic and feasible. In contrast, that which is entirely out of one’s possibility is
discarded as unrealistic and therefore not worthy of effort or even dreaming. People
who are more optimistic and experience positive emotions set this marker high,
meanwhile more pessimistic people set this marker low (Seligman, 1991; Fredrickson
and Soiner, 2002). Therefore, we believe optimism must be incorporated into the
components that affect a person’s experience of hope. This helps to explain why some
people claim, or consciously claim that they do not dream. They do not want to be
disappointed. They lack optimism, or a sense of possibility, and therefore, place no
The ideal self
629
Downloaded by Technická univerzita vo Zvolene At 18:41 30 October 2015 (PT)
affect on the desired image of the future, or even worse, they place a negative affective
label on it turning the positive force into a restraining force.
Snyder (2000c) said that people with high hope are also producing alternative routes
to their goals, and when in situations when they face goal impediment (Irving et al.,
1998; Snyder et al., 1991, 1996; Tierney, 1995). Agency thinking is an additional
motivational component of Snyder’s definition of hope (Snyder, 2000c). But this is
where we believe Snyder’s concept confuses different constructs, that of the emotional
condition toward and the conceptual image of the desired future. We believe, by
separating these two concepts, we achieve greater clarity on the internal mechanism of
a person’s ideal self.
In addition, in the above literature of hope there are overlaps and similarities with
the concept of efficacy, as described in motivational and personality literatures as self
efficacy and optimism. Literature on control beliefs shows confusion with agency and
pathways thinking. Aspinwall and Leaf (2002), and Skinner’s (1996) review of a large
number of control-related constructs found three related sets of beliefs, namely beliefs
about agent-ends relations (personal control beliefs), beliefs about agent means
relations (efficacy expectations) and means ends relations (response efficacy,
optimism). Agency thinking seems close to a combination of the first two sets of beliefs
(agent-ends and agent-means), while the pathways concept is parallel to means-ends
thinking (Aspinwall and Leaf, 2002).
In the model of the ideal self proposed in this article, optimism and efficacy are seen
as the main determinants and generators of hope, and therefore, key determinants of
the ideal self. Efficacy and optimism research provided insights on the nature and the
difficulty of goals selected and the mechanisms through which the ideal self becomes a
motivational force within the self, guiding the individual on goals selection, review, as
well as goal adaptation, focus and change in behaviours or the goals themselves the
face of adversity, integration of both negative and positive information. Self-efficacy
related research also helps to understand how the person can sacrifice immediate
rewards for the sake of accomplishment of the important ones.
Between the 1960s and 1970s, there had been a lot of research and theory developed
on the role of self-referent thought in psychological functioning (DeCharms, 1968;
Rotter et al., 1972; Lefcourt, 1976; Perlmuter and Monty, 1979; Garber et al., 1980).
Bandura’s research on self-efficacy (1986, 1982, 1977) emphasized the centrality of
self-perception of efficacy in human agency, through its influence on psychological
functioning during anticipatory and actual transactions with a person’s environment.
Self-efficacy is found to be the cognitive mediator in the relationship between
knowledge and actions (Bandura, 1997). A person’s perception of his/her capability
also determines what kind of goals people will be chosen, how much effort will be
invested and how much and how long one will maintain persistence in the face of
obstacles or aversive experiences (Bandura, 1997). This is where the hope component
of the ideal self interacts with the core identity component. That is, the person’s
awareness of his/her enduring capability and dispositions.
Strength of a person’s efficacy predicted behavior change (Kolb and Boyatzis, 1970;
Bandura, 1982). The stronger the perceived efficacy the more likely are people to
persist in their efforts until they succeed (Bandura, 1982). In Bandura’s social learning
theory, a source of cognitive motivation is directly linked to goal setting (Bandura,
1977, 1982). It requires personal standards of excellence against which one is able to
JMD
25,7
630
Downloaded by Technická univerzita vo Zvolene At 18:41 30 October 2015 (PT)
evaluate his/her own performance. People create self-incentives for their actions
(intrinsic motivation) by linking self satisfaction with a certain level of performance
mastery self motivation is sustained through the adoption of feasible sub goals that
lead to large future goals (Bandura, 1977). According to Bandura (1977; 1982),
self-efficacy beliefs may vary on three dimensions:
(1) particular level of difficulty of the goal (magnitude or level dimension);
(2) certainty of the person about performing particular level of goals (strength
dimension); and
(3) generality across contexts and situations.
Recent research has focused in the third dimension, termed general self-efficacy, which
is a more stable dimension of self-efficacy, seen by many researchers as another
motivational trait (Chen et al., 2001), which they believe is resistant to ephemeral
influences and is developed through the aggregation of previous life experiences and
the role of successes and failures in an individual’s life history.
Attitudinal optimism has received much attention and been a driving force in the
positive psychology movement (Seligman, 1991). Scheier and Carver (1985) define
optimism as a stable personality trait of cognitive nature that is operationalized as a
measure of generalized positive expectancies in certain and uncertain times, thus
stressing the role of outcome expectancies in the prediction of goal-directed behavior.
Attitudinal optimism has been seen as enabling:
.
vigorous and effective goal pursuit;
.
cognitive evaluation and useful integration of negative information about the
self;
.
capability for adaptation to changes in life brought about by unexpected
negative life events; and
.
ability for the individual to select which are the critical goals to engage, as well
as ability to disengage from goals that become irrelevant, not able to lead to
success, unsolvable, or misleading (Aspinwall et al., 2001, 2000, 1999, 1996;
Carver et al., 1993; Taylor et al., 1992; Scheier et al, 1986).
As mentioned previously, we believe this has confused the desired end states, the
goals, with the belief that they are possible to be achieved and the affective tone of each
of these thoughts or images. In our model, we are attempting to clarify each of these
elements, separately, to document their primary interactions and facilitate their
accurate assessment.
Current research on hope converges on the underlying base of hope as cognitions
(Snyder, 2000c; Snyder, Feldman, Taylor, Schroeder and Adams, 2000; Snyder, Ilardi,
Cheavens, Michael, Yamhure and Sympson, 2000; Snyder, Ilardi, Michael and
Cheavens, 2000; Snyder and McCullough, 2000; Snyder, 1995, 1994, 1991). In their view,
emotions are not seen to be at the core of hope, as they are seen as rather reactive and
evaluative in nature, with “feelings playing an important albeit contributory role”
which is not further defined (Aspinwall and Leaf, 2002). Affect is seen to be following
cognitive appraisals of goal related activities (Snyder, 1991). As individuals proceed to
goal attainment, emotional feedback reinforces agency thinking, resulting in the
The ideal self
631
Downloaded by Technická univerzita vo Zvolene At 18:41 30 October 2015 (PT)
continuation of activity toward the goal (Aspinwall and Leaf, 2002). This confusion as
to the image, or goals, and the affect is a major problem.
Although we view emotion as a part of each of the components of the ideal self and
the driving force of a person’s intentional change, the positive emotion involved in the
experience of Hope is central to the power of the ideal self. Positive emotion emerges
from the sense of agency or self-efficacy, and the belief that there will be feasible routes
to the accomplishment of the hoped for image or state. All of this adds to the person’s
degree of optimism, resulting in the aggregate positive affect encoding of the images or
dreams of the future and the person’s core identity (their sense of their own enduring
dispositions, strengths, traits, and such). As we said earlier, positive emotion is defined
as “a state of high energy, full concentration, and pleasurable engagement, while
negative affect is defined as “a general dimension of subjective distress and
unpleasurable engagement that subsumes a variety of aversive mood states, including
anger, contempt, disgust, guilt, fear and nervousness” (Watson and Tellegen, 1985).
Does this mean that people who are relatively lower in self-efficacy and optimism
experience less hope? Yes. We believe that the experience of hope drives the energy,
through positive emotions, attached to the image or dream of a desired future. Without
these positive emotions, we believe that the person becomes defensive, loses “hope,”
and withdraws energy or commitment to the effort of change.
The image
If the hope component is the affective driver of the ideal self, and the core identity is the
personal context, the dream or image of a desired future is the content of the ideal self.
It is the picture of what is hoped to be. Again, whenever we refer to the dream or the
image of a desired future, it does not imply that a person has to change. For example, a
person may be in a current state that he/she feels in perfect or the best balance that is
possible. In such a situation, the dream of a future state is the continuation of the
current state. In such a situation, we believe there is still the need to formulate and
activate the ideal self for the investment of time and energy needed to maintain or
sustain this present condition. All of the dynamics of ICT apply here as well.
The dream or image of a desired future comes from many sources. Dreams and
fantasies are the expression of one’s inner needs, wishes, fears (Murray, 1938;
McClelland, 1985). They interact with each other in an on-going manner, over time. So
there is a dynamics quality to the dream. Among the major sources are one’s values
and philosophy. These are created and nurtured by a person’s family of origin, current
family, reference groups, and social identity groups to which one belongs or aspires to
belong (Boyatzis et al., 2000). A person history and enduring dispositions, including
those characteristics labelled as strengths, become a continuous input into one’s values
and philosophy. In other words, such perceptions of one’s own qualities, and the extent
to which they are positively valued (i.e. seen as strengths), will affect one’s values and
be interpreted by one’s operating philosophy.
But the images of a desired future is also a function of a person’s career and life
stage. What you consider a noble and worthwhile aspiration when you are 21 is
typically different from what you dream about doing or being when you are 58. In
addition, a person’s discovery of their purpose, or calling, also feeds into the dream.
Being aware of your own passion, that which makes you feel life is worth living and
you are fulfilling a promise of some higher being or life force, is your calling.
JMD
25,7
632
Downloaded by Technická univerzita vo Zvolene At 18:41 30 October 2015 (PT)
Throughout history of mankind, humans are driven by their imagination and their
ability to see images of the desired future. Leaders, poets, writers, composers, artists,
dreamers, athletes have been able to be inspired, stay inspired and inspire others
through such images. These images, once shared, have the power to become a force,
and in that sense an inspiration for social development and growth, for intentional
change at many levels of social organization, not just for the individual.
The dream or image of a desired future is often a compilation of a variety of
preferences, aspirations, wishes, and fantasies. But the specifics of each dream or
fantasy are not necessarily insight into the person’s ideal self. Just as with dream
analysis, you must probe below the manifest level and extrude the insight from the
latent themes which are inherent in these dreams and fantasies (McClelland, 1985;
Boyatzis, 1998).
The dream or image of desired future is not what a person fears and wants to avoid.
it is a mental and emotional state in which a person is elated and aroused, we believe, in
their parasympathetic nervous system (i.e. PSNS). This is quite the opposite
neuro-endocrine process to that which is engaged when a person contemplates
something they fear or want to avoid. In such case, a person’s limbic system is aroused,
starting with the amygdale and their sympathetic nervous system (i.e. SNS) is aroused
(Sapolsky, 2004). This involves a stress response. Once engaged, arousal of the SNS
has the effect of limiting thought but focusing neural circuits on the object of fear or to
be avoided (Boyatzis, Smith, and Blaize, in press). The resulting “fixation” blocks out
other neural circuits or thoughts. At the same, the arousal of the SNS causes cortisol to
be secreted into the bloodstream and has the resulting effect of inhibiting neurogenesis
(i.e. the creation of new neural tissue from stem cells, which create the possibility of
new learning) and shrinkage or death of older neurons from over stimulation (Boyatzis
and McKee, 2005). All of this means that fear and stress arousal not only lead to
feelings of nervousness, depression, or sadness and fear, but also limit the person’s
access to their current neural circuits (their brain) and learning. It is believed that
dreaming about the future, experiencing hope, and arousing images of a desired future
leads to arousal of the PSNS and all of the positive effects on new learning, access to
more of one’s neural circuits, and a perception of wanting to spend more time in
thinking about these images (Boyatzis and McKee, 2005). The power of the ideal self is
not just emotional. It is physical in that it involves neuro-endocrine processes that
allow the body to renew itself, while ameliorating the ravages caused by chronic stress
(Boyatzis, and McKee, 2005).
But the process of evaluating or judging the worthiness of one’s dreams
immediately invokes stress. This results in limiting your openness to new ideas and
possibilities. This is the essential difference in the positive and negative emotional
attractors. Similarly, increasing a person’s commitment to their ought self, to the extent
it is different from their ideal self, can have the effect of limiting flexibility about even
considering the ideal self (Berlinger, 1994).
The core identity one’s context and resources
As we mentioned earlier, the core identity is the compilation of the person’s enduring
dispositions. Their unconscious motives, traits, roles taken consistently in social
settings, and other habits become the basis for their core identity. In addition, this is
evaluated by their reference groups, social identity groups, and anticipatory
The ideal self
633
Downloaded by Technická univerzita vo Zvolene At 18:41 30 October 2015 (PT)
socialization (groups within which they would like to included). So these dispositions
take a relatively positive or negative value. In this sense, like Trafinow, Triandis and
Goto claimed, our concept of core identity is comprised of “social identity” in group
memberships, affiliations, and connections to different social collectives, and “personal
identity” often referred to as one’s attitudes, traits, feelings and behavior (Abrams and
Hogg, 2001, 2003).
A growing interest in techniques called “strength based” approaches to
development or training seek to help the person identify strengths they have shown
in the past. In a typical exercise, a person is asked to interview ten to 20 people with
whom they work, live, and play, asking them to recall “a time when I was at my best.”
Then, each respondent is asked to tell about the person’s actions and their impact on
others. The person takes all of these stories and conducts a thematic analysis, looking
for themes and patterns. The resulting list of ways they act when they were “at their
best” constitutes an inventory of their strengths as observed, experienced, and
remembered by others. It is a powerful exercise that makes people feel a surge of
self-confidence and often accompanying self-esteem. We believe this burst of positive
emotion and self-evaluation provides a boost to their sense of self-efficacy, and
therefore, hope about the future. It also arouses, we believe, the PSNS and creates a
neural condition in which they are open to new ideas. These may include new ideas
about the future.
But it is precisely this effect that is important but not sufficient to yield a potent
ideal self and driver of intentional change. Each person needs a clear image of a desired
future. By building the articulation and awareness (consciousness) of the core identity,
the person is prepared for the development of the image of a desired future, and the
accompanying sense of hope. Without this additional jump into the fantasized future, a
person may feel compelled to recreate conditions of the past in order to continue utilize
their “strengths” and not experiment with new behavior.
The three processes leading to a healthy ideal self
There are three paths or processes by which a person can develop a healthy and robust
(i.e. meaningful and useful) ideal self, as shown in Table I. One is to increase one’s
Phase change criteria When it is low
Is the ideal self
articulated, explicit?
Mindfulness or consciousness The person experiences catastrophic
jumps, surprises, or emergence. The
person is mindless or in denial of a
desired future
Is the ideal self
important?
Salience or intensity of desire for
the components of the ideal self
Like New Year’s Eve resolutions, the
person makes superficial commitments
to change
Is it integrated with the
rest?
Coherence or a holistic inclusion of
all components of the person’s
desired life and future
The person experiences surprises and
unintended consequences in other parts
of their life even when making progress
or changes in other parts of their life or
work
Table I.
Three paths leading to a
healthy ideal self
JMD
25,7
634
Downloaded by Technická univerzita vo Zvolene At 18:41 30 October 2015 (PT)
mindfulness about the ideal self and its components. Another is to test the salience or
importance of the components. A third is the determination of the coherence of the
image is it an holistic image of a desired future?
Each of these processes enables the person to establish increased congruence
between his/her unconscious and conscious, between his/her seemingly fanciful
dreams and aspirations, and thoughts and feelings. Without mindfulness or awareness
of the ideal self and its components, it would be difficult for a person to sort out and
evaluate the degree of importance each component has. Similarly, it would be
impossible to test the holistic nature of one’s image of the desired future. How can a
person discover that he/she forgot to consider his/her partner or spouse without
awareness of both the components and their spouse or partner’s wishes and dreams?
The power of the ideal self, in accessing more neural circuits, more learning
possibilities, and the emotional state of elation provides a fertile ground for
contemplating the future. The person is more likely to be engaging all aspects of
his/her hopes, dreams, and strengths. It diminishes the possibility of repressed wishes
or fears and less subject to undesired effect of their ego defence mechanism (Freud,
1936).
Implications for research and practice
This article proposed a model in which the ideal self becomes a motivational core and
the locus of positive emotion within the self. Further, it is our contention that this ideal
self drives the personal vision which, in turn, drives sustainable, intentional change.
Similarly, we believe collective, shared desired images of the future, shared hope, and
shared sense of a group’s identity and distinctiveness, in the same way, become the
shared vision that drives sustainable, intentional change at these other levels of human
and social organization as discussed in more detail in other articles in this issue.
Drawing from advances in positive psychology as well as classic psychological theory,
we proposed a novel and more detailed determination of the construct of the ideal self
and its determinants.
This is a prescriptive model that needs to be refined and tested through empirical
research. Research validating this model should primarily focus in three directions:
First, the links between the variables of hope and the degree of activation of a person’s
ideal self (increased consciousness, salience and coherence) need to be established and
validated through research. Second, the offered model linking the constructs of self
efficacy and optimism with the construct of hope needs to be further explored. In the
relevant literature the measures offered for these constructs seem to be seen as
unrelated and as measuring three distinct psychological processes. We believe that the
inherent assumptions in existing work require new ways of measuring the constructs,
as well as clarity as to their distinctions form each other. Third, the sources and roles of
one’s dreams, aspirations, and the components of a desired image of the future need to
be studies. This would include how a person’s values and operating philosophy is
affected by one’s life and career stages, the role of passion and calling, and so forth.
Conceptualizing and recognizing the ideal self as the driver of sustainable change
reverses common practice and common assumptions at all levels of change. For
individuals, people often believe a shock drives change. In organizations, the
assumption is that urgency and threat can provoke change. Intentional change theory
helps us to see that the ideal self at the individual level, shared vision and dreams at
The ideal self
635
Downloaded by Technická univerzita vo Zvolene At 18:41 30 October 2015 (PT)
collective levels are the real driver of change. But even with the recent increase in
popularity and research in positive psychology, the actual mechanism is elusive and
often misrepresented as seeming to require mere feeling good and change and good
things will happen. The model of the ideal self and its counterpart at collective levels of
social organization provides a more detailed model of how emotion, conceptualization
of a desired state and one’s past and dispositions might fit into a personal or shared
vision.
While stories abound with this impact for sports figures, what is needed is careful
research to support or refute or modify the elements of this model and how they affect
each other. Current measures often confuse one of more of these concepts and make
divergent validity difficult to establish. The use of qualitative methods of thematic
analysis may be required to access and encode a person’s dreams, or a team’s or
organization’s. Cultures express shared dreams in their mythology and folklore (or the
current version of that as highly viewed movies, listened to songs, and repeated
archetypal stories). Research would then follow to help us understand how best to
arouse and help people articulate these dreams for themselves and shared dream that
will drive collective intentional change.
References
Abrams, D. and Hogg, M.A. (2001), “Collective identity: group membership and self-conception”,
in Hogg, M.A. and Tindale, S. (Eds), Group Processes, Blackwell Publishers, Malden, MA,
pp. 425-60.
Anderson, J.R. (1983), The Architecture of Cognition, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.
Ashby, F.G., Isen, A.M. and Turken, A.U. (1999), “A neurological theory of positive affect and its
influence on cognition”, Psychological Review, Vol. 106, pp. 529-50.
Aspinwall, L.G. and Brunhart, S.M. (1996), “Distinguishing optimism from denial: optimistic
beliefs predict attention to health threats”, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
Vol. 22, pp. 993-1003.
Aspinwall, L.G. and Brunhart, S.M. (2000), “What I do know won’t hurt me: optimism, attention
to negative information, coping and health”, in Gillham, J.E. (Ed.), The Science of Optimism
and Hope: Research Essays in Honour of Martin E.P. Seligman, Templeton Foundation
Press, Philadelphia, PA, pp. 163-200.
Aspinwall, L.G. and Leaf, S.L. (2002), “In search of the unique aspects of hope: pinning our hopes
on positive emotions, future-oriented thinking, hard times and other people”, Psychological
Inquiry, Vol. 13 No. 4, pp. 276-321.
Aspinwall, L.G., Richter, L. and Hoffman, R.R. (2001), “Understanding how optimism ‘works’: an
examination of optimists’ adaptive moderation of belief and behaviour”, in Chang, E.C.
(Ed.), Optimism and Pessimism: Theory, Research and Practice, American Psychological
Association, Washington, DC, pp. 217-38.
Bandura, A. (1977), “Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behaviour change”, Psychological
Review, Vol. 84, pp. 191-215.
Bandura, A. (1982), “Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency”, American Psychologist, Vol. 37,
pp. 122-47.
Bandura, A. (1986), Social Foundations of Thought and Action, Prentice-Hall, New York, NY.
Bandura, A. (1997), Self Efficacy: The Exercise of Control, Freeman, New York, NY.
JMD
25,7
636
Downloaded by Technická univerzita vo Zvolene At 18:41 30 October 2015 (PT)
Baumeister, R.F. (1998), “The self”, in Gilbert, D.T., Fiske, S.T. and Lindzey, G. (Eds),
The Handbook of Social Psychology, 4th ed., McGraw-Hill, New York, NY, pp. 680-740.
Berlinger, L. (1994), “Commitment as a moderator of flexibility”, unpublished doctoral
dissertation, University of Texas, Austin, TX.
Boldero, J. and Francis, J. (1999), “Ideals, oughts, and self-regulation: are there qualitatively
distinct self-guides?”, Asian Journal of Social Psychology, Vol. 2, pp. 343-55.
Boyatzis, R.E. (1973), “Affiliation motivation: a review and a new perspective”, in
McClelland, D.C. and Steele, R.S. (Eds), Human Motivation: A Book of Readings,
General Learning Press, Morristown, NJ, pp. 252-78.
Boyatzis, R.E. (1998), Transforming Qualitative Information: Thematic Analysis and Code
Development, Sage, Thousand Oaks, Sage, CA.
Boyatzis, R.E. and McKee, A. (2005), Resonant Leadership: Renewing Yourself and Connecting
with Others Through Mindfulness, Hope, and Compassion, Harvard Business School
Press, Boston, MA.
Boyatzis, R.E., Murphy, A. and Wheeler, J. (2000), “Philosophy as the missing link between
values and behaviour”, Psychological Reports, Vol. 86, pp. 47-64.
Boyatzis, R.E., Smith, M. and Blaize, N. ((in press)), “Sustaining leadership effectiveness through
coaching and compassion: it’s not what you think”, Academy of Management Journal on
Learning and Education.
Cantril, H. (1967), “Sentio, ergo sum: ‘motivation’ reconsidered”, Journal of Psychology:
Interdisciplinary and Applied, Vol. 65 No. 1, pp. 91-107.
Carver, C.S., Pozo, C., Harris, S.D., Noriega, V., Scheier, M.F., Robinson, D.S., Ketcham, A.S.,
Moffat, F.I. and Clark, K.C. (1993), “How coping mediates the effect of optimism on
distress: a study of women with early stage breast cancer”, in Suinn, R.M. and
VandenBos, G.R. (Eds), Cancer Patients and Their Families: Readings on Disease Course,
Coping and Psychological Interventions, American Psychological Association, Washington,
DC, pp. 92-127.
Chen, G., Gully, S.M. and Eden, D. (2001), “Validation of a new general self-efficacy scale”,
Organizational Research Methods, Vol. 4, pp. 62-83.
Curry, L., Snyder, C.R., Cook, D., Ruby, B. and Rehm, M. (1997), “The role of hope in academic and
sport achievement”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 73, pp. 1257-67.
DeCharms, R. (1968), Personal Causation, Academic Press, New York, NY.
Erickson, R.C., Post, R.D. and Paige, A.B. (1975), “Hope as a psychiatric variable”, Journal of
Clinical Psychology, Vol. 31, pp. 324-9.
Farber, M.L. (1968), Theory of Suicide, Funk & Wagnall’s, New York, NY.
Frank, J.D. (1968), “The role of hope in psychotherapy”, International Journal of Psychiatry, Vol. 5,
pp. 383-95.
Frank, J.D. (1973), Persuasion and Healing, rev. ed., Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore,
MD.
Fredrickson, B.L. and Soiner, T. (2002), “Positive emotions trigger upward spirals toward
emotional well being”, Psychological Science, Vol. 13, pp. 162-75.
French, T.M. (1952), The Integration of Behavior: Vol. 1. Basic Postulates, University of Chicago
Press, Chicago, IL.
Freud, A. (1966), The Writings of Anna Freud, Volume II: The Ego and the Mechanisms of
Defence, (originally published 1936), International Universities Press, New York, NY.
The ideal self
637
Downloaded by Technická univerzita vo Zvolene At 18:41 30 October 2015 (PT)
Garber, J., Fencil-Morse, E., Rosellini, R.A. and Seligman, M.E. (1980), “‘Abnormal fixations’ and
‘learned helplessness’: inescapable shock as a weanling impairs adult discrimination
learning in rats”, Behaviour Research and Therapy, Vol. 17, pp. 197-206.
Gottschalk, L.A. (1974), “A hope scale applicable to verbal samples”, Archives of General
Psychiatry, Vol. 30, pp. 779-85.
Groopman, J. (2004), The Anatomy of Hope: How People Prevail in the Face of Illness, Random
House, New York, NY.
Higgins, E.T. (1989), “Self-discrepancy theory: what patterns of self-beliefs cause people to
suffer?”, in Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 22,
Academic Press, New York, NY, pp. 93-136.
Irving, L.M., Snyder, C.R. and Crowson, J.J. (1998), “Hope and coping with cancer by college
women”, Journal of Personality, Vol. 66, pp. 195-214.
James, W. (1897), The Will to Believe, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.
Kelner, S. (1990), “Interpersonal motivation: positive, negative, and anxious”, unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Department of Psychology and Social Relations, Harvard University,
Boston, MA.
Kolb, D.A. and Boyatzis, R.E. (1970b), “Goal-setting and self -directed behavior change”, Human
Relations, Vol. 23 No. 5, pp. 439-57.
Lee, T.W., Locke, E.A. and Latham, G.P. (1989), “Goal setting theory and job performance”,
in Pervin, L.A. (Ed.), Goal Concepts in Personality and Social Psychology, Erlbaum,
Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 291-326.
Lefcourt, H.M. (1976), “Locus of control and the response to aversive events”, Ontario
Psychologist, Vol. 8, pp. 41-9.
Lewin, K. (1938), “The conceptual representation and measurement of psychological forces”,
Contributions to Psychological Theory, Vol. 1, pp. 1-36.
Lewin, K., Dembo, T., Festinger, L. and Sears, P.S. (1944), “Level of aspiration”, in Hunt, J.M.
(Ed.), Personality and Behavior Disorders, Ronald Press, New York, NY.
Locke, E.A. and Latham, G.P. (1990), A Theory of Goal Setting and Task Performance,
Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.
Ludema, J. (1996), “Narrative inquiry: collective storytelling as a source of hope, knowledge, and
action in organizational life”, unpublished doctoral dissertation, Case Western Reserve
University, Cleveland, OH.
McAdams, D. (1980), “A thematic coding system for the intimacy motive”, Journal of Research in
Personality, Vol. 14, pp. 413-32.
McClelland, D.C. (1985), Human Motivation, Scott Foreman & Co., Glenview, IL.
Markus, H. and Nurius, P. (1986), “Possible selves”, American Psychologist, Vol. 41, pp. 954-69.
Matthews, K.A., Raikkomen, K., Sulton-Tyrell, K. and Kuller, L.H. (2004), “Optimistic attitudes
protect against progression of carotid atherosclerosis in healthy middle-aged women”,
Psychosomatic Medicine, Vol. 66, pp. 640-4.
Melges, R. and Bowlby, J. (1969), “Types of hopelessness in psychopathological processes”,
Archives of General Psychiatry, Vol. 20, pp. 690-9.
Menninger, K. (1959), “The academic lecture on hope”, American Journal of Psychiatry, Vol. 116,
pp. 481-91.
Mowrer, O.H. (1960), The Psychology of Hope, Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, CA.
Moyers, B. (1993), Healing and the Mind, Doubleday, New York, NY.
JMD
25,7
638
Downloaded by Technická univerzita vo Zvolene At 18:41 30 October 2015 (PT)
Murray, H.A. (1938), Explorations in Personality, Oxford University Press, New York, NY.
Nasby, W. (1997), “Self-consciousness and cognitive prototypes of the ideal self”, Journal of
Research in Personality, Vol. 31, pp. 543-63.
Oettingen, G. (1996), “Positive fantasies and motivation”, in Gollwitzer, P.M. and Bargh, J.A.
(Eds), The Psychology of Action: Linking Cognition and Motivation to Behavior, Guilford,
New York, NY, pp. 236-59.
Perlmuter, L.C. and Monty, R.A. (Eds) (1979), Choice and Perceived Control, Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, New York, NY.
Pervin, L.A. (Ed.) (1989), Goal Concepts in Personality and Social Psychology, Erlbaum, Hillsdale,
NJ.
Rotter, J.B., Chance, J.E. and Phares, E.J. (1972), Application of Social Learning Theory of
Personality, Holt, Rinehart & Winston, Oxford, UK.
Sapolsky, R.M. (2004), Why Zebras Don’t get Ulcers, 3rd ed., Harper Collins, New York, NY.
Schecter, D.A. (1974), “The ideal self and other”, Contemporary Psychoanalysis, Vol. 10 No. 1,
pp. 103-15.
Scheier, M.F. and Carver, C.S. (1985), “Optimism, coping and health: assessment and implications
of generalized outcome expectancies”, Health Psychology, Vol. 4, pp. 219-47.
Scheier, M.F., Weintraub, J.K. and Carver, C.S. (1986), “Coping with stress: divergent strategies of
optimists and pessimists”, Journal of Personality and Social psychology, Vol. 51, pp. 1257-64.
Seligman, M.E.P. (1991), Learned Optimism, Knopf, New York, NY.
Seligman, M.E.P. and Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000), “Positive psychology: an introduction”,
American Psychologist, Vol. 55, pp. 5-14.
Skinner, E.A. (1996), “A guide to constructs of control”, Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, Vol. 71, pp. 549-70.
Snyder, C.R. (1991), “The will and the ways: development and validation of an
individual-differences measure of hope”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Vol. 60 No. 4, pp. 570-85.
Snyder, C.R. (1994), The Psychology of Hope: You Can Get There From Here, Free Press,
New York, NY.
Snyder, C.R. (1995), “Conceptualizing, measuring and nurturing hope”, Journal of Counseling and
Development, Vol. 73, pp. 355-60.
Snyder, C.R. (1996), “To hope, to lose and hope again”, Journal of Personal and Interpersonal
Loss, Vol. 1, pp. 3-16.
Snyder, C.R. (1998), “A case for hope in pain, loss and suffering”, in Harvey, J.H., Omarzu, J. and
Miller, E. (Eds), Perspectives on Loss: A Sourcebook, Taylor and Francis, Washington, DC.
Snyder, C.R. (2000a), “Hope theory: rainbows in the mind”, Psychological Inquiry, Vol. 13 No. 4,
pp. 149-275.
Snyder, C.R. (2000b), “The past and possible futures of hope”, Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology, Vol. 19 No. 1, pp. 11-28.
Snyder, C.R. (Ed.) (2000c), Handbook of Hope: Theory, Measures and Applications, Academic
Press, San Diego, CA.
Snyder, C.R. and McCullough, M.E. (2000), “A positive psychology field of dreams: if you build it,
they will come ...”, Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, Vol. 19, pp. 151-60.
Snyder, C.R., Cheavens, J. and Sympson, S.C. (1997), “Hope: an individual motive for social
commerce”, Group dynamics: Theory, Research and Practice, Vol. 1 No. 2, pp. 107-18.
The ideal self
639
Downloaded by Technická univerzita vo Zvolene At 18:41 30 October 2015 (PT)
Snyder, C.R., Irvine, L.M. and Anderson, J.R. (1991), “Hope and health”, in Snyder, C.R. and
Forsyth, D.R. (Eds), Handbook of Social and Clinical Psychology: The Health Perspective,
Pergamon Press, Elmsford, NY, pp. 285-305.
Snyder, C.R., Rand, K.L. and Sigmon, D.R. (2002), “Hope theory: a member of the positive
psychology family”, in Snyder, C.R. and Lopez, S.J. (Eds), The Handbook of Positive
Psychology, Wiley, New York, NY.
Snyder, C.R., Rand, K.L., King, E.A., Feldman, D.B. and Woodward, J.T. (2002), “False hope”,
Journal of Clinical Psychology, Vol. 58, pp. 1003-22.
Snyder, C.R., Ilardi, S., Michael, S.E. and Cheavens, J. (2000), “Hope theory: updating a common
process for psychological change”, in Snyder, C.R. and Ingram, R.E. (Eds), Handbook of
Psychological Change: Psychotherapy Processes and Practices for the 21st Century, Wiley,
New York, NY, pp. 128-53.
Snyder, C.R., Feldman, D.B., Taylor, S.D., Schroeder, L.L. and Adams, V.H. (2000), “The roles of
hopeful thinking in preventing problems and enhancing strengths”, Applied and
Preventive Psychology, Vol. 15, pp. 262-95.
Snyder, C.R., Ilardi, S.S., Cheavens, J., Michael, S.T., Yamhure, L. and Sympson, S. (2000), “The
role of hope in cognitive-behavior therapies”, Cognitive Therapy and Research, Vol. 24,
pp. 747-62.
Snyder, C.R., Sympson, S.C., Ybasco, F.C., Borders, T.F., Babyak, M.A. and Higgins, R.L. (1996),
“Development and validation of the State Hope Scale”, Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, Vol. 70, pp. 321-35.
Stotland, E. (1969), The Psychology of Hope, Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, CA.
Taylor, S.E., Kemeny, M.E., Aspinwall, L.G. and Schneider, S.G. (1992), “Optimism, coping,
psychological distress, and high risk sexual behavior among men at risk for Acquired
Immuno-deficiency Syndrome (AIDS)”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Vol. 63, pp. 460-73.
Taylor, S.E., Pham, L.B., Rivkin, I.D. and Armor, D.A. (1998), “Harnessing the imagination:
mental simulation”, American Psychologist, Vol. 53, pp. 429-39.
Tierney, A.M. (1995), “Analysis of a new theory of hope and personality as measured by the
California Psychological Inventory”, Dissertation Abstracts International, 55 (10-B), 4616.
VandeWalle, D., Cron, W.L. and Slocum, J.W. Jr (2001), “The role of goal orientation following
performance feedback”, Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 86 No. 4, pp. 629-40.
Watson, D. and Tellegen, A. (1985), “Toward a consensual structure of mood”, Psychological
Bulletin, Vol. 98, pp. 219-35.
Further reading
Abramson, L.Y., Seligman, M.E. and Teasdale, J.D. (1978), “Learned helplessness in humans:
critique and reformulation”, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Vol. 87, pp. 49-74.
Aspinwall, L.G. (1998), “Rethinking the role of positive affect in self-regulation”, Motivation and
Emotion, Vol. 22, pp. 1-32.
Aspinwall, L.G. and Richter, L. (1999), “Optimism and self-mastery predict more rapid
disengagement from unsolvable tasks in the presence of alternatives”, Motivation and
Emotion, Vol. 23, pp. 221-45.
Bandura, A. (1989), “Human agency in social cognitive theory”, American Psychologist, Vol. 44,
pp. 1175-84.
JMD
25,7
640
Downloaded by Technická univerzita vo Zvolene At 18:41 30 October 2015 (PT)
Baumeister, R.F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M. and Tice, D.M. (1998), “Ego depletion: is the
active self a limited resource?”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 74,
pp. 1252-65.
Beck, A.T. (1974), “The measurement of pessimism: the hopelessness scale”, Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Vol. 42, pp. 861-5.
Boyatzis, R.E. (1982), The Competent Manager: A Model for Effective Performance, John Wiley &
Sons, New York, NY.
Boyatzis, R.E. (1999), “Self-directed change and learning as a necessary meta-competency for
success and effectiveness in the 21st century”, in Sims, R. and Veres, J.G. (Eds), Keys to
Employee Success in the Coming Decades, Greenwood Publishing, Westport, CT, pp. 15-32.
Boyatzis, R.E. (2001), “How and why individuals are able to develop emotional intelligence”, in
Cherniss, C. and Goleman, D. (Eds), The Emotionally Intelligent Workplace: How to Select
For, Measure, and Improve Emotional Intelligence in Individuals, Groups, and
Organizations, Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, CA, pp. 234-53.
Boyatzis, R.E. ((in press)), “Using tipping points of emotional intelligence and cognitive
competencies to predict financial performance of leaders”, Psicothemia, p. 17.
Carver, C.S. and Scheier, M.F. (2003), “Three human strengths”, in Aspinwall, L.G. and
Staudinger, U.M. (Eds), A Psychology of Human Strengths: Fundamental Questions and
Future Directions for a Positive Psychology, American Psychological Association,
Washington, DC, pp. 87-102.
Carter, C., Macdonald, A., Ursu, S., Stenger, A., Ho Sohn, M. and Anderson, J. (2000), “How the
brain gets ready to perform”, paper presented at the 30th Annual Meeting of The Society
of Neuroscience, New Orleans, LA.
Chang, E.C. (Ed.) (2001), Optimism and Pessimism: Theory, Research and Practice, American
Psychological Association, Washington, DC.
Edelman, G. (1987), Neural Darwinism: The Theory of Neuronal Group Selection, Basic Books,
New York, NY.
James, W. (1890), The Principles of Psychology, Henry Holt, New York, NY.
Keliman, G., Koch, C. and Fried, I. (2000), “Imagery neurons in the human brain”, Nature, Vol. 408,
pp. 357-61.
Maddux, J. (1991), “Self-efficacy”, in Snyder, C.R. and Forsyth, D.R. (Eds), Handbook of Social and
Clinical Psychology: The Health Perspective, Pergamon Press, Elmsford, NY, pp. 57-8.
Maddux, J.E. (1995), “Self-efficacy theory: an introduction”, in Maddux, J.E. (Ed.), Self-efficacy,
Adaptation, and Adjustment: Theory, Research, and Application, Plenum, New York, NY,
pp. 3-33.
Monty, R.A., Geller, E.S., Savage, R.E. and Perlmuter, L.C. (1979), “The freedom to choose is not
always so choice”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory,
Vol. 5, pp. 170-8.
Rotter, J.B. (1966), “Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of
reinforcement”, Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, Vol. 80 No. 1, p. 609.
Scheier, M.F. and Carver, C.S. (1987), “Dispositional optimism and physical well-being: the
influence of generalized outcome expectancies”, Journal of Personality, Vol. 55, pp. 169-210.
Seligman, M.E.P. (1998), Learned Optimism: How to Change Your Mind and Your Life, Pocket
Books, New York, NY.
Seligman, M.E.P. (2002), Authentic Happiness: Using the New Positive Psychology Realize Your
Potential for Lasting Fulfillment, Free Press, New York, NY.
The ideal self
641
Downloaded by Technická univerzita vo Zvolene At 18:41 30 October 2015 (PT)
Skinner, E.A. (1996), “A guide to constructs of control”, Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, Vol. 71, pp. 549-70.
Taylor, S.E. and Brown, J.D. (1988), “Illusion and well-being: a social psychological perspective
on mental health”, Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 103, pp. 193-210.
Zaccaro, S.J., Blair, V., Peterson, C. and Zazanis, M. (1995), “Collective efficacy”, in Maddux, J.E.
(Ed.), Self-Efficacy, Adaptation, and Adjustment: Theory, Research and Application,
Plenum, New York, NY, pp. 305-30.
About the authors
Richard E. Boyatzis is a Professor in the Weatherhead School of Management, Case Western
Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio, USA. He is the corresponding author and can be contacted
at: richard.boyatzis@case.edu
Kleio Akrivou is a PhD candidate in the Department of Organizational Behavior, Case
Western Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio, USA.
JMD
25,7
642
To purchase reprints of this article please e-mail: reprints@emeraldinsight.com
Or visit our web site for further details: www.emeraldinsight.com/reprints
Downloaded by Technická univerzita vo Zvolene At 18:41 30 October 2015 (PT)
This article has been cited by:
1. Nathalie Beaulieu, Julia Santos Silva, Steve Plante. 2015. Using a vision of a desired future in climate
change adaptation planning: lessons learned in the municipality of Rivière-au-Tonnerre (Québec,
Canada). Climate and Development 1-11. [CrossRef]
2. Kathy K. Overbeke, Diana Bilimoria, Toni Somers. 2015. Shared vision between fathers and daughters in
family businesses: the determining factor that transforms daughters into successors. Frontiers in Psychology
6. . [CrossRef]
3. Richard E. Boyatzis, Kylie Rochford, Scott N. Taylor. 2015. The role of the positive emotional attractor in
vision and shared vision: toward effective leadership, relationships, and engagement. Frontiers in Psychology
06. . [CrossRef]
4. Martin R. Fellenz. 2015. Forming the Professional Self: Bildung and the ontological perspective on
professional education and development. Educational Philosophy and Theory 1-17. [CrossRef]
5. Kathleen R. Buse, Diana Bilimoria. 2014. Personal vision: enhancing work engagement and the retention
of women in the engineering profession. Frontiers in Psychology 5. . [CrossRef]
6. Stephen P. Miller. 2014. Next-generation leadership development in family businesses: the critical roles
of shared vision and family climate. Frontiers in Psychology 5. . [CrossRef]
7. Daan Stam, Robert G. Lord, Daan van Knippenberg, Barbara Wisse. 2014. An Image of Who We Might
Become: Vision Communication, Possible Selves, and Vision Pursuit. Organization Science 25, 1172-1194.
[CrossRef]
8. Claire Mason, Mark Griffin, Sharon Parker. 2014. Transformational leadership development. Leadership
& Organization Development Journal 35:3, 174-194. [Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]
9. Suk Park Jin. 2014. After pain comes joy: identity gaps in employees' minds. Personnel Review 43:3,
419-437. [Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]
10. Heli Aramo-Immonen, Andrea Bikfalvi, Núria Mancebo, Hannu Vanharanta. 2013. Project Managers’
Competence Identification. International Journal of Human Capital and Information Technology
Professionals 2:10.4018/jhcitp.20110101, 33-47. [CrossRef]
11. Alexander Kaiser, Birgit Feldhusen, Birgit FordinalVision Development as a Knowledge Creating Process
3593-3602. [CrossRef]
12. George M. Slavich, Philip G. Zimbardo. 2012. Transformational Teaching: Theoretical Underpinnings,
Basic Principles, and Core Methods. Educational Psychology Review 24, 569-608. [CrossRef]
13. Martin Lamb. 2012. A Self System Perspective on Young Adolescents’ Motivation to Learn English in
Urban and Rural Settings. Language Learning 62:10.1111/lang.2012.62.issue-4, 997-1023. [CrossRef]
14. Jui-Kuei Chen. 2012. The assessment of vision re-creation indices for the automotive industry in Taiwan:
A hybrid fuzzy model approach. Measurement 45, 909-917. [CrossRef]
15. Linda M. Pittenger, Sheri Perelli, Toni SomersIT professionals: Maximizing engagement in the rapidly
changing business environment 248-256. [CrossRef]
16. Richard E. Boyatzis, Ceferi Soler. 2012. Vision, leadership and emotional intelligence transforming family
business. Journal of Family Business Management 2:1, 23-30. [Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]
17. Endnotes 153-161. [CrossRef]
Downloaded by Technická univerzita vo Zvolene At 18:41 30 October 2015 (PT)
18. Loren R. Dyck, Kathi J. Lovelace. 2012. Finding a Fit With Fitness: Applying Intentional Change
Theory in Worksite Health Promotion Programming. Journal of Workplace Behavioral Health 27, 12-31.
[CrossRef]
19. Kleio Akrivou, Hilary Bradbury-Huang. 2011. Executive catalysts: Predicting sustainable organizational
performance amid complex demands. The Leadership Quarterly 22, 995-1009. [CrossRef]
20. Jin-Suk Yang, 김김김. 2011. The L2 Motivational Self-System and Perceptual Learning Styles of Chinese,
Japanese, Korean, and Swedish Students. English Teaching 66, 141-162. [CrossRef]
21. Angela Passarelli, Eric Hall, Mallory Anderson. 2010. A Strengths-Based Approach to Outdoor and
Adventure Education: Possibilities for Personal Growth. Journal of Experiential Education 33, 120-135.
[CrossRef]
22. Larry Froman. 2010. Positive Psychology in the Workplace. Journal of Adult Development 17, 59-69.
[CrossRef]
23. David McNabb, Michael Webster. 2010. Qualities and practices of professional social work leadership
in an interdisciplinary mental health service: an action learning approach. Action Learning: Research and
Practice 7, 41-57. [CrossRef]
24. Thomas Andersson. 2010. Struggles of managerial being and becoming. Journal of Management
Development 29:2, 167-176. [Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]
25. Kellie Buckley-Walker, Trevor Crowe, Peter Caputi. 2010. Exploring identity within the recovery process
of people with serious mental illnesses. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal 33, 219-227. [CrossRef]
26. Richard E. Boyatzis, Melvin L. Smith, Ellen Van Oosten. 2010. Le coaching compatissant, allié de la
santé et du renouveau. Gestion 35, 41. [CrossRef]
27. Cam Caldwell. 2009. Identity, Self-Awareness, and Self-Deception: Ethical Implications for Leaders and
Organizations. Journal of Business Ethics 90, 393-406. [CrossRef]
28. Richard E. Boyatzis. 2009. Competencies as a behavioral approach to emotional intelligence. Journal of
Management Development 28:9, 749-770. [Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]
29. Davar Rezania, Tony Lingham. 2009. Coaching IT project teams: a design toolkit. International Journal
of Managing Projects in Business 2:4, 577-590. [Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]
30. Melvin L. Smith, Ellen B. Van Oosten, Richard E. BoyatzisCoaching for sustained desired change
145-173. [Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF] [PDF]
31. Davar Rezania. 2008. A framework for team coaching: using self‐discrepancy theory. Development and
Learning in Organizations: An International Journal 22:5, 12-13. [Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]
32. Anita Howard, Duncan Coombe. 2006. National level intentional change: a story of two countries. Journal
of Management Development 25:7, 732-742. [Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]
33. Richard E. Boyatzis. 2006. An overview of intentional change from a complexity perspective. Journal of
Management Development 25:7, 607-623. [Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]
34. Scott N. Taylor. 2006. Why the real self is fundamental to intentional change. Journal of Management
Development 25:7, 643-656. [Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]
35. Kleio Akrivou, Richard. E. Boyatzis, Poppy L. McLeod. 2006. The evolving group: towards a prescriptive
theory of intentional group development. Journal of Management Development 25:7, 689-706. [Abstract]
[Full Text] [PDF]
36. Heli Aramo-Immonen, Andrea Bikfalvi, Núria Mancebo, Hannu VanharantaProject Managers’
Competence Identification 17-31. [CrossRef]
Downloaded by Technická univerzita vo Zvolene At 18:41 30 October 2015 (PT)
... The scholarly exploration of the ideal self began primarily as a contrast to the actual or real self in forming a discrepancy (Higgins, 1987) or as one of the possible selves (Markus and Nurius, 1986). Theorizing has offered propositions on the constituent elements of the ideal self (Boyatzis and Akrivou, 2006). Organizational psychology research has alluded to an ideal work self in studies on identity development and change (Ashforth and Dukerich, 2001;Dutton et al., 2010;Ibarra, 1999). ...
... The objective of this paper is to update, enhance and refine the Boyatzis and Akrivou's (2006) model and develop a temporal understanding of the dynamics of the ideal self. We examine how key factors such as a person's understanding of their purpose, values, operating philosophy, and personal and social identities trigger emergent changes in the ideal self. ...
... The ideal self has been described as having components of one's purpose, personal values and philosophy, personal and social identity (Boyatzis and Akrivou, 2006). We review and refine these concepts in this section based on recent findings. ...
Article
Purpose The ideal self has had a place in management literature in recent years with reference to identity and role change. However, except for a JMD article in 2006, there has been little theorizing on the ideal self, which is often treated as a static construct. The purpose of this article is to update and refine the concept and explain the dynamic nature of the construct. Design/methodology/approach This conceptual paper is based on a review of the recent management and psychology literature related to the ideal self and its components. Findings The authors propose a dynamic theory of the emerging ideal self and delineate how its components evolve over time. Research limitations/implications The ideal self, or one's personal vision, is a major motivator of learning and change and the sustainability of such efforts. The time dynamic theory would encourage and guide longitudinal research using better variables and measures as well as help in conceptualizing the role of socialization, social identity and life/career stages. Practical implications With a better theory of the ideal self, trainers, consultants, coaches and teachers can help people update their deep sense of purpose and the sustaining driver of learning and change the ideal self. It could help people and organizations address a major determinant of engagement. Originality/value This theory offers a temporal understanding of how the ideal self can motivate learning and change at different life and career eras, which can help in designing future research on identity-related transitions.
... As a practical approach, we throw light on the importance of coaching with a focus on the ideal self. The ideal self is the aspirational personal vision of one's future [6]. Such coaching is an effective workplace application of this concept to improve the personal sustainability of oneself and others [7,8]. ...
... The implication of the above discussion is that renewal involves a movement from SNS to PNS activation. This constitutes what is referred to as movement across "tipping points" between two distinct psycho-physiological states comprised of distinct emotional, psychological, physiological, and neurological characteristics [5,6]. These two states, called the Positive Emotional Attractors (PEA) and Negative Emotional Attractors (NEA), are emotional in the sense that they are primarily affective in nature, and attractors in the sense that when plotted on a three-dimensional graph, they constitute a bounded region in phase-space towards which the forward trajectories of most nearby points converge. ...
... As we know that negative emotions have a stronger impact compared to positive emotions [38], people will tend to move into an NEA state over time if there is no effort to maintain the PEA state. Therefore, salient emotional events, or a series of such smaller events, that can act as a "tipping point" to move the person from PEA to NEA are required at regular intervals to help maintain the PEA state [6,9]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Research about stress management has traditionally focused on reducing possible stressors. However, more recent studies have highlighted the importance of renewal as an antidote to stress. The purpose of this paper is to provide a theoretical explanation of how renewal activities can invoke a psycho-physiological response that enhances personal sustainability. By drawing upon recent evidence from the fields of medicine, psychology, and management, we developed a conceptual understanding of how renewal activities are characterized by the movement of the individual from a negative to a positive psycho-physiological state. Such a transition happens over a tipping point, which people can enact in themselves and in others through emotional and social intelligence competencies. We illustrate this proposition with the specific case of ideal self-based coaching in the workplace that can enhance personal sustainability among both leaders and their team members.
... The deep affect creates a cognitive pattern for people's behavior and thoughts, the ideal self-acts like a visualization of our craved future achievements and accomplishments. The ideal self can become a motivational core and the generator for positive emotion (Boyatzis & Akrivou 2006) [12]. Occasionally, people choose an already exist individual to be a substantialized representation of their ideal self. ...
... The deep affect creates a cognitive pattern for people's behavior and thoughts, the ideal self-acts like a visualization of our craved future achievements and accomplishments. The ideal self can become a motivational core and the generator for positive emotion (Boyatzis & Akrivou 2006) [12]. Occasionally, people choose an already exist individual to be a substantialized representation of their ideal self. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
... Fear has negative effects, such as discomfort, anxiety, stress, and even depression (Leppin and Aro, 2009). Even so, fear will generate defensive emotions (Boyatzis and Akrivou, 2006) or defensive behaviour (McFarland, 1987). Terror management theory (TMT) suggests a dual defence model: proximal defence and distal defence (Pyszczynski et al., 1999). ...
... Therefore, fear is a self-protection mechanism (Loewenstein and Lerner, 2003;Plutchik, 1984). Fear has a vital role in dealing with threats (Leppin and Aro, 2009) because it can generate defensive emotions (Boyatzis and Akrivou, 2006) or defensive behaviour (McFarland, 1987) and motivate various behaviours to reduce anxiety (Greenberg et al., 1997). ...
... If a person has a dream and tries to sustain it by imagining to his or her future image, the dream can come true, according to Markus [17] and Wenger [18]. In addition, Boyatzis and Akrivou [19] stated that humans have been driven by imagination and the ability to visualize desired future images. For example, in many occupations, people must be inspired, keep inspired and also make other people inspired through the images which will become the motivation to move towards these future images. ...
... Meanwhile, ought-to self can be established a value that language learners have to have by learning a language. Unlike that of ideal self, the value on ought-to self is ascribed to learners not because they want to but because they have to as they need to represent obligation or responsibility (Boyatzis & Akrivou, 2006;Dörnyei et al., 2006). To illustrate, students who possess an ideal self while learning a language might visualise the ideal version of themselves and the societal value and economic benefit they are finally able to gain when they become fluent in the target language. ...
Article
Full-text available
Motivation has been long believed to be an essential contributor to students’ success in learning English. Many studies (e.g., Gbollie & Keamu, 2017; Wang, 2008; Zhang & Xiao, 2006) have been dedicated to investigating the relationship between motivation and success in learning English. While these studies have contributed to shedding light on the importance of motivation on students’ achievement in learning English, insufficient research has been conducted to address the concept of motivation itself, how the concept has evolved since the first time it was developed, and to what extent motivation affects both teachers and students’ performances at school. Owing to this fact, the present article seeks to discuss the notion and conceptualization of motivation by following several steps. First, the present paper took on arguments from prominent figures on the field of motivation to explore the essence of motivation and why it is essential for English as second or foreign language (ESL/EFL) learning. The conversation then built on the current theoretical framework of types and factors influencing student and teacher motivation. Following that, a plethora of discussions on the possible ways to increase student and teacher enthusiasm and motivation inside EFL classrooms were elaborated.
Chapter
It is not unusual for long-standing, submerged conflicts to exist and erupt in family firms. This chapter presents an illustrative case example of such a family conflict and how the consultant intervened to help them resolve it. Other problems a consultant may need to address that are not part of the case described are also noted.
Article
Purpose Leading with vision while granting employees autonomy is one effective organizational response to the demands of a dynamic external environment. The former is thought to align followers' behavior by providing guidance, the latter to increase variance in their behavior by relinquishing control; both exert beneficial but distinct effects on organizational performance. What has remained uncharted heretofore is how these leader behaviors shape their followers' cognition and, subsequently, yield improvements in performance. The authors argue that a leader's vision communication transforms followers' cognitive representation of their work. This not only enables them to specify their goals in alignment with the vision (goal clarity) but also to locate the meaning of their work within the bigger picture of the vision (construal level). By contrast, perceived autonomy in terms of power-sharing might directly affect followers' work engagement more narrowly. Design/methodology/approach The authors tested the model on a sample of 408 employees from eleven enterprises of a holding company. In the survey, employees reported perceived vision communication and autonomy provided by their leader. Furthermore, the authors assessed the employees' goal attainment. To capture how employees represent their daily work activities, the authors measured their construal level and their goal clarity. Findings The results show that both perceived vision communication and granted autonomy improve employees' goal achievement. Moreover, two processes mediate the relationship between vision communication and goal achievement in followers: first, specifying goals in terms of clarity; second, composing a higher-level mental construal of their work. In contrast, no mediation of empowering leader behaviors was found. Originality/value Better goal achievement through visionary leadership is therefore achieved through cognitive alignment of followers, while leader-granted autonomy acts as a motivational tool directly on performance.
Article
Purpose The purpose of this study was to examine predictors of leader flow (well-being) at work, based on conservation-of-resources theory. The authors also introduce the concept of fuel, the proactive and strategic use of physical wellness behaviors to generate the energy needed to manage personal stress, maximize performance and to thrive in life. Specifically, the authors examined the main and interactive relationships of leader self-reports of ideal self and fuel on flow at work. Design/methodology/approach Three different samples of leaders were surveyed online over a three-year period. Findings In all three samples, leaders high in self-reported fuel had strong, positive relationships between their ideal selves and flow at work. Leaders low in self-reported fuel had negative or non-significant relationships between ideal selves and flow. Practical implications Leaders can be coached to develop and use a healthy ideal self and to proactively engage in physical wellness behaviors, to enhance their workplace well-being. This coaching can be strengthened by emphasizing the connection between possessing an ideal self, and engaging in physical wellness behaviors, and resulting leader well-being. Originality/value This is the first study to examine and demonstrate that a combination of proactive leader wellness behaviors is critical to their experience of flow at work, an indicator of work-related well-being.
Chapter
The problem I will be addressing in this chapter is the new context in which business operates—the Anthropocene era—and its implications for, and demands on, leaders. A decade ago the surveys about awareness of sustainability and the importance of CSR provided mixed results: organizational leaders were slowly acknowledging its importance but were not yet acting on that understanding. Since then, the context has been changing dramatically, and the economic, social, and environmental circumstances have increasingly impacted business decisions and have caused organizations to re-write their carefully crafted plans. Covid-19 is just one, if the major, such circumstance. As if this were not enough, the voices of a new generation are clamoring for a new way of doing things. They don’t have the answers, but they are loudly requesting review of the operating system of our business and governments, and adjustment to the changed realities. They are calling for solutions and are taking an active role in effecting change. This chapter will address the invisible aspects that anchor current problems and suggest a possible lever of transformation. It is called the Sustainability Mindset. Based on her research over the past decade, the author will share the principles for a sustainability mindset, which constitute a scaffolding for developing the Thinking and Being dimensions which leaders can utilize to face the Anthropocene’s challenges. These leaders must start shaping the world we want to live in, and which we want to leave for succeeding generations. The main message of this chapter is that once we identify the elements of our mindset that continue to contribute to existing problems, we will be in a better position to change by developing new ways of acting and being in the world. And importantly, by starting the transformation with self, leaders will, in the words of Mohandas Gandhi‚ have the opportunity to be the change they want to see.
Article
Full-text available
This study examined the relationship of goal orientation and performance over a series of 2 challenging performance events. After providing performance feedback on the 1st event, the authors found that the relationship between a learning goal orientation and performance remained positive for the 2nd event, the relationship between a proving goal orientation and performance diminished from a positive to a nonsignificant level, and the relationship between an avoiding goal orientation and performance remained negative. Data analysis also indicated that the relationships between the 3 goal orientation dimensions and the performance event were differentially mediated by goal setting, self-efficacy, and effort.
Article
The relations of dispositional hope to various self-reported cancer-related coping activities were examined in 115 college women. Dispositionally high- as compared to low-hope women were more knowledgeable about cancer, and this relationship remained when the shared variances due to previous academic achievement, experience with cancer among family or friends, and positive and negative affectivity were removed. Additionally, high- as compared to low-hope women reported more hope-related coping responses in four separate imagined phases of cancer (prevention/risk, detection, temporal course, and impact), and these relationships remained when shared variances related to previous academic achievement, knowledge about cancer, experience with cancer, and negative affectivity were removed. Hope is discussed as means of maintaining a “fighting spirit” for coping with cancer.
Article
Presents an integrative theoretical framework to explain and to predict psychological changes achieved by different modes of treatment. This theory states that psychological procedures, whatever their form, alter the level and strength of self-efficacy. It is hypothesized that expectations of personal efficacy determine whether coping behavior will be initiated, how much effort will be expended, and how long it will be sustained in the face of obstacles and aversive experiences. Persistence in activities that are subjectively threatening but in fact relatively safe produces, through experiences of mastery, further enhancement of self-efficacy and corresponding reductions in defensive behavior. In the proposed model, expectations of personal efficacy are derived from 4 principal sources of information: performance accomplishments, vicarious experience, verbal persuasion, and physiological states. Factors influencing the cognitive processing of efficacy information arise from enactive, vicarious, exhortative, and emotive sources. The differential power of diverse therapeutic procedures is analyzed in terms of the postulated cognitive mechanism of operation. Findings are reported from microanalyses of enactive, vicarious, and emotive modes of treatment that support the hypothesized relationship between perceived self-efficacy and behavioral changes. (21/2 p ref)
Article
Through a practical application in the municipality of Rivière-au-Tonnerre on the Northern coast of the Gulf of Saint Lawrence, this article explores the advantages of using a vision of future desired conditions for planning adaptation to climate change. Such an approach was used in small “kitchen meetings” for the elaboration of an adaptation action plan that was approved by the municipal council in September 2013 and revised in November 2014. The impacts of climate hazards were discussed as obstacles to the desired future conditions expressed by participants. This approach allowed the planning of a number of adaptation options, to define roles and responsibilities of different actors involved and to articulate different plans that are either in existence or being developed, linking them with adaptation. Based on our observations and on other studies in the fields of social movements, psychology and risk perception, we suggest that the use of a vision in discussion meetings with local actors could increase their motivation by placing the avoidance of undesired situations in the context of reaching their aspirations, in other words by embedding a prevention problem in promotion logic. The expression of a vision of a desired future could allow them to express their values and could facilitate the “framing” of a plan in a way that is coherent with them. The visions could then also be used to frame awareness-raising activities along the values they express.