Content uploaded by Bernard Rimé
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Bernard Rimé on Sep 23, 2015
Content may be subject to copyright.
To appear in the first issue (January 2009) of Emotion Review, a new journal of ISRE
(International Society for Research on Emotion), SAGE Publishing.
Emotion Elicits the Social Sharing of Emotion:
Theory and Empirical Review
Bernard Rimé
University of Louvain (UCL) at Louvain-la-Neuve,
Belgium
2
Emotion Elicits the Social Sharing of Emotion:
Theory and Empirical Review
Abstract
This review demonstrates that an individualist view of emotion and regulation is
untenable. First, I question the plausibility of a developmental shift away from social
interdependency in emotion regulation. Second, I show that there are multiple reasons for
emotional experiences in adults to elicit a process of social sharing of emotion, and I review
the supporting evidence. Third, I look at effects that emotion sharing entails at the
interpersonal and at the collective levels. Fourth, I examine the contribution of emotional
sharing to emotion regulation together with the relevant empirical evidence. Finally, the
various functions that the social sharing of emotion fulfils are reviewed and the relevance of
the social sharing of emotion for emotion scientists is discussed.
3
Emotion Elicits the Social Sharing of Emotion:
Theory and Empirical Review
Emotion regulation in children is universally recognized as an interpersonal process
since the advent of attachment theory (Bowlby, 1969; Harlow, 1959). In contrast, prevailing
concepts for emotion regulation and emotional coping in adults are generally devoid of a link
with interdependency or social relationships (Dunahoo, Hobfoll, Monnier, Hulsizer, &
Johnson, 1998)—e.g., problem-focused and emotion-focused coping (Lazarus & Folkman,
1984), personal agency (Bandura, 1982), meaning-making coping (Park & Folkman, 1997),
and emotional expression and suppression (Gross, 2007). Current views thus imply that in the
course of their development, individuals eliminate social dependence. To become mature
adults, individuals should demonstrate their ability to regulate adaptation problems
independently of external intervention. Such an assumption fits prevailing values of Western
psychological science that emphasize autonomy and separation over relationality (Gilligan,
1982; Sampson, 1983). In psychological research, the healthy individual is largely seen as one
who is “self-contained, independent and self-reliant, capable of asserting himself and
influencing his environment” (Riger, 1993, p. 280). This view was termed “rugged
individualism” (Riger, 1993), or the Lone Ranger, “man against the elements” perspective
(Dunahoo et al., 1998). It pits man against the elements in a fight for survival. In such a
context, control and action are highlighted whereas social and communal aspects of coping
are ignored (Riger, 1993).
The present review demonstrates that in the field of emotion and regulation, a rugged
individualist view is untenable. I proceed as follows. First, taking a close look at the
developmental course, I review the knowledge and abilities acquired by children with respect
to emotion regulation, and I then question the plausibility of a developmental shift away from
4
social interdependency in this respect. Second, based upon an examination of the impact of
emotional experiences, I show that there are multiple reasons to predict that emotional
experiences in adults open upon social interaction and emotion sharing and I review the
evidence in support of this prediction. Third, I formulate theoretical propositions predicting
that any private emotional experience entails a number of consequences at the interpersonal
level, as well as at the collective level, and I review the supporting facts. In a final step, I
advance theoretical considerations on the contribution of emotional sharing to emotion
regulation and I examine the relevant empirical evidence. I conclude by briefly reviewing the
various functions that the social sharing of emotion fulfils and by discussing the relevance of
emotion scientists taking the social sharing of emotion into account.
Interpersonal Emotion Regulation in the Developmental Course:
Is a Shift Away from Interdependence Plausible?
The Developmental Course of Emotion Regulation
Interpersonal processes contribute to children’s emotion regulation in many different
ways. First, newborns signal all their discomfort and needs by emitting emotional signals.
Caregivers react to these signals with regularity and continuity thereby bringing the infants’
emotional arousal back to a baseline. Once an attachment is established between an infant and
a caregiver, the infant actively seeks the caregiver in a distressing situation. The caregivers’
presence, contact, and comfort efficiently appease the infant’s emotional state (e.g.,
Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978; Bowlby, 1969; Harlow, 1959; Sroufe & Waters,
1976).
Second, by appeasing children, caregivers also allow them to explore threatening
situations and to develop the cognitive tools necessary to prevent further emotional stress. In
this sense, caregivers’ actions not only constitute preconditions to children’s physical and
5
psychological survival, but also grant them access to the world. Infant rhesus monkeys raised
in social isolation chronically avoided incoming stimuli by upholding an encapsulated posture
and by sheltering their sense organs (Harlow, 1959).
Third, whereas mammals essentially make use of body contact for regulating their
offspring’s emotion, human beings also employ language. Long before children are able to
understand any speech, adults talk to them in distressing situations: they label the situation
and the resulting experience, clarify causes and effects, suggest remedies, formulate
regulation advice, and propose ways of coping. What is obvious from lay observation in this
regard was fully confirmed in recordings of parents’ and doctor’ speech in a routine pediatric
consultation in which children aged 9, 15, 18 or 21 months old underwent a vaccination shot
(Claes & Rimé, 2008). Childrens’ emotional consciousness thus emerges in the context of
adults’ words and sentences. Such a context gives children guidelines for event appraisal,
emotion labelling, and perception of bodily changes, emotional responding, regulation, and
coping.
Fourth, caregivers endow children with cultural information and meanings necessary
to protect them from the impact of the mysterious and hazardous world in which they live
(Becker, 1973) and to shelter them from the distressing impact of raw reality (Berger &
Luckmann, 1967). Such knowledge structures allow them to face the world with no fear and
even with relative confidence.
Fifth, adults’ conversations expose children to a social environment replete with
narration and stories. An average of one story is told every seven minutes of recorded adult
conversation in the presence of children (Bruner, 1990; Heath, 1983; Miller, 1982). Recorded
stories predominantly resulted from situations in which shared beliefs had been violated—i.e.,
from emotion-eliciting situations. While they are listening to adults’ accounts, children learn
to discriminate everyday events from exceptional ones, to discover both what should normally
6
occur and what might happen, to enlarge their theoretical knowledge about the world, to
exploit the negotiation of meaning that adult accounts entail, and to develop negotiating skills
in turn when they are facing events that did not evolve as they had anticipated (Bruner, 1990).
Sixth, children are actively trained to develop a capacity that is unique to mankind:
they learn to become narrators (Fivush, 1994; Fivush, Haden, & Reese, 1999). They gain the
capacity to inform their conspecifics about what happened to them, to exchange comments
about this experience, to inform them of what their needs are in this regard, and how they
should be taken care of. This capacity also allows children to negotiate with others ex post
facto the appraisal—the meaning and the implications of situations they encountered. By the
same token, children inform others of what may happen, of how one might react, of what the
consequences of such a situation may be, and of the actions to be taken to prevent similar
situations from occurring in the future. The benefit of the capacity for the development of a
shared knowledge on emotional situations and emotional experiences is thus immense.
What was Learned?
This quick review of the developmental course regarding emotion regulation
demonstrates the accumulation of regulation-relevant knowledge and abilities that children
acquire in this process. To sum up, children learn that interpersonal processes (1) have
soothing capacities with regard to emotional distress, (2) can help them to cognitively
confront and process distressing elements, which helps to reduce these elements’ distressing
power, (3) can help them to verbally apprehend and comprehend emotional experiences, (4)
generate cultural knowledge and meanings appropriate for protecting them from the
emotional impact of existential conditions, (5) endow them with stories and narratives replete
with information on what may happen and how one might negotiate the meaning of such
situations, (6) offer them the opportunity to share their emotional experiences with others, to
7
exchange comments and point of view on such experiences, and by the same token, to
contribute, in their turn, to the shared knowledge on emotion. In sum, children progressively
gain the capacity to enter their own experiences of failed expectations into their cultural
narrative, and to propose possible explanations or meanings for these experiences. They learn
to bring their own stories to the cultural construction that shelters them, and thus to join in the
continuous effort through which members of their community strive to consolidate the
securing dome under which they all live.
The Subsequent Developmental Course: Two Contrasted Hypotheses
Two hypotheses can now be contrasted with regard to the subsequent developmental
course. On the one hand, an individualist view predicts that this complex process involving
the development of interpersonal contacts, of social exchange, and of collective constructions
about emotional experiences vanishes at adolescence. To become mature adults, young people
abandon social interdependence and instead develop their ability to regulate adaptational
problems independently of any external intervention. On the other hand, I claim that the
various processes of interdependent emotion regulation in the developmental course do not
vanish. On the contrary, initially restricted to parental caregivers, children are progressively
exposed to a broader social circle during adolescence and adulthood. This circle essentially
involves elective attachment figures such as friends and romantic partners. From this
perspective, interpersonal contacts, social exchange, and collective constructions about
emotional experiences as they emerge in childhood simply represent consecutive
developmental products that pave the way to interdependent practices that lie at the very core
of adult emotion regulation. As is the case among children, interdependent processes buffer
adults’ emotions, stimulate adults’ cognitive processing of emotional experiences, increase
adults’ personal knowledge about emotion, and contribute to the strengthening of their
8
interpersonal relationships and social integration. Above all, interdependent processes nourish
the construction of a massive knowledge base and the production of meaning pursued
collectively in every culture since the dawn of human awareness.
A few simple questions can help decide which of these two views is more plausible.
Why would the developmental course involve such remarkable emotion regulation tools if the
fate of these tools is to vanish at the edge of adult life? Why would the developmental course
progressively grant children the tools appropriate for contributing to social knowledge, to
meaning production, and to cultural constructions about emotions if the fate of these tools is
to be substituted with the Lone Ranger attitude? And how would the thousands-years-old
cultural construction be pursued if the Lone Ranger attitude is systematically adopted among
adolescents? I first review successively theoretical and empirical arguments in support of my
alternative hypothesis. Thereafter, I examine the consequences that emotional experiences
entail among mature individuals. This will generate multiple theoretical arguments in support
of my hypothesis.
Consequences of Emotional Experiences
Let me first stress a major and often overlooked characteristic of human emotion. In
the animal world, emotional responding ends when emotional circumstances vanish. When a
predator pounces on its prey, intense emotional displays result in the surrounding flock. For
example, once a cheetah has caught an antelope and carries it away leaving the scene, the
other antelopes return to their grazing activities as if nothing had happened. No preoccupation
or trouble of any kind is manifested in their behavior after the incident. Human beings differ
diametrically from animals in this respect. In our species, any emotion leaves long-lasting
cognitive and social consequences. This is the case not only for important life events and for
traumatic emotional experiences, but also for current life emotions such as joy, anger, fear,
9
sadness, shame, and the like. Animal models that inspired emotion research (e.g., Cannon,
1917, 1927; Darwin, 1872; MacLean, 1949; Ekman & Friesen, 1972) are simply blind to
these consequences. Hereafter, I will focus first on the consequences that negative emotions
involve, and then on the consequences that positive emotions entail.
Consequences of Negative Emotions
Human beings are continuously engaged in goal-reaching activities (see Fig. 1a).
Negative emotions occur when circumstances interfere with these activities. If the pursuit of a
goal is substantially slowed down, or if it is blocked, this results in a negative emotional state
(e.g. sadness, anger, fear, shame, etc.) (Carver & Scheier, 1990, 2001). Yet, current views
generally ignore that such a state is no more than the visible part of an iceberg. A negative
emotional state fuels cognitive work. It also stimulates social exchange. In addition, it
activates the attachment system.
Negative emotions fuel cognitive work. Goal-reaching activities rely upon two
complementary capacities: (1) the capacity to predict states of the world and the consequences
of one’s action, and (2) the capacity to control situations and to bring forth states of the world
one had planned (see Fig. 1a). Exerting these capacities presumes a continuous reliance upon
a large knowledge base that individuals possess under various formats—schemas, models of
the world, implicit theories, world views, assumptions, and so forth. Dotted lines in Figure 1a,
represent how a feedback system links goal-reaching activities and the knowledge base. Thus,
when conditions interfere with the pursuit or attainment of a goal, components of this
knowledge base are invalidated. When elements of individuals’ experience disconfirm aspects
of their’ schemas, models, theories, or assumptions (Fig. 1b), a state of cognitive dissonance
results. This leads to the prediction that negative emotions necessarily are at the beginning of
cognitive efforts toward dissonance reduction (Festinger, 1957).
10
--------------------------------------
Insert Fig. 1
about here
--------------------------------------
Such reasoning was anticipated by classic authors. Hadley Cantril (1950) viewed
emotions as occurring when people confront events for which their assumptive world did not
prepare them. He states that in situations that challenge people’s world comprehension,
emotions supply a search for meaning. Similarly, Georges Kelly (1955) maintains that
emotions take place in moments when events “do not stick” with constructions that one
applies to them, thus compelling individuals to modify these constructions. Both Cantril and
Kelly view emotion as sparking cognitive work.
Research on conditions that initiate cognitive work led to very similar considerations.
Hastie (1984) demonstrates that deviation from an expected course of events offers the most
basic instigation of causal reasoning. Kruglanski (1996) views cognitive activity as typically
initiated when a discrepancy is perceived between an actual and a desired state. Martin and
Tesser (1989) argue that when the progression toward a goal is blocked or when a
discrepancy occurs between the current state of affairs and the expected situation, conditions
for the development of a ruminative cognitive activity are met. That such conditions relate to
emotion was made evident by Mandler (1984) who posits that two types of conditions can
cause emotion: when something which was not expected occurs, or when something which
was expected does not occur. Thus, conditions eliciting emotion overlap those eliciting a
cognitive search. Dissensions, surprises, unexpected events, incidents, accidents, and even
catastrophes all mobilize attention to the production of meaning. When forecasts fail, when
expectations are disconfirmed, when activities in progress are blocked, meaning production
emerges at its best (Weick, 1995).
11
Numerous data confirms that negative emotions fuel cognitive work. After a negative
emotional event, people engage in an active information search—as made clear in
laypersons’ behaviors after the 9/11 attacks (Rimé, Delfosse, & Corsini, 2005). Memories of
negative events also resurface into consciousness under the guise of intrusive thoughts,
mental images, or mental rumination (Martin & Tesser, 1989; Tait & Silver, 1989). Mild
laboratory-induced emotions readily elicited this effect (Horowitz, 1969; Horowitz and
Becker, 1971; for a review, see Horowitz, 1992). Everyday emotions elicit recurrent thoughts
in the subsequent days and weeks in 95% of cases (e.g., Rimé, Philippot, Boca & Mesquita,
1992). Traumatic episodes especially provoke recurrent intrusive recollections months or
even years after the event (e.g., Bawnes, Gorman & Sayer, 1991; McCammon, Durham,
Allison & Williamson, 1988; Tait & Silver, 1989; Weisaeth, 1989; Wilkinson, 1983). In the
aftermath of collective emotional events, such as the assassination of a nation’s president
(e.g., Curci, Luminet, Finkenauer, & Gisle, 2001), the death of a king (e.g., Finkenauer,
Luminet, Gisle, El-Ahmadi, Van der Linden, & Philippot, 1998), the death of a princes (e.g.,
Hornstein, Brown, Mulligan, 2003) or dramatic events such as 9/11 in New York (e.g.,
Luminet, Curci, Marsh, Wessel, Constantin, Gencoz, & Yogo, 2004), event-rehearsal
proliferates and is demonstrated to mediate the formation of the memory of the event
(Finkenauer, Gisle and Luminet, 1997). Altogether, these various observations support the
proposition that negative emotional experiences fuel cognitive work.
Negative emotions stimulate social comparison, narration, and conversation
Individualist biases lead people in our culture to be convinced that their sense organs are
essentially the source of their information. This is markedly incorrect. In the developmental
process, children continuously combine two sources of information: their sense organs and
people around them. It is this double reliance which allows them to label, categorize,
understand, and endow objects and events that they come across with meanings. That this
12
process does not vanish among adults was particularly well-captured by what Festinger
(1954) termed social comparison. Under this concept, Festinger addressed the fact that
individuals are continuously motivated to assess their own perceptions and opinions. To this
aim, they compare their views with those of the individuals around them. Through this
process, individuals reach agreement upon world views, values, opinions, concepts, and so
forth. Their social consensus produces a social reality which is as objective as objects
themselves. Social comparison is especially at work when people lack objective standards or
when they face a confusing experience. As evidenced by the cognitive work they elicit,
emotional experiences typically constitute confusing experiences and they are thus expected
to stimulate social comparison.
Schachter (1959) demonstrates that being exposed to an emotional condition elicits a
person’s motivation to seek social contact. In his classic studies, participants who were
anxious at the prospect of being administered electric shocks expressed a preference for
waiting in the company of other persons whereas those for whom the threat was low preferred
to wait alone. This “stress and affiliation” effect generated considerable research (Cottrell &
Epley, 1977). Schachter himself adopted a social comparison explanation of the effect. In his
view, individuals facing stress would attempt to reduce the elicited anxiety by verbally
interacting with others sharing the same fate thereby using others as a gauge for evaluating
their own emotional state. Schachter’s view leads to the inference that negative emotions will
fuel verbal exchange and social comparison.
Schachter was not alone in viewing emotion as opening up social communication.
Bruner (1990) regards narration as playing a central role in the context of emotional
circumstances. In his perspective, no story can emerge as long as events fit common sense
expectations. Motives for narration come forward only when expectations are violated—
which overlaps with the definition of emotional circumstances adopted in this article.
13
According to Bruner, a story aims to provide unusual circumstances with a form which makes
sense and renders them understandable. It is particularly well-suited to bridging the
exceptional with the ordinary. Thus, Bruner said, if you come across an exception to what is
normally the rule, and if you ask people to explain what happened, their answer generally
takes the form of a story. A motive or an intention is brought forth and a possible world is
sketched in a context in which the encountered exception makes sense. In line with this view,
it can be posited that a negative emotional experience stimulates the production of narration.
In his theory of social representations, Moscovici (1984) stressed that members of a
group continually nourish conversations in social life. In doing so, they converge towards
common representations and shared knowledge. Thus, conversation is the tool by which
people expand an implicit stock of images and ideas which they then take for granted.
Unfamiliar or atypical objects or events instantly trigger a feeling of chaos among individuals
because such objects or events shatter the basis upon which mutual comprehension rested.
Moscovici viewed the production of social representations as the optimal means for absorbing
such threats. Through conversations, social thinking transforms unfamiliar objects or events
into social representations. Emotional events, therefore, fit the conditions described by the
theory of social representations. They question what was taken for granted and they compel
people to reconsider the grounds upon which their beliefs rested. The theory of social
representations thus leads to infer that emotions stimulate conversation in social life.
Negative emotion, distress, and attachment. When a condition interferes with the
pursuit or attainment of a goal, the resulting invalidation of elements of the person’s
knowledge base extends to the self-system who coordinated components of this knowledge
base with plans and actions (see Fig. 1b). As a result, the self is weakened. Its “deflation” is
manifested in reduced self-confidence, reduced feelings of self-efficacy and lower self-
esteem. In everyday life, individuals behave in a context of apparent order and meaning. By
14
disconfirming expectations, models, and world views, negative emotions undermine this
delicate architecture. Traumatic situations have been shown to be particularly deleterious in
this regard (Epstein, 1973, 1990; Janoff-Bulman, 1992; Parkes, 1972). But any emotion has
some impact on this symbolic architecture because emotion develops precisely at its
fissures—or where things are unexpected or get out of control (Corsini & Rimé, 2007;
Epstein, 1973, 1990). By making fissures apparent, emotion makes individuals feel the
weakness of the construction, thereby eliciting a wave of collateral emotional feelings:
anxiety, insecurity, helplessness, estrangement, alienation, loss of self-esteem, and so forth. In
sum, besides the obvious emotional state itself, negative experiences induce a subtle effect—
an effect which is most often overlooked. Negative experiences entail a temporary
destabilization of the person, a generalized distressing condition that a person is highly
motivated to reduce.
Though he favored a social comparison explanation of his “stress and affiliation”
effect, Schachter (1959) also considered other motives to be at play. He mentions emotional
support, or direct distress reduction through the presence of others. Shaver and Klinnert
(1982) advocate that the work on stress and affiliation yield data fully consistent with findings
from research on attachment initiated by Bowlby (1969). This research (e.g., Ainsworth,
Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978; Sroufe and Waters, 1976) indeed provided ample evidence that
both primate and human infants seek contact with others at times of uncertainty and distress.
Bowlby (1969) himself defines attachment as a resource that children activate when in
distress. Shaver and Klinnert (1982) note that this early form of affiliation was found to serve
two distinct but related functions: direct anxiety reduction and increased cognitive clarity.
They concluded that for both human and nonhuman primates the relationship between distress
and affiliation is maintained throughout life. This leads me to contend that the generalized
15
distress that negative emotions produce motivate adults to search for emotional support and to
turn to their attachment figures for this purpose.
Conclusions. Negative emotional conditions leave a large wake of cognitive and
social consequences behind them. Negative emotions initiate a state of cognitive dissonance
and are thus at the beginning of important cognitive efforts toward dissonance reduction.
Ample data already exist in support of the general hypothesis that emotions fuel cognitive
work. But, intrapersonal manifestations in the context of information search, mental
rumination, intrusive thoughts, or search for meaning do not close the list of the
consequences that negative emotions entail. For multiple reasons, negative emotions stimulate
social interaction in many forms: social comparison, story-telling and narration, conversation,
and last but not least, a search for emotional support through contact with attachment figures,
or their elective substitutes in adulthood.
Substantial evidence shows that mentally or socially reactivating the memory of an
emotional experience rekindles response components of this experience, such as mental
images, associated feelings, bodily sensations, and physiologic arousal (e.g., Pennebaker &
Beall, 1986; Rimé, Noël & Philippot, 1991b; Schaeffer & Philippot, 2005). Thus, talking
about a negative episode should involve an aversive experience and individuals should avoid
doing so. Yet, the theoretical reasoning has opened up strong arguments in the opposite
direction. Before examining what the relevant empirical evidence reveals in this regard, let me
turn to positive emotions.
Consequences of Positive Emotions
Positive emotions result from circumstances which facilitate goal-reaching activities.
They develop when the pursuit of a goal is substantially accelerated and is attained sooner
than anticipated (Carver & Scheier, 1990, 2001). When this occurs, the subject’s knowledge
16
base is proved efficient and his or her theories and models are supported. By the same token
(Fig. 1b), the self-system is strengthened and manifests an enhanced self-confidence, stronger
feelings of self-efficacy and a higher self-esteem. Positive emotions thus enhance a subject’s
well-being in two different ways: (1) by the increase of the level of positive affect that they
entail and (2) by the positive feedback that a successful experience brings to the knowledge
base and the self.
As is the case for past negative episodes, re-accessing past positive episodes revives
related feelings and sensations. Consequently, thinking back or talking about a past positive
emotional experience elicits pleasurable emotional feelings. This leads up to the hypothesis
that people are motivated to mentally ruminate upon positive episodes that they have
experienced. Experimental studies conducted long ago by Horowitz and colleagues
(Horowitz, 1969, 1975; Horowitz & Becker, 1971, 1973) support this expectation. They
exposed participants to either a positive emotion-inducing movie or to a negative one. In a
post-film observational session, participants had to signal the occurrence of movie-related
thoughts or mental images while completing a mental task. Compared to a neutral movie
condition, participants who had seen an emotionally-arousing film evidenced significantly
more frequent thoughts and mental images. The respective impact of emotionally-arousing
positive and negative movies was perfectly comparable in this regard. That positive emotional
experiences elicit mental rumination as frequently as negative emotional experiences was later
confirmed in several studies in which the recall of autobiographic emotional episodes was
investigated (Rimé, Mesquita, Philippot, & Boca, 1991a; Rimé, Philippot, Boca, & Mesquita,
1992). To illustrate, as was the case for episodes of sadness, fear, or anger, episodes of joy or
of love-affection were ruminated in more than 90% of occurrences (Rimé et al., 1991a, study
2). Thus, there is little doubt that positive emotional experiences fuel subjects’ thinking.
17
Langston (1994) views positive emotional episodes as opportunities on which to seize
or “capitalize”—he notes that Bryant (1989) used the term savoring in this respect. Langston
adds that capitalizing on positive emotional experiences can be achieved by seeking social
contacts and letting others know about the event. These situations would offer another
opportunity to enhance one’s positive affect. In two different studies, Langston confirms that
communicating positive events to others was indeed associated with an enhancement of
positive affect far beyond the benefits due to the valence of the positive events themselves.
Gable, Reis, Impett and Asher (2004) confirm these findings. In addition, they observe that
close relationships in which one’s partner typically responds enthusiastically to capitalization
were associated with higher relationship well-being (e.g., intimacy, daily marital satisfaction).
Thus, sharing positive emotions not only boosts individuals’ positive affect, it also enhances
their social bonds.
To sum up, for multiple reasons, negative emotional experiences stimulate social
interaction and communication of the experience. Such a social process develops despite the
fact that talking about a negative episode involves a markedly aversive experience. As regards
positive emotional experiences, such restraint is irrelevant. On the contrary, the research
suggests that positive emotions are savored and capitalized upon, that a good way to achieve
this capitalization is to talk about them, and that doing so is beneficial to existing links with
those with whom such an experience is shared.
Conclusions
The theoretical analysis of the consequences of negative and positive emotions leads
to predict the following. For very different reasons, both negative and positive emotions
stimulate important social interaction. More specifically, I predict that, independently of the
type or valence of the emotions involved, an emotional experience elicits the social sharing of
18
this experience. Because the consequences of emotions examined in this section hold
independently of the specificities of the personal characteristics of those who experience an
emotion, this prediction should hold whatever the subjects’ gender, education, or culture. In
the next section, relevant empirical data are reviewed.
The Social Sharing of Emotion:
Empirical Evidence
I first define what the social sharing of emotion encompasses. Next I review basic
evidence that emotions are socially shared. I then turn to the exceptions: When is the social
sharing of emotion eluded? Finally, I examine the temporal evolution of the social sharing of
an emotional experience.
What is the Social Sharing of Emotion?
At a street intersection in town, a car hits two youngsters riding a motorbike. The two
severely wounded victims are lying down. In a minute, the crowd of gaping people around
them is so dense that the ambulance stops at a distance and the medics can hardly get through.
Particularly striking is the following fact. Most witnesses are using their cellular phone. They
are reporting the emotional scene on-line to a close person. This anecdotal observation
illustrates the fact that people who experience an emotion then evidence an imperious need to
share it and to talk about it. Before the advent of cellular phones, this need was first expressed
on-site. Witnesses talked to one another, and when back home, they again talked about the
scene with their intimates. These various situations illustrate what I call “the social sharing of
emotion", a process that takes place in the minutes, hours, days, even weeks and months—and
sometimes years, or even an entire life—following an emotional episode. The process entails
a description of the emotional event in a socially-shared language by the person who
19
experienced it to another (Rimé, 1989; Rimé et al., 1991a). In its full form, the social sharing
of emotion occurs in discourse, when individuals communicate openly with one or more
persons about the circumstances of the emotion-eliciting event and about their own feelings
and emotional reactions. In attenuated forms, it consists of latent or indirect communications
in which the addressee is present only at a symbolic level, as is the case with letters or diaries.
Social Sharing of Emotion: Basic Evidence
Autobiographic data. The first studies which tested this prediction relied upon
autobiographic data. Participants were instructed to recall and briefly describe an emotional
experience from their recent past corresponding to a specified basic emotion (e.g., joy, anger,
fear, shame, or sadness). They then answered questions about their sharing of this episode:
Did they talk about the episode with others? With whom? How long after the emotion? How
often? (Mesquita, 1993; Rimé, et al., 1991a, 1991b; Vergara, 1993). Eight independent
studies of this type were reviewed (Rimé et al., 1992). They involved 1384 emotional
episodes reported by 913 respondents ranging in age from 12 to 72 years. The data showed
that, according to the study, 88 to 96 % of the collected episodes were socially shared. The
modal pattern for an emotion sharing was to be initiated early after the episode, with the first
sharing round taking place during the day the episode happened in about 60% of the cases
across studies. Modal responses also indicated that participants talked about the event
“several times” with “several persons”, suggesting emotion sharing to be a repetitive process
involving a variety of targets. Extent of sharing (i.e., number of repetitions and number of
recipients) was positively correlated with the rated intensity of the emotional experience: The
more intense the emotion was, the more participants talked about it. Thus these early data
strongly supported the view that “every emotion tends to be socially shared”. Obviously,
20
however, the ex-post facto nature such data were at risk of being unduly inflated. Respondents
might indeed have selectively remembered episodes that they did socially share.
Diary studies. Diary methods were then used in order to reduce the time elapsed
between the emotional episode and data collection. Participants completed a short
questionnaire every night before going to bed. They briefly described the event that had
affected them most that day, they rated it on emotion scales and they then answered questions
about this event, including questions concerning social sharing. This procedure reduced risks
of selective and reconstructive biases and allowed collecting data for current life emotion of
low or moderate intensities. Several studies of this type were conducted (Rimé, Philippot,
Finkenauer, Legast, Moorkens, & Tornqvist, 1994). Previous autobiographic studies
consistently showed that an emotion is shared on the day it happens in about 60% of the
cases. Consequently, diary-reported daily emotional episodes were expected to be shared
before the next night for approximately 60% of them. The findings were precisely in line with
this prediction. To illustrate, in one of the diary studies, emotional episodes were collected
from 34 participants during 14 consecutive nights (Rimé et al., 1994, study 2). On average,
58% of the 461 events reported were shared the day they happened. With the exception of a
marked trend indicating less sharing for cases of shame, this result did not vary as a function
of specific emotions. To conclude, findings from diary studies in which the interval between
the episode and its recall is at maximum one day, replicated those from autobiographic studies
involving intervals of weeks, months, or even years. Thus, it does not seem that findings from
autobiographic studies can be explained by selective or by reconstructive memory biases:
social sharing behaviors appear to be a typical consequence of exposure to emotion, even if
the emotion is mild.
Follow-up procedures. This conclusion was further tested using “follow-up”
procedures. Having the investigators “pre-select” a target event prevented any selection bias
21
by respondents. Participants were contacted when they were exposed to some important
emotional event and they were then followed up for several weeks. In six such studies, the
target emotional episodes involved respectively (a) bereavement, (b) an important
examination; (c) first blood donation; (d) attending the dissection of a human corpse among
medical students; (e) performing first dissection; (f) first delivery for young mothers (Rimé,
Finkenauer, Luminet, Zech, & Philippot, 1998). In each of these studies, follow-up
questionnaires assessed the occurrence of social sharing immediately after the event and again
at various time intervals. All the follow-up studies offered evidence consistent with earlier
findings. Social sharing occurred during the week following the episode at rates closely
matching those of autobiographic studies (i.e., around 90% of the cases across studies). With
the exception of blood donation, which is a relatively minor emotional episode, the proportion
of episodes that still elicited sharing during the second week was virtually the same as in the
first week. Marked decreases were then generally observed in the following weeks or months.
Steeper extinction slopes were found for less intense emotional events. Altogether, these
findings in which selective memory biases were precluded confirmed that people who were
exposed to emotion engage in social sharing.
Experimental data. As this proposition clearly involves a causal relation,
experimental tests needed to be conducted. Three conditions of emotional intensity were
induced in the lab by exposing participants to short movie excerpts (3 minutes) of respectively
high, moderate, or low emotional intensity (Luminet, Bouts, Delie, Manstead, & Rimé, 2000).
These movies were proved comparable for the type of emotion elicited. Volunteer students
participated with a friend. One member of each pair was randomly assigned to view one of
the movies while the other completed a filler task in another room. After the movie, the
target-participant and friend were brought together in a waiting room and left alone. Their
conversation was unobtrusively tape-recorded. Independent judges later rated the recordings
22
for time of movie-related talk and for the proportion of words referring to the movie. Three
independent experiments based upon this model showed that compared to participants in the
two other conditions, those exposed to the highly emotional movie talked more about their
experience. To illustrate, nearly 40% of the words spoken by these participants referred to the
movie, as compared to less than 5% of the words spoken by those in the other two conditions.
Thus a 3-minute emotional movie suffices to elicit social sharing. In all three studies, the
moderately intense movie failed to elicit more sharing than the non-emotional control one,
which suggested that social sharing occurs only when an intensity threshold is exceeded.
Additional analyses revealed that individual differences in emotional reactions to the movies
were markedly correlated with the extent of social sharing: the more participants were
emotionally aroused by the movies, the more they talked about it in the waiting room. In one
of the studies, participants came back to the lab two days later and rated their sharing in this
elapsed period. The results paralleled the waiting room findings, thus supporting their
ecological validity.
Altogether, these experiments demonstrated that being exposed to an emotion elicits
the social sharing of this emotion, thereby confirming autobiographic, diary, and follow-up
findings. In a recent study, social sharing of dreams was examined (Curci & Rimé, 2007).
Data showed that the intensity of the emotion experienced in a dream was the better predictor
of the extent to which the dream was socially shared.
Generality of the social sharing of emotion. Studies consistently show that the social
sharing process is initiated early after the eliciting event, that it is generally repetitive, and
that it is addressed to several target persons. The generality of these social consequences of
emotion is largely supported (for reviews, see Rimé et al., 1992, 1998). Social sharing of
emotion occurrs independently of age and gender. Contrary to common stereotypes, women
were not found to be more prone than men to share their emotions. In addition, neither the
23
type of basic emotion involved nor the emotional valence affected the proportion or extent of
sharing. Fear, anger, and sadness were shared as widely as happiness or love. However, as
will be discussed later, shame and guilt were the exceptions and were shared to a lesser degree
(Finkenauer & Rimé, 1998a). Despite arguments that putting emotion into words might be a
function of verbal abilities or education, the propensity to share emotions was comparable
whether people held a university degree or had an elementary school education (Curci, Rimé,
Gisle, & Baruffol, 2007; Zech, Rimé, De Soir, Versporten, & Van Oyen, 2007). Cross-
cultural comparison also failed to evidence significant differences for the rate of sharing,
which occurred with comparable importance across European samples (e.g., Belgium, France,
the Netherlands, the Basque Country in Spain, Italy), across samples of immigrants in Europe
(from Surinam, or Turkey) (Mesquita, 1993), or across populations as diverse as Asian (South
Korea, Singapore, India, Japan) (e.g., Singh-Manoux & Finkenauer, 2001; Yogo & Onoe,
1998) and North American (e.g. Rimé, Yogo, & Pennebaker, 1996). Yet, the way people
shared fluctuated broadly. Latency, recurrence, or target all varied considerably with culture
(e.g., Singh-Manoux & Finkenauer, 2001).
Intensity of emotion and extent of sharing. The notion of social sharing of emotion
opens upon the prediction that the more an episode is emotional, the more frequently it will be
shared. Thus, a positive linear correlation is expected between the intensity of emotion
elicited by emotional episodes and the extent to which episodes are shared. Studies of
autobiographic recall involving nine independent samples from eight nationalities offer a
moderate support of the predicted relationship (Rimé et al., 1998). Observed correlations were
significant and positive, but in the low range—Pearson r’s ranged from .21 to .35. Laboratory
studies in which error variance was better controlled yielded considerably higher coefficients.
Data from eight laboratory studies relating the intensity of laboratory-induced emotion and
the extent of its sharing in the next few days indeed provided coefficients ranging from .30 to
24
.63. The curve relating intensity of emotion and extent of sharing manifested a step-function
rather than a linear one. Extent of sharing increased at moderate levels of emotion and
reached a ceiling at higher levels. As I will discuss later, this ceiling effect might result from
the fact that listeners limited their availability to the sharing process.
Targets of social sharing. Who are the targets that people select to share their
emotions with? Interesting trends emerge from the comparison of age groups (Rimé, et al.
1991a, 1991b, 1992; Rimé, Dozier, Vandenplas, & Declercq, 1996). Children aged 6 and 8
who had been exposed to an emotion-eliciting narrative later manifested virtually no sharing
with peers in their classroom. Yet, most of them did share the episode with their father and
mother when they were back home. Other family members were rarely involved in the
sharing of emotions in this age group.
Preadolescents (8 to 12 years old) were surveyed after a night game at summer camp
in which, according to the children’s ratings, the game induced a moderate intensity emotion
in them. They went back home the next day. Three days later, parents’ ratings of their child’s
sharing showed that the night game had been shared by 97% of the participants. Parents
clearly emerged as the privileged sharing partners —mothers in 93% of the cases, and fathers
in 89% of the cases. Siblings served as recipients in 48% of the cases, best friends in 33%,
peers in 37%, and grandparents in only 5%.
Among adolescents (12–18 years old), family members, and predominantly parents,
were by far the most often mentioned sharing targets both among boys and girls. Friends were
the recipients of about one-third of emotional sharing. Romantic partners were rarely
mentioned, either because there was none, or because they were not yet eligible as sharing
partners. But, as age cohorts became older, friends—including girlfriends, boyfriends, and
female best friends—became increasingly important. Other people were rarely mentioned.
25
Among young adults (18–33 years old), the role of family members was considerably
reduced, especially among males. As a counterpart for both genders, spouses and partners
emerged as important actors on the social sharing stage whereas friends maintained the same
importance as in the adolescents’ data. The role of family members decreased again in
middle-aged adults (40–60 years old) perhaps in part because parents are no longer available.
Additionally, a considerable drop in the importance of friends occurred for male adults, but
not for females. In this age group, spouses and partners markedly predominated as sharing
targets. In men in particular, the spouse or partner was virtually an exclusive target among
more than three-quarters of them. Data collected on elderly people (65–95 years old) simply
replicated this pattern. From adulthood on, spouses and partners became the main sharing
targets (over 75%), followed by family members (over 30%), and friends (about 20%). As
previously mentioned, other categories, such as strangers or professionals, were rarely
addressed (less than 5% of the cases).
In sum, across age groups, targets of social sharing were consistently found to be
intimates (i.e., parents, brothers, sisters, friends, or spouse/partner). Non-intimates hardly
played a role in the sharing process. Professionals (e.g., priests, physicians, teachers,
psychologists, etc.), unfamiliar, or unknown persons were unlikely to be selected for this role.
In both genders, a remarkable evolution was observed with age in selected targets.
Attachment figures (father and mother) are the focus of social sharing of emotion in
childhood. As was predicted in the first section of this review, sharing targets progressively
evolve from parental figures to newly-elected attachment figures, such as friends among
adolescents, friends and spouses/partners among young adults, and predominantly
spouses/partners among mature adults.
These conclusions are somewhat qualified by observations regarding professional
rather than personal emotional episodes. Hospital nurses working in emergency units or in
26
coronary care units were asked with whom they shared emotional episodes that occurred in
their work (Laurens, 2000). Results revealed that in this case colleagues and other
professionals were the primary sharing partners. Intimates were also involved in the sharing
process, but were addressed to a lesser extent. In another study, police, medical and
psychosocial intervention personnel involved in a rescue operation after a deadly train crash
were surveyed. Though they had shared their emotions about the catastrophe primarily with
their spouses/companion (61%), the primary sharing partners were their colleagues and
members of their team for 39% of them (Zech, Ucros, Rimé, & De Soir, 2002). Colleagues
can thus play an important role in sharing after professional situations, perhaps because they
are seen as more knowledgeable.
Conclusions. Data collected through a variety of methods confirm my prediction that
both positive and negative emotional experiences elicit a process of social sharing of these
experiences. Individuals narrated their experiences in an overwhelming proportion of cases,
and their modal response was that they shared the same episode several times with several
people. As predicted, this behavior was observed independent of emotional valence or of the
type of emotion. In addition, social sharing manifestations occurred at a comparable rate
among people varying in gender, education, and culture. In line with these predictions, data
demonstrated that the intensity of an emotional experience increased the frequency with
which it was shared.
The generality of these findings suggests that emotional sharing is a basic component
of emotion, together with appraisal, expression, bodily changes, action tendencies, and the
like. Data unambiguously confirm that emotional interdependence does not vanish with age.
From adolescence on elected attachment figures succeeded natural attachment figures in the
social sharing of emotion. Parental caregivers were found to be the privileged sharing partners
among children. Adolescents progressively turned to elected attachment figures, with friends
27
becoming important sharing targets. Among young adults, romantic partners became the main
focus. Finally, among both male and female adults, spouses or romantic partners—core
figures of adult attachment (e.g., Mikulincer & Shaver (2007)—were the predominant sharing
target. These observations radically challenge the Lone Ranger individualist view which
predicted that at adolescence individuals would forevermore abandon social interdependence
in emotion regulation.
When is the Social Sharing of Emotion Eluded?
Despite empirical arguments that support the generality of the social sharing of
emotion, there are, of course, exceptions. At least three types of circumstances can be seen to
restrain the social sharing of emotion. First, when an emotional episode elicits self-conscious
emotions such as shame and guilt, these feelings counteract the propensity to share emotion.
This observation follows from the fact that the phenomenal experience characterizing such
emotions is a wish to hide or disappear (Lewis, 2000). People who experience shame or guilt
are thus expected to actively refrain from further exposing themselves and they avoid
narrating these experiences to others.
A second situation in which a phenomenal experience can counteract the propensity to
share an emotion is found in extremely intense or traumatic emotions. On the one hand, in
line with the fact that more intense emotional experiences are shared more extensively,
extremely intense and traumatic emotional experiences should also be shared more
extensively. On the other hand, the notion of trauma (e.g., Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder
(PTSD) as described in DSM-IV, American Psychiatric Association, 1994) implies that the
cues associated with a traumatic experience elicit avoidance. Such a response will most likely
interfere with the propensity to share emotions. This leads to expect the occurrence of a
behavioral compromise between these conflicting motivational forces. Whereas individuals
28
would talk frequently about a high intensity or traumatic episode through which they have
lived, they would refrain from mentioning the most sensitive issues, thus leaving definite
blanks or holes in their narration.
Finally, although emotion elicits a need for social sharing, the social environment
might not be receptive. In particular, members of the social environment may limit or even
deny the social sharing of emotion for their own protection. This should occur when the
episode to be shared involves elements that are likely to elicit harmful emotions among
listeners. I now turn to the empirical findings relevant to each of these three issues.
Shame and guilt. Investigating emotional experiences that people keep secret tested
the hypothesis that shame and guilt play a critical role in restraining the social sharing of
emotion. In two different studies, participants answered questions either about emotional
memories that they had socially shared, or about emotional memories that they had kept
secret (Finkenauer & Rimé, 1998a). The assumption was that the participants would answer
questions about secrets provided that (1) absolute anonymity would be granted, and
(2) participants would in no way be asked to reveal their secret. The results consistently
showed that neither the intensity of the emotion felt when the event occurred, nor the intensity
of the emotion still felt when responding, discriminated shared from non-shared episodes.
Also, non-shared emotional experiences were no more or no less negatively valenced than
shared ones. However, in line with my prediction, non-shared emotional episodes elicited
more intense feelings of shame and guilt than shared ones in each of two studies. In addition,
emotional appraisal ratings revealed that emotional experiences that were kept secret involved
greater personal responsibility for the event than shared experiences. Finally, data revealed
that non-shared experiences were initially associated with attempts to hide one’s feelings or
emotions, a tendency typical of shamed persons (e.g., Tangney, 1991). These findings thus
support my prediction that when an emotional episode elicits self-conscious emotions such as
29
shame and guilt, these emotional feelings counteract the powerful propensity to share
emotion.
Extreme intensity and traumatic emotions. Findings converged in showing that
traumatic emotions are frequently shared (e.g., Pennebaker & Harber, 1993; Sydor &
Philippot, 1996) and that approximately 80% of trauma victims manifested the need to share
their experience (e.g., Ersland, Weisaeth, & Sund, 1989; Mitchell & Glickman, 1977). But,
what about blanks or gaps in social sharing? A survey of a large group (N = 1,027) of adults
exposed at various degrees to a catastrophic gas-pipe explosion in 2004 in Belgium provide
relevant findings (Zech, & al., 2007). In this cohort, frequency of sharing about the disaster
was a linear function of the intensity of PTSD symptomatology. However, when sharing
modalities were examined in more details, marked differences discriminated between
participants who fulfilled the criteria for PTSD from those who did not. The former
mentioned much more frequently than the latter aspects (1) that they had never shared, and (2)
that they did not want to share. Thus, in conformity with the prediction, people who were
most affected by an event shared their emotion as often as those who were less affected. Yet,
they also evidenced marked proclivities toward not revealing at least some aspects of their
traumatic experience. It is probable that these undisclosed aspects are particularly critical with
regard to the post-traumatic symptomatology they developed. Future investigation should
explore further the content of undisclosed aspects of traumatic exposure. More generally,
studies of the social sharing of emotion should focus more on the aspects of traumatic
experience which people keep secret.
Social constraint. Studies of responses to collective trauma found enhanced levels of
social sharing and of manifestations of solidarity in the affected community within a period of
two to three weeks following the traumatic event. After this emergency stage, a sharp drop in
social sharing is generally observed. Yet, solidarity is still manifested at a high level for
30
another month. Two months after the event, solidarity begins a slow decline and spontaneous
sharing disappears from the scene (Pennebaker & Harber, 1993; Gortner & Pennebaker, 2003;
Collins, 2004). At this stage, individuals who are still inclined to share their experience find
themselves rejected. Thus, in their study of the Loma Prieta earthquake, Pennebaker & Harber
(1993) observed important social sharing in the week following the disaster. As time went on,
survivors still wanted to talk, but they did not want to listen to others sharing their
earthquake-related thoughts and emotions about the disaster anymore. At this point, they
started wearing t-shirts that read, “Thank you for not sharing your earthquake experience.”
Thus, receptiveness can work against the propensity to share emotions, a phenomenon which
Pennebaker & Harber (1993) labeled “social constraint.”
Both seriously ill patients and victims of life eventsoften find themselves confronted
with social constraint. In particular, cancer patients claim that expressing their illness-related
issues are particularly problematic to them and disturbing to others around them. Only half of
these patients were satisfied with the social support they received (Peters-Golden, 1982;
Wortman & Dunkel-Schetter, 1979; for a review see Lepore & Revenson, 2006). Similarly, a
majority of victims report unhelpful behaviors from their social environment. They were
exposed to minimization, rude remarks, and the like, which discouraged them from sharing
(Lehman, Ellard, & Wortman, 1986). In interviews with patients in chronic pain, lack of
understanding and lack of listening from their intimates were mentioned among the most
common difficulties in their lives (Herbette, 2002; Herbette & Rimé, 2004). Data confirmed
that nurses, as well as patients’ families, try to avoid or to limit geriatric patients’ discussions
about death (Kastenbaum & Aisenberg, 1972). A majority of healthy people said that they
would do their best to cheer up cancer patients if confronted by them (Peters-Golden, 1982).
Yet, they generally viewed emotional expression as non-adaptive for suffering persons, and
they considered that sharing was not beneficial to them “because it might shatter their will to
31
fight” (Peters-Golden, 1982; Wortman & Dunkel-Schetter, 1979). It is clear that although the
need for sharing emotion is particularly compelling among patients suffering from severe
illness, potential listeners often exert social constraints in this regard. People who are
particularly in need of social support and social integration are precisely exposed to social
signals in the opposite direction.
Conclusions. The findings confirm that a narrator’s experience of shame and guilt is
the source of restraint in social sharing and favors secrecy. The findings also confirm that a
traumatic response to an extremely intense emotional episode is the source of both frequent
sharing as well as blanks or gaps in this sharing. Finally, many findings confirm that members
of the social environment limit or deny the social sharing of emotion when they anticipate
being exposed to harmful emotions.
Temporal Evolution of the Social Sharing of Emotion
What is the fate of emotional sharing? When do people stop talking about the same
emotional experience? Under what conditions do they keep talking about a given episode?
These questions remain largely open to investigation. Hereafter, I tentatively propose a model
of the temporal evolution of social sharing (see Fig. 2) and I then examine the relevant
empirical findings when available.
---------------------------------------------
Insert Fig. 2 about here
---------------------------------------------
Immediately after an emotional episode, the experience recurs frequently in a person’s
working memory, increasing his or her thoughts of this experience, and the need to share it.
With each of these emotional memories, the phenomenology of the past episode largely re-
accesses the subject’s consciousness. As time passes, emotional memories of the past episode
32
become less frequent and its impact on current adaptation becomes progressively negligible.
The slope of this extinction is a function of the initial intensity of the emotional experience
with more intense emotions generating slower extinction slopes. Extinction is attributable to,
at least in part, the fact that new experiences mobilize the subject’s attention so that less and
less resources are available for older memories. At the final stage of evolution, the memory of
an emotional episode becomes “dormant”. Dormant memories have no further impact on
current adaptation. Yet, “dormant” implies that the memory of an emotional experience might
never totally fade away. Recall will occur if the appropriate cues are met in the present
context. When such cues are encountered, the forgotten episode re-emerges and impacts anew
upon current adaptation. Appropriate contextual cues can powerfully reactivate even very
ancient emotional experiences. Emotional memories can also fail to reach a dormant stage. In
this case, they can keep mobilizing current attentional resources to the detriment of novel
situations. People are generally well aware of the “unrecovered” status of such memories.
They overtly express a feeling “to still carry on” a past episode, or “not to be in peace” with it
(Tait & Silver, 1989). Emotional memories which fail to reach a dormant stage still elicit a
need to socially share them. Therefore, a perpetual need to share a past episode indicates that
this episode failed to reach a dormant stage and remains emotionally influential.
In a later section, I will examine the conditions for emotional recovery and the social
sharing of emotion’s contribution to emotional recovery. I now examine the evidence
available with regard to the various hypotheses just mentioned: emotional memories and their
extinction, dormant memory and reactivation, nonextinction and unrecovered memories, and
unrecovered episodes and perpetual sharing.
Emotional memories and their extinction. Several follow-up studies support the
hypothesis that as time passes, emotional memories become less frequent (Rimé et al., 1998).
To illustrate, one week after an important exam, 100% of concerned students still talked about
33
it; two weeks later 94% of them still did so; three weeks later, however, the rate fell to 50%.
Comparatively, three months after a loss, the proportion of bereaved persons who still shared
their experience was close to 80% (Zech, 1994). As there is little doubt that the loss of a close
person is a more severe emotional experience than an academic exam, such data are consistent
with the prediction that more intense emotions generate slower extinction slopes. In function
of the event intensity, extinction can be expressed in hours, days, weeks, months or even
years. Tait & Silver (1989) asked participants to report on the current impact of the “worse
event of their life.” Although the events collected under this category took place on average
more than 22 years before, they still elicited emotional memories in about half of the sample.
Dormant memory and reactivation. No data are available to specifically test the
prediction that reactivating a dormant memory temporarily reinstates the need to share it. In
his masterpiece, In search of lost time, French author Marcel Proust (1913) gave a famous
description of the flood of sensations, feelings, and other memories of his childhood which
were revived in him by the mere ingestion of a typical French cookie called a “Madeleine.”
His beautiful literary depiction of the experience launched by this recalling cue illustrated
how fast and how far forgotten emotionally-loaded memories are reactivated when the
appropriate conditions are met. In addition, the fact that Proust wrote this piece with exacting
detail points to the notion that reactivated emotional memories reinstate the need to share
them.
Nonextinction and unrecovered memories. The current impact of unrecovered
emotional memories was investigated in a study in which half of the participants were asked
to retrieve from memory a past experience “that still currently affected them” (a recovered
episode), whereas the other half were asked to select a memory “that did not affect them any
more” (an unrecovered episode) (Rimé, Hayward, & Pennebaker, 1996). After recall, all of
the participants answered a questionnaire examining the initial and present impact the recalled
34
experience produced. Data showed that at the time they occurred, episodes in the two
conditions were fully comparable. They did not differ in emotional impact, their social
sharing had been initiated equally quickly, and sharing had been repeated with comparable
frequency. In contrast, the two types of episodes differed in every regard in their present
impact. Compared to recovered episodes, unrecovered episodes elicited much more intense
emotions at recall. They also aroused a greater need to share, and they were shared more
often. Thus, participants aptly distinguished between recovered and unrecovered emotional
experiences in their autobiographical memory. The data also demonstrated that when a past
emotional memory still affects someone, the person keeps manifesting the need to share it.
This is quite consistent with the general assumption that emotion elicits sharing.
Unrecovered episode and perpetual sharing. That an emotional memory which fails
to reach a dormant stage still elicits social sharing was confirmed in a review of eight
different studies (Rimé et al., 1998). In each study, participants rated the emotional intensity
they felt when remembering a past emotional episode (“residual emotional intensity”) and the
extent to which they (a) still felt the need to talk about it (“residual need to share”), and
(b) still actually talked about it (“actual residual sharing”). The time elapsed between the
target emotional event and these ratings ranged from a week to several years, according to the
study. For each data set, correlations were computed between residual emotional intensity on
the one hand and each of two indices of residual sharing on the other hand. All sixteen
coefficients were positive and significant—they ranged from r = .20 to r = .64. Thus, a quite
consistent link was observed between residual emotional intensity and residual sharing.
A longitudinal observation conducted in the context of a study on life stress
experienced by women during their pregnancy confirmed that unextinguished emotional
experiences elicit an ongoing need to share these experiences (Curci et al., 2007). A sample of
346 women in their 10th to 17th week of pregnancy was interviewed about emotions they
35
experienced during, or shortly before, pregnancy. Half of them mentioned an important life
event in this period. During the second phase (22nd–30th week of pregnancy), and again at
the third phase of the study (4–5 days after the delivery), these women completed a
questionnaire on the current impact of the event they had reported at the first interview.
Results confirmed that a poorer recovery manifested several weeks or several months after an
episode was associated with a higher residual sharing during the same period. In addition,
structural analyses demonstrated that the extent of residual sharing at a given time strongly
predicted a poorer recovery at the next measurement time.
These results support the view that as long as an emotional memory maintains its
impact, it also elicits a need to share it, as well as actual sharing. But does this mean that
people who fail to recover from an emotion keep talking about it without limit? It was found
that, as was the case for initial emotional intensity and initial sharing, residual sharing
increased at lower levels of residual emotionality whereas a ceiling effect occurred at higher
levels (Rimé et al., 1998). In addition, correlations between residual emotional intensity and
actual sharing were generally lower than those between residual intensity and the need for
sharing. This suggests that people who fail to recover from an emotion feel the need to share
it, but that their actual sharing is somehow restricted. It is likely that these people’s intimates
experienced satiation and developed attitudes of social constraint. In other words, as long as a
memory elicits emotion, people feel the need to talk about it and they actually do so to some
extent. But their propensity to share endlessly is quite probably moderated by discouraging
responses from habitual targets.
Conclusion. The fate of the social sharing of an emotion is to progressively decline
and to finally become a dormant memory. The best known predictor of the slope of this
evolution is the initial intensity of the shared emotion. Some emotional experiences are
exceptional and continue to demand attention to the detriment of current adaptation. In this
36
case, the emotional experience continues to stimulate a need to share. However, social
constraints exerted by targets usually restrict people from sharing endlessly.
Interpersonal and Collective Consequences of Emotion Sharing
Although social constraints by recipients of social sharing of emotion have already
been mentioned twice in this article, they are by no means frequent responses to social sharing
situations. In this section, I will focus on the effects that sharing situations have on targets and
on the sharing person’s social environment. It will be shown that if individuals who
experienced an emotion are motivated to share this experience with others, the latter generally
lend themselves quite willingly to this type of social interaction. In addition, far from
neglecting thereafter what they were told, the social sharing target more often propagate the
shared content later with their own usual sharing targets. I first develop theoretical views on
targets’ responses to a social sharing round. Next, I examine predictions regarding the
propagation of the social sharing of emotion. Finally, I examine the empirical evidence
related to these two topics.
Interpersonal Dynamic of the Social Sharing of Emotion
When driving by a traffic accident, drivers slow down to watch. Pedestrians change
their route to look at a building in flames. People are attracted to emotional stories in the
media, as well as in movies, novels, plays, drama, opera, songs, images, and so forth. A
fascination for emotional material literally permeates everyday life (Rimé et al., 2005). I thus
predict that a similar fascination is elicited among listeners of the social sharing of emotion.
Once listeners expose themselves to the expressed emotions of a narrator, predictions of the
Perception-Action model of empathy should hold (Preston & de Waal, 2002). This model
states that “attended perception of the object’s state automatically activates the subject’s
37
representations of the state, situation, and object, and that activation of these representations
automatically primes or generates the associated autonomic and somatic responses, unless
inhibited” (Preston & de Waal, 2002, p. 4). In conjunction with this model, data indeed show
that being exposed to an emotional narrative is in itself an emotion-eliciting situation (Archer
& Berg, 1978; Lazarus, Opton, Monikos, & Rankin, 1965; Shortt & Pennebaker, 1992; Strack
& Coyne, 1983). Listeners in a social sharing situation are thus expected to experience
emotional responses in their turn. Moreover, the intensity of these responses should vary as a
direct function of the intensity of the emotional experience to which they are exposed.
Furthermore, empathy and a feeling of emotional communion should result from the aroused
emotional feelings. Once empathy is elicited, prosocial behavior should follow. More
specifically, I view a social sharing situation as an adult-life analogue of attachment behaviors
as they are described in the caregiver-child relationship. Caregivers respond to infants’
distress by comforting them and by demonstrating love, care, availability, proximity, or
contact (e. g. Bowlby, 1969; Harlow, 1959). I thus expect emotion sharing situations to
stimulate attachment behaviors among targets.
This reasoning suggests that in the social sharing of emotions, a characteristic
interpersonal dynamic takes place. A similar dynamic has already been documented in the
study of self-disclosure (e.g., Reis & Patrick, 1996). This dynamic proceeds as follows (Fig.
3). Person A experiences an emotion and shares it with B. The latter manifests a marked
interest for the shared content and this stimulates Person A to express the emotion more and
more. Consequently, emotional arousal is activated in Person B. A reciprocal stimulation of
emotion thus develops among the interactants and leads to enhanced empathy in Person B.
The latter then experiences a willingness to help and support Person A. The higher the
emotional intensity of the shared episode, the more Person B gives up using verbal expression
and switches to a nonverbal communication mode, with body contact or touching. As a result,
38
B’s affection for A increases. Similarly A, who is the focus of B’s attention, interest,
empathy, and support, experiences enhanced affection for the latter. This opens upon the
prediction that sharing emotion is effective in bringing a narrator and listener closer to one
another. In sum, social sharing interactions are expected to strengthen social bonds, link the
interactants, and end in enhanced social integration. As sharing targets are predominantly
intimates, the sharing process thus appears to be an efficient tool for refreshing and
consolidating intimacy. In daily life, individuals are busy with their own occupations, and
their social ties loosen. Every emotional experience would thus create a new opportunity to
reinstate intimacy between individuals and their targets.
---------------------------------------------
Insert Fig. 3 about here
---------------------------------------------
To sum up, five specific predictions are formulated with regard to the interpersonal
consequences of sharing an emotion. If they lend themselves to the interaction, exposed
targets are expected to manifest (1) interest, (2) emotional contagion, (3) empathy and
sympathy, (4) attachment behaviors, and (5) enhanced affection for the narrator.
The Social Sharing of Emotion Propagates
As previously discussed, being exposed to an emotional narrative is in itself an
emotion-eliciting situation (Archer & Berg, 1978; Lazarus et al., 1965; Shortt & Pennebaker,
1992; Strack & Coyne, 1983). Consequently, the following hypothesis should be formulated
for the social sharing of emotion. If experiencing an emotion elicits the social sharing of this
emotion, then the listener should in turn share the emotion with a third person. In other words,
a “secondary social sharing” should follow any “primary” sharing interaction. Furthermore,
secondary social sharing is also expected to elicit an emotional response in listeners and,
39
therefore, a tertiary social sharing is expected to occur. This means that an emotional event
activates the dissemination of related emotional information across social networks. In sum, a
social sharing propagation chain is created. As I suggest in Figure 4, when an intense
emotional event affects someone, a considerable number of people in this person’s
community are expected to hear about it through the sharing chain within the next few hours.
---------------------------------------------
Insert Fig. 4 about here
---------------------------------------------
Individuals are thus likely to keep track of the emotional experiences affecting their
peers. Such a process would be particularly efficient for the construction of collective
knowledge about emotional events, emotional responses, and their consequences. A continual
updating of this collective knowledge would occur as a function of every new private
emotional experience. Since emotions occur when events are unexpected or unpredicted and
since emotional situations require a quick and appropriate response, the spread of emotion
sharing offers people a particularly effective prevention tool for future situations. The social
consequences of emotions will be considerably enhanced when emotions result from media-
displayed collective events, such as collective loss, victory, defeat, success, failure,
catastrophe, accident, or common threat (Rimé, 2007a). In the case of a private emotion,
sharing propagates essentially in an exocentric direction. When collective emotions are
experienced, there are as many narrators as there are people in the audience and the
propagation should diffuse in many directions. With a private emotion, propagation is
expected to decrease with the social distance from the initial source. In collective emotion,
each sharing round would reactivate felt emotions in as many initial sources, thus reloading
the propagation flow. Consequently, collective emotional events are expected to spark a
social process resembling nuclear chain reactions.
40
Empirical Evidence
Social-integrative functions of social sharing. Christophe & Rimé (1997) compared
the emotional effects that shared episodes of low, moderate, or high intensity had on listeners.
Supporting the prediction that people are attracted to emotional stories, listeners in all three
conditions manifested a very high level of interest for the shared episode. Most of them rated
the emotion of interest at maximal value on the rating scale. Next, results confirmed the
prediction that listening to another person’s emotional story is emotion-eliciting. Moderate or
high intensity episodes were rated as more influential than low intensity ones, and more
intense episodes elicited stronger negative emotions (fear, sadness, or disgust). Thus, listeners
manifested both positive (interest) and negative affects—this might explain why sharing takes
place even when markedly unpleasant emotions are involved. Further results revealed that
listeners’ responses to the sharing person also varied with the intensity of the shared episode
(Christophe & Rimé, 1997). For low intensity episodes, listeners’ responses consisted mostly
of verbal manifestations and verbal comments aimed at diminishing the seriousness of the
event. These types of responses decreased linearly with increasing intensity of the shared
episode. Conversely, higher intensity episodes generated more displays of nonverbal
behaviors such as touching, body contact, taking into the arms, or kissing, by the recipients. In
sum, increasing levels of emotional intensity prompt sharing interactions to become
decreasingly verbal and increasingly nonverbal. This is in line with the prediction that social
sharing situation would elicit attachment responses on the part of exposed individuals.
Meta-analytic data confirm that people who engage in intimate disclosure tend to be
more well-liked than people who disclose at lower levels, and that additionally, people like
others as a result of having disclosed to them (Collins & Miller, 1994). Laurenceau, Feldman-
Barrett, and Pietromonaco (1998) stressed that emotion is a crucial ingredient in this regard
41
when they observed that: “self-disclosure of emotion emerged as a more important predictor
of intimacy than did self-disclosure of facts and information” (p. 1238). Espitalier,
Tcherkassof and Delmas (2002) demonstrated that emotion sharing can enhance participants’
social integration. In this study, participants viewed a movie and were then assigned to a
discussion group of four people. The groups were randomly assigned to discuss either
emotional or technical aspects of the movie. Participants in control groups had no discussion.
The strength of the social ties between group participants was then assessed. Compared to the
other two groups, the average strength of these ties was significantly higher in discussion
groups where emotions were shared. Consistent with these findings, Peters & Kashima (2007)
observed that emotion sharing creates a coalition between the narrator and the audience.
When emotion sharing occurred, participants were more bonded with narrators. These authors
concluded from four studies that emotion sharing had powerful consequences for social
structures and group action.
The spread of emotion sharing. Studies tested the prediction that a secondary social
sharing would generally follow primary sharing situations (Christophe & Rimé, 1997). In one
study, participants were asked to recall a recent listening situation in which they heard an
episode resembling those in a list of twenty. By random assignment, they received either a list
of low intensity episodes (i.e., lower end of a classic life-events scale), or of moderate
intensity episodes (i.e., higher end of the same scale), or of high intensity episodes (i.e., a list
of potential elicitors of post traumatic stress). The participants then rated their secondary
social sharing of the recalled episode. The study revealed that secondary social sharing was
mentioned by most respondents (overall, in 78% of the cases), with no significant differences
across conditions. However, as was evidenced for primary social sharing (Rimé et al., 1998),
the frequency of secondary sharing was related to the intensity of the episode. More intense
42
episodes indeed elicited more repetition of secondary sharing, and had been addressed to a
larger number of partners.
These observations were replicated and expanded upon by Curci and Bellelli (2004).
In one of their studies, volunteer students completed a daily diary for 15 days. Every night,
they reported an episode that someone had shared with them during the day. The data showed
that on average participants encountered such a situation every day and a half (or in 10 out of
the 15 days). Thus, 875 episodes were collected (302 positive and 573 negative). On the day
they were heard, 54% of these episodes were secondarily shared, with no difference as a
function of valence—a result which virtually matches those collected for primary sharing
(i.e., 60% on average, see above). At the end of the diary procedure, participants rated their
total secondary sharing for each of the events they had reported. The data revealed that 55%
of the events that were not shared on the day they were heard were shared on a later day. In
this manner, 75% of all the episodes collected in this study were shared, closely replicating
previous findings (Christophe & Rimé, 1997) relying on autobiographic recall.
Because targets of secondary sharing also experience emotion while listening, a
tertiary sharing should follow. Christophe (1997) found that for one third of participants,
episodes heard in a secondary sharing were indeed shared again with several new listeners,
whereas for another third of the participants, it was shared with one new listener. In total,
two-thirds of secondary listeners manifested some tertiary sharing. This confirmed that an
emotional event sparks the spread of emotional information across social networks. A field
study conducted by Harber & Cohen (2005) supported the propagation chain described in
Fig. 2. In this study, 33 college students visited a hospital morgue within the framework of a
class. Students’ intensity of emotional reactions to this visit was found predictive of the
number of people they told (primary sharing), of the number of people their friends told
(secondary sharing), and of the number of people their friends’ friends told (tertiary sharing).
43
The study showed that within 10 days, nearly 900 people had heard about this event through
these cascading levels of social sharing. Consistent with these observations, research on
rumors or urban legends demonstrate that the circulation of stories relies upon emotional
rather than informational selection (Heath, Bell, & Sternberg, 2001). When informational
aspects of truth and usefulness were controlled, people were more willing to pass along
stories that elicited stronger emotion. It was also found that more emotional legends were
distributed more widely.
Conclusions
Data confirm that the social sharing of emotion opens up important social
consequences. When receivers lend themselves to the sharing process, a state of emotional
communion is likely to follow. Additionally, in line with my speculation that emotion sharing
is the adult equivalent of attachment manifestations, sharing situations indeed favor the
occurrence of attachment behavior on the part of sharing targets. Further, a good deal of data
reveal that, consistent with hypotheses about secondary and tertiary social sharing, listeners
indeed become actors in their turn in the transmission of emotional episodes, and that a
process of propagation of emotional information develops in this manner in the surrounding
social network. Through this process, everyone in a group learns what happened to one of its
members. The process thus offers a powerful social tool at the service of continual updating
of shared knowledge, theories, and representations. At the same time, each sharing interaction
in a propagation chain brings interactants closer to one another. In sum, beside the outcome
that this person-to-person sharing process has for collective knowledge, it also consolidates
the social integration of the various participants. It can thus be concluded that, unknown to
most, individual emotional experiences have important potential consequences for both social
knowledge and group integration.
44
Social Sharing of Emotion and Emotion Regulation
Our final topic concerns the contribution of the social sharing of emotion to the
regulation of the shared emotional experience. In this context, I will focus entirely upon
negative episodes. I will begin by considering two facts. First, people are quite eager to share
negative emotions. Second, it is commonly assumed that talking about a negative emotion is a
relief.
The first fact is well illustrated in a study in which participants were asked to describe
a past emotional experience (of joy, anger, fear, or sadness, according to the condition) in
detail, and then answered a questionnaire examining what they had experienced while sharing
(Rimé et al., 1991b). Nearly all of the participants reported mental images of the emotional
event as well as related feelings and bodily sensations. Thus, they re-experienced the shared
episode. Next, they were asked to what extent the sharing was pleasant or painful to them.
Not surprisingly, sharing an experience of joy was rated as more pleasant than sharing a
negative emotion. Yet, sharing fear, sadness, or anger was rated only by a minority as painful
or very painful. Notwithstanding the reactivation of a negative experience, social sharing did
not appear to be as averse as expected. A final question asked whether participants would be
ready to share another past emotional event of the same kind “here and now.” Overall, 93.7%
of the participants gave a positive answer, and their answers did not differ significantly
whether the emotion shared previously was positively (joy) or negatively valenced (fear,
sadness, or anger). These observations reveal a paradox. On the one hand, social sharing
reactivates the components of the emotion which, in the case of a negative emotion, should be
experienced as aversive. On the other hand, sharing an emotion, whether positive or negative,
is a situation to which people are quite willingly inclined. If people are so willing to engage in
a social sharing process despite the reactivation of negative feelings, they should find notable
benefits in these situations.
45
As regards the second fact mentioned above, common sense often suggests to “talk
out” a negative emotion, or “to get it off your chest.” It is thus assumed that talking would
bring relief. Some 80% of a large survey sample in a Western country endorsed the view that
talking out emotions is helpful and this was then replicated in Hong-Kong (Zech, 2000). Most
psychologists share a similar view since the earliest times of psychotherapy. Freud initially
considered repressed emotions as the main cause of neurosis and he viewed emotional
expression as the appropriate cure (Breuer & Freud, 1885/1955). Though he later changed his
view , many psychologists still emphasize the therapeutic effects of emotional expression (see
Scheff, 1979 for a review) and believe that merely talking about an emotional experience
dissolves its emotional impact. If data could confirm this “expression-discharge” hypothesis,
it would shed light on the reasons why people so unanimously recommend talking out
emotions.
The Double Impact of a Past Negative Emotional Experience
In this section, I examine what the social sharing of emotion brings and what it does
not bring to emotional recovery. First, I re-examine my distinction between the obvious and
the subtle consequences of a negative emotional experience, and I then propose several
hypotheses on the type of action needed to buffer each of these consequences respectively.
Figure 5 summarizes my major points in this regard. Finally, I review the relevant empirical
evidence.
---------------------------------------------
Insert Fig. 5 about here
---------------------------------------------
Obvious impact of a past negative emotion. I argue that a negative emotional
experience maintains an impact upon current adaptation when cognitions involved in the
46
corresponding memory network sustain the signaling function of emotion. Such cognitions
cause the network to re-access the working memory and to rouse mental rumination, intrusive
thoughts and images, a need to talk, and actual sharing. As previously noted, in such cases
individuals express the feeling not to be at peace with the past episode. Cognitions of three
kinds can provoke such emotional memories (Rimé, 2005; 2007b): (1) representation of goals
which were blocked by the emotion-eliciting situation (Carver & Scheier, 1990; Dembo,
1931; Klinger, 1975; Mandler, 1984; Martin & Tesser, 1989); (2) expectations, schemas,
models, or self- and worldviews which were disconfirmed by the situation (Epstein, 1990,
1991; Janoff-Bulman, 1992; Horowitz, 1976, 1979; Piaget, 1946; Taylor, 1983; Taylor &
Brown, 1988); and (3) persistent initial appraisal of the encoded emotional situation—if the
person’s appraisal of the recalled situation duplicates the initial appraisal of this situation, the
same emotion is triggered every time the memory is accessed. To eliminate the current impact
of a past episode, and thus to achieve emotional recovery, emotion regulation should thus turn
off each of these various memory-sustaining cognitions. This requires completion of all the
relevant cognitive tasks: abandonment of one’s frustrated goals, reorganization of one’s
hierarchy of motives, accommodation of one’s models and schemas, re-creation of meaning,
and assimilation of the event through reframing or reappraisal. A thorough completion of
these cognitive tasks would lead to emotional recovery.
Subtle effects of a past negative emotional experience. As I stressed earlier, negative
emotional episodes have collateral effects involving a momentary destabilization of the
person. I argued that the source of such a distressing condition is multifaceted. It involves all
at once goal frustration, discomfirmation of expectations, diccomfirmation of models and
worldviews, loss of symbolic buffers, loss of meaning, and experience of a defeated self. The
condition manifests itself in the form of anxiety, insecurity, helplessness, estrangement,
alienation, loss of self-esteem, loss of self-confidence, and a feeling of loneliness. How could
47
this distressing condition be buffered? I argue that as a legacy from early attachment schemes,
distress arouses potent socio-affective needs in adults and thus opens up a quest for
appeasement, comfort, love, care, availability, proximity, or contact (e.g,. Bowlby, 1969;
Harlow, 1959); for material help and assistance (e.g. Stroebe & Stroebe, 1996; Thoits, 1984);
for support, reassurance, and esteem (e.g. Epstein, 1973); and for social recognition and
validation (e.g. Wortman & Lehman, 1985). In sum, distressed individuals experience socio-
affective needs. In my view, this quest plays an important role in individuals’ motivation to
socially share their emotional experiences profusely and to do it quite willingly.
Social Sharing Modes and their Effects
To what extent does the social sharing of emotion buffer both the obvious and the
subtle impact of a past negative emotion? I propose that two types of sharing modes need to
be considered. On the one hand, a cognitive mode takes place when social sharing targets
stimulate a narrator’s cognitive work, prompting them to abandon their frustrated goals,
reorganize their hierarchy of motives, accommodate their models and schemas, re-create
meaning, and reframe or re-appraise the episode. On the other hand, the socio-affective mode
contributes to the fulfillment of the socio-affective needs of the narrator by providing him or
her with responses that offer help, support, comfort, consolation, legitimization, attention,
bonding, and empathy. Social sharing rounds developed along a cognitive route can
contribute to the extinction of a past negative experience, and thus achieve emotional
recovery. Social sharing rounds which develop along the socio-affective route would not
contribute to the extinction of a past negative experience. However, they are well-suited to
alleviating narrators’ insecurity, anxiety, helplessness, loneliness, and so forth. When this
happens, narrators experience deep feelings of relief and they describe the situation as highly
beneficial to them. However, when the social sharing process develops along the socio-
48
affective route in the absence of any cognitive contribution, its relieving effects are expected
only to be temporary. Because socio-affective responses do not extinguish the source of the
subtle effects of a past negative episode, the impact of this episode would reemerge shortly
after the sharing round, opening upon new memories of the episode and a renewed need to
share it. I argue that in routine conditions, social sharing interactions develop predominantly
along the socio-affective route and that this constitutes a major reason why sharing is modally
a repetitive manifestation. At least five arguments support such a view.
First, as it develops routinely, the social sharing of emotion essentially favors socio-
affective processes. Both narrators’ demands and targets’ responses concur with this
conclusion. Narrators’ most immediate demands arise from the activation of their attachment
system. They primarily address their intimates in view of a reduction of their distress. They
thus stimulate attachment responses from their addressees. This favors the interpersonal
dynamic which was described and documented in the previous section and which
prototypically fits the socio-affective mode.
A second argument in favor of a preponderant socio-affective mode lies in the fact that
distressing situations hardly elicit complex responses from nonvictims. Simplistic approaches
and simplistic interventions usually prevail because nonvictims dramatically underestimate a
victim’ situation (e.g. Coates, Wortman, & Abbey, 1979; Goffman, 1963; Wortman &
Lehman, 1985). Exposure to negative circumstances indeed generates anxiety among
bystanders. For the sake of their own protection, their predominant concern is generally a
quick resolution of the crisis. Simple solutions, such as eliminating the cause or extricating
the victim from the problematic situation, dominate (Burleson, 1985; O’Keefe & Delia,
1987). Comforting interventions often consist of low level imperatives focused on action and
recommendations such as “get it off your chest...!” are typical of this kind. Most laypersons
49
are unable to take into account the complex consequences negative emotional experiences
entail.
A third argument favoring the preponderance of the socio-affective mode in emotional
sharing is that most of the sharing process develops in the period immediately following the
episode. In this period, individuals are generally not ready for the changes implied by the
cognitive work. Emotion-inducing situations, such as goal-blocking, consistently elicited an
initially enhanced concentration on the unattained goal, with invigoration and repetitive
efforts (e.g., Dembo, 1931; Klinger, 1975; Martin & Tesser, 1989). In other words, soon after
an emotion, individuals generally refuse to abandon their frustrated goals, they do not
consider modifying their hierarchy of motives, they stick to their existing schemas, they do
not want to change their representations, they stand by their initial appraisal of the emotional
situation, they do not feel ready to reframe it, nor to change their perspective.
A fourth argument in the same vein comes from the demonstration that when more
intense emotional experiences are socially shared, listeners’ responses become less and less
verbal and increasingly nonverbal (Christophe & Rimé, 1997). Obviously, nonverbal
manifestations cannot be expected to contribute to the cognitive mode. Thus again, at least in
the case of intense emotions, social sharing situations fail to favor this mode.
Fifth, an investigation of the alleged motives for engaging in sharing reveal that
motives of the socio-affective kind prevailed. Four sets of data are available in this regard.
(Delfosse, Nils, Lasserre & Rimé, 2004; Finkenauer & Rimé, 1996; Nils, Delfosse and Rimé,
2005; Wetzer, Zeelenberg, and Pieters, 2005). Three of these data sets came from respondents
who were prompted to recall recent emotional experiences and then asked to list the reasons
why they had engaged in social sharing. The fourth data set (Wetzer et al., 2005) was theory-
based and relied upon the literature on social sharing, on word-of-mouth communication, and
on social interaction. Categorizing the various motives provided 9, 8, 9 and 7 categories,
50
respectively, with large overlaps between them. A synthesis of these categories is displayed in
Table 1 under twelve classes of motives. Most of the motives in the list manifest considerable
demands addressed to social sharing targets. Partners are indeed expected to provide help and
support, comfort and consolation, legitimization, clarification and meaning, and advice and
solutions. This long list of specific social solicitations is extended further with less specific
and more personally involving demands, such as attention, bonding, and empathy. Thus, the
alleged motives for sharing emotions especially meet socio-affective needs. They also meet
some cognitive regulation needs, such as finding clarification and meaning. Yet, major
cognitive needs such as abandonment of goals, reorganization of motives, reconstruction of
schemas, reframing and reappraisal of the event are simply absent from social demands for
regulation. Participants simply might not be aware of such motives. In any case, the study of
motives for sharing confirm the preponderance of socio-affective demands in the sharing
process.
---------------------------------------
Insert Table 1
about here
---------------------------------------
Thus, all five elements concur to suggest that spontaneous sharing does generally not
fuel the cognitive route. It should be stressed that nothing precludes listeners from providing
socio-cognitive responses, and therefore the possibility exists for sharing situations to
contribute to emotional recovery. Yet, observing spontaneous social sharing situations
suggests that this is far from being common. Ample opportunity is then open for professional
intervention focused on the cognitive processing of emotional episodes.
This two-mode model leads to an important prediction regarding what can and cannot
be expected, as a result of spontaneous sharing situations. I predict that spontaneous sharing
51
situations bring narrators an important sense of relief, but no effect upon their emotional
recovery. This prediction stands in sharp contrast to the expression-discharge, or cathartic
hypothesis, which predicts that merely talking about an emotional experience can dissolve its
emotional impact. I believe that what individuals most often experience in sharing situations
are the temporary benefits of the completion of their socio-affective needs. Because people
are indiscriminate in this regard, they equate emotional relief with emotional recovery, and
the benefits they perceive from talking out emotion in an interpersonal context thus reinforces
the false belief that sharing entails "cathartic" effects .
Does the Mere Sharing of Emotions Bring Emotional Recovery? Empirical Evidence
In this section, I examine and discuss the various empirical facts relevant to assessing
the hypothesis that sharing emotions brings emotional recovery. I also examine direct tests of
the respective effects of both the cognitive and socio-affective sharing modes previously
defined.
Written disclosure. In the last two decades, inspired by James Pennebaker’s studies,
scientific interest emerged regarding the effects of written disclosure of emotion on well-
being. In these studies (e.g., Pennebaker & Beall, 1986; Pennebaker, Colder, & Sharp, 1990;
Pennebaker, Kiecolt-Glaser, & Glaser, 1988), participants wrote about past stressful or
traumatic events in their lives for short sessions (15 to 30 minutes) held daily on several
consecutive days. In control groups, participants wrote about trivial topics. The basic finding
of these studies was that when compared to the control group, participants who wrote about
trauma showed both less frequent illness-related visits to a health center and fewer self-
reported illness symptoms at follow-up (Pennebaker and Beall, 1986). Further studies
extended these findings to other health outcomes, including improved immune function
(Esterling, Antoni, Fletcher, Margulies, & Schneiderman, 1994). A meta-analytic review
52
involving 146 randomized writing studies confirmed written disclosure to be beneficial for
one’s psychological health, physical health, and overall functioning (Frattaroli, 2006).
These findings are generally understood as supporting the view that “putting emotion
into words” is conducive to emotional recovery. It is indeed largely accepted that the
beneficial effects obtained from writing result from mediation of the emotional turmoil
associated with memories of distressing episodes. Whereas writing studies clearly document
the independent (writing) and dependent (health and well-being) variables, they do not
examine whether the hypothetical mediator can explain the relations established between
these variables. In particular, these studies do not assess whether putting a specific emotional
episode into words brings about significant relief of this specific episode’s memory. This is
due in large part to the standard instructions inviting participants to “write about as many
traumatic experiences of their past” as they wished. Admittedly, it can hardly be assessed
whether the memory of many traumatic experiences would have been alleviated as a result of
the procedure. However, the possibility of testing the effects of writing upon emotional
recovery exists. It would suffice to limit the writing instructions to a single, well specified,
emotional episode. This task is still open to future investigation.
Shared vs. secret emotional events. Several studies compared shared and secret
emotions. Participants were asked if they could recall an important emotional life event which
they had kept secret. Consistent with the previous findings by Pennebaker (1989), participants
who reported the memory of a non-shared emotion also reported a higher number of illnesses
than those who did not have such a memory (Finkenauer & Rimé, 1998b). In addition, those
who had not shared at least one emotion scored lower on various markers of life satisfaction.
However, when shared and secret emotional episodes were re-accessed and compared for the
intensity of their emotional impact, no significant difference was observed (Finkenauer &
Rimé, 1998a). Thus, nonshared memories were no more no less emotionally arousing when
53
re-accessed than shared ones. These findings thus fail to support what the expression-
discharge view would predict.
Extent of social sharing and emotional recovery. The expression-discharge
hypothesis has been further tested in studies wherein participants were observed after a
specific emotional event. The research designs generally involved the assessment of (1) the
initial intensity of emotions elicited by the episode, (2) the extent of sharing that developed
after, and (3) the intensity of emotions elicited when the memory of the episode was later
activated. For each participant, the degree of emotional recovery was established by the
difference between (1) and (3), and it was evaluated by the extent to which this variable
correlated positively with social sharing. Results of these studies—which remained
unpublished due to their negative results—failed to yield such correlations. To illustrate,
(Rimé et al., 1994), participants in a diary study described their most intense emotional
experience of the previous day every evening for three weeks. They then answered questions
about the experience, including intensity of impact and extent of sharing. Two to three weeks
later, participants were reintroduced to six emotional events taken from these diary reports
(three intense, three weak), and they rated each of the events for extent of current sharing,
extent of current rumination, and intensity of current emotional impact. A recovery index was
calculated by subtracting the follow-up impact from the initial impact. Both for initially weak
events and for initially intense ones, this recovery index was negatively correlated with
follow-up sharing. This confirmed again that people who have recovered less from an episode
keep sharing it. But, the recovery index was in no case significantly related to initial sharing
so that the expression-discharge view again failed to be supported.
Experimental induction of social sharing. Pennebaker and Beall (1986) observed that
writing about factual aspects of an emotional episode did not affect their health variables,
whereas writing about emotional aspects did. Experiments involving such variations in
54
sharing were thus conducted with emotional recovery as a dependent variable (Zech, 2000;
Zech & Rimé, 2005). In three studies, psychology students interviewed a relative about a
recent negative emotional event. In a fourth, participants extensively shared the most
upsetting event of their life with an experimenter. In each of the four studies, participants
were instructed to emphasize either the factual aspects of the episode, or their related feelings.
In control groups, the participants talked about a nonemotional topic. The current emotional
impact that the shared event had when re-accessed was assessed through various indices, for
example, emotional intensity of the memory, intensity of bodily sensations, and action
tendencies. The assessments were done before the sharing interview, immediately after, and
again a couple of days later. In one of the studies, additional assessments were conducted two
months later. In contrast to what the expression-discharge model predicts, none of the studies
yielded effect of sharing type on these indices of emotional impact. However, compared to
those in the factual sharing and in the control conditions, participants in the felt emotions
condition consistently rated the sharing session as more emotionally alleviating, more
cognitively helpful, and more interpersonally beneficial. Thus, the impact of the emotional
memory was not altered in the sharing situation which did not involve the cognitive mode.
Yet, participants who had shared their emotions clearly reported the benefits which were
predicted for sharing situations of the socio-affective kind.
Psychological debriefings. Clinical research conducted on the effects of psychological
debriefing techniques provided data relevant to the expression-discharge hypothesis.
Psychological debriefing is a group intervention technique developed for implementation
immediately after a traumatic event (see Dyregrov, 1997; Mitchell & Everly, 1995, for
overviews). Participants in small groups each describe what happened from their perspective.
They are asked to express their prominent thoughts about the event and to communicate the
worse aspects of their experience. The purpose of this technique is to prevent the development
55
of post-traumatic stress disorders (PTSD) which involve manifestations of intrusion and of
avoidance of memories of the traumatic episode. It is thus well suited to testing the
expression-discharge view. Recently, several meta-analytic reviews assessed the findings of
controlled studies conducted in this perspective (Arendt & Elklit, 2001; Rose & Bisson, 1998;
Van Emmerik, Kamphuis, Hulsbosch, & Emmelkamp, 2002). These reviews consistently
concluded that debriefings have no efficacy in reducing symptoms of post-traumatic stress
disorders or other trauma-related symptoms. Adverse effects were even found. However, in
spite of these inconclusive results regarding emotional recovery, psychological debriefing
procedures have generally demonstrated clear perceived benefits among participants. Quite
generally, people who have been exposed to a traumatic situation report that taking part in a
psychological debriefing was useful and beneficial to them. Thus, with the absence of a
recovery effect and a wealth of expressed benefits, the findings from studies of psychological
debriefings closely parallel those from social sharing studies.
The social sharing of laboratory-induced emotions. Finally, in three laboratory
studies, a movie-clip emotional induction was followed by either a social sharing session
about the movie, or by some control condition in which sharing was absent. Their findings
regarding recovery effects were hardly consistent. Both Mendolia and Kleck (1993) and
Lepore, Ragan and Jones (2000) observed recovery effects when participants were re-exposed
to the movie 48 hours later. Compared to the control groups, those who had shared their
emotion manifested lower perceived stress levels (Lepore et al., 2000) or reported feeling
more positive (Mendolia & Kleck, 1993). In addition, Mendolia and Kleck (1993) recorded
physiological recovery effects, but Lepore et al. (2000) did not replicate this finding. In a third
investigation, Lepore, Fernandez-Berrocal, Ragan, and Ramos (2004) assigned participants
who had viewed an emotional movie to either (1) a challenging partner, (2) a validating
partner, (3) a talk alone condition, or (4) a no-talk control condition. Compared to the “no
56
talk” group, participants in the challenging condition manifested a clearly superior recovery.
However, this result was not consistent with the findings from the two previously mentioned
studies. In both of those, recovery was favored by emotion talking—a condition which
compares with a validating (supportive) condition, and certainly not with a challenging one.
Conclusion. No consistent empirical support was found for the common view that
putting an emotional experience into words can resolve it. The abundance of null findings in
studies of the expression-discharge hypothesis strongly suggest that merely sharing an
emotion cannot change the emotional memory. Interestingly, Kennedy-Moore and Watson
(1999) reached a similar conclusion in their review of empirical studies on the “venting
hypothesis.” This hypothesis claims that the expression of distress, such as crying, or the
expression of anger, has direct and immediate effects of relief. At the end of their review,
Kennedy-Moore and Watson (1999) wrote: “At this point, it should be clear that the venting
hypothesis and its corollaries are myths. Whether or not expression of distress is beneficial
depends on what is expressed, to whom, and how” (p. 58).
A Direct Test of the Two Social Sharing Modes
Several studies directly tested the respective effects of the cognitive mode and the
socio-affective mode by varying listeners’ responses in social sharing situations (Nils &
Rimé, 2007b). In a random assignment, listeners adopted either a socio-affective response or
responses that would prompt cognitive work by the sharing person. Predictions were as
follows.
Predicted effects of socio-affective responses. If listeners adopt socio-affective
responses, should emotional recovery follow for the initiator? This model predicts a negative
answer. Emotional feelings could temporarily increase as a result of reviving the emotional
experience. Consequently, mental rumination, intrusive thoughts, and the need for sharing
57
may be enhanced in the period immediately following a sharing round. Should changes in
cognitions and motivations result from this socio-affective sharing? A negative answer is
implied by the model. Indeed, the initial appraisal of the event would be left unchanged,
frustrated goals would stay at the foreground, gaps in representations and schemas would not
be fulfilled, and nothing would have contributed constructively to the search for meaning
elicited by the episode. Should the destabilizing impact of the episode be buffered? In this
case, the model predicts a positive answer. After the sharing session, the initiator is expected
to report feelings of being recognized, validated, supported, the impression of relief, reduction
of loneliness, an increase in social integration, as well as a strengthening of shared beliefs. It
should be stressed, however, that these effects are at risk of being only temporary. Because
emotional recovery failed to be achieved in such an interaction, the emotional impact of the
episode can persist and may reactivate the destabilization. This is likely an explanation for
observations that demonstrate that despite recurrent sharing, people who failed to recover
from a given emotional experience keep manifesting the need to share it.
Predicted effects of cognitive responses. If listeners adopt responses that prompt
cognitive work, should emotional recovery follow for the initiator? The model predicts a
positive answer and thereby a subsequent reduction in mental rumination, intrusive thoughts,
and the need for sharing. Would change in cognitions and motivations be recorded after such
social interactions? Yes, indeed, according to the model. There are chances that, due to the
prompted cognitive work, the initial appraisal of the event would be modified, that frustrated
goals would be abandoned, that gaps in representation and schemas would be fulfilled, and
that the search for meaning would be brought to completion. Should the destabilizing impact
of the episode be buffered? The model proposes a negative answer. Nothing in these cognitive
responses condition would elicit feelings of being recognized, validated, and supported. There
58
would be no reduction of loneliness, and no enhancement of the feeling of integration. There
would be no strengthening of shared beliefs.
Experimental findings. In one of the experiments (Nils & Rimé, 2008), volunteer
students were individually induced into a negative emotional state using a movie clip.
Immediately after the movie, participants shared their movie experience with a friend of theirs
who came to the lab at the same time. Friends were used as targets because social sharing
studies repeatedly show that sharing very generally addresses friends. While the participant
watched the movie, the friend was instructed to maintain a systematic attitude in the sharing
session. In one condition, the friend had to maintain an empathetic attitude, which
corresponded to a socio-affective response. In a second condition, the friend had to manifest
positive reframing, which corresponded to one modality of cognitive responding. In a third,
control condition, the friend had to maintain a neutral attitude. Dependent variables were
measured immediately after the sharing session, and then 48 hours later after re-exposure to
movie. Participants completed scales assessing emotional, cognitive, and social variables with
emotional distress, beliefs and world views, and degree of loneliness, respectively.
The results showed that movie-related emotional distress was significantly lower in
the cognitive condition than in the two others. This effect was again observed after re-
exposure to the movie, thus confirming that a cognitively-oriented sharing brings emotional
recovery. In addition, after both sharing and movie re-exposure, beliefs and world views that
were challenged by the movie content (i.e., scenes of human cruelty toward animals), were
significantly less dampened in the cognitive condition than in the two others, thus confirming
that a cognitively-oriented sharing contributes to the reconstruction of assumptions. Finally,
after both sharing and movie re-exposure, loneliness scores were much lower in the socio-
affective condition than in the two others, thereby confirming the benefits of interpersonal and
59
social integration brought about by a socio-affectively-oriented sharing. Altogether, these
results, and those from other experiments, were quite consistent with the model.
Additional consistency emerged from studies mentioned earlier in which Lepore et al.
(2004) exposed students to a dramatic video (real-life gang scene) on two different days.
Participants were assigned to either (1) a challenging partner condition, comparable to Nils
and Rimé’ (2007) cognitive reframing condition, (2) a validating partner condition, which
resembled Nils and Rimé’ (2007) empathetic or socio-affective condition, (3) a condition in
which they talked alone about their reaction, or (4) a no talk control condition. The talk alone
and the validating condition did not differ, or only marginally, when compared to the no talk
group. Quite consistent with the proposed model, however, participants in the cognitive
reframing condition showed the greatest adjustment to stress. These participants reported the
lowest level of emotional distress during re-exposure to the video as well as the lowest levels
of intrusive thoughts and avoidance reactions.
Conclusions. These findings confirm that when social sharing situations stimulate
cognitive work, they can attenuate movie-related emotional arousal and rumination. Thus, in
compliance with the proposed model, a sharing situation that prompts cognitive responses can
produce the recovery effect that former studies, which focused on mere social sharing,
systematically failed to observe. The results also show that an empathetic sharing situation
provoke no effects, or only weak ones, on emotional arousal and rumination. This confirms
that emotional recovery effects are specific to cognitive work and cannot be achieved when
the social sharing situation is focused on socio-affective variables. However, in line with the
model, participants in the empathetic sharing condition rated their partner as more similar,
friendlier, and more empathic than the challenging or cognitive-reframing confederate. In
other words, whereas empathetic conferedates did not help to decrease movie-related
emotional arousal and rumination, they fulfilled participants' socio-affective needs better.
60
Thus, when listeners adopt socio-affective responses in the social sharing process, the
emotional distress elicited by the shared negative experience can be relieved, at least for some
time. The model predicts that in the absence of recovery effects, the benefits resulting from a
sharing situation of the socio-affective type would be only temporary.
In this study, the target emotion was the result of a laboratory induction. Because the
stress level from such an induction was probably minimal, participants could easily lend
themselves to cognitive work immediately afterward. This is unlikely to happen after a real-
life intense emotional experience. True cognitive work that involves change and creativity
cannot be expected on the part of a person experiencing intense emotional distress. This leads
to predict that an optimal timing exists for the respective onset of each of the socio-affective
and the cognitive sharing modes. In the period immediately following an emotional episode,
socio-affective sharing aimed at buffering the emotional distress should take precedence. As
time goes by, and as the emotional distress wanes, social sharing focused on cognitive work
should progressively take place. Future studies should examine the respective effects of these
two social sharing modes in the context of real-life emotional episodes of various levels of
intensity, and these studies should take into account the timing hypothesis.
General Conclusions
I conclude this article by taking stock of the theoretical considerations and empirical
findings which were reviewed. To this purpose, two specific questions will be addressed.
First, what are the various functions that the social sharing of emotion can serve? Second,
what does emotion research gain from the study of the social sharing of emotion?
Functions Served by Sharing Emotion
Hereafter, I will sum up the various functions that the social sharing of emotion fulfils,
or can fulfill. As will be apparent, there are many. Some of these functions have already been
61
submitted to empirical investigation, others are still completely open to investigation. In any
case, most of the work needed to clarify social sharing manifestations remains to be
conducted. I hope that the following overview will stimulate further research.
A first, very basic, function of emotion sharing is found in rehearsing, reminding, or
reexperiencing. For positive emotions, rehearsing essentially serves the purpose of
“capitalization.” Individuals can enhance their, and their targets’, current level of positive
affect by retelling a positive emotional episode. For negative emotions, rehearsing can fulfill
important functions with regard to the memory of emotional episodes. In the study of
collective emotional events, social sharing emerged as part of a more general rehearsal
process that also involved following news in the media. By talking about a public emotional
event, people both constructed a collective memory of the event and consolidated their own
memory of the personal circumstances under which they first heard about it (Finkenauer,
Gisle & Luminet, 1997). The storage of episodic information which rehearsing favors is
probably of high survival value in the case of personal emotional events.
The social sharing of emotion can cover many functions of the socio-affective kind.
Such functions are well captured by laypersons, as was evidenced by the investigation of the
motives they allege for sharing an emotional episode. As noted in Table 1, emotional sharing
can be instrumental in gaining social attention and interest from targets, or in arousing
empathy among targets. In line with such functions, emotional sharing appeared as a powerful
tool for stimulating bonding and for strengthening social ties. In addition, laypersons were
well aware of the various levels of help they can expect form targets when they share a
negative emotional experience: these levels can include help and support, comfort and
consolation, legitimization and validation, as well as reception of advice and solutions.
Besides these various socio-affective contributions, the social sharing of emotion can
fulfill many functions of the cognitive or of the symbolic type. As was apparent from
62
investigations of alleged motives for sharing, these motives are much less obvious to lay
observation. I will thus develop them in more detail.
First of all, emotion is a dense and diffuse experience in need of cognitive articulation.
By using language and by addressing others, individuals “unfold” the emotional material,
label it, and organize it into sequential relationships conforming to the rules of rational
thinking (Rimé, 1983, 1987; Werner & Kaplan, 1967). Repetitive communication about an
emotional experience lends itself to an evolution of its mental representation. For instance,
script-like formats can be imposed upon the shared experience; some elements can be
emphasized whereas others can be played down. The process of cognitive articulation can
contribute to a progressive distancing from this experience and to adoption of a decentered
point of view on the emotional material.
Next, emotion usually involves complex and unexpected eliciting circumstances, as
well as the perception of a complex set of abundant and often ambiguous exteroceptive and
interoceptive stimulations. As predicted by Festinger (1954), individuals confronted with
ambiguous information look for clarification in their social environment. That such a social
comparison process is aroused by emotional conditions was documented by Schachter’s
(1959) work on the stress-affiliation effect. Other cognitive-social processes are likely to be
involved in the social sharing of emotion. As emotions occur when forecasts fail, when
expectations are disconfirmed, and when activities in progress are blocked, they generally
bring forth a considerable loss of meaning (Weick, 1995). They challenge models and
assumptions about oneself and the world that are held by individuals to preserve a sense of
coherence, predictability, and control (e.g., Janoff-Bulman, 1992; Tait & Silver, 1989).
Festinger (1957) argued that when prophecies fail, believers are motivated to develop a
cognitive work of dissonance reduction. Cognitive dissonance research showed that people
who engage in dissonance reduction typically initiate communication (e.g. Festinger, Riecken
63
& Schachter, 1956). The process of social sharing is well-suited to respond to the quest for
meaning aroused by an emotion and to contribute to meaning production. In addition, story-
telling and the construction of a narrative can be at play in the social sharing of emotion. As
was stressed by Bruner (1990), unusual or exceptional circumstances stimulate the production
of narration because it allows for the sketching of a world within a context where the
encountered exception makes sense. Within the framework of a story, exceptions can be made
comprehensible. In addition, emotional circumstances generally involve unfamiliar or atypical
objects or events that are likely to shatter collective representations and shared knowledge. In
line with Moscovici’s (1984) views, the social sharing of emotion provides a frame in which
conversation develops. Conversation can transform and absorb unfamiliar elements into
social representation.
More generally, it was shown that emotional sharing propagates across a community.
This collective process can contribute to the construction and dissemination of social
knowledge about emotional episodes and emotional responses. Successive sharing rounds
about the same episode probably involve a process analogous to Bartlett’s (1932) “serial
reproduction,” which involves transmission of information through a chain of persons.
Bartlett observed that participants in studies on serial reproduction (a) transformed the
original material into a caricature, (b) changed meaningless aspects into meaningful ones, and
(c) reconstructed transmitted information in a manner fitting their prior knowledge and
expectations. These observations suggested to Bartlett the notion of schemata. Schemata
liberate cognitive resources, allowing the person to focus on more novel and unexpected
aspects of the incoming information.
What Does Emotion Research Gain from the Study of the Social Sharing of Emotion?
64
Investigating the social sharing of emotion can change our view of what an emotion is
in many respects.
In contrast to classic homeostatic views rooted in animal models of emotion, social
sharing studies demonstrate that emotion is not a rapidly vanishing state bound to disappear as
soon as the emotional circumstances vanish. Emotion impregnates the subsequent life of the
affected persons. This impact can be long-lasting: the higher the intensity of the experience,
the stronger its hold on later life. Thus, an emotional experience is only the starting point for
important subsequent processes simultaneously involving cognitive, symbolic, affective and
social aspects. In such a context, emotion emerges not as a problem or state of confusion, but
as a response through which attention is allocated to the production of meaning. Contrary to
common views, emotion is in no manner antagonistic to cognition. In the present context,
emotions appear to be at the service of cognitive consistency and of meaning production.
They signal the occurrence of cognitive discrepancies between the assumed state of the world
and the current experience. They also exert a continuous motivational pressure on the person
in the direction of discrepancy reduction. Emotions are thus manifested as playing a central
role in the vicissitudes of human adaptation. They can be viewed as essential moments of
potential evolution and growth in the individuals’ life span. They contribute to expanding,
adapting, transforming and repairing the individual’s models, theories, assumptions and other
views of themselves and the world.
The study of the social sharing of emotion generated many arguments that discard the
Lone Ranger individualist perspective of adult emotional regulation. In sharp contrast with
the latter perspective, much data revealed that an emotional experience is virtually indivisible
of a social response. This response involves not only the need to be with others, as was
foreseen by Stanley Schachter’s (1959) pioneering work on stress and affiliation, but also the
need to talk to others, to tell them what happened, and to share with them what was felt and
65
thought. When individuals experience difficulties or obstacles in reaching their goals, they
urgently turn to other people around them with mutliple demands. Emotion is thus revealed as
a fundamentally interdependent process. It is in no manner limited to an internal process
constrained within the boundaries of an affected individual.
I proposed that the social manifestations elicited by emotional episodes originated in
attachment processes which were at play since infancy. According to my view, social
responses elicited by emotion in adults are no less than mature forms of these early
interdependent processes of emotional regulation. Thus, adults are not alone when they face
an obstacle, a loss, or other difficulties signaled by emotion. In such circumstances, their
deeply rooted social nature immediately comes into play. Even if the underlying motives
differ radically, positive emotional experiences elicit exactly the same social orientation as
negative ones. Theories of emotion will thus need to consider the fact that emotional
circumstances are systematically sparking social communication among human beings. The
available evidence now suggests that social responses following emotion can safely be added
to the various manifestations—facial-expressive, physiologic, behavioral, cognitive, and
phenomenal—traditionally regarded as the prototypical features of emotional responding.
Data from the study of the social sharing of emotion demonstrate the unexpected
breadth of the social consequences of emotion. They not only reveal that interactants
strengthen their ties and come close to one another by sharing emotion, they also show that a
social sharing interaction is the beginning of a propagation process in which all the successive
interactants experience similar effects for their own ties. Thus, the consequences of emotion
are far from being limited to the individual who experienced it. On the contrary, emotion
opens up processes that can enhance social integration and social cohesion within the larger
community. By the same token, emotion appears to open up a communal spreading of
emotional episodes through which individuals have lived, as well as of related questioning
66
and search for meaning. In this manner, every individual emotional experience acts as the
potential source of a shared process of cognitive construction to which many contribute.
Within the horizon of such findings, every individual emotion is to be conceived as another
opportunity for contributing to the construction of culturally-shared protection tools, such as
common sense, social knowledge, shared assumptions, and social representations. Thus, I
want to conclude this review by stressing that emotions experienced by individuals are not
only instruments at the service of individual adaptation, they are also major tools serving the
adaptation of members of a community.
References
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: a
psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, Fourth Edition. Washington DC.: American Psychiatric Association.
Archer, R. L., & Berg, J. H. (1978). Disclosure reciprocity and its limits: A reactance
analysis. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 527-540.
Arendt, M., & Elklit, A. (2001). Effectiveness of psychological debriefing. Acta Psychiatrica
Scandinavica, 104, 423-437.
Bandura, A (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist, 37,
142-147.
Bawnes, I. T., O’Gorman, E. C., & Sayers, A. (1991). Assault characteristics and
posttraumatic stress disorder in rape victims. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 88, 27-
30.
Becker, E. (1973). The denial of death. New York : Free Press.
67
Berger, P. L. & Luckmann, T. (1967). Social construction of reality. Garden City, NY:
Doubleday.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
Breuer, J. & Freud, S. (1885/1956). Studies on hysteria. In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), The
standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 2, pp. 1-
305). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1895).
Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bryant, F. B. (1989). A four factor model of perceived control: Avoiding, coping, obtaining,
and savoring. Journal of Personality, 57, 773-797.
Burleson, B. R. (1985). The production of comforting messages: Social-cognitive
foundations. Journal of language and social psychology, 4, 253-212.
Cantril, H. (1950). The why of man’s experience. New York: Mac Millan.
Cannon, W. B. (1915). Bodily changes in pain, hunger, fear and rage: an account of
researches into the function of emotional excitement. New York and London: D.
Appleton and Company.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect:
A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97, 19-35.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2001). On the self-regulation of behaviour. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Christophe, V. & Rimé, B. (1997). Exposure to the social sharing of emotion: Emotional
impact, listener responses and the secondary social sharing. European Journal of
Social Psychology, 27, 37-54.
Christophe, V. (1997). Le partage social des émotions du point de vue de l’auditeur. [Social
sharing of emotion under the angle of the listener]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Université de Lille III, France.
68
Claes, C., & Rimé, B. (2008). [Parents’ and doctor’s talk to the child in a routine pediatric
consultation involving a vaccination shot]. Unpublished raw data.
Coates, D., Wortman, C. B., & Abbey, A. (1979). Reactions to victims. In I. H. Frieze, D.
Bar-Tal, & J. S. Carrol (Eds.), New approaches to social problems ((pp. 461-489). San
Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Collins, N. L., & Miller, L. C. (1994). Self-Disclosure and liking: A meta-analytic review.
Psychological Bulletin, 116, 457-475.
Collins, R. (2004). Rituals of solidarity in the wake of terrorist attack. Sociological Theory, 22,
53-87.
Corsini, S. & Rimé, B. (2007). Everyday emotional events and basic beliefs. Manuscript
submitted for publication.
Cottrell, N. B. & Epley, S. W. (1977). Affiliation, social comparison, and socially mediated
stress reduction. In J. M. Suls & R. L. Miller (Eds.), Social comparison processes:
Theoretical and empirical perspectives (pp. 43-68). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.
Curci, A., & Bellelli, G. (2004). Cognitive and social consequences of exposure to emotional
narratives: Two studies on secondary social sharing of emotions. Cognition and
Emotion, 18, 881-900 .
Curci, A., & Rimé, B. (2007). Dreams, emotions, and the social sharing of dreams. Cognition
and Emotion (in press).
Curci, A., Luminet, O., Finkenauer, C., & Gisle, L. (2001). Flashbulb memories in social
groups: A comparative test-retest study of the memory of French President
Mitterrand’s death in a French and a Belgian group. Memory, 9, 81-101.
Curci, A., Rimé, B., Gisle, L., & Baruffol, E. (2007). Social sharing after emotion, its
temporal evolution and consequences for emotional recovery. Manuscript submitted
for publication.
69
Darwin, C. (1872/1965). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Delfosse C., Nils, F., Lasserre S., & Rimé B. (2004). Les motifs allégués du partage social et
de la rumination mentale des émotions: comparaison des épisodes positifs et négatifs
[Alleged motives for social sharing and mental rumination of emotions: a comparison
of positive and negative episodes ]. Cahiers Internationaux de Psychologie Sociale,
64, 35-44.
Dembo, T. (1931). Der anger als dynamisches problem [Anger as a dynamic problem].
Psychologische Forschung, 15, 1-144.
Dunahoo, C. L., Hobfoll, S. E., Monnier, J., Hulsizer, M. R., & Johnson, R. (1998). There’s
more than rugged individualism in coping. Part 1: Even the lone ranger had tonto.
Anxiety, Stress & Coping, 11, 137-165.
Dyregrov, A. (1997). The process in psychological debriefings. Journal of Traumatic Stress,
10, 589-605.
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V. & Ellsworth, P. C. (1972). Emotion in the human face: guidelines
for research and an integration of findings. New York: Pergamon
Epstein, S. (1973). The self-concept revisited, or a theory of a theory. American Psychologist,
28, 404-416.
Epstein, S. (1990). Cognitive-experiential self-theory. In L. Pervin (Ed.). Handbook of
personality: Theory and research (pp. 165-192). New York: Guilford Press.
Epstein, S. (1991). The self-concept, the traumatic neurosis, and the structure of personality.
In D. Ozer, J. M. Healey, Jr., & A. J. Stewart (Eds.), Perspectives on personality (Vol.
3, Part A, pp. 63-98). London: Jessica Kingsley.
Ersland, S., Weisaeth, L., & Sund, A. (1989). The stress upon rescuers involved in an oil rig
disaster. “Alexander Kielland” 1980. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 80, 38-49.
70
Espitalier, Tcherkassof, A., & Delmas (2002). Partage social des émotions et cohésion de
groupe [Social sharing of emotion and group cohesion]. In J.-M. Colletta & A.
Tcherkassof (Eds.), Perspectives actuelles sur les émotions. Cognition, langage et
développement (pp. 90-94). Sprimont, Belgique: Mardaga.
Esterling, B. A., Antoni, M. H., Fletcher, M. A., Margulies, S. & Schneiderman, N. (1994).
Emotional disclosure through writing or speaking modulates latent Epstein-Barr Virus
antibody titers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 130-140.
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7,117-140.
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University
Press.
Festinger, L., Riecken, H. W., & Schachter, S. (1956). When prophecy fails. Minneapolis:
University of Minnesota Press.
Finkenauer, C. & Rimé, B. (1998a). Socially shared emotional experiences vs. emotional
experiences kept secret: Differential characteristics and consequences. Journal of
Social and Clinical Psychology, 17, 295-318.
Finkenauer, C. & Rimé, B. (1998b). Keeping emotional memories secret: Health and
subjective well-being when emotions are not shared. Journal of Health Psychology, 3,
47-58.
Finkenauer, C., Gisle, L. & Luminet, O. (1997). When collective memories are socially
shaped: Flashbulb memories of socio-political events. In J. W. Pennebaker, D. Paez,
& B. Rimé (Eds.). Collective memories of political events: Social and psychological
perspectives (pp.191-208). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Finkenauer, C., Luminet, O., Gisle, L. El-Ahmadi, A., Van der Linden, M. & Philippot, P.
(1998). Flashbulb memories and the underlying mechanism of their formation:
Toward an emotional-integrative model. Memory and Cognition, 26, 516-531.
71
Finkenauer, F. & Rimé, B. (1996). [Motives for sharing emotion]. Unpublished raw data.
Fivush, R. (1994), Constructing narrative, emotion, and self in parent-child conversations
about past. In U. Neisser, & R. Fivush (Eds.), The remembering Self. Construction and
accuracy in the self-narrative (pp. 136-156). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Fivush, R., Haden, C., & Reese, E. (1999). Remembering, recounting, and reminiscing: The
development of autobiographical memory in social context. In D.C. Rubin (Ed.)
Remembering our past: Studies in autobiographical memory (pp. 341-359).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Frattaroli, J. (2006). Experimental Disclosure and Its Moderators: A Meta-Analysis.
Psychological Bulletin, 132, 823-865
Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., Impett, E. A., & Asher, E. R. (2004). What do you do when things
go right? The intrapersonal and interpersonal benefits of sharing positive events.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 228-245.
Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and women’s development.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma: Notes on the management of spoiled identity. Englewood Cliffs,
NJ.: Prentice Hall.
Gortner, E.M. & Pennebaker, J. (2003). The archival anatomy of a disaster: media coverage
and community-wide health effects of Texas A&M bonfire tragedy. Journal of Social
and Clinical Psychology, 22, 580-603.
Gross, J. J. (2007) (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation. New York: Guilford Publications.
Harber, K. D. & Cohen, D. J. (2005). The emotional broadcaster theory of social sharing.
Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 24, 382-400.
Heath, C., Bell, C., & Sternberg, E. (2001). Emotional selection in memes: The case of urban
legends. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,81, 1028-1041.
72
Harlow, H. F. (1959, June). Love in infant monkeys. Scientific American, 200, 68-74.
Hastie, R. (1984). Causes and effects of causal attribution. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 46, 44-56.
Herbette, G. & Rimé, B. (2004). Verbalization of emotion in chronic pain patients and their
psychological adjustments. Journal of Health Psychology, 9, 661-676.
Herbette, G. (2002). Chronic Illness and the response of others: Emotional, cognitive, and
social aspects. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Catholic University of Louvain,
Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium.
Hornstein, S.L., Brown, A.S., & Mulligan, N.W. (2003). Long-term flashbulb memory for
learning of Princess Diana’s death. Memory, 11, 293-306.
Horowitz, M. J. (1969). Psychic trauma: Return of images after a stress film. Archives of
General Psychiatry, 20, 552-559.
Horowitz, M. J. (1975). Intrusive and repetitive thoughts after experimental stress: A
summary. Archives of General Psychiatry, 32, 1457-1463.
Horowitz, M. J. (1976). Stress response syndromes. New York: Aronson.
Horowitz, M. J. (1979). Psychological response to serious life events. In V. Hamilton & D.
M. Warburton (Eds.), Human stress and cognition: An information processing
approach (pp. 235-263). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Horowitz, M. J. (1992). Stress response syndromes. Northvale, NJ : Jason Aronson (original
work published 1976).
Horowitz, M. J. & Becker, S. S. (1971). Cognitive response to stress and experimental
demand. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 78, 86-92.
Horowitz, M. J., & Becker, S. S. (1973). Cognitive response to erotic and stressful films.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 29, 81-84.
73
Janoff-Bulman, R. (1992). Shattered assumptions. Towards a new psychology of trauma. New
York: The Free Press.
Kastenbaum, R., & Aisenberg, R. (1972). The Psychology of Death. New York, NY:
Springer.
Kelly, G. A. (1955). A theory of personality: A psychology of personal construct. New York:
Norton.
Kennedy-Moore, E. & Watson, J. C. (1999). Expressing emotion: Myths, realities, and
therapeutic strategies. New York: Guilford Press.
Klinger, E. (1975). Consequences of commitment to and disengagement from incentives.
Psychological Review, 82, 1-25.
Kruglanski, A. (1996). Motivated social cognition: Principles of the interface. In E. T.
Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles
(pp. 493-521). New York: The Guilford Press.
Langston, C. A. (1994). Capitalizing on and coping with daily-life events: expressive
responses to positive events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1112-
1125.
Laurenceau, J.-P., Feldman-Barret, L., Pietromonaco, P. R. (1998). Intimacy as an
interpersonal process: The importance of self-disclosure, partner disclosure, and
perceived partner responsiveness in interpersonal exchanges. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 74, 1238-1251.
Laurens, F. (2000). La souffrance des infirmier(e)s : comment prévenir leur burn-out?
[Nurses suffering. How to prevent burn-out among nurses?]Mémoire de licence non
publié, Université Catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgique.
Lazarus, R. S. & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal and coping. New York: Springer.
74
Lazarus, R. S. , Opton, E. M. ,Monikos, M. S. , & Rankin, N. O. (1965). The principle of
short-circuiting of threat: Further evidence. Journal of Personality, 47, 909-917.
Lehman, D. R., Ellard, J. H. & Wortman, C. B. (1986). Social Support for the bereaved:
Recipients’ and providers’ perspectives on what is helpful. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 54(4), 438-446.
Lepore, S. J., Ragan, J. D., & Jones, S. (2000). Talking facilitates cognitive-emotional
processes of adaptation to an acute stressor. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 78, 499-508.
Lepore, S.J., & Revenson, T.A. (2006). Resilience and posttraumatic growth: Recovery,
resistance, & reconfiguration. In L. Calhoun R. G. Tedeschi (Eds.), The Handbook of
Posttraumatic Growth: Research and Practice (pp. 24-46). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Lepore, S.J., Fernandez-Berrocal, P., Ragan, J. & Ramos, N. (2004). It’s not that bad: social
challenges to emotional disclosure enhance adjustment to stress. Anxiety, Stress and
Coping, 17, 341-361.
Lewis, M. (2000). The emergence of human emotions. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland (Eds.),
Handbook of emotions (pp. 265-280). New York: Guilford Press.
Luminet, O., Bouts, P., Delie, F., Manstead, A. S. R., & Rimé, B. (2000). Social sharing of
emotion following exposure to a negatively valenced situation. Cognition and
Emotion, 14, 661-688.
Luminet, O., Curci, A., Marsh, E. J., Wessel, I., Constantin, T., Gencoz, F., & Yogo, M.
(2004). The cognitive, emotional and social impacts of the September 11th attacks:
Group differences in memory for the reception context and the determinants of
flashbulb memory . Journal of General Psychology, 131 (3), 197-224.
MacLean, P. D. (1949). Psychosomatic disease and the “visceral brain”: recent developments
bearing on the Papez theory of emotion. Psychosomatic Medicine, 11, 338-353.
75
Mandler, G. (1984). Mind and body: Psychology of emotion and stress. New York: Norton.
Martin, L. L., & Tesser A., (1989). Toward a motivational and structural theory of ruminative
thought. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended Thought (pp. 306-326).
New York: Guilford.
McCammon, S., Durham, T. W., Allison, E. & Williamson, J. E. (1988). Emergency
workers’ cognitive appraisal and coping with traumatic events. Journal of Traumatic
Stress, 1, 353-371.
Mendolia, M., & Kleck, R. E. (1993). Effects of talking about a stressful event on arousal:
Does what we talk about make a difference? Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 64, 283-292.
Mesquita, B. (1993). Cultural variations in emotion: A comparative study of Dutch,
Surinamese and Turkish people in the Netherlands. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
Mikulincer, M. & Shaver, P. R. (Eds) (2007) Attachment in Adulthood: Structure,
Dynamics, and Change. New York: The Guilford Press.
Mitchell, G. W., & Glickman, A. S. (1977). Cancer patients: Knowledge and attitude. Cancer,
40, 61-66.
Mitchell, J. T., & Everly, G. S. (1995). Critical incident stress debriefing (CISD) and the
prevention of work-related traumatic stress among high risk occupational groups. In
G. S. Everly & J. M. Lating (Eds.), Psychotraumatology. Key papers and core
concepts in post-traumatic stress (pp. 267-280). New York: Plenum Press.
Moscovici, S. (1984). The phenomenon of social representations. In R. M. Farr and S.
Moscovici (Eds.), Social Representations pp. 3-69. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
76
Nils, F., & Rimé, B. (2008). Social sharing of emotion, listeners’ reponses and emotion
regulation. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Nils, F., Delfosse, C., & Rimé, B. (2005). Partage social des émotions : Raisons invoquées et
médiateurs perçus [Social sharing of emotions: Alleged reasons and perceived
mediators]. Manuscript submitted for publication. University of Louvain at Louvain-
la-Neuve, Belgium.
Park, C. L. & Folkman, S. (1997. Meaning in the context of stress and coping, Review of
General Psychology, 2, 115–144
Parkes, C. M. (1972). Bereavement: Studies of grief in adult life. London, UK: Tavistock
Publications
Pennebaker, J. W. & Beall, S. (1986). Confronting a traumatic event: Toward an
understanding of inhibition and disease. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 274-
281.
Pennebaker, J. W. & Harber, K. (1993). A social stage model of collective coping: The Loma
Prieta earthquake and Persian Gulf war. Journal of Social Issues, 49, 125-145.
Pennebaker, J. W. (1989). Confession, inhibition and disease. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 22, pp. 211-244). Orlando, FL:
Academic Press.
Pennebaker, J. W., Colder, M., & Sharp, L. K. (1990). Accelerating the coping process.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 528-537.
Pennebaker, J. W., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. & Glaser, R. (1988). Disclosure of traumas and
immune function: Health implications for psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 56, 239-245.
Peters, P. & Kashima, Y. (2007). From social talk to social action: shaping the social triad.
with emotion sharing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 780-797.
77
Peters-Golden, H. (1982). Breast cancer: Varied perceptions of social support in the illness
experience. Social Science and Medicine, 16, 483-491.
Piaget, J. (1946). La psychologie de l’intelligence [The psychology of intelligence]. Paris:
Collin.
Preston, S. D. & de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Brain
and Behavioral Sciences, 25, 1-72
Proust, M. (1913). A la recherche du temps perdu: Du côté de chez Swann [In search of lost
time: Swann’s way]. Paris: Gallimard.
Reis, H. T., & Patrick, B. C. (1996). Attachment and intimacy: Component processes. In E. T.
Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles
(pp. 523-563). New York: Guilford Press.
Riger, S. (1993). What’s Wrong with Empowerment», American Journal of Community
Psychology, 21, 279-292.
Rimé, B. (1989). Le partage social des émotions.[The social sharing of emotion] In B. Rimé
& K. Scherer (Eds.), Les émotions (pp. 271-303). Neuchâtel: Delachaux et Niestlé.
Rimé, B. (2005). Le partage social des émotions [The social sharing of emotions]. Paris:
Presses Universitaires de France.
Rimé, B. (2007a). The social sharing of emotion as an interface between individual and
collective processes in the construction of emotional climates. Journal of Social
Issues, 63, 307-322.
Rimé, B. (2007b). Interpersonal emotion regulation. In J. J. Gross (ed.), Handbook of emotion
regulation (pp. 466-485). New York: Guilford Publications.
Rimé, B., Delfosse, C., & Corsini, S. (2005). Emotional fascination. Responses Elicited by
Viewing Pictures of September 11 Attack. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 923-932.
78
Rimé, B., Dozier, S., Vandenplas, C. & Declercq, M. (1996). Social sharing of emotion in
children. In N. Frijda (Ed.), ISRE 96. Proceedings of the IXth Conference of the
International Society for Research in Emotion (pp. 161-163). Toronto, Ontario: ISRE.
Rimé, B., Finkenauer, C., Luminet, O., Zech, E., & Philippot, P. (1998). Social sharing of
emotion: New evidence and new questions. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.),
European review of social psychology (Vol. 9, pp. 145-189). Chichester, UK: John
Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Rimé, B., Hayward, M. S. & Pennebaker, J. W. (1996). [Characteristics of recovered vs.
unrecovered emotional experiences]. Unpublished raw data.
Rimé, B., Mesquita, B. , Philippot, P., & Boca, S. (1991a). Beyond the emotional event: Six
studies on the social sharing of emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 5, 435-465.
Rimé, B., Noël, P., & Philippot, P. (1991b). Episode émotionnel, réminiscences cognitives et
réminiscences sociales. Cahiers Internationaux de Psychologie Sociale , 11, 93-104
Rimé, B., Philippot, P., Boca, S., & Mesquita, B. (1992). Long lasting cognitive and social
consequences of emotion: Social sharing and rumination. In W. Stroebe & M.
Hewstone (Eds.), European Review of Social Psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 225-258).
Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Rimé, B., Philippot, P., Finkenauer, C., Legast, S., Moorkens, P. & Tornqvist, J. (1994).
Mental rumination and social sharing in emotion: Diary investigations of the
cognitive and social aftermath of emotional events. Unpublished manuscript,
University of Louvain at Louvain-la-Neuve.
Rimé, B., Yogo, M., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1996). [Social sharing of emotion across cultures].
Unpublished raw data.
Rose, S., & Bisson, J. (1998). Brief early psychological interventions following trauma: A
systematic review of the literature. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 11, 697-710.
79
Sampson, E. E. (1983).Justice and the critique of pure psychology. New York: Plenum Press.
Schachter, S. (1959). The psychology of affiliation. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Schaefer, A & Philippot, P. (2005). Selective effects of emotion on the phenomenal
characteristics of autobiographical memories. Memory, 13, 148-160
Scheff, T. J. (1979). Catharsis in healing, ritual, and drama. Berkeley, CA: University of
California Press.
Shaver, P. & Klinnert, M. (1982). Schachter’s theories of affiliation and emotion:
Implications of developmental research. Review of Personality and Social Psychology,
3, 37-71.
Shortt, J. W. & Pennebaker, J. W. (1992). Talking versus hearing about Holocaust
experiences. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 13, 165-179.
Singh-Manoux, A. & Finkenauer, C. (2001). Cultural Variations in social sharing of
emotions: An intercultural perspective on a universal phenomenon. Journal of Cross-
Cultural Psychology, 32, 647-661.
Sroufe, L. A. & Waters, E. (1976). The ontogenesis of smiling and laughter: A perspective on
the organization of development in infancy. Psychological Review, 83, 173-189.
Strack, F. & Coyne, J. C. (1983). Shared and private reaction to depression. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 798-806.
Stroebe W. et Stroebe M., 1996). The Social Psychology of Social Support. In E. T. Higgins
and A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles (pp.
597-621). New York and London: The Guilford Press.
Sydor, G. & Philippot, P. (1996). Conséquences psychologiques des massacres de 1994 au
Rwanda [Psychological consequences of massacres in Rwanda in 1994]. Santé
Mentale au Québec, 21, 229-248.
80
Tait, R. & Silver, R. C. (1989). Coming to term with major negative life events. In J. S.
Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 351-382). New York: The
Guilford Press.
Tangney, J. P. (1991). Moral affect: The good, the bad, and the ugly. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 61, 598-607.
Taylor, S. E. (1983). Adjustment to threatening events. A theory of cognitive adaptation.
American Psychologist, 38, 1161-1173.
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological
perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193-210.
Thoits, P. A. (1984). Coping, social support, and psychological outcomes. In P. Shaver (Ed.),
Review of Personality and Social Psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 219-238). Beverly Hills:
Sage.
Van Emmerik, A. A., Kamphuis, J. H., Hulsbosch, A. M., & Emmelkamp, P. M. (2002).
Single session debriefing after psychological trauma: A meta-analysis. The Lancet,
360, 766-771.
Vergara, A. (1993). Sexo e identidad de genero: Diferencias en el conomiento social
de las emociones en el modo de compatirlas [Sex and gender identity:
Differences in social knowledge on emotions and in ways of sharing them] .
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Universidad del Pais Vasco, San Sebastian,
Spain.
Weick, K. (1995). Sensemaking in organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Weisaeth, L. (1989). A study of behavioral responses to an industrial disaster. Acta
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 80 (Suppl. 355), 13-24.
81
Wetzer, I. M., Zeelenberg, M., & Pieters, R. (2005). Motivations for socially sharing
emotions: Why being specific matters. Manuscript sublitted for publication. Tilburg
University, NL: Department of Psychology.
Wilkinson, C. B. (1983). Aftermath of a disaster: The collapse of the Hyatt Regency Hotel
skywalks. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 1134-1139.
Wortman, C. B. & Dunkel-Schetter, C. (1979). Interpersonal relationships and cancer: a
theoretical analysis. Journal of Social Issues, 35, 120-155.
Wortman, C. B., & Lehman, D R. (1985). Reactions to victims of life crises: Support attempts
that fail. In I. G. Sarason & B. R. Sarason (Eds.), Social support: Theory, research,
and applications (pp. 463-489). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoof.
Yogo, M. & Onoe, K. (1998). The social sharing of emotion among Japanese students. Poster
session presented at ISRE ‘98, The Biannual conference of the International Society
for Research on Emotion, held in Wuerzburg, Germany, August 4-8, 1998.
Zech, E. & Rimé, B. (2005) Is it talking about an emotional experience helpful? Effects on
emotional recovery and perceived benefits. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy,
12, 270-287.
Zech, E. (1994). La gestion du deuil et la gestion des émotions [Coping with grief and coping
with emotions]. Unpublished licentiate thesis, University of Louvain, Louvain-la-
Neuve, Belgium.
Zech, E. (2000). The effects of the communication of emotional experiences. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation. Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium: University of Louvain
Zech, E., Rimé, B., DeSoir, E., Versporten, A. & Van Oyen, H. (2007). Social Sharing of
Emotion or Suppression of Social Sharing After a Traumatic Experience? Conflicting
proclivities coocur in PTSD victims of the Ghislenghien Disaster. Manuscript
submitted for publication.
82
Zech, E., Ucros, C., Rimé, B., & DeSoir, E. (2002). [The emotional impact of a train crash
among federal police service personnel]. Unpublished raw data.
83
Table 1 A synthesis of alleged motives for sharing an emotional episode
(Rimé, 2007b)
Classes of motives
Typical members of the class
Rehearsing Reminding, re-experiencing, remembering
Venting Expressing, looking for relief, letting off steam, alleviating,
looking for catharsis
Help and support
Obtaining comfort, support, sympathy, help, receiving
comprehension, being listened to
Comfort/consolation Receiving consolation, receiving comfort
Legitimization, validation Being legitimized, approved, understood
Clarification and
meaning Finding understanding, finding explanation, clarification,
meaning
Advice and solutions Obtaining advice, feedback, guidance, receiving the perspective
of another person, receiving advice, finding solutions,
suggestions
Bonding, strengthening
social ties
Being in touch, relating, escaping loneliness or the feeling of
abandonment, strengthening social ties, decreasing
interpersonal distance, feeling closer to others
Arousing empathy
Touching/moving others, affecting the target, moving the
listener
Gaining attention
Receiving attention, eliciting interest, impressing others,
distinguishing oneself
Entertaining
“Lubricating” social interactions
84
Figure Captions
Figure 1 (a) Key elements in the dynamic of goal-pursuit in current life; (b)Multilevel
consequences of emotional experiences.
Figure 2. A model of the temporal evolution of emotional memories.
Figure 3. Interpersonal dynamic of the social sharing of emotion.
Figure 4. The social sharing of emotion propagation wave (from Rimé, 2005)
Figure 5. A model of the double impact of a past negative emotional episode