ArticlePDF Available

Differences in Parental Investment Contribute to Important Differences Between Men and Women

Authors:

Abstract

Parental investment theory addresses sex differences that result from the trade-off between parenting effort and mating effort. For example, relative to men, women spend more time caring for offspring, are more selective in assenting to sexual intercourse, are more upset by a partner's emotional infidelity than by a partner's sexual infidelity, and are better able to inhibit their behaviors in certain situations. These and other sex differences are attributable to evolved mechanisms that work in interaction with the physical and social environments.
86 VOLUME 8, NUMBER 3, JUNE 1999
Sex differences in behaviors tra-
ditionally have been attributed to
“social” factors, because of the pre-
sumed flexibility of human behav-
ior. Proposals that evolved mecha-
nisms explain why men and
women behave as they do have
been regarded warily because of
the belief that “biological” causa-
tion (including evolutionary caus-
es) implies biological determinism.
Modern evolutionary psychologi-
cal theory, however, makes no such
claims, but argues that contrasting
“social” with “evolutionary” expla-
nations is a false dichotomy (see
Tooby & Cosmides, 1992). From
this perspective, human nature in-
cludes evolved psychological
mechanisms that require input
such as social norms and cultural
beliefs for their operation, and dif-
ferent behavioral outcomes will re-
sult from these evolved mecha-
nisms in different environments. In
this article, we apply a theory moti-
vated by such evolutionary psy-
chological logic, parental invest-
ment theory (Trivers, 1972), to
interpret some well-documented
sex differences.
According to an evolutionary
psychological perspective (see
Buss, 1994), ancestral men and
women faced different adaptive
problems that threatened their sur-
vival and reproduction. As a result,
they evolved different psychologi-
cal mechanisms, and evidence of
this ancient heritage is apparent in
modern humans (for a review, see
Buss, 1994). Parental investment
theory accounts for many of these
differences. According to this theo-
ry, there is a conflict for both males
and females in how much time, ef-
fort, and resources to invest in mat-
ing versus parenting. For many
species, including humans, males
need to invest substantially less in
parenting than females to achieve
successful reproduction. In mam-
mals, fertilization and gestation
occur within the female, and, after
birth, mothers provide the primary
nutritional support for their off-
spring until they are weaned. Male
investment in offspring may be as
little as the sperm produced during
copulation. In humans, paternal in-
vestment is not essential for a
man’s offspring to reach adult-
hood, and although the amount of
time men spend in child care varies
across cultures, fathers spend less
time interacting with and caring for
their children than do mothers in
all cultures that have been studied
(see Geary, 1998).
Yet human males spend more
time caring for their offspring than
is typical for male mammals. One
factor contributing to this unusually
high level of paternal investment is
humans’ extended childhood.
Humans spend more time as juve-
niles than other primates, needing
many years to develop the brain
and the knowledge necessary to
navigate the complexities of society.
This prolonged immaturity requires
a supportive environment to which
both mothers and fathers ideally
contribute. Evidence from many
cultures and historical records indi-
cates that the death rate of offspring
increases as a function of father’s
absence, particularly in harsh envi-
ronments (see Geary, 1998). Thus,
whereas mothers opt to invest sub-
stantially in their children’s care
after birth in order to ensure their
offsprings’ survival, the case is not
as clear-cut for fathers, particularly
when one considers the number of
additional offspring men can have
by investing more effort in mating.
Women also may choose to invest
less in the care of a child and more
in mating, but the caloric costs and
duration of pregnancy and nursing
(in traditional societies today, and
surely in our evolutionary past,
nursing extends to the age of 3 or 4
years) reduce the number of chil-
dren a woman can expect to have in
her lifetime.
There is evidence that paternal
investment influences socialization
practices and the amount of
parental investment subsequent
generations devote to their off-
spring (Belsky, Steinberg, & Draper,
1991). In environments where fa-
thers are absent or where there is
marital discord, the resulting stress
produces harsh and inconsistent
child care and insecure attachment.
Published by Blackwell Publishers, Inc.
Differences in Parental Investment
Contribute to Important Differences
Between Men and Women
David F. Bjorklund and Todd K. Shackelford1
Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, Boca Raton, Florida
Abstract
Parental investment theory
addresses sex differences that
result from the trade-off be-
tween parenting effort and
mating effort. For example,
relative to men, women spend
more time caring for offspring,
are more selective in assenting
to sexual intercourse, are more
upset by a partner’s emotional
infidelity than by a partner’s
sexual infidelity, and are better
able to inhibit their behaviors
in certain situations. These and
other sex differences are attrib-
utable to evolved mechanisms
that work in interaction with
the physical and social envi-
ronments.
Keywords
parental investment theory;
evolutionary psychology; sex-
ual strategies
CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 87
Under such situations, children
reach puberty early, form short-
term and unstable relationships,
and invest relatively little in their
own offspring. In stressful and un-
certain environments, there is a ten-
dency to invest more in mating (for
both sexes) than in parenting. The
pattern is reversed for children
growing up in harmonious homes
and homes where the father is pres-
ent; such children mature later,
postpone sexual activity, and dis-
play greater investment in the fewer
children they produce. In sum, the
availability of resources, which is
related to paternal investment and
spousal harmony, leads to different
patterns of socialization, resulting
in differential parental investment
in the next generation.
The differential investment of
males and females in their off-
spring has resulted in the evolution
of different ways of behaving and
thinking in men and women.
Although these differences are gen-
erated by evolved psychological
mechanisms, they are more vari-
able than is typical in mammals.
Here, we discuss two areas in
which men and women behave and
think differently, as predicted by
parental investment theory: sexual
strategies and inhibitory abilities.
One class of sex differences that
can be understood in terms of
parental investment theory is sexu-
al strategies (Buss, 1994). Males in
most mammals can achieve
tremendous reproductive success
by inseminating many females,
making males relatively indiscrim-
inate when it comes to choosing a
sex partner. Females, in contrast,
have much more invested, at least
potentially, in a single copulation.
The possibility of pregnancy, and
the time and energy spent caring
for the resulting offspring, favored
ancestral females who were selec-
tive in mating. Because of the long
period of immaturity in humans,
ancient women’s reproductive in-
terests were often best served by
selecting a mate who not only
would provide good genes (e.g., as
signaled by facial symmetry;
Shackelford & Larsen, 1997), but
who also would invest in her and
her offspring. Over evolutionary
time, it was also in men’s reproduc-
tive interests to see to it that their
offspring received the support nec-
essary to survive to reproductive
age. But the amount and duration
of investment necessary to ensure
the survival of offspring was less
for men than for women. Thus, al-
though both men and women
shared a common reproductive
goal (getting their offspring to
adulthood), the optimal level of in-
vestment to achieve this goal was
unequal for the sexes, placing
males’ and females’ reproductive
interests in conflict.
Men are not indiscriminate
when it comes to selecting a mate,
especially when selecting a long-
term partner. Men around the
world want long-term partners
who are attractive, intelligent, and
kind. Women also desire attractive,
intelligent, and kind men as hus-
bands, but, as predicted by
parental investment theory, they
rate financial resources as more im-
portant in a mate than do men
(Buss, 1989). Despite the selectivity
that men and women universally
display in choosing a long-term
partner, and consistent with
parental investment theory, women
are more selective in granting sex,
and men are more eager to have ca-
sual sex (see Buss, 1994).
Men and women have evolved
different psychological mecha-
nisms as solutions to the adaptive
problems unique to their sex. For
example, fertilization occurs within
the female, and it is the female who
gestates and gives birth to the off-
spring. This greater prenatal in-
vestment of the female comes at
considerable cost but brings with it
certainty of maternity. In contrast,
males, who may invest only sperm
and the energy necessary to copu-
late, cannot be certain of paternity.
Furthermore, because women con-
ceal ovulation and are potentially
sexually receptive throughout their
menstrual cycle, it is difficult for a
man to know when copulation is
likely to result in pregnancy. As a
result, being the unwitting social
father to another man’s genetic off-
spring is a possibility for men. This
in fact occurs in between 2% and
30% of all births; the rate is similar
in traditional societies, and there is
no reason to believe that it differed
substantially for our ancestors (see
Baker & Bellis, 1995). Women, of
course, cannot so easily be fooled
into rearing another woman’s child
who they believe is their own, al-
though they do risk losing a mate’s
investment to another woman.
One consequence of this sex dif-
ference in the certainty of genetic
parenthood can be seen in men’s
and women’s reactions to a long-
term partner’s infidelity. When
asked to imagine that their long-
term partner is either (a) having ca-
sual sex with another person or (b)
developing a close emotional rela-
tionship with another person, men
and women respond differently.
Verbal reports and measures of
physiological arousal indicate that
women are more upset by a part-
ner’s emotional infidelity, which
could signal a loss of resources,
whereas men are more upset by a
partner’s sexual infidelity (Buss,
Larsen, Westen, & Semmelroth,
1992).
In conclusion, human sex differ-
ences in parental investment predict
sex differences in sexual strategies.
These sex differences include the
greater inclination of men to pursue
short-term casual sex; the greater
selectivity of women in choosing a
mate, especially in the context of
short-term mating; and the greater
SEXUAL STRATEGIES
Copyright © 1999 American Psychological Society
88 VOLUME 8, NUMBER 3, JUNE 1999
distress of men in response to a
long-term partner’s sexual infideli-
ty and of women in response to a
partner’s emotional infidelity.
Inhibition refers to the withhold-
ing of a response in situations in
which that response otherwise
would be made. The ability to in-
hibit inappropriate sexual and ag-
gressive responses is important for
the success of both men and women
in modern (and presumably ances-
tral) society and may have played a
role in the evolution of human intel-
ligence, particularly social intelli-
gence (Bjorklund & Harnishfeger,
1995). However, ancestral women
may have needed greater inhibitory
abilities than ancestral men in
certain contexts, because of their
different reproductive strategies
(Bjorklund & Kipp, 1996). For exam-
ple, because of ancestral women’s
greater investment in the potential
consequences of an act of copula-
tion, it might have been in their re-
productive interests to have greater
control of their sexual arousal and
related behaviors in order to more
closely evaluate the value of a man
before assenting to sex. Ancestral
women also may have needed sub-
stantial political skill in order to
keep sexual interests in other men
hidden from a mate. Male response
to suspected female infidelity can
be violent, and even when adultery
does not lead to aggression, it often
leads to divorce, which both histor-
ically and in contemporary societies
is more detrimental to a woman and
her offspring than to a man (see
Buss, 1994).
Similarly, the bulk of child-care
responsibilities falls to women, and
these also may require enhanced
inhibitory abilities. For example,
parents often must put the needs of
their infants ahead of their own,
delaying gratification of their own
desires and resisting distractions
that would take them away from
their infants. They also must inhib-
it aggression toward infants or
young children who may cry con-
tinuously, disobey, and damage
personal property.
In support of these hypotheses,
research has shown that women
are better able to control the ex-
pression of their emotions than are
men, despite the fact that women
are more emotionally expressive
than men. In studies in which peo-
ple are asked to display a positive
emotion after a negative experience
(e.g., pretending that a foul-tasting
drink tastes good) or vice versa, fe-
males (from the age of 4 years and
up) are better able to control their
emotional expressions (i.e., fool a
judge watching their reactions)
than are males (e.g., Cole, 1986). In
other research, there is evidence
that females are better than males
on tasks that involve resisting
temptation and delaying gratifica-
tion (e.g., Kochanska, Murray,
Jacques, Koenig, & Vandegeest,
1996), precisely the pattern one
would predict if selection pressures
associated with child care were
greater on ancestral females than
on ancestral males. And there is
limited evidence that women are
better able to inhibit sexual arousal
than are men (Cerny, 1978; Rosen,
1973). In contrast, there is no fe-
male advantage in inhibitory abili-
ties for cognitive tasks such as se-
lective attention, suggesting that
the sex differences are relatively
domain-specific, restricted to abili-
ties related to sexual and parenting
contexts, as predicted by parental
investment theory.
Different psychological mecha-
nisms in men and women, attribut-
able to sex differences in minimum
parental investment over evolu-
tionary history, provide the skeletal
features for adapted behaviors.
They represent biases that, in the
distant past, served men’s and
women’s reproductive fitness well.
However, humans exert greater in-
tentional control over sexual be-
havior than any other animal, and
the evolved sexual and child-care
strategies of men and women are
not invariantly manifested, but in-
stead are responsive to physical
and social environments over the
course of development. From this
perspective, nature and nurture are
not alternative explanations, but
instead are two sides of the same
explanatory coin.
Note
1. Address correspondence to either
David F. Bjorklund, Department of
Psychology, Florida Atlantic Uni-
versity, Boca Raton, FL 33431, e-mail:
dbjorklund@fau.edu, or Todd K.
Shackelford, Division of Science—
Psychology, Florida Atlantic Uni-
versity, 2912 College Ave., Davie, FL
33314, e-mail: tshackel@fau.edu.
References
Baker, R.R., & Bellis, M.A. (1995). Human sperm
competition. London: Chapman & Hall.
Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991).
Childhood experience, interpersonal develop-
ment, and reproductive strategy: An evolu-
Acknowledgments—We would like to
thank David Buss, David Geary, Erika
Hoff-Ginsberg, Martha Hubertz, Gregg
LeBlanc, Santo Tarantino, Robin
Vallacher, and Viviana Weekes, who pro-
vided helpful comments and sugges-
tions that improved this article.
CONCLUSIONS
INHIBITORY ABILITIES
Published by Blackwell Publishers, Inc.
Recommended Reading
Bjorklund, D.F., & Kipp, K. (1996).
(See References)
Buss, D.M. (1994). (See References)
Buss, D.M. (1999). Evolutionary psy-
chology: The new science of the
mind. Needham Heights, MA:
Allyn & Bacon.
Geary, D.C. (1998). (See References)
Trivers, R. (1972). (See References)
CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 89
tionary theory of socialization. Child
Development, 62, 647–670.
Bjorklund, D.F., & Harnishfeger, K.K. (1995). The
role of inhibition mechanisms in the evolution
of human cognition. In F.N. Dempster & C.J.
Brainerd (Eds.), New perspectives on interference
and inhibition in cognition (pp. 141–173). New
York: Academic Press.
Bjorklund, D.F., & Kipp, K. (1996). Parental invest-
ment theory and gender differences in the evo-
lution of inhibition mechanisms. Psychological
Bulletin, 120, 163–188.
Buss, D.M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate
preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in
37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12,
1–49.
Buss, D.M. (1994). The evolution of desire. New York:
Basic Books.
Buss, D.M., Larsen, R.J., Westen, D., &
Semmelroth, J. (1992). Sex differences in jeal-
ousy: Evolution, physiology, and psychology.
Psychological Science, 3, 251–255.
Cerny, J.A. (1978). Biofeedback and the voluntary
control of sexual arousal in women. Behavior
Therapy, 9, 847–855.
Cole, P.M. (1986). Children’s spontaneous control
of facial expression. Child Development, 57,
1309–1321.
Geary, D.C. (1998). Male, female: The evolution of
human sex differences. Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Kochanska, G., Murray, K., Jacques, J.Y., Koenig,
A.L., & Vandegeest, K.A. (1996). Inhibitory
control in young children and its role in
emerging internalization. Child Development,
67, 490–507.
Rosen, R.C. (1973). Suppression of penile tumes-
cence by instrumental conditioning.
Psychometric Medicine, 35, 509–514.
Shackelford, T.K., & Larsen, R.J. (1997). Facial
asymmetry as an indicator of psychological,
emotional, and physiological distress. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 72,
456–466.
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological
foundations of culture. In J. Barkow, L.
Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind
(pp. 19–136). New York: Oxford University
Press.
Trivers, R. (1972). Parental investment and sexual
selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selection
and the descent of man (pp. 136–179). New York:
Aldine de Gruyter.
Copyright © 1999 American Psychological Society
The phenomena of attempted
and completed suicide are trou-
bling and mysterious enough in
themselves; the possibility that sui-
cide is socially contagious, even
more so. This article considers
whether suicide clusters exist, and
if so, whether “contagion” process-
es can account for them.
There is a potentially important
distinction between the terms sui-
cide cluster and suicide contagion.
A cluster refers to the factual oc-
currence of two or more complet-
ed or attempted suicides that are
nonrandomly “bunched” in space
or time (e.g., a series of suicide at-
tempts in the same high school or
a series of completed suicides in
response to the suicide of a
celebrity). The term cluster implies
nothing about why the cluster
came to be, only that it came to be.
By contrast, contagion refers to a
possible explanation (as I argue
later, a fairly vague explanation) of
why a cluster developed. Clusters
(of a sort) appear to occur, but the
status of contagion as the reason
for such occurrences is more
equivocal.
Given that attempted and com-
pleted suicides are relatively rare,
and given that they tend to be
more or less evenly distributed in
space and time (e.g., suicides occur
at roughly the same rate in various
regions of the United States and
occur at roughly the same rate re-
gardless of the day of the week or
the month), it is statistically un-
likely that suicides would cluster
by chance alone. Yet cluster they
do, at least under some circum-
stances. (Such clustering is often
termed the “Werther effect,” after
a fictional character of Goethe’s
whose suicide purportedly in-
spired actual suicides in 18th-cen-
tury Europe.) Two general types of
suicide cluster have been dis-
cussed in the literature: mass clus-
ters and point clusters. Mass clus-
ters are media related; point
clusters, local.
CLUSTERS—OF A SORT—
APPEAR TO OCCUR
The Clustering and Contagion
of Suicide
Thomas E. Joiner, Jr.1
Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida
Abstract
Two general types of sui-
cide cluster have been dis-
cussed in the literature;
roughly, these can be classi-
fied as mass clusters and
point clusters. Mass clusters
are media related, and the ev-
idence for them is equivocal;
point clusters are local phe-
nomena, and these do appear
to occur. Contagion has not
been conceptually well devel-
oped nor empirically well
supported as an explanation
for suicide clusters. An alter-
native explanation for why
suicides sometimes cluster is
articulated: People who are
vulnerable to suicide may
cluster well before the occur-
rence of any overt suicidal
stimulus, and when they ex-
perience severe negative
events, including but not lim-
ited to the suicidal behavior of
one member of the cluster, all
members of the cluster are at
increased risk for suicidality
(a risk that may be offset by
good social support).
Keywords
suicide clusters; suicide con-
tagion
... The information exchanged in stories and gossip is likely to help people solve recurrent adaptive challenges. For women, one such challenge is avoiding threats to reproductive choice posed by sexual coercion and assault (e.g., Bjorklund & Shackelford, 1999;McDonald et al., 2021). Past research has demonstrated that women are motivated to consume information that they believe will help them prevent their own sexual assault victimization (McDonald et al., 2021;Vicary & Fraley, 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
Despite its bad reputation, gossip plays an important role in communicating and policing the social norms, morals, and values of a community. People are likely to be particularly attuned to gossip that helps solve recurrent adaptive challenges. Among women, sexual assault is a pervasive threat to reproductive choice that exacts serious costs on women’s reproductive fitness. Research has demonstrated that women fear sexual assault and are motivated to engage in behaviors to reduce the threat of being victimized. Here we propose that women may gossip about sexual assault as a means of protecting themselves and others. Participants read a series of vignettes describing instances of sexual assault of a female victim and were asked to indicate how likely they would be to share that information with a variety of recipients, and what factors motivated their sharing intentions. Results indicated that, overall, sexual assault gossip was especially likely to be shared with proximal female family and friends, as well as authority figures. Women were more likely to share sexual assault gossip than men, and this gender effect was strongest when sharing gossip with female family and friends. The strongest motivations for sharing gossip were to warn the recipient, damage the reputation of the perpetrator, and check agreement with the recipient, with women being more motivated to damage the perpetrator’s reputation than men were. Women who expressed a greater fear of rape were more likely to share the information with all recipients except proximal male friends, and reported stronger motivations to share in order to damage the perpetrator’s reputation and check agreement with the recipient. Results are consistent with the idea that women may use gossip to create a whisper-network of information exchange that helps women protect themselves and others.
... While it has been robustly documented across multiple studies that men are more likely than women to prefer short-term mating (e.g., Buss and Schmitt 1993;Gangestad and Simpson 2000;Kenrick et al. 1990Kenrick et al. , 1993Li and Kenrick 2006), humans are also among the few mammals where males develop long-term bonds with their mates and invest in care and nurturance of offspring (Geary 2005). As humans spend more time in a juvenile state than other mammals, requiring more years of brain and social development before being capable of functioning independently (Björklund and Shackelford 1999), fatherhood as an adaptation carries with it improved fitness of offspring, such as lower child mortality and greater social competitiveness of offspring later in life (e.g., Kaplan et al. 1998). Additionally, human males have relatively higher levels of paternity certainty and lower levels of reproductive success from multiple short-term matings compared to other mammals (Geary 2005), the latter resulting from adaptations in our female ancestors that are both physical (e.g., concealed ovulation; Shackelford et al. 2004) and psychological (e.g., aversion to casual sex and preference for commitment; Schmitt et al. 2001). ...
... Fathers and mothers play different roles in individual development [23], and the traditional family model in China influences division of labor, with mothers primarily responsible for direct caregiving while fathers typically provide material support [24]. Research indicates that individuals with lower caregiver education levels are at higher risk of experiencing emotional and behavioral problems following parent-child separation [25]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective The present study aimed to investigate the long-term effects of parent–child separation during infancy and early childhood on depression, social relationships including parent–child and peer relationships, and academic performance during adolescence and early adulthood. Methods Data from the China Family Panel Studies (CFPS) were analyzed, which included a sample of 3829 children aged 4–15 years from 25 provinces over a period of 8 years. The study examined the association between early parent–child separation and outcomes related to depression, social and academic performance, comparing outcomes between individuals with and without early separation experiences. A series of subgroup analyses were conducted to further explore these associations. Results Parent–child separation lasting 3 months or longer was found to be associated with moderate to severe levels of depression and impaired social relationships during adolescence and early adulthood, particularly among males, adolescents, urban dwellers, and those with less educated mothers. Children who experienced parent–child separation for 3 months or longer showed a positive correlation between separation duration and depression. Short-term separations under 3 months did not show this association. The duration of separation also had a negative correlation with parent–child and peer relationships, as well as academic performance. Conclusion Early parent–child separation has significant adverse effects on the mental health, social and academic performance of adolescents and early adulthood, especially among males, adolescents, urban residents, and those with lower maternal education. The severity of depression was found to be related to the duration of separation, highlighting the importance of minimizing separation to less than 3 months for children under the age of 3. These findings underscore the critical role of early parental care and the need for targeted interventions for high-risk populations.
Article
Full-text available
Based on the tenets of parental investment theory, the authors postulate that there was greater pressure to inhibit potentially maladaptive emotional, social, and sexual responses on prehistoric women than men in some contexts, resulting in enhanced inhibitory abilities in women in some domains. They reviewed studies whose researchers examined gender differences on social, behavioral, and cognitive tasks involving inhibition and found gender differences favoring female humans most consistent for social tasks (e.g., control of emotions), somewhat less pronounced for behavioral tasks (e.g., delay of gratification), and weak and inconsistent for cognitive tasks (e.g., conceptual tempo). This pattern was interrupted as being consistent with the position that gender differences in inhibition are relatively domain specific in nature, with women demonstrating greater abilities on tasks related to reproduction and childrearing, which is consistent with parental investment theory.
Chapter
Full-text available
Article
Full-text available
In species with internal female fertilization, males risk both lowered paternity probability and investment in rival gametes if their mates have sexual contact with other males. Females of such species do not risk lowered maternity probability through partner infidelity, but they do risk the diversion of their mates' commitment and resources to rival females. Three studies tested the hypothesis that sex differences in jealousy emerged in humans as solutions to the respective adaptive problems faced by each sex. In Study 1, men and women selected which event would upset them more—a partner's sexual infidelity or emotional infidelity. Study 2 recorded physiological responses (heart rate, electrodermal response, corrugator supercilii contraction) while subjects imagined separately the two types of partner infidelity. Study 3 tested the effect of being in a committed sexual relationship on the activation of jealousy. All studies showed large sex differences, confirming hypothesized sex linkages in jealousy activation.
Article
Full-text available
provide an account of the evolution of human cognitive and behavioral inhibition mechanisms / propose that in hominid phylogeny pressures related to intraspecific cooperation and competition led to enlargement of the neocortex, including connections between the prefrontal lobe and the limbic system / these connections afforded greater voluntary inhibitory control over sexual and aggressive behaviors, which in turn led to enhanced social harmony / as inhibition mechanisms came increasingly under cortical control, neural circuits, initially evolved to inhibit emotional responding, were recruited for purposes of inhibiting other social behaviors and cognitions, contributing to our current cognitive/behavioral system / before presenting evidence for this evolutionary account, [the authors] review the role of inhibition in contemporary human cognition and behavior, its development, and the neural basis for such inhibition (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Contemporary mate preferences can provide important clues to human reproductive history. Little is known about which characteristics people value in potential mates. Five predictions were made about sex differences in human mate preferences based on evolutionary conceptions of parental investment, sexual selection, human reproductive capacity, and sexual asymmetries regarding certainty of paternity versus maternity. The predictions centered on how each sex valued earning capacity, ambition— industriousness, youth, physical attractiveness, and chastity. Predictions were tested in data from 37 samples drawn from 33 countries located on six continents and five islands (total N = 10,047). For 27 countries, demographic data on actual age at marriage provided a validity check on questionnaire data. Females were found to value cues to resource acquisition in potential mates more highly than males. Characteristics signaling reproductive capacity were valued more by males than by females. These sex differences may reflect different evolutionary selection pressures on human males and females; they provide powerful cross-cultural evidence of current sex differences in reproductive strategies. Discussion focuses on proximate mechanisms underlying mate preferences, consequences for human intrasexual competition, and the limitations of this study.
Chapter
Anthropologists have long recognized that cultural evolution critically depends on the transmission and generation of information. However, between the selection pressures of evolution and the actual behaviour of individuals, scientists have suspected that other processes are at work. With the advent of what has come to be known as the cognitive revolution, psychologists are now exploring the evolved problem-solving and information-processing mechanisms that allow humans to absorb and generate culture. The purpose of this book is to introduce the newly crystallizing field of evolutionary psychology, which supplied the necessary connection between the underlying evolutionary biology and the complex and irreducible social phenomena studied by anthropologists, sociologists, economists, and historians.
Article
Why do girls tend to earn better grades in school than boys? Why are men still far more likely than women to earn degrees in the fields of science, technology, engineering, and mathematics? And why are men on average more likely to be injured in accidents and fights than women? These and many other questions are the subject of both informal investigation in the media and formal investigation in academic and scientific circles. In his landmark book "Male, Female: The Evolution of Human Sex Differences", author David Geary provided the first comprehensive evolutionary model to explain human sex differences. Using the principles of sexual selection such as female choice and male-male competition, the author systematically reviewed and discussed the evolution of sex differences and their expression throughout the animal kingdom, as a means of not just describing but explaining the same process in Homo sapiens. Now, over ten years since the first edition, Geary has completed a massive update, expansion and theoretical revision of his classic text. New findings in brain and genetic research inform a wealth of new material, including a new chapter on sex differences in patterns of life history development; expanded coverage of genetic research (e.g. DNA finger printing to determine paternity as related to male-male competition in primates); fatherhood in humans; cross-cultural patterns of sex differences in choosing and competing for mates; and, genetic, hormonal, and socio-cultural influences on the expression of sex differences. Finally, through his motivation to control framework (introduction in the first edition and expanded in "The Origin of Mind", 2005), Geary presents a theoretical bridge linking parenting, mate choices, and competition, with children's development and sex differences in brain and cognition. The result is an even better book than the original - a lively and nuanced application of Darwin's insight to help explain our heritage and our place in the natural world.
Article
Spontaneous expressive control of negative emotion was examined in 2 studies of children aged 3-9 using an experimental "disappointing" situation. In Study 1, facial expressions, verbalizations, and spontaneous references to emotional expression control were examined in terms of the child's age and sex and the experimental manipulation (neutral, positive, and "disappointing" mood segments). Results suggested that children attempted to control the display of negative emotion with positive displays and that females did so more than males. No effect of age on expressive behavior was found. Age differences were found for children's spontaneous reference to expressive control, with such references increasing with age. To further examine the possibility that preschoolers engaged in expressive control, Study 2 examined the expressive behavior of 20 preschool girls in the disappointing situation in 2 conditions, alone or with the examiner present. These data indicated that females, aged 3-4, inhibited negative displays when in the presence of the examiner.
Article
Originally published in Contemporary Psychology: APA Review of Books, 1995, Vol 40(7), 711. Replies to comments made in the S. Mendoza and P. Shaver review (see record 2004-17632-002) of the present author's book, The Evolution of Desire: Strategies of Human Mating (see record 199497188-000). Within the broad context of seeming agreement, Buss also notes disagreement. He argues that there is strong evidence that both men and women have a complex repertoire of mating strategies that includes short-term mating and long-term mating. He also answers Mendoza's analysis of two areas in which initial formulations of hypotheses anchored in sexual strategies theory did not pan out exactly as expected empirically and had to be modified--men's concern about chastity as a potential solution to the problem of paternity uncertainty and women's desire for older men as a solution to the problem of resource acquisition. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)