ArticlePDF Available

Mind Over Matter: Mental Training Increases Physical Strength

Authors:

Abstract

This study tested whether mental training alone can produce a gain in muscular strength. Thirty male university athletes, including football, basketball and rugby players, were randomly assigned to perform mental training of their hip flexor muscles, to use weight machines to physically exercise their hip flexors, or to form a control group which received neither mental nor physical training. The hip strength of each group was measured before and after training. Physical strength was increased by 24% through mental practice (p = .008). Strength was also increased through physical training, by 28%, but did not change significantly in the control condition. The strength gain was greatest among the football players given mental training. Mental and physical training produced similar decreases in heart rate, and both yielded a marginal reduction in systolic blood pressure. The results support the related findings of Ranganathan, Siemionow, Liu, Sahgal, and Yue (2004).
Mind Over Matter: Mental Training Increases
Physical Strength
Erin M. Shackell and Lionel G. Standing
Bishop's University
This study tested whether mental training alone can produce a gain in
muscular strength. Thirty male university athletes, including football,
basketball and rugby players, were randomly assigned to perform mental
training of their hip flexor muscles, to use weight machines to physically
exercise their hip flexors, or to form a control group which received
neither mental nor physical training. The hip strength of
each
group was
measured before and after training. Physical strength was increased by
24%
through mental practice (p = .008). Strength was also increased
through physical training, by
28%,
but did not change significantly in the
control condition. The strength gain was greatest among the football
players given mental training. Mental and physical training produced
similar decreases in heart rate, and both yielded a marginal reduction in
systolic blood pressure. The results support the related findings of
Ranganathan, Siemionow, Liu, Sahgal, and Yue (2004).
The idea of using mental practice to enhance performance has
recently become common. In this procedure, participants are asked to
rehearse a motor or cognitive skill by using mental imagery of
themselves performing it successfully, without any overt behavior or
muscular activity on their part. In the field of
sports
psychology, websites
and books now proliferate which promote mental training to enhance
athletic performance (e.g., Cohn, 2006; Ungerleider, 1996). Professional
teams today often utilize mental training programs for their athletes, and
some have suggested that mental strength training should receive as
much emphasis as physical training.
There are now several hundred papers in the literature which support
the idea that prior mental practice produces measurable gains in skilled
performance, for both cognitive and physical tasks, as summarized in
meta-analyses by Feltz and Landers (1983) and Driskell, Copper, and
Moran (1994). Concurrent mental set or mood may also of course
influence motivation and skilled performance either positively, as shown
by improved scores for speed and accuracy in subjects who adopt a
positive mental attitude (e.g., Ainscoe & Hardy, 1997; Spencer &
Norem, 1996), or negatively, as shown by reduced scores when the test
Author
info:
Correspondence should be sent
to:
Erin
M.
Shackell & Lionel G.
Standing, Department of
Psychology,
Bishop's University, Sherbrooke QC,
Canada JIM 0C8.
North American Journal of Psychology, 2007, Vol. 9, No. 1 189-200
©NAJP
190 NORTH AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY
instructions include anxiety-inducing cues (e.g., Steele & Aronson
1995).
However, the more controversial claim has also been made that prior
mental training alone can also substantially increase performance in a
task which requires simple muscular strength, as opposed to skill or fine
motor control in space and time (Cupal & Brewer, 2001; Reiser, 2005;
Smith, Collins, & Hoknes,
2003;
Smith & Collins, 2004). This claim has
yet to be validated conclusively, partly because methodological issues
abound. The claimed effect is, however, conceptually similar to
experimental demonstrations that hypnosis can produce various
physiological changes, such as wart-removal or breast enlargement
(Barber, 1984; Ewin, 1992; Willard, 1977), and is also parallel to the
many findings in the field of medicine that mental changes can create
measurable physical benefits, such as recovery of ovulation produced by
psychotherapy (Berga, Marcus, Loucks, Hlastala, Ringham, & Krohn,
2003),
or reduction in blood pressure induced by classical music (Chafm,
Roy, Gerin, & Christenfeld, 2004). Mental practice is now recommended
during treatment in some medical cases (e.g., stroke recovery; Bell &
Murray, 2004). The influential bio-informational theory of Lang (1979),
which suggests that in mental training the prepositional content of mental
imagery can directly modify the efferent activity of the brain, provides a
possible conceptual basis for this claim.
Ranganathan, Siemionow, Liu, Sahgal, and Yue (2004) have recently
tested in detail whether strength gains in the little finger abductor and
elbow flexor muscles may be produced by mental training. They also
examined the cortical functions that control contractions of these muscle
groups, as well as EMGs, blood pressure, and heart rate. After mental
training it was found that the finger abductor group muscles had
significantly increased their abduction strength by 35% above baseline,
or 40% when measured four weeks after the training had ended. (The
physical training group increased in finger abduction strength by 53%).
The elbow flexion group increased their strength by
13.5%;
however, this
value was not statistically significant. The authors conclude that mental
training enhances the cortical output signal, causing a higher activation
level and an increase in strength, although the EMG signal is not
measurably affected by training.
The present experiment tests the same general hypothesis by using a
different muscle group, the hip flexors, to determine whether a
measurable gain in strength may be induced through mental training.
Representing a modified version of the study by Ranganathan et al.
(2004),
this experiment attempted to replicate their central finding in a
shorter time fi-ame of two weeks, as opposed to 12 weeks. Hip flexion
uses primarily the iliacus and psoas muscles (Andersson, Nilsson, Maa,
Shackell & Standing STRENGTH VIA IMAGERY 191
6 Thorstensson, 1997), and was selected because these muscles cannot
readily be exercised in other contexts or with free weights.
METHOD
Participants
Thirty male undergraduate students, who were football, basketball
and rugby players at Bishop's University, were used as participants (18,
7 and 5 subjects, respectively). Of these 30 participants, 10 were
randomly assigned to a mental training group which mentally practiced
hip flexions. Ten more participants were assigned to a physical training
group, which exercised using a hip flexor weight machine. A further 10
participants formed a control group, which was given no mental or
physical training. The subjects' ages, heights, and body weights are
summarized in Table 1, while Table 2 indicates that their pre-test scores
fall into the healthy-normal range for blood pressure and pulse.
TABLE
1
Subject Characteristics in the Mental Training, Physical
Training, and Control Groups
Age in years
Height (cm)
Body weight (Ib)
Mental
M SD
19.8
190.0
218.7
n = 10 for each group
1.40
4.22
36.39
Physical
M SD
19.2
185.7
213.2
1.23
7.63
36.07
Control
M
21.3
85.1
216.9
SD
2.45
9.68
51.08
As an incentive to take part in the study, the participants were
rewarded with one Blast protein bar for each 15 minute session.
Materials
Subjects were measured pre-training and post-training in each
condition to determine whether strength gain occurred. A hip flexor
weight machine (Atlanta Precision), measuring pounds of force in
increments of 5 lb from a baseline of 30 lb, was employed to assess
initial and flnal strength, as well as being used in the training process for
the physical training group. An electronic sphygmomanometer (Physio
Logic Automatic Inflation) was utilized to measure systolic and diastolic
blood pressure together with heart rate, and a balance beam scale was
used to weigh each participant.
192 NORTH AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY
Procedure
Each participant was tested for strength at the beginning and the end
of the study, using a hip flexor task. In this task, the subject stands next
to a padded bar and attempts to raise his left leg sideways while the
weight of
the
bar is varied by the experimenter, the fmal weight achieved
being the measure of strength.
TABLE 2 Scores Obtained Pre- and Post- Mental Training, Physical
Training, and Control Treatment, for Weight Lifted in Hip Flexions
(Ib),
Blood Pressure (mmHg), and Heart Rate (beats per min)
Mental Physical Control
M SD M_ SD M_ SD
Weight lifted (pre) 135.0" 25.5 127.5" 30.2 171.5"" 47.3
Weight lifted (post) 167.0^376.0 163.5'' 37.7 177.5 43.7
Blood pressure, systolic (pre) 131.4 13.2 135.9 12.7 128.8 7.0
Blood pressure, systolic (post) 128.1 9.7 133.8 8.9 129.3 8.1
Blood pressure, diastolic (pre) 79.6 9.8 89.7 9.0 85.7 5.3
Blood pressure, diastolic (post) 78.1 6.5 86.0 8.3 84.4 6.0
Heart rate (pre) 59.2° 5.8 61.8^ 6.6 63.1 4.8
Heart rate (post) 58.3"= 5.5 60.0^ 6.6 63.2 5.5
n = 10for each group: ""V < 01;
"'''p
< .05
All testing was performed on an individual basis by a female
experimenter (EMS). Participants were recruited at random in a
university sports center, during personal workouts, or before/after team
practices. After each participant had given informed consent, he was
randomly assigned to the mental training, physical training or control
group. During the first session, the experimenter recorded the
participant's height, body weight, age, blood pressure, heart rate, and
initial weight lifted. Subjects in the mental or physical training condition
were instructed as specified below, and asked to return every working
day for the following two weeks at 10:00 a.m., 2:00 p.m., or 4:00 p.m., to
receive further instruction. Participants in the control condition were
tested for strength initially, as for the other two groups; however, they
were told not to return until the end of
the
two week study period. At the
end of two weeks, all participants were again tested for blood pressure
and heart rate, and then repeated the weight-lifting task. Participants were
thanked and debriefed following the last testing session. There were no
drop-outs.
Shackell & Standing STRENGTH VIA IMAGERY 193
Mental Training
The mental training was carried out for two weeks, with five training
sessions of 15 min each per week. During each training session the
participant was instructed to mentally envision himself using the hip
flexor machine for four sets of eight repetitions, each set being followed
by a 60 second period of rest. During each session, he was instructed to
imagine himself using the hip flexor machine and increasing the lifted
weight by five pounds each day. It was emphasized to the participant that
this mental exercise was not simply a visualization of himself performing
the task, but that he was required mentally to imagine an increase in
weight lifted with each five pounds added. This mental process is
referred to by Ranganathan et al. (2004) as "visualization-guided brain
activation training." Five pound increments were used in order to provide
a change for each session that could be readily imagined. In this process,
the mental training subjects were asked to first visualize themselves
standing on the platform attached to the hip fiexor machine. The subjects
were then requested to imagine themselves extending their left leg
sideways as far as they could within their range of motion.
Physical Training
Physical training was the same as the mental training, except that
each step was physically carded out by the subject instead of being
mentally visualized. An additional weight was added only every three
sessions, as the available weights on the hip flexor machine increased by
15 Ib steps. As in the other two conditions, pre-training and post-training
tests were administered to evaluate muscular strength.
A third group of subjects did not mentally or physically exercise their
hip flexors, but were given the same pre-training and post-training
muscular strength test as the other groups.
Because physiological responses such as heart rate and blood pressure
commonly decrease with training, these variables were also recorded at
the start and the conclusion of
the
study to indicate possible changes.
RESULTS
Effect of Mental and Physical Training on Hip Strength
Descriptive statistics for weight lifted, blood pressure, and heart rate,
before and after training, are given in Table 2. To examine the effect of
training type on strength, a 3 x 2 mixed ANOVA (training type x
practice) was perfonned on the scores for weight lifted, showing that hip
strength overall was higher for the post-test than the pre-test, F(l, 27) =
33.1,
p = .001. Strength overall did not differ significantly between the
three types of training, F(2, 27) = 1.85, p = .\%. h Training Type x
Practice interaction was found, F(2, 27) =
4.81,
;>
= .016. This occurred
194 NORTH AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY
because mental and physical training both produced substantial increases
in strength, of
32
Ib and 36 Ib respectively, whereas the control treatment
yielded a nonsignificant increase of only 6 Ib, t{9) =
1.50,
p
=
.168. These
means are shown in Figure 1.
When tested individually, the increases in strength produced by
mental training and by physical training were both statistically reliable,
t{9) = 3.40,p = .008; t{9) = 4.61,p =
.001.
These two increments did not
differ significantly, /(18) =
.327,
p = .75.
200 n
175-
150
75 ^
50
25-1
0
i Pre-test
d Post-test
Mental Physical
Training TrainingControl
FIGURE
1
Mean weight (Ib) lifted in hip flexion task, before and after
mental training, physical training control, and control treatment. Error
bars represent the standard error of the mean.
It should be noted that due to sampling variation, the control group
was stronger in the pre-test than both the mental training group, r(18) =
2.15,
p = .046, and the physical training group, /(18) = 2.48,^3 = .023. By
Shackell & Standing STRENGTH VIA IMAGERY 195
the end of
training,
however, it was no stronger than these groups, /(18) =
.56,
p = .57 and r(18) =
.77,
p = .45, respectively.
The subjects' pre- and post- training scores in the control group were
highly correlated, r(8) = .964, /? = .001. Within the mental and physical
training groups, this correlation was .60 and .76, p = .066 and .011,
respectively.
Strength Changes in Players of Different Sports
The eighteen subjects who played football were compared against the
others, who played basketball or rugby; these latter were pooled, due to
the small number of subjects in these categories: 7 and 5, respectively. A
3x2x2 mixed ANOVA (training type x practice x sport category)
indicated that there was a significant Practice x Sport interaction, F{\,
24) =
5.23,/J
=
.031.
This interaction represented the larger improvement
that training produced in the footballers compared to the other athletes. In
the mental training group, this gain in strength was 44.17 and of 13.75 Ib,
respectively, whereas in the physical training group, the strength of the
footballers and the other athletes increased by 42.5 and 26.25 Ib,
respectively. At the pre-test before training, the hip strength of the
football players at 146.1 Ib was intermediate between that of the
basketballers and the rugby players (130 and 170 Ib, respectively). Their
body weight at 221.4 Ib was also intermediate among the three groups
(198.4 and 222.8 Ib, respectively). The positive effects of mental training
are not due to the number of football players being greater for this group,
since there were 6 football players in each of the three training groups.
There was no Training type x Practice x Sport interaction, F(4, 21) = .27,
Effect of Mental and Physical Training on Blood Pressure and Heart
Rate
In two 3
X
(2) mixed ANOVAs, no significant changes in systolic or
diastolic blood pressure (mmHg) were observed from the pre-test to the
post-test, F(l, 27) = .86, p = .36, and F(l, 27) = 1.37, p = .25
respectively. However, systolic pressure dropped by 3.3 and 2.1 mmHg
respectively during the mental and physical training conditions, whereas
it increased trivially by 0.5 mmHg for the control condition. This
reduction in systolic pressure, while non-significant for each group alone
(p > .05), is reliable for the two training groups pooled, x^(l) = 5 0, p =
.025.
No reliable trend was found for diastolic changes.
A 3
X
2 mixed ANOVA indicated a significant effect of training in
reducing heart rate, shown as a Heart rate x Training type interaction,
F(2,
27) =
5.15,;?
= .013. This interaction represents a significant pulse
decrease in the mental and physical groups during training, of 0.9 and 1.8
196 NORTH AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY
beats per minute respectively, as compared to an increase in the control
group of
0.1
bpm. The mental and physical training groups did not differ
significantly regarding this decrease in heart rate, according to the
Scheffe test, p = .71. No difference between football players and other
athletes was found in blood pressure or heart rate as a function of
training, all/j > .10. An association was noted between the subjects' pre-
test heart rate and their weight, pooled over all conditions, r(28) = .67, p
=
.001.
However, no significant correlations were found between strength
changes and heart rate, weight, age, or height, allp > .05.
DISCUSSION
The present data indicate that while only a trivial and non-significant
gain in strength occurred for the control subjects, who performed no
physical or mental exercises during the study, both the mental and the
physical training treatments caused a significant increase in the weight
that subjects could lift in hip flexions. This increase in strength was
similar between the two training groups, and was substantial (23.7% and
28.3%,
respectively). It also resembles the magnitude of the effect
reported by Ranganathan et al. (2004), and supports the central
conclusions of that study. Since the present experiment differs from the
study of Ranganathan et al. in employing a novel and larger muscle
group, with short-term isotonic rather than long-term isometric training
and some changes in methodology, these positive results support the
extemal validity of Ranganathan's findings. The results also correspond
to the conclusions of Reiser (2005), although this latter study obtained an
increase in bench press strength of only 5.7% from mental training, and
14%
from physical training.
The data presented by Ranganathan et al. (2004) suggest that the
basis of the mental training effect is central rather than peripheral:
substantial changes in EEG occur in mental training and correlate well
with the observed strength increase. The same conclusion applies to the
recent observation of changes in brain function as a result of mental
practice in a fmger-tapping task (Nyberg, Eriksson, Larsson, &
Marklund, 2006). However, muscular action potentials during imagery
also have often been reported (e.g., Shaw, 1940). Many past studies have
found reductions in blood pressure and heart rate after either physical
exercise or mental relaxation training (e.g., Amigo, Gonzalez, & Herrera,
1997;
Pawlow & Jones, 2002), but the effect of mental exercise on these
measures has not been reported previously. Although the reductions
found here for heart rate and systolic pressure both reached statistical
significance, as they were not large these findings call for replication.
Also,
the measurement of blood pressure may be problematic, as it is
intrinsically labile and varies with many extraneous variables including
Shackell & Standing STRENGTH VIA IMAGERY 197
stress,
nervousness, fatigue, etc. (McAlister & Straus, 2001); therefore it
is necessary to assume that these intruding factors function similarly
across training conditions.
Although the present data clearly support the experimental
hypothesis, assuming that the participants followed the specified
protocol, possible demand characteristics of the task and experimenter
effects should be considered (Ome & Evans, 1965; Rosenthal, 1976).
While a conscious effort was made by the test administrator to keep from
suggesting any expected trends to the participants, the increase in final
strength scores conceivably could be due to the participant's desire to
produce positive results, or from criterion shifts influencing the point at
which he decides that he cannot increase his effort due to discomfort
(Rollman, 1979). Such unwitting experimenter effects and demand
characteristics of the task are emphasized, for example, by the effects of
expectancy upon relaxation-induced blood pressure lowering (Agras,
Home, & Tayor, 1982). However, the persistence ofthe mental training
effect in the study by Ranganathan et al. (2004) several weeks after the
training period had ended, and the strong effects of mental training
observed upon cortical motor potentials, argue against any simple
dismissal of the phenomenon as an artifact, although further analysis of
altemative hypotheses is still called for. There is also the empirical
problem that some studies report positive results for the effects of mental
training on strength (Yue & Cole, 1992; Yue, Wilson, Cole, & Darling,
1996),
while others do not (Herbert, Dean & Gandevia, 1998), a
discrepancy which possibly may be attributed to uncontrolled incidental
factors such as the superiority of intemal over extemal imagery
(Ranganathan et al, p. 954; Hinshaw, 1991/1992; Wang & Morgan,
1992).
While not part of the experimental hypothesis, the strength gain of
each subject in relation to his chosen sport was also examined. Greater
benefits were found among the football players than the other athletes,
for both physical and mental training. This effect is of some interest, as
the football players were no stronger at the pre-test than the other
subjects, nor heavier. Therefore the greater absolute increase in strength
observed for football players also represents a greater percentage
increase.
This study used only male participants, to control subject gender as a
variable which might interact with the gender of the experimenter
(always female in this case), but presumably to the same extent in each
training condition. Future research in this area ideally would use both
male and female participants, as well as male and female experimenters.
The hip fiexor exercise was chosen as a means of measuring strength
gain based on the fact that this muscle group is one that none of the
198 NORTH AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY
athletes had previously and consciously made any effort to exercise. It
seemed logical to predict that greater strength gain could be
demonstrated in a muscle group that rarely is used rather than one that is
already well exercised before the study. The high correlation between the
hip flexion scores obtained before and after the control treatment
indicates that this measure shows statistical reliability here, although this
is not always the case (Emery, Maitland, & Meeuwisse, 1999). The much
lower correlations found after training may be attributed to the marked
variations that occur between different individuals in response to the
same training program (Bouchard, An, Rice, Skinner, Wilmore, Gagnon,
Pemsse, Leon, & Rao, 1999).
It would be valuable to conduct simultaneous mental and physical
practice, to examine their combined effect on strength gain, as is
sometimes recommended in the literature on mental practice (Jackson,
Doyon, Richards, & Malouin, 2004). It is hoped that subsequent studies
with prolonged mental training will explore the limits of the effect, and
provide more evidence as to the ultimate powers of mental imagery.
However, stating the issue as one of mind over matter sets up an
unnecessary dichotomy since all mental states are intrinsically embodied
as physical events in the nervous system, if we reject the existence of
disembodied mind and accept central state theory (Armstrong, 1968).
Therefore, rather than referring to the conventional 'mind over matter',
we may summarize the present results as showing the power of CNS
activity over long-term muscular strength.
REFERENCES
Agras, W. S., Home, M., & Taylor, C. B. (1982). Expectation and the blood-
pressure-lowering effects of relaxation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 44, 389-
395.
Ainscoe, M., & Hardy, L. (1997). Cognitive warm-up in a cyclical gymnastics
skill. International Journal of Sports Psychology, 18, 269-275.
Amigo, I., Gonzalez, A, & Herrera, J. (1997). Comparison of physical exercise
and muscle relaxation training in the treatment of mild essential hypertension.
Stress Medicine, 13, 59-65.
Andersson, E. A., Nilsson, J., Maa, Z., & Thorstensson, A. (1997). Abdominal
and hip flexor muscle activation during various training exercises. European
Journal of Applied
Physiology,
75, 115-123.
Armstrong, D. M. (1968). A materialist theory of the
mind.
New York:
Humanities Press.
Barber, T. X. (1984). Changing "unchangeable" bodily processes by (hypnotic)
suggestions: A new look at hypnosis, cognitions, imaging, and the mind-body
problem. In A. A. Sheikh
{Ei.),
Imagery and healing
{'p'p.l-AQ).
Farmingdale,
New York: Baywood.
Shackell & Standing STRENGTH VIA IMAGERY 199
Bell, A. R., & Murray, B. J. (2004). Improvement in upper limb motor
performance following stroke: The use of mental practice. British Journal of
Occupational
Therapy,
67, 501-507.
Berga, S. L., Marcus, M. D., Loucks, T. L., Hlastala, S., Ringham, R., & Krohn,
M. A. (2003). Recovery of ovarian activity in women with functional
hypothalamic amenorrhea who were treated with cognitive behavior therapy.
Fertility and
Sterility,
80, 976-81.
Bouchard, C, An, P., Rice, T., Skinner, J. S., Wilmore, J. H., Gagnon, J.,
Perusse, L, Leon, A. S., & Rao, D. C. (1999), Familial aggregation of VO2
ma, response to exercise training: results from the HERITAGE Family Study.
Journal of Applied
Physiology,
87, 1003-1008.
Chafm, S., Roy, M., Gerin, W. & Christenfeld, N, (2004). Music can facilitate
blood pressure recovery from stress.
British
Journal of Health Psychology, 9,
393-403.
Cohn, P. J. (2006). Peak performance sports: Instilling confidence for a
competitive edge. Retrieved December 14, 2006, from
http://www.peaksports.com/
Cupal, D, D., & Brewer, B. W. (2001). Effects of relaxation and guided imagery
on knee strength, reinjury anxiety, and pain following anterior cruciate
ligament reconstruction. Rehabilitation Psychology, 46,
28-43.
Driskell, J. E., Copper, C, & Moran, A. (1994). Does mental practice enhance
performanceT
Journal of Applied
Psychology,
79,
481-492.
Emery, C. A., Maitland, M. E., & Meeuwisse, W. H. (1999). Test-retest
reliability of isokinetic hip adductor and flexor muscle strength. Clinical
Journal of Sports
Medicine,
9, 79-85.
Ewin, D. M, (1992), Hypnotherapy for warts (Verruca Vulgaris): 41 consecutive
cases with 33 cures,
American
Journal of Clinical
Hypnosis,
35, 1-10,
Feltz, D, L., & Landers, D. M. (1983). The effects of mental practice on motor
skill leaming and performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sports
Psychology, 5, 25-57.
Herbert, R. D., Dean, C, & Gandevia, S. C. (1998). Effects of real and imagined
training on voluntary muscle activation during maximal isometrics
contractions, Acta Physiologica
Scandinavica,
163, 361-368.
Hinshaw, K. E. (1991/1992), The effects of mental practice on motor skill
performance: Critical evaluation and meta-analysis. Imagination, Cognition
and
Personality,
11, 3-35.
Jackson, P. L., Doyon, J., Richards, C, & Malouin, F, (2004), The efficacy of
combined physical and mental practice in the leaming of
a
foot-sequence task
after stroke: A case report, Neurorehabilitation cS Neural Repair 18 106-
111,
' '
Lang, P. J. (1979). A bio-informational theory of emotional imagery. Psycho-
physiology,
16,
495-512.
McAIister, F. A., & Straus, S. E, (2001). Measurement of blood pressure: An
evidence based review, British Medical Journal, 322,
908-911,
Nyberg, L,, Eriksson, J,, Larsson, A,, & Marklund, P, (2006). Leaming by doing
versus leaming by thinking: An fMRI study of motor and mental training.
Neuropsychologia, 44, 711-717.
200 NORTH AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY
Ome, M. T., & Evans, F. J. (1965). Social control in the psychological
experiment: Antisocial behavior and hypnosis. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, /, 189-200.
Pawlow, L. A., & Jones, G. E. (2002). The impact of abbreviated progressive
muscle relaxation on salivary cortisol. Biological Psychology, 60, 1-16.
Ranganathan, V. K., Siemionow, V., Liu, J. Z., Sahgal, V., & Yue, G. (2004).
From mental power to muscular strength - gaining strength by using the
mind. Neuropsychologia, 42, 944-956.
Reiser, M. (2005). Strength gains by motor imagery of maximal muscle
contractions / Kraftgewinne durch Vorstellung maximaler
Muskelkontraktionen. Zeitschrift
JUr
Sportpsychologie, 12,11-2.
Rollman, G. B. (1979). Signal detection theory pain measures: Empirical
validation studies and adaptation-level effects. Pain, 6. 9-21
Rosenthal, R. (1976). Experimenter effects in behavioral research. New York:
Irvington.
Shaw, W. A. (1940). The relation of muscular action potentials to imaginal
weight lifting. Archives of Psychology, No.247,1-50.
Smith, D., Collins, D., & Holmes, P. (2003). Impact and mechanism of mental
practice effects on strength. International Journal of Sport and Exercise
Psychology, I, 293-306.
Smith, D., & Collins, D. (2004). Mental practice, motor performance, and the late
CNV. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 26, 412-426.
Spencer, S. M., & Norem, J. K. (1996). Reflection and distraction, defensive
pessimism, strategic optimism, and performance. Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin,
22, 354-365.
Steele, C, & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test
performance of African-Americans. Journal of Personality & Social
Psychology, 69,191-8\\.
Ungerleider, S. (1996). Mental training for peak performance: Top athletes
reveal the mind exercises they use to excel. Emmaus, PA: Rodale.
Wang, Y., & Morgan, W. P. (1992). The effect of imagery perspectives on the
psychophysiological responses to imagined exercise. Beliavioral Brain
Research, 52, 167-174.
Willard, R. D. (1977). Breast enlargement through visual imagery and hypnosis.
American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 19, 195-200.
Yue,
G., & Cole, K. J. (1992). Strength increases from the motor program:
Comparison of training with maximal voluntary and imagined muscle
contractions. Journal ofNeurophysiology, 67, 1114-1123.
Yue,
G. H., Wilson, S. L., Cole, K. J., & Darling, W. G. (1996). Imagined muscle
contraction training increases voluntary neural drive to muscle. Journal of
Psychophysiology, 10, 198-208.
Author Note. The authors are grateful to the participants for conscientious
participation, and to the reviewers for their helpful suggestions. Communications
may be sent to eshackell03@ubishops.ca or lstandin@ubishops.ca.
... The underlying mechanism of MI training on muscle strength is believed to be based on neural adaptation. Specifically, it is shown that MI leads to elevation of Movement-Related Cortical Potential (MRCP) over both the primary motor and supplementary motor cortices (Herbert et al., 1998;Shackell and Standing, 2007). Here, it is suggested that MI strengthens the brain-to-muscle command processes and improves both motor unit recruitment and activation, leading to greater muscle forces (Shackell and Standing, 2007). ...
... Specifically, it is shown that MI leads to elevation of Movement-Related Cortical Potential (MRCP) over both the primary motor and supplementary motor cortices (Herbert et al., 1998;Shackell and Standing, 2007). Here, it is suggested that MI strengthens the brain-to-muscle command processes and improves both motor unit recruitment and activation, leading to greater muscle forces (Shackell and Standing, 2007). While most of the literature reports an increase of brain activations (measured via evoked potentials and changes in blood flow (fMRI)) when comparing MI with rest (i.e., an online effect), Grosprêtre et al. (2018) reported that MI practice can influence cortical descending neural drive (Grosprêtre et al., 2018). ...
... Subsequent studies have indicated that imagery training (i.e., with different intervention periods ranging from 4 to 7 weeks) can improve muscle strength in various muscle groups (Paravlic et al., 2018;Liu et al., 2023). Specifically, upper limb muscle groups, comprising the elbow flexor (Herbert et al., 1998;Smith et al., 2003;Shackell and Standing, 2007), dorsal extension and ulnar abduction and fifth finger abductor digiti minimi muscle (Herbert et al., 1998;Smith et al., 2003;Smith and Collins, 2004), and lower limb muscle groups, namely, the plantar flexor (Zijdewind et al., 2003;Bouguetoch et al., 2021), ankle dorsiflexor (Sidaway et al., 2005), and hip flexor (Yao et al., 2013). The previous studies showing improvements in strength/ performance outcomes, mostly adopted a cognitive imagery type of intervention. ...
Article
Full-text available
Motor imagery training could be an important treatment of reduced muscle function in patients and injured athletes. In this study, we investigated the efficacy of imagery training on maximal force production in a larger muscle group (hip abductors) and potential bilateral transfer effects. Healthy participants (n = 77) took part in two experimental studies using two imagery protocols (∼30 min/day, 5 days/week for 2 weeks) compared either with no practice (study 1), or with isometric exercise training (study 2). Maximal hip abduction isometric torque, electromyography amplitudes (trained and untrained limbs), handgrip strength, right shoulder abduction (strength and electromyography), and imagery capability were measured before and after the intervention. Post intervention, motor imagery groups of both studies exhibited significant increase in hip abductors strength (∼8%, trained side) and improved imagery capability. Further results showed that imagery training induced bilateral transfer effects on muscle strength and electromyography amplitude of hip abductors. Motor imagery training was effective in creating functional improvements in limb muscles of trained and untrained sides.
... Table 1 presents details of each included article regarding sample, measures, and key results. The sample size of the MIT groups in the included studies ranged from 6 to 18, three studies with a sample size of 6 ( Leung et al., 2013;Yao et al., 2013;Jiang et al., 2017), two studies with a sample size of 8 (Ranganathan et al., 2004;Bahari et al., 2011), two studies with a sample size of 9 (Lebon et al., 2010;Grôspretre et al., 2017), nine studies with a sample size of 10 (Yue and Cole, 1992;Sidaway and Trzaska, 2005;Fontani et al., 2007;Shackel and Standing, 2007;Wright and Smith, 2009;de Ruiter et al., 2012;Darvishi et al., 2013;Jiang et al., 2016;Bouguetoch et al., 2021), three studies with a sample size of 12 (Cornwall et al., 1991;Goudarzian et al., 2017), one study with a sample size of 15 (Mamone, 2013), three studies with a sample size of 16 (Smith et al., 2003;Niazi et al., 2014;Alenezi, 2018), and one study with a sample size of 18 (Herbert et al., 1998). All the selected studies included a non-exercise and/or a non-imagery control group. ...
... Fourteen selected studies included an additional PT group (Yue and Cole, 1992;Herbert et al., 1998;Smith et al., 2003;Ranganathan et al., 2004;Sidaway and Trzaska, 2005;Fontani et al., 2007;Shackel and Standing, 2007;Wright and Smith, 2009;de Ruiter et al., 2012;Darvishi et al., 2013;Leung et al., 2013;Mamone, 2013;Niazi et al., 2014;Jiang et al., 2016;Goudarzian et al., 2017). Four studies examined the effect of MIT on older subjects (Darvishi et al., 2013;Mamone, 2013;Jiang et al., 2016;Goudarzian et al., 2017). ...
... The selected studies varied in training volume, ranging from 1 to 12 weeks, 3 to 7 sessions per week, and 6 to 120 trials per session ( Table 1). In addition, the selected studies varied also in muscles examined, nine studies trained lower extremity (LE) muscles (Cornwall et al., 1991;Sidaway and Trzaska, 2005;Shackel and Standing, 2007;de Ruiter et al., 2012;Niazi et al., 2014;Goudarzian et al., 2017;Grôspretre et al., 2017;Alenezi, 2018;Bouguetoch et al., 2021), ten studies trained upper extremity (UE) muscles (Herbert et al., 1998;Ranganathan et al., 2004;Fontani et al., 2007;Wright and Smith, 2009;Bahari et al., 2011;Leung et al., 2013;Mamone, 2013;Yao et al., 2013; Jiang et al., 2016Jiang et al., , 2017, and four studies trained finger muscles (Yue and Cole, 1992;Smith et al., 2003;Ranganathan et al., 2004;Darvishi et al., 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective The current review was aimed to determine the effectiveness of mental imagery training (MIT) on the enhancement of maximum voluntary muscle contraction (MVC) force for healthy young and old adults. Data sources Six electronic databases were searched from July 2021 to March 2022. Search terms included: “motor imagery training,” “motor imagery practice,” “mental practice,” “mental training,” “movement imagery,” “cognitive training,” “strength,” “force,” “muscle strength,” “performance,” “enhancement,” “improvement,” “development,” and “healthy adults.” Study selection and data extraction Randomized controlled trials of MIT in enhancing muscle strength with healthy adults were selected. The decision on whether a study met the inclusion criteria of the review was made by two reviewers independently. Any disagreements between the two reviewers were first resolved by discussion between the two reviewers. If consensus could not be reached, then it would be arbitrated by a third reviewer. Data synthesis Twenty-five studies including both internal MIT and external MIT were included in meta-analysis for determining the efficacy of MIT on enhancing muscle strength and 22 internal MIT were used for subgroup analysis for examining dose-response relationship of MIT on MVC. Results MIT demonstrated significant benefit on enhancing muscle strength when compared with no exercise, Effect Size (ES), 1.10, 95% confidence interval (CI), 0.89–1.30, favoring MIT, but was inferior to physical training (PT), ES, 0.38, 95% CI, 0.15–0.62, favoring PT. Subgroup analysis demonstrated that MIT was more effective for older adults (ES, 2.17, 95% CI, 1.57–2.76) than young adults (ES, 0.95, 95% CI, 0.74–1.17), p = 0.0002, and for small finger muscles (ES, 1.64, 95% CI, 1.06–2.22) than large upper extremity muscles (ES, 0.86, 95% CI, 0.56–1.16), p = 0.02. No significant difference was found in the comparison of small finger muscles and large lower extremity muscles, p = 0.19 although the ES of the former (ES, 1.64, 95% CI, 1.06–2.22) was greater than that of the later (ES, 1.20, 95%, 0.88–1.52). Conclusion This review demonstrates that MIT has better estimated effects on enhancing MVC force compared to no exercise, but is inferior to PT. The combination of MIT and PT is equivalent to PT alone in enhancing muscle strength. The subgroup group analysis further suggests that older adults and small finger muscles may benefit more from MIT than young adults and larger muscles.
... So, the rescue time measured in the test is less than the actual rescue in Tables 2-5. According to Shackell et al.'s research [46], it was tested whether mental training alone can produce a gain in muscular strength. Te results showed that physical strength was increased by 24% through mental practice. ...
Article
Full-text available
It is a very important task to construct a life-saving passage in building ruins rapidly and scientifically in the process of earthquake rescue. Currently, the virtual scene is built to train rescuers to construct the life-saving passage quickly and scientifically. However, there are problems such as high cost, small quantity, and single form. A new method of constructing building ruins and life-saving passage was proposed based on the combined finite element (FE) and finite-discrete element (F-DE) method and restarted function of LS-DYNA program. First, taking the RC frame structure ruins as research objection, the different types of life-saving passages were constructed. What’s more, a simple and reasonable optimization method of life-saving passage is proposed based on the rescue technologies with the shortest time. Meanwhile, the timing test of four typical rescue technologies was performed considering the influence factors of various rescue situations. Finally, the practicability and validation of the optimization method was verified through comparing with actual earthquake rescue case. The results show that the restart function of ANSYS/LS-DYNA program can construct the life-saving passage rapidly and reasonably, and simultaneously, the optimal method of life-saving passage can give the optimal rescue route intuitively and accurately. The numerical simulation method of construction and optimization of life-saving passages is expected to provide theoretical guidance for rescue drills for on-site earthquake rescue.
... Underlying alterations in muscle activity are explained through changes in motor unit firing patterns and also motor unit synchronization, measured by surface electromyography (Gabriel, Kamen & Frost, 2006). This concept of the important role of neural adaptations following practice is supported by other MI studies (Ranganathan et al., 2004;Shackell & Standing, 2007;Yue & Cole, 1992) and also of those one that investigating early stages of neuromuscular adaptation on resistance training (Moritani & Devries, 1980;Häkkinen & Häkkinen, 1994;Häkkinen, Kallinen, Linnamo, Pastinen, Newton, et al. 1996;Häkkinen, Kraemer, Newton, & Alen, 2001). Therefore, it seems that same patterns are present during MI and physical practice (Di Rienzo, Blache, Kanthack, Monteil, Collet, et al. 2015;Yao, Ranganathan, Allexandre, Siemionow & Yue, 2013) where greater motor unit recruitment and activation leads to greater muscle output. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
The objective of this study is to determine the incidence of lordosis in children aged 15 years and under in the Republic of Serbia. The following electronic databases were searched in order to collect the relevant studies conducted to date: PubMed, SCIndeks, KOPSON, and Google Scholar. Only studies conducted between 2005 and 2015 were included in the review. In order for a study to be included for analysis, the following criteria had to be satisfied: the study had to include participants assessed for lordosis, and the participants in the study had to be of pre‐school and elementary school age in Serbia. A significant number of studies (195) were excluded based on the pre‐set criteria, and the remaining 13 studies met all the criteria agreed upon. The studies reviewed in the present paper assessed a total of 8,528 children. Lordosis as a deformity was diagnosed in 1,673 children, yielding an incidence of 19.617%. Recommendations, based on the results obtained, include a reduced Napoleon Volanski method, insprection, somatometry, and somatoscopy, as well as the “Spinal mouse” instrument for lordosis assessment. Studies have found a very high incidence of lordotic deformity in children aged under 15 years.
... According to the studies in the literature, it was revealed that mental training is a psychological skill that should be added into the training programs, which include technique and tactics, so as to enhance the performance of the athlete (Özdal, Akcan, Abakay & Dağlıoğlu, 2013;Altıntaş & Akalan, 2008;Peluso, Ross, Gfeller & LaVoie, 2005;Weinberg & Williams, 2006). Undoubtedly, there are many studies which explain that mental skills are a decisive element of sportive success and many successful athletes add mental skills development activities in their training programs in addition to physical training activities (Cumming & Williams, 2012;Levy et al., 2015;Slimani & Cheour, 2016;Shackell & Standing, 2007;Cankurtaran, 2020;Güler & Erhan, 2017;Slimani, Tod, Chaabene, Miarka & Chamari, 2016;Amasiatu, 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study was carried out to examine the effect of mental training skills of athlete students, who are studying at the Sport Sciences Faculty, in predicting the level of commitment to sports. In the study, sub-dimensions of the mental training scale in sports were considered as predictive variables and it was purposed to observe the impact of these variables on the level of commitment to sports. The study group was comprised of 240 athlete students, who are interested in different sport branches and pursuing their education in the Sport Sciences Faculty in a State University in Türkiye. In the data collection of the study, the Mental Training Inventory in Sport (MTIS) and Scale of Engagement to Sports (SES) were utilized. In addition, the frequency analysis was used to analyze the dispersion of participants’ demographic data. After the scale scores were obtained, their obedience to the normal dispersion was examined with Kolmogorov Smirnov and Shapiro Wilks test and it was found that the scale scores were appropriate for the normal dispersion. In addition, the regression model was utilized to examine the impact of MTIS on commitment to sports. Moreover, the t-test and one-way analysis of variance in independent groups were utilized to analyze whether the scales differed significantly or not, according to demographic information. The analyses were performed with utilizing the SPSS 20.0 software with the confidence level of 95%. By virtue of this study, it was revealed that the level of commitment to sports of national athletes is high, the level of commitment to sports increases with the increase in the age, and the level of commitment to sports is partially high for those who are interested in team sports. In addition, the multivariate regression results showed that sports commitment did not predict the mental training skills in sports. According to the findings of the study, it can be stated that mental training skills in sports do not have an effect on the sports commitment of the participants.
... Gaggioli et al. (2013) revealed that the combined mental and physical training performed in order to improve the motor performance in basketball increased performance. Shackell and Standing (2007) determined that the increase in strength between the mental and physical trainingsof the groupswas similar in football, basketball and rugby players, which they divided into 3 groups as the mental and physical training group and control group. ...
Article
Full-text available
The study aimed to examine the effect of service trainings performed with eyes closed on the target service success percentage in volleyball players. The study was carried out with 22 volunteer male volleyball players. The volleyball half court was divided into six zones, each zone with a distance of 3 meters. In the trainings, the athletes were asked to serve 10 to each predetermined target zone with their eyes tied with a black eye patch. In the pre-test and post-test application, following the appropriate warm-up process, the athletes were asked to serve 20 to each zone with their eyes open. For the statistical analysis of the data obtained, SPSS 21.0 package program was used. In the evaluation of the data; descriptive statistics, non-parametric Wilcoxon signed-rank test and Friedman test were used. P<0.05 was accepted for statistical significance. As a result of the applications performed, it was found that the difference between the pre-test and post-test service success percentages in the zones 1, 2, 3, 4 and 6 was significant. In the post-test values, the percentages of the total serve into the zones were higher than the pre-test serve percentages. In the applications, the percentage distribution of the serve with eyes closed was found to be higher according to the days. The service percentage values in the first and last training sessions increased significantly. As a result, it was determined that the service trainings performed with eyes closed affected the success percentage in the service.
... Günümüzde artık sporcular, fiziksel antrenmanların yanında zihinsel antrenmanlardan da yararlanmaktadırlar (Altıntaş ve Akalın, 2008). Profesyonel takımlar, sporcuları için sıklıkla zihinsel antrenman programları kullanmakta ve zihinsel antrenmanların fiziksel antrenman kadar işlenmesi gerektiğini vurgulamaktadır (Shackell and Standing, 2007). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Günümüzde artık sporcular, fiziksel antrenmanların yanında zihinsel antrenmanlardan da yararlanmaktadırlar (Altıntaş ve Akalın, 2008). Profesyonel takımlar, sporcuları için sıklıkla zihinsel antrenman programları kullanmakta ve zihinsel antrenmanların fiziksel antrenman kadar işlenmesi gerektiğini vurgulamaktadır (Shackell and Standing, 2007). Spor bilimlerinde neredeyse her alanda öne çıkan rolünden dolayı yaratıcılık, takım sporlarında da akademik merakı uyandırmıştır. Buna bağlı olarak sportif anlamda yaratıcılıkla ilgili farklı yaklaşımlar ortaya çıkmıştır. Bu yaklaşımlar yaratıcılık kavramını oyuncuların karar verme yeteneği, özgünlüğü ve olağandışı becerilerin ortaya konması, sporcunun özgün, estetik ve dahi olması, farklı durumlarda sporcu tarafından başarılı bedensel doğaçlamaların yapılması ve olağandışı eylemlerin ortaya konulması olarak ifade edilmiştir (Hendry, Williams and Hodges, 2018; Lacerda and Mumford, 2010; Tanggaard, Laursen and Szulevicz, 2016). Bu derlemede futbolcuların daha iyi bir performans sergileyebilmeleri için tercih edilen zihinsel antrenmanlar ve alışılmışın dışında bir farklılık oluşturan yaratıcılık hakkında bilgi vermek, alan yazında yer alan çalışmaların özetini sunmak ve futbolda zihinsel antrenman ile yaratıcılık hakkında bazı önerilerde bulunmak amaçlanmıştır.
... Günümüzde artık sporcular, fiziksel antrenmanların yanında zihinsel antrenmanlardan da yararlanmaktadırlar (Altıntaş ve Akalın, 2008). Profesyonel takımlar, sporcuları için sıklıkla zihinsel antrenman programları kullanmakta ve zihinsel antrenmanların fiziksel antrenman kadar işlenmesi gerektiğini vurgulamaktadır (Shackell and Standing, 2007). ...
Book
Full-text available
Futbolda, "stresi yönetemiyoruz", "kendimizi maça veremedik", zihinsel olarak hazır değildik" gibi sözleri mağlubiyetlerden sonra sıkça duyarız. Doktora tezimden üretmiş olduğum bu kitap tam olarak bu gibi söylentileri ortadan kaldırmayı amaçlamaktadır. Giyilebilir teknolojinin kullanıldığı bu çalışma futbol antrenörlerini zihinsel olarak daha iyi hazırlamayı hedeflemektedir. Kitap içinde teorik ve uygulamalı bilgileri sonuçları ve etkileri ile bulabilirsiniz.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to examine whether acute relaxation training, conducted on two separate occasions, would be associated with reliable reduction in subjective and physiological indices of stress. Forty-six experimental subjects were led through Abbreviated Progressive Relaxation Training (APRT) exercises during two laboratory sessions spaced exactly 1 week apart. Fifteen control subjects experienced two laboratory sessions where they sat quietly for an equal amount of time. Results indicated that a brief relaxation exercise led to experimental subjects having significantly lower levels of post-intervention heart rate, state anxiety, perceived stress, and salivary cortisol than control subjects, as well as increased levels of self-report levels of relaxation. The results of this study may have implications for the use of relaxation training in enhancing immune function.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to examine changes in maximal voluntary abduction Force of the human fifth finger following five weeks of training with imagined, maximal contractions of the abductor digiti minimi (ADM) muscle. The metacarpophalangeal joints of the Fifth and fourth fingers of the nondominant hand of 10 young subjects were immobilized in a plaster cast for five weeks. Five of the 10 subjects imagined producing 15 maximal abduction contractions with the fifth finger of the casted hand each day during the immobilization period. The other five subjects (control group) wore the cast and did not participate in any training. The abduction force exerted during maximal voluntary contractions (MVCs) of the fifth finger and the associated integrated electromyograms (EMG) of the ADM muscle were measured before and after immobilization. Cross-sectional area of the casted ADM muscle of three subjects was assessed with magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and muscle atrophy occurred in all three subjects (7, 13, and 22%) after immobilization indicating that the muscle activity was reduced during the immobilization period. The maximal abduction force of the casted fifth finger increased 9.28% on average For the imaginary-trained subjects, despite muscle atrophy, and that for the control subjects decreased by 1.54%. The integrated EMG of the casted ADM muscle during MVCs increased 48.56% (P < 0.05) for the imaginary-trained group and that for the control group decreased by 12.07%. The maximal abduction Force and EMG of the contralateral uncasted hand of the imaginary-trained group increased 12.45 and 58.23%, respectively (P < 0.05), after the imagined-contraction training of the ipsilateral hand. These results support previous findings that voluntary strength increases can be achieved without repeated muscle activation, and that these muscle force gains appear to result From practice effects on central motor commands.
Article
A theory of emotional imagery is described which conceives the image in the brain to be a conceptual network, controlling specific somatovisceral patterns, and constituting a prototype for overt behavioral expression. Evidence for the hypothesis that differentiated efferent activity is associated with type and content of imaginal activity is considered. Recent work in cognitive psychology is described, which treats both the generation of sensory imagery and text comprehension and storage as examples of the processing of propositional information. A similar propositional analysis is applied to emotional imagery as it is employed in the therapeutic context. Experiments prompted by this view show that the conceptual structure of the image and its associated efferent outflow can be modified directly through instructions and through shaping of reports of image experience. The implications of the theory for psychopathology are considered, as well as its relevance to therapeutic behavior change.
Article
This research concerns the interaction between performance strategies and imagery conditions. Defensive pessimists (DPs, who set low expectations, feel anxious, and rehearse possible outcomes) and strategic optimists (OPTs, who set high expectations, feel calm, and avoid reflecting) were randomly assigned to three conditions: a coping imagery condition (imagining correcting mistakes), a mastery imagery condition (imagining a flawless performance), or a relaxation condition (relaxation imagery). DPs performed better in the coping imagery condition, which was similar to their typical strategy, than in the relaxation condition, which was hypothesized to interfere with their performance. The opposite was true for OPTs. Both groups performed worst in the mastery imagery condition. Discussion highlights the importance of both reflective process and thought content and the costs and benefits of each strategy.
Article
Zusammenfassung. In der vorliegenden Trainingsstudie wurde der Effekt imaginierter Muskelkontraktionen (IMC-Training) auf die isometrische Maximalkraft (MVC) untersucht. In der Literatur finden sich hierzu teils widerspruchliche Befunde (Herbert, Dean & Gandevia, 1998; Yue & Cole, 1992. Im Rahmen eines vierwochigen kontrollierten Trainingsprogramms trainierten Versuchspersonen (N = 34) die Kraftubung Bankdrucken entweder physisch (Gruppe “MaxKraft“, n = 12), d. h. mit maximalen isometrischen Kontraktionen oder indem sie die entsprechenden Kontraktionen so lebhaft als moglich imaginierten (Gruppe “Mental“, n = 11). Die Kontrollgruppe (n = 11) hatte kein Training. Vor, wahrend (nach 7 bzw. 14 Tagen) und am Ende der Trainingsphase wurde die Relativkraft (MVC relativiert am Korpergewicht) erfasst. Im Gegensatz zur Kontrollgruppe verzeichnet die mental ubende Gruppe einen signifikanten Kraftgewinn (5.7 %; p < .001). Der starkste Vorstellungseffekt findet sich dabei zu Beginn der Trainingsphase (η2 = .58). Der ...
Article
Mental practice is a technique that involves imagery and rehearsal of movement without movement actually occurring. This study considers the evidence that indicates whether mental practice is successful in improving upper limb motor performance after a stroke. The use of mental practice in the traditional fields of sport, music and dance is identified and a theoretical premise for its application in stroke rehabilitation is presented. Eight studies on the use of mental practice in the rehabilitation of motor performance following a stroke are critiqued. These studies suggest that mental practice improves upper limb motor ability and appears to be applicable to a range of participants, especially those with moderate impairment, although good cognitive and communication skills are required. The article suggests reasons that this relatively new approach should be considered by occupational therapists involved in stroke rehabilitation. The limited number of studies and small sample sizes are highlighted. Further research is recommended in order to identify people who will benefit from mental practice, to investigate the generalisation of results and to establish guidelines for the effective provision of mental practice in terms of length, format and content in stroke rehabilitation.