ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Previous studies have explained children’s experience of place and their special preferences for the unbuilt and unstructured environment. However, the impact of a natural environment on children’s learning and development has been a topic of low priority within child research and the importance of natural playscapes for children has also been neglected in physical planning. The present study focuses on a natural environment, a small forest, as a playscape for children. The forest was used by a kindergarten as a supplement to their traditional outdoor playground and the impact such a landscape might have on children’s motor development was investigated through an experimental study on kindergarten children aged 5, 6 and 7 years of age. The landscape was described and analyzed by methods of landscape ecology and geomorphology implemented in a geographical information system (GIS). We found that the natural landscape had qualities to meet the children’s needs for a stimulating and varied play environment. Landscape ecology metrics showed high values for diversity, evenness and heterogeneity for the study area. The diversity of vegetation and topography corresponded to function-related structures that afforded versatile play. The study indicated a strong relation between landscape structure and play functions. The diversity of the vegetation was related to phytosociology and physiognomy, while the diversity of topography was related to slope and roughness. Diversity in landscape elements such as vegetation and topography might be considered a dimension of quality for a natural playscape. This playscape comprised the ground for training of motor fitness in children. Through all-round playing and exploring the natural playscape, the children’s motor fitness was improved. This proved the learning effects from a natural playscape on children’s motor abilities. This paper will focus mainly on the landscape descriptions and the affordance of versatile play.
Content may be subject to copyright.
The natural environment as a playground for children
Landscape description and analyses of a
natural playscape
Ingunn Fjùrtoft
*
, Jostein Sageie
Department of Teacher Education/Department of Computer Science and Mathematics,
Telemark College, 3679 Notodden, Norway
Received 31 May 1999; received in revised form 21 October 1999; accepted 14 January 2000
Abstract
Previous studies have explained children's experience of place and their special preferences for the unbuilt and unstructured
environment. However, the impact of a natural environment on children's learning and development has been a topic of low
priority within child research and the importance of natural playscapes for children has also been neglected in physical
planning. The present study focuses on a natural environment, a small forest, as a playscape for children. The forest was used
by a kindergarten as a supplement to their traditional outdoor playground and the impact such a landscape might have on
children's motor development was investigated through an experimental study on kindergarten children aged 5, 6 and 7 years
of age. The landscape was described and analyzed by methods of landscape ecology and geomorphology implemented in a
geographical information system (GIS). We found that the natural landscape had qualities to meet the children's needs for a
stimulating and varied play environment. Landscape ecology metrics showed high values for diversity, evenness and
heterogeneity for the study area. The diversity of vegetation and topography corresponded to function-related structures that
afforded versatile play. The study indicated a strong relation between landscape structure and play functions. The diversity of
the vegetation was related to phytosociology and physiognomy, while the diversity of topography was related to slope and
roughness. Diversity in landscape elements such as vegetation and topography might be considered a dimension of quality for
a natural playscape. This playscape comprised the ground for training of motor ®tness in children. Through all-round playing
and exploring the natural playscape, the children's motor ®tness was improved. This proved the learning effects from a natural
playscape on children's motor abilities. This paper will focus mainly on the landscape descriptions and the affordance of
versatile play. #2000 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Environments for play and development; Natural playscapes; Landscape ecology
1. Introduction
``Climbing rocks is more fun than climbing trees Ð
but climbing trees is more fun than the boring play-
ground equipment.'' These words from a kindergarten
Landscape and Urban Planning 48 (2000) 83±97
*
Corresponding author. Tel.: 47-35026-333;
fax: 47-35026-201.
E-mail address: ingunn.fjortoft@hit.no (I. Fjùrtoft)
0169-2046/00/$20.00 #2000 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.
PII: S 0169-2046(00)00045-1
boy may serve as an example of how children consider
the traditional playground and how they ®nd the
natural playscape more attractive and exciting (Tit-
man, 1994; Moore and Wong, 1997). Previous studies
have explained children's experience of place, their
special preferences for the unbuilt and unstructured
environment, and how children interpret place and
space (Hart, 1979; Moore, 1986). Children need green
playgrounds including ®elds to play on, trees for
climbing and bushes for shelter and hiding (Titman,
1994). Prescott (1987) concluded that kindergarten
environments were too simple in comparison with the
natural environment. She mentioned three unique
qualities about nature; the high diversity, the fact that
nature is not made by man, and the impression of
timelessness. Furthermore, several studies indicate
that a diverse and adventurous playground stimulates
creative play (Frost and Campbell, 1985; Frost and
Strickland, 1985; Moore, G.T., 1985; Moore, R.C.,
1986; Steel and Neuman, 1985; Winter, 1985; Frost
and Wortham, 1988; Hart, 1993). Scandinavian stu-
dies report positive impact from playing in nature on
the children's social play, concentration and motor
ability (Bang et al., 1989; Grahn, 1991; Fjùrtoft,
1995a, 1998; Grahn et al., 1997). However, few
studies systematically document the impact a natural
environment might have on children's development
(Bjerke, 1994). One reason may be an apparent lack of
suitable methods for describing and analyzing natural
environments as playscapes.
Frost (1992) introduced the concept of `playscape'
for describing different play environments. He
argued that natural features are important qualities
of playgrounds, and that the natural features allow a
wide range of learning opportunities not available
from other playground options. When planning plays-
capes for children, it is important to focus on
which environmental features are available for play.
The possibilities to choose their own activities and
create their own playscape in the environment are
important elements in children's play (Moore, 1985).
Green structures and nature are of high priority
in children (Moore and Young, 1978; Hart, 1979,
1982; Heft, 1988). Variation and diversity also
seem to be important characteristics of the play habi-
tats and so does the unstructured and manipulative
environment (Nicholson, 1971; Moore, 1986; Laursen,
1991).
1.1. Aims and objectives
In Norway, nature is still a part of culture. The
landscape has not only been an object of utilitarian
value but also a scene for outdoor life and leisure
activities (FRIFO, 1992; Kaltenborn and og Vorkinn,
1993). Nature is a part of people's local environment
and Norwegian children have usually grown up in
places where the natural environment was the arena
for free play and physical activities (Fjùrtoft, 1995b).
The present study aimed at describing a natural envir-
onment as a playscape for children in Norway.
Furthermore, it was focused on the learning effects
that all-round play in nature might have on children's
motor development (Fjùrtoft, 1998, 1999). For this
presentation, we have concentrated on the description
and analyses of the landscape as a playground for
children, based on the affordance of the landscape for
versatile play. It was hypothesized that variation and
diversity in the landscape corresponded with diversity
in play activities. The main objectives were (1) to
describe and analyze a woodland area as a playground
for children, and (2) to describe, analyze and visualize
how children's play was related to important land-
scape elements such as vegetation and topography.
This involved exploring methods from landscape
ecology and geomorphology for description, analysis
and visualization of landscape characteristics as
important elements in playscapes for children.
1.2. Study area
The study area, the forest (Fig. 1), was a 7.7 ha
woodland vegetation with varied topography, located
close to a kindergarten in Bù, Telemark County in
Norway. The forest was the outdoor playscape, outside
the kindergarten fence. The closer parts of it were used
spontaneously by the children on their own, while
adults accompanied the children to the parts further
away.
2. Methods
To our knowledge, there is no established metho-
dology to describe the natural environment as a play-
ground for children. In environmental psychology, the
focus is on how we mentally perceive the environment
84 I. Fjùrtoft, J. Sageie / Landscape and Urban Planning 48 (2000) 83±97
(Lynch, 1960; Elfstro
Èm, 1991). A semantic environ-
mental description is based on experienced dimen-
sions of the landscape (Ku
Èller, 1972; Kaplan and
Kaplan, 1989; Sorte, 1989; Lindgren, 1990). Applica-
tion of these methods for describing natural environ-
ments as playgrounds for children have been explored
(Fjùrtoft, 1993; Berggren-Ba
Èrring and Grahn, 1995),
but for this purpose, the semantic environmental
description based on adults' visual perceptions and
qualitative appreciation of a landscape is insuf®cient.
As adults, we perceive the landscape as forms,
whereas children will interpret the landscape and
the terrain as functions (Gibson, 1979; Heft, 1988).
Therefore, methods focusing on structures and func-
tions of the environment are more appropriate for
describing natural landscapes as playscapes for chil-
dren.
2.1. Landscape ecology
The theories of landscape ecology are based on
composition,structure,function and change of phy-
sical landscapes (Forman and Godron, 1986). Quan-
titative metrics have been developed to describe the
relations between the structure and the function in
spatial patterns (Forman, 1997). A common way of
describing landscapes from a species perspective is to
study habitat variables that are likely to affect the
species' utilization of the habitat and landscape of
interest, e.g. vegetation and topography (Ims, 1992;
Forman, 1997). In the context of landscape ecology,
we might therefore introduce the concept of human
play habitats, i.e. the different landscape elements
and structures that afford children's play. The affor-
dances of play are based on the features that the
environment can provide (Gibson, 1979). We focused
on vegetation and topography because these elements
comprise the physical patterns of a landscape and can
be expressed as composition, structure and function of
an environment (Ska
Ênes, 1997). The landscape
metrics were computed according to McGarigal and
Marks (1998).
2.2. Vegetation
Vegetation mapping was done by ®eld inventory
and aerial photo-interpretation (Paine and Luba, 1980;
Ihse, 1989, 1995). The vegetation was classi®ed
according to the Norwegian classi®cation system
(Fremstad, 1997). Identi®ed patches in the ®eld inven-
tory were classi®ed into main vegetation types, based
on phytosociology and physiognomy (Heywood and
Watson, 1995; Fremstad, 1997). Thematic maps of
physiognomy were derived from the vegetation map
by use of geographic information system (GIS) (Tom-
lin, 1990; Bernhardsen, 1995; Worboys, 1995).
2.3. Topography
Elevation data were contour lines extracted from the
base-map transformed into digital representation as a
digital elevation model (DEM). The DEM was the
basis for calculating slope and roughness of the terrain
Fig. 1. (a) Localization of the study area; (b) the kindergarten and
the forest.
I. Fjùrtoft, J. Sageie / Landscape and Urban Planning 48 (2000) 83±97 85
(Burrough, 1996). Detailed terrain measurements by
use of geodetic methods were used for three reference
pro®les to determine the terrain con®guration for a
frequently used sliding slope, a steep cliff and a rough
climbing track (Weibel and Heller, 1993; Wangen,
1998).
Methods for deriving and analyzing topography and
vegetation data were implemented in the GIS-software
ARC/INFO (Environmental Systems Research Insti-
tute (ESRI), 1997). For landscape ecology analysis,
we used FRAGSTATS*ARC (innovative GIS solu-
tions (IGIS) McGarigal and Marks, 1998).
2.4. Playscape
Global positioning systems (GPSs) were used to
map speci®c play habitats. Calculation of this survey-
ing was done as differential GPS (DGPS) (Simensen,
1998). The play activities were de®ned and classi®ed
into three categories (Frost, 1992). (1) Functional play
comprised gross-motor activities and basic skills and
were implemented in games like play tag, chase and
catch, leapfrog, hide and seek, catch a tree, making
angels in the snow, and other games involving basic
movements. (2) Construction play was the type of play
that was afforded by landscape structures and loose
parts, e.g. building shelters, dens and other construc-
tions like a pirate ship, building with cones and sticks
and other moveable things. In the winter season, snow
was an excellent building material. (3) Symbolic play
included socio-dramatic play and was recorded as role
play and fantasy play such as play house, pirates, play
farm with cones and sticks etc. As a natural environ-
ment might challenge several movements and physical
mastering, play in a natural environment was, there-
fore, assumed to have an impact on the children's
motor development. Consequently, an experimental
study was carried out, describing how all-round play
activities in a natural environment might have an
impact on children's motor development (Fjùrtoft,
1999). The playscape was used by, in all, 46 children
in the age group 5±7 years old for 2 h every day when
they attended kindergarten. The study lasted for a
period of 9 months, from September to June. The
play activities and the localities for play were
observed and reported by the kindergarten teachers
(Robson, 1997). The spatial relations between the
different landscape characteristics and the observed
play activities were visualized by using spatial overlay
analysis and correlation plots (Berry, 1997).
3. Results
3.1. Vegetation
The landscape spatial pattern formed by the vegeta-
tion of the forest, total area 7.7 ha, was a mosaic of 34
patches of different types of woodland interspersed
with some open spaces consisting of rocks, ®elds and
meadows (Fig. 2, Table 1).
The woodlands consisted of six different vegetation
types, and were dominated by rich deciduous wood-
land in the south-western parts of the area (Fig. 2a).
Two types of rich decidious woodland were registered
(Fig. 2a, Table 2). The wych elm small-leaved-lime
woodland was the most widespread one with maple
(Acer platanoides), alder (Alnus incana), hazel (Cor-
ylus avellana), ash (Fraxinus excelsior) and elm
(Ulmus glabra) as common tree species. In the
north-eastern parts, the coniferous and mixed wood-
Table 1
Landscape metrics of the total area and the playscape
Landscape metrics Ta
a
NP
b
LPI
c
MPS
d
PR
e
SIDI
f
SIEI
g
Total area 7.71 34 21.2 0.23 10 0.82 0.92
Playscape 4.89 22 33.5 0.22 9 0.78 0.88
a
TA: total area (ha).
b
NP: number of patches.
c
LPI: largest patch index.
d
MPS: mean patch size (ha).
e
PR: patch richness.
f
SIDI: Simpson's diversity index.
g
SIEI: Simpson's evenness index.
86 I. Fjùrtoft, J. Sageie / Landscape and Urban Planning 48 (2000) 83±97
Fig. 2. Vegetation map and derived tematic maps on edge contrasts between vegetation types and physiognomy of trees and shrubs. Playscape
is indicated by border lines.
I. Fjùrtoft, J. Sageie / Landscape and Urban Planning 48 (2000) 83±97 87
lands dominated the forest (Fig. 2a, Table 2). The
coniferous woodland consisted of pine woodland with
Scots pine (Pinus sylvestris), while the Norway spruce
(Picea abies) dominated the bilberry woodland. The
low-herb woodland was a mixture of pine, spruce and
deciduous trees like birch (Betula pubescens) and
aspen (Populus tremula) and a more scattered occur-
rence of sallow (Salix caprea) and mountain ash
(Sorbus aucuparia). This woodland was dominated
by scattered deciduous shrub vegetation (Fig. 2a,
Table 2), and a mixture of trees and shrubs (Fig. 2c±e).
3.2. Landscape metrics
Based on the vegetation data, several landscape
metrics were computed (Tables 1 and 2). Mean patch
size (MPS) of 0.23 ha and the largest patch index
(LPI) of 21.2 illustrated the ®ne-grained pattern in the
landscape mosaic. The Simpson diversity index (SIDI)
of 0.82 and the Simpson evenness index (SIEI) of 0.92
indicated variation and heterogeneity in the area.
At the class level, the wych elm-small-leaved wood-
land had an LPI of 14.9, and covered 23.8% of the total
area (PLAND) with an MPS0.5. It also had the
highest class area (CA) of 1.8 and was distributed
on four different patches (Table 2). The low-herb
woodland was the second largest class of woodland
in the area (CA1.3) distributed on four patches, with
the LPI0.9 and the MPS0.3. The coniferous wood-
land consisted of six patches of bilberry woodland
(CA0.8, PLAND9.8) and three patches of pine
woodland (CA0.3, PLAND4.4). The open space
was dominated by meadows and ®elds with weed
vegetation on road embankments and waste places
Table 2
Class metrics of the total area (TA) and the playscape (PL)
Vegetation types Area CA
a
NP
b
PLAND
c
LPI
d
MPS
e
MSI
f
Wych elm-small-leaved lime woodland TA 1.8 4 23.8 14.9 0.5 1.8
PL 0.4 1 7.7 7.7 0.4 1.7
Low-herb woodland TA 1.3 4 17.2 9.9 0.3 1.6
PL 1.3 3 25.7 15.5 0.4 1.7
Bilberry woodland TA 0.8 6 9.8 6.7 0.1 1.4
PL 0.8 6 15.5 10.6 0.1 1.4
Pine woodland TA 0.3 3 4.4 3.4 0.1 1.4
PL 0.3 3 6.9 5.4 0.1 1.4
Alder-ash woodland TA 0.2 1 2.2 2.2 0.2 2.1
PL 0.2 1 3.4 3.4 0.2 2.1
Tall-fern woodland TA 0.1 1 1.1 1.1 0.1 1.7
PL 0.1 1 1.7 1.7 0.1 1.7
Woodland border shrub TA 0.5 2 6.4 4.3 0.2 1.9
PL ± ± ± ± ± ±
Vegetation on road embankments and waste places TA 2.0 2 26.1 21.2 1.0 1.5
PL 1.6 1 33.4 33.5 1.6 1.6
Weed vegetation in cultivated fields TA 0.5 6 6.3 1.9 0.1 1.7
PL 0.1 3 2.4 1.0 0.0 1.8
Rocks TA 0.2 5 2.7 1.1 0.0 1.5
PL 0.2 3 3.3 1.8 0.5 1.5
a
CA: class area (ha).
b
NP: number of patches.
c
PLAND: percent of landscape (%).
d
LPI: largest patch index.
e
MPS: mean patch size (ha).
f
MSI: mean shape index.
88 I. Fjùrtoft, J. Sageie / Landscape and Urban Planning 48 (2000) 83±97
as the largest ground (CA0.5, PLAND26.1) (Table
2, Fig. 2a).
The edge parameters indicated varying contrasts in
the study area (Fig. 2b, Table 3). The highest contrast
appeared where open ®elds met dense woodland (edge
contrast1). The longer borders also had higher edge
contrast when coupled with the weighted contrast for
the vegetation types (weighted length63.6±125.6).
The contrast between the different woodlands was
low, due to short borders and low weighted contrasts
(Fig. 2b, Table 3). Even spaces with open rocks had a
low edge contrast, i.e. tall-fern woodland/rocks0.2
(Table 3). This may also be explained by the very short
perimeter (weighted length0.4±25.5, Fig. 2b).
3.3. Topography
The topography was undulating with terraces and
slopes and a dominating steep slope traversing the
area. Topography analysis results are presented as
pro®les, slope-map, roughness-map, and statistical
®gures (Fig. 3).
Directional derivatives of the surface provided
information about slope and roughness as the most
important descriptive variables of topography. Slope
(the ®rst derivative) was calculated in degrees and
showed a range from min 0 to max 72 with a mean
value 14.5 within the total area (Fig. 3a). Roughness
(the second derivative) described the variation of slope
(Fig. 3c). This variable can be calculated in different
directions; we used the pro®le curvature that showed
the change of steepest slope across the surface (SUR-
FER User's Guide, 1997). Avalue around 0 indicated a
smooth terrain and higher values a more rough land-
scape. The statistical ®gures in Table 4 show that the
mean value for roughness for different play habitats
was approximately 0, which means nearly the same
volume of convex and concave changes in the topo-
graphical curvature. The standard deviation illustrated
the variation of topography within different play
habitats, where a standard deviation of 1.5 indicated
a smoother topography and 4.8 a more `dramatic'
landscape.
3.4. Playscape
The play habitats were located next to the kinder-
garten area (Fig. 1b, Fig. 2a) and covered 63% (4.9 ha)
of the total landscape. The playscape had a patch
richness of 9 out of 10 possible vegetation types
and included all the six woodland types, rocks, ®elds
Fig. 3. Geomorphology: slope and roughness; playscape indicated
with the locality of terrain; measured pro®les.
I. Fjùrtoft, J. Sageie / Landscape and Urban Planning 48 (2000) 83±97 89
Table 3
Edge contrasts: contrasts between vegetation types based on observations and a reasoned guess
Vegetation
types
Alder-ash
woodland
Bilberry
woodland
Low-herb
woodland
Pine
woodland
Rocks Tall-fern
woodland
Vegetation on
road embankments
and waste
Weed vegetation
in cultivated
fields
Woodland
border
scrub
Wych elm-small-
leaved lime
woodland
Alder-ash woodland ±
Bilberry woodland 0.3 ±
Low-herb woodland 0.3 0.3 ±
Pine woodland 0.4 0.3 0.2 ±
Rocks 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 ±
Tall-fern woodland 0.8 0.8 0.8 0.8 0.2 ±
Vegetation on road embankments
and waste
0.8 0.8 0.8 0.7 0.2 0.2 ±
Weed vegetation in cultivated fields 0.8 0.8 0.8 0.8 0.2 0.2 0.1 ±
Woodland border scrub 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.3 ±
Wych elm-small-leaved lime
woodland
0.1 0.3 0.3 0.4 1.0 0.9 0.8 0.8 0.5 ±
90 I. Fjùrtoft, J. Sageie / Landscape and Urban Planning 48 (2000) 83±97
and meadows (Fig. 2a). The low-herb woodland was
the dominating woodland type, covering 26% of the
playscape and representing an LPI of 15.5 (Tables
1and 2). The diversity index for the play habitats was
0.78 and the evenness index 0.88, i.e. a little lower
than for the total landscape (Table 1). The physiog-
nomy of tree species was shaped by pine, spruce,
deciduous trees and mixed (Fig. 2c). The physiog-
nomy of the shrubs was mixed and deciduous and the
density of shrubs was scattered only with some dense
areas in the outskirts, bordering the neighboring ®eld
(Fig. 2d and e). The edge contrasts showed low to
medium contrast values between the vegetation types
in the play area (Fig. 2b). The shape index for vegeta-
tion types ranged from 1.4 to 2.1, indicating a rela-
tively commensurable shape for the different
vegetation types (Table 2). The 5-, 6- and 7-year
old children in the kindergarten used the playscape
for approximately 2 h every day (Fig. 4). The diversity
in woodland types afforded all the three categories of
play activities (Fig. 4, Table 4). Woodland types
constituted climbing areas, with the pine as the most
frequent climbing tree, while areas with scattered
shrub cover were preferred for construction play, like
building dens and shelters, and symbolic play like
playing house and pirates. Shrubs like the Juniper bush
turned into a nice home, the stump might represented a
nice pilothouse for the pirate ship, while the meadows
Table 4
Play activities related to landscape characters
a
Landscape characters Play activities
Vegetation Class Climbing
rocks
Climbing
trees
Running Sliding Skiing Symbol
play
Construction
play
Physiognomy trees Deciduous ± 28% ± ± ± 35% 40%
Spruce ± ± ± ± ± 18% ±
Pine ± 34% ± ± ± 20% ±
Mixed ± 7% ± ± ± 6% 14%
Pine/spruce ± 20% ± ± ± 11% 38%
Physiognomy shrubs Deciduous ± ± ± ± ± 46% 48%
Mixed ± ± ± ± ± 48% 53%
Pine/spruce ± ± ± ± ± ± ±
Density of shrubs Open 13% ± ± 12% 98% 7% ±
Scattered 79% ± 100% 79% ± 86% 96%
Dense 9% ± ± 9% ± 8% 4%
Topography (slope) Mean degree 22.5 ± 11.6 22.3 7.0 17.2 10.3
(S.D.) (7.8) ± (4.1) (7.7) (8.3) (7.7) (3.4)
Topography (roughness) Mean value ÿ0.8 ± ÿ0.2 ÿ0.8 0.2 0.2 ÿ0.1
(S.D.) (4.8) ± (2.4) (4.8) (2.3) (4.0) (1.5)
a
Physiognomy of vegetation reported in % playscape area, topography reported as mean values and (S.D.) of slope (degrees) and
roughness (second derivative of height).
Fig. 4. Playscape and core areas.
I. Fjùrtoft, J. Sageie / Landscape and Urban Planning 48 (2000) 83±97 91
and open spaces might be a battle®eld. Symbolic play
and construction play were more dependent on both
topography and vegetation. Whereas the topography
had a smooth character, the vegetation was more
complex in tree species, shrubs and density of shrubs.
Traditional games like hide and seek, catch and run
were located to smooth topography with open space or
scattered shrub density (Fig. 2c±e). The open, large
meadow patch of vegetation on waste places and road
embankments next to the kindergarten was solely used
for skiing activities in the winter and was used daily in
the period from January to April. Functional play
activities were linked to activities such as climbing
rocks, climbing trees, running, sliding and skiing
(Table 4).
The traversing steep slope including rocks and cliffs
dominated the topography of the playscape. Fig. 3b
illustrates pro®les at different sites of the slope. Pro®le
1 was a steep slope of 358and Pro®le 2 was a rough
slope, but less steep. These two pro®les represented
typical areas for climbing rocks. Pro®le 3 illustrated a
long and smooth 21.58slope which represented a long
`bob-run' used in the winter season. All three terrain
measured pro®les were located within the area
mapped for sliding and climbing habitats and the
statistical ®gures for roughness were the same for
those areas (Table 4). The roughness values showed
that the smoothest topography was found within areas
for construction play (meanÿ0.1, S.D.1.5), while
the most `dramatic' topography was found within
areas for climbing rocks and sliding (meanÿ0.8,
S.D.4.8).
There were some core areas in the playscape that the
children used more frequently. The children named
those areas, and the names may illustrate their func-
tional use. The activities taking place at those areas
were closely linked to the structures and the functions
they represented, e.g. `the Spaceship' was a big ship-
shaped rock randomly placed underneath the steep
slope. `The Cone War' was located in a patch of
bilberry woodland of big spruces. `The Cliff' was a
steep end of the long traversing slope where daring
sliding activities took place in wintertime. The activity
map in Fig. 4 shows the location of the core areas.
Seasonal changes in the use of the play habitats
were observed. The meadow next to the kindergarten
(Figs. 2a and 4) was an arena for skiing activities. The
snow and a suitable geomorphology afforded sliding
(Figs. 3b and 4). Typical winter play activities like
`angels in the snow' and construction play with snow
(®gures, caves) took place in the forest. With a deep
snow cover, the young deciduous trees became more
accessible for climbing.
4. Effects on motor development
By all-round playing in a diverse and rough land-
scape, the children's motor ®tness was improved.
Results from the experimental study showed consid-
erable improvements in motor ®tness in the experi-
mental group (n46) compared to the reference group
(n29). Signi®cant difference (p<0.01) between the
groups was found in balance and co-ordination skills.
Those were considered important competencies
regarding motor learning. The age and maturation
related competencies such as strength and ¯exibility
were improved in both groups, but were not related to
training in the same way as other motor ®tness skills
(Fjùrtoft (in preparation)).
5. Discussion
5.1. The natural playscape
In this study, we found that a diverse natural land-
scape had the qualities to meet the children's needs for
a varied and stimulating play environment where the
composition and structures of the landscape were
conducive to different play functions. The landscape
ecology analyses showed a heterogeneous environ-
ment rich in woodland types. The number and spatial
pattern of patches constituted a varied landscape
mosaic (Forman, 1997) and the diversity and the
evenness indexes for the playscape were high com-
pared to the total study area (Fig. 2, Tables 1 and 2).
The shape and edge metrics also illustrated the varia-
tion and the heterogeneity of the landscape (Fig. 2b,
Tables 2 and 3). The total amount of edge in a land-
scape is directly related to the degree of spatial
heterogeneity, and the edge effects are in¯uenced
by the degree of contrasts between the patches
(McGarigal and Marks, 1998). We found soft bound-
aries between woodland types in the playscape
(Fig. 2b). Soft boundaries together with convoluted
92 I. Fjùrtoft, J. Sageie / Landscape and Urban Planning 48 (2000) 83±97
patch shapes increased interaction with the surround-
ings (Dramstad et al., 1996; Forman, 1997). Convo-
luted and low edge contrasts in our study made it easy
for the children to move about and this might in¯uence
play behaviour.
5.2. Affordances and the impact on children
In the interpretation of the relevance of landscape
ecology metrics and the topography for playscapes for
children, we have focused on the affordances of the
landscape for play. The structure of the landscape,
such as the physiognomy of vegetation and the geo-
morphology, ®lled the functions for play. The children
perceived the functions of the landscape and used
them (Gibson, 1979). They selected the habitats that
afforded play (Fig. 4 and Table 4). This was the
observed behavioural response of the children to the
environment, i.e. in ecological terms, their habitat
selection (Begon et al., 1990). The play habitats were
related to diversity in play activities and seasonal
changes (Table 4). The characteristics of the habitats
determined the use and the seasonal preferences
(Begon et al., 1990; Ims, 1992; Ska
Ênes, 1997).
Nicholson (1971) argued that there is evidence that
all children love to interact with physical environ-
mental features, such as materials and shapes, gravity,
smell, and other things, which they can discover,
explore, and experiment with. The stimulation of
inventiveness and creativity, and the possibility of
discovery are directly related to the number and the
kind of features in the environment. Table 4 illustrates
the relationship between the play activities and the
landscape characteristics. The physiognomy of trees
played a signi®cant role not only for climbing trees but
also for symbolic play and construction play. The
areas for climbing trees were dominated by pine trees
(34%), deciduous trees (28%) and mixed pine and
spruce (20%). For climbing trees, the other landscape
characteristics were considered to be of no signi®-
cance. Deciduous trees such as birch and sallow were
popular climbing trees especially during wintertime
when they became more accessible due to a high snow
layer. In summer time, some ropes and rope ladders
made the pines more accessible for climbing, hanging,
dangling and swinging. The sites for symbol play were
characterized by deciduous trees (38%), but with other
tree species also present. The same was the case for
construction play, where deciduous trees dominated.
Materials from deciduous trees were more easily
accessible for building huts and shelters and this
corresponds well to Nicholson's theory of playing
with loose parts (1971). The physiognomy of shrubs
seemed to have an in¯uence on all the play activities
except for climbing trees. At the sites for symbolic
play and construction play, the deciduous shrubs and
mixed shrub vegetation were dominating, while the
density of shrubs was scattered, 87% of symbol play
habitats and 96% of construction play habitats. The
importance of shrub vegetation linked to those two
play activities was obvious for the purpose of hiding,
playing house, building shelters etc. The skiing and
sliding areas were used in the winter only. The shrub
cover in those areas was mainly open and scattered
and did not hinder the activities. The topography of the
playscape was linked to the activities by slope and
roughness. The more challenging activities like climb-
ing rocks and sliding took place in the steepest part of
the playscape with a mean slope of 22.58(S.D.7.8),
but ranged from 15 to above 308. High values in
roughness (meanÿ0.8, S.D.4.8) favoured climb-
ing activities, but disfavoured sliding. The reason why
Table 4 shows the same values of topographical data
for both climbing rocks and sliding is because data
have been captured from the same sites for both
activities. The pro®les in Fig. 3b illustrate topography
for climbing and sliding. The other activities are
obviously favoured by a less steep and a smoother
topography (Table 4). These spatial and play activity
relations proved to have an impact on children's motor
development (Fjùrtoft, 1999 (in press)). Motor mas-
tering is an important quality in children's behaviour
and a quali®cation to be included in different social
activities. Grahn et al. (1997) also found the same
effects in kindergarten children playing in an enriched
natural environment compared to a traditional city
kindergarten. He also found that an enriched environ-
ment improved the children's concentration abilities.
Health conditions in children in the nature kindergar-
ten were also improved, as they reported less sickness
absence compared to the urban kindergarten.
5.3. Implications for physical planning
An important aspect, which needs to be further
explored, is how and to what extent diversity indices
I. Fjùrtoft, J. Sageie / Landscape and Urban Planning 48 (2000) 83±97 93
should be related to playscapes for children. In our
study, landscape diversity was related to different
structures in the topography and the vegetation, which
were important for children's spontaneous play and
activities. Diversity is also synonymous with an
enriched environment, which again stimulates and
promotes play and learning (Frost, 1992; Titman,
1994; Rivkin, 1995; Moore and Wong, 1997). Learn-
ing effects due to diversity in the landscape have been
documented in this paper (Grahn et al., 1997; Fjùrtoft,
1998 (in preparation)). It is obvious that other learning
effects from a diverse natural landscape such as nature
studies may occur through children's play and activ-
ities. Learning science from nature has not been the
focus of this paper, but learning from nature when
playing in nature may be characterized as the hidden
curriculum (Titman, 1994).
A natural environment as playscape for children
may represent a challenge demanding new attitudes in
policy and planning. In existing planning directives,
there tend to be three main criteria for playground
planning: distance from residential areas, kindergar-
tens, schools etc., area size, and safe access to the sites.
The physical planning of playgrounds has not
addressed children's needs for a diverse and stimulat-
ing playscape. In schools and kindergartens, the out-
door grounds have not been corresponding with
children's needs for affordances and challenges for
play, nor has it re¯ected the policy of the prevailing
curriculum (Adams, 1990). It is, therefore, reasonable
to suggest some new criteria to be included in planning
directives to insure playscapes with landscape quali-
ties representing affordances and challenges for chil-
dren (Fjùrtoft and Sageie, in preparation). In such a
perspective, it is also necessary to discuss an accep-
table level of risks. Playscapes with the highest level
of security tend also to represent areas with the lowest
affordances and challenges. Consequently, diversity in
landscape elements, affordances for play, challenges
and safety, accessibility and wear resistance may be
important criteria in the planning and management of
future playscapes for children.
In this study, methods from landscape ecology, and
geomorphology implemented in GIS for description
and analysis of the natural environment's suitability as
a playground for children were applied. For this
purpose, we successfully used some of the methods,
but more remains to be explored. Applied as a tool for
planning and management for natural playscapes,
some implications of the methods should be
addressed. Necessary spatial resolution in data
depends on the purpose of the study. The geometric
accuracy range of the mapping methods was 10±15 m
for the less accurate data. For our purpose, descrip-
tions of the topography and the vegetation types are at
a typical scale for botanical studies, i.e. some m
2
,
tentatively estimated to be suf®cient. Consequently,
for practical planning use, the vegetation map may be
useful but not a must for describing and selecting
suitable playscapes. But for more scienti®c descrip-
tion and analyses of play habitats, a vegetation map
adds information. For detailed analyses of the land-
scape, ecology detailed vegetation data are most
appropriate.
For the slope and roughness analyses, we have not
explored all the possibilities available in GIS. The
divergence in roughness value (Table 4), and evalua-
tion results of the reference pro®les (Fig. 3b) showed
that a more distinct classi®cation of play habitats is
necessary. The children's use of the landscape
over time might in¯uence its ecology. In this study,
the time perspective was too short to register any
ecological change due to the children's use of the
landscape. However, this might be a topic of serious
concern in planning and selecting natural playscapes
for children.
For the calculation of edge contrasts, the patches
were weighted subjectively, i.e. a reasoned guess
based on observed contrasts between the patches
(McGarigal and Marks, 1998). This of course in¯u-
enced the reliability of these data.
In theory, the appropriate scale of study is the
smallest scale which an organism perceives and
responds to (e.g. Forman, 1997; McGarigal and
Marks, 1998). In our case, the user was the child,
and the child perceives functions rather than forms
(Gibson, 1979). Perhaps, the scale should be more
function-related and linked to points and special fea-
tures in the playscape, for example, a special feature, a
climbing tree or a sliding slope.
6. Conclusion
The focus on the natural environment as a play-
ground and learning arena is a way of rediscovering
94 I. Fjùrtoft, J. Sageie / Landscape and Urban Planning 48 (2000) 83±97
nature's way of teaching, or `learning from nature'.
Hart (1982), Naylor (1985), Moore (1986), Heft
(1988), Matthew (1992), Titman (1994), Rivkin
(1995) and Moore and Wong (1997) have discussed
the importance of the natural environment as a part
of children's growth and development. The key
concepts from previous studies and the present one
are the importance of `green structures and loose
parts' and multivariable possibilities for versatile play.
Our study showed a strong relation between landscape
structures and play functions. Different landscape
elements afforded different and speci®c possibilities
for play. There was a positive relation between play
activities and the diversity in vegetation types and
physiognomy of trees and shrubs, i.e. building dens
was linked to scattered shrub vegetation, climbing
trees was linked to pines in the summer time and to
young deciduous trees in winter. The diversity of
topography expressed as slope and roughness also
provided diversity in play activities. Steep slopes were
linked to sliding and steep and rough cliffs were
challenging for climbing. More even landscape struc-
tures were used for running activities, role-play and
games. This corresponded with Gibson's theory of
affordances (1979) which explained the composition
of the environment as functions for use. The children
interpreted these affordances and adapted them into
functions for play. These functions have proved an
impact on motor development in children as well as
preventive health effects. The overall conclusion from
the present study may be that natural landscapes
represent potential grounds for playing and learning
and this has to be taken into serious consideration for
future policy and planning of outdoor grounds for
children.
Acknowledgements
The authors appreciate the regular communication
with the producers of FRAGSTATS*ARC spatial
pattern analysis program. We would like to acknowl-
edge the supervision of Senior Engineer J. Hofsten on
®eld inventory and vegetation mapping. We are
indebted to Professor J. Heggenes, Dr. W. Dramstad
and Professor K. Nicolaysen for comments and sug-
gestions on this manuscript, and to Assistant Professor
P.K. Halle for proof-reading.
References
Adams, E., 1990. Learning through landscapes. The Final
Report.
Bang, C., Braute, J., Kohen, B., 1989. Naturleikeplassen. Ein stad
for leik og lñring. Universitetsforlaget, Oslo.
Begon, M., Harper, J.L., Townsend, C.R., 1990. Ecology.
Individuals, Populations and Communities, 2nd Edition. Black-
well Scienti®c Publications, Oxford.
Berggren-Ba
Èrring, A.-M., Grahn, P., 1995. Gro
Ènstrukturens bety-
delse fo
Èranva
Èndingen. Rapport 95:3. Sveriges Lantbruksuni-
versitet, Alnarp.
Bernhardsen, T., 1995. Geogra®ske informasjonssystemer. Vett &
Viten.
Berry, K.J., 1997. Beyond Mapping: Concepts, Algorithms, and
Issues in GIS, 4th Edition. GIS World, Inc., Fort Collins, USA.
Bjerke, T., 1994. BARN Ð NATUR Ð FRILUFTSLIV. A/S
Landbruksforlaget.
Burrough, P.A., 1996. Principles of Geographical Information
Systems for Land Resources Assessment. University of
Utrecht, Netherlands, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Dramstad, W.E., Olson, J.D., Forman, R.T.T., 1996. Landscape
Ecology Principles in Landscape Architecture and Land Use
Planning. Harvard University Graduate School of Design,
Island Press, American Society of Landscape Architects,
Washington.
Elfstro
Èm, J., 1991. Landskapsbildanalys. Stad & Land 97.
Fjùrtoft, I., 1993. Hundremeterskogen -et sted for leik og lñring.
Prosjektarbeid i barnehager i Bù. BARN. Nytt fra forskning om
barn i Norge. 3, 71±93.
Fjùrtoft, I., 1995a. Fysisk miljù og sansemotorisk stimulering.
Forsknings- og utviklingsarbeid med barn i naturen. Kroppsùv-
ing 1, s.2±5.
Fjùrtoft, I., 1995b. Hundremeterskogen Ð va
Êr barndoms skog. I:
Grimeland (red.): Under bladet ligger det en liten larve...Om
opplevelse, utvikling og lñring gjennom utelek. Artikkelsaml-
ing for planleggere av barns utemiljù. Barne- og familiede-
partementet, Oslo.
Fjùrtoft, I., 1998. Ut i skogen Ð opp i trñrne! Naturen Ð et sted
for lek og lñring. In: Konferensrapport. Barn och Friluftsliv.
Friluftsfra
Èmjandet.
Fjùrtoft, I., 1999. The Natural Environment As a Playground for
Children. The Impact of Out-Door Play Activities in Pre-
Primary School Children. In: Proceedings of OMEP's 22nd
World Congress and 50th Anniversary on The Child's Rights to
Care, Play and Education, Copenhagen, Denmark, 13±16
August.
Fjùrtoft, I. The Natural Enivronment ± a landscape for learning the
impact of natural environments on childrens motor develop-
ment, in preparation.
Fjùrtoft, I., Sageie, J. Natural environments as playgrounds for
children: Potential suitability criteria for natural playscapes, in
preparation.
Forman, R.T.T., 1997. Land Mosaics: the Ecology of Landscapes
and Regions. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (reprint).
Forman, R.T.T., Godron, M., 1986. Landscape Ecology. Wiley,
New York.
I. Fjùrtoft, J. Sageie / Landscape and Urban Planning 48 (2000) 83±97 95
Fremstad, E., 1997. Vegetasjonstyperi Norge. NINA Temahefte 12.
Norsk institutt for naturforskning Trondheim.
FRIFO, 1992. Undersùklese av nordmenns friluftsliv, deres
naturverdier, holdninger og adferd. MMI Rapport.
Frost, J.L., 1992. Play and Playscapes. Delmar Publishers, New
York.
Frost, J.L., Campbell, S.D., 1985. Equipment choices of primary-age
children on conventional and creative playgrounds. In: Frost, J.L,
Sunderlin, S. (Eds.), When Children Play. Proceedings of the
International Conference on Play and Play Environments.
Association for Childhood Education International.
Frost, J.L., Strickland, E., 1985. Equipment choices of young
children during free play. In: Frost, J.L., Sunderlin, S. (Eds.),
When Children Play. Proceedings of the International Con-
ference on Play and Play Environments. Association for
Childhood Education International.
Frost, J.L., Wortham, S., 1988. The evolution of American
playgrounds. Young Children 19±28.
Gibson, J., 1979. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception.
Houghton Mif¯in Company, Boston.
Grahn, P., 1991. Om parkers betydelse. Stad & Land nr. 93/1991
Sveriges lantbruksuniversitet, Alnarp.
Grahn, P., Ma
Êrtensson, F., Lindblad, B., Nilsson, P., Ekman, A.,
1997. UTE pa
ÊDAGIS. Stad & Land 45: Movium, Sveriges
lantbruksuniversitet, Alnarp.
Hart, R., 1979. Children's Experience of Place. Irvington Publish-
ers, New York.
Hart, R., 1982. Wildlands for children: consideration of the value of
natural environments in landscape planning. LandshaftStadt
14(1) 34±39. Eugen Ulmer GmbH & Co., Stuttgart.
Hart, C.H. (Ed.), 1993. Children on Playgrounds. Research
Perspectives and Applications. State University of New York
Press, Albany.
Heft, H., 1988. Affordances of children's environments: a
functional approach to environmental description. Children's
Environ. Quarterly 5(3).
Heywood, V.H. (Ex. Ed.), Watson, R.T. (Chair), 1995. Global
Biodiversity Assessment. Published for the United Nations
Environment Programme. Cambridge University Press, Cam-
bridge.
Ihse, M., 1989. Air photo interpretation and computer cartography
Ð tools for studying the changes in the cultural landscape. In:
Berks, J. (Ed.), The Cultural Landscape Ð Past, Present and
Future. Cambridge University press, Cambridge.
Ihse, M., 1995. Swedish agricultural landscapes Ð patterns and
changes during the last 50 years, studied by aerial photos.
Landscape and Urban Planning 31, 21±37.
Ims, R.A., 1992. Hva er landskapsùkologi. Problem og metoder.
Fauna 43, 151±171.
Kaltenborn, B.P., og Vorkinn, M., 1993. Va
Êrt Friluftsliv. Aktiviteter,
miljùkrav og forvaltningsbehov.
Kaplan, R., Kaplan, S., 1989. The Experience of Nature. A
Psychological Perspective. Cambridge University Press, Cam-
bridge.
Ku
Èller, R., 1972. A semantic model for describing perceived
environment. Doktorsavhandling. Lund universitet, Tekniska
fakulteten. Statens institut fo
Èr byggforskning.
Laursen, S.E., 1991. Legeplassen Forlaget Bùrn & Unge, 2. opplag.
Kùbenhavn.
Lindgren, C.A., 1990. Upplevda skilnader mellan skogsbesta
Ênd.
Stad & Land 87.
Lynch, K., 1960. The Image of the City. MIT Press, Cambridge.
Matthew, M.H., 1992. Making Sense of Place. Children's Under-
standing of Large-Scale Environments. Harvester Wheatsheaf,
Barnes & Noble Books, Hermel Hempstead.
McGarigal, K., Marks, B., 1998. FRAGSTATS: Spatial Pattern
Analysis Program for Quantifying Landscape Structure.
Manual.
Moore, G.T., 1985. State of the art in play environments. In: Frost,
J.L., Sunderlin, S. (Eds.), When Children Play. Proceedings of
the International Conference on Play and Play Environments.
Association for Childhood Education International.
Moore, R.C., 1986. Childhood's Domain: Play and Space in Child
Development. Croom Helm, London.
Moore, R., Young, D., 1978. Childhood outdoors: toward a social
ecology of the landscape. In: Altman, I., Wohlwill, J.F. (Eds.),
Children and the Environment. Plenum Press, New York and
London.
Moore, R., Wong, H.H., 1997. Natural Learning. Creating
Environments for Rediscovering Nature's Way of Learning.
The Life History of an Environmental Schoolyard. MIG
Communications. Berkley, CA.
Naylor, H., 1985. Design of outdoor play: an observational study. In:
Frost, J.L., Sunderlin, S. (Eds.), When Children Play. Proceedings
of the International Conference on Play and Play Environments.
Association for Childhood Education International.
Nicholson, S., 1971. How NOT to cheat children. The theory of
loose parts. Landscape Architecture 62, 30±34.
Paine, D., Luba, T., 1980. Principles of Aerial Photo Interpretation,
Oregon State University, OR. http://www.cof.orst.edu/cof/
teach/for220/slides/.
Prescott, E., 1987. The physical environment and cognitive
development in child-care centres. In: Weinstein, C.S., David,
T.G. (Eds.), Spaces for Children. Plenum Press, New York.
Rivkin, M.S., 1995. The Great Outdoors. Restoring Children's
Rights to Play Outside. National Association for the Education
of Young Children, Washington, DC.
Robson, C., 1997. Real World Research. A Resource for Social
Scientists and Practitioner-Researchers. Blackwell, Oxford and
Cambridge.
Simensen, G., 1998. Stalittma
Êlinger. In: Skogseth, T. (Ed.),
Grunnleggende landma
Êling. Universitetsforlaget.
Ska
Ênes, H.M., 1997. Towards an integrated ecological±geographi-
cal landscape perspective. A review of principal concepts and
methods. Norwegian J. Geogr. 51(3). Scandinavian University
Press.
Sorte, G., 1989. Perception of public green spaces Ð a park for
Homo urbaniensis. Department for Landscape Planning,
Swedish University of Agriculture Sciences, Alnarp.
Steel, C., Neuman, M., 1985. Infant's play on outdoor equipment. In:
Frost, J.L., Sunderlin, S. (Eds.), When Children Play. Proceedings
of the International Conference on Play and Play Environments.
Association for Childhood Education International.
SURFER User's Guide. Golden Software, Inc.
96 I. Fjùrtoft, J. Sageie / Landscape and Urban Planning 48 (2000) 83±97
Titman, W., 1994. Special Places, Special People. The Hidden
Curriculum of School Grounds. WWF UK (World Wide Fund
For Nature)/Learning Through Landscapes.
Tomlin, C.D., 1990. Geographic Information Systems and Carto-
graphic modeling.
Wangen, H., 1998. Hùyder. Ma
Êling og beregning. In: Skogseth, T.
(Ed.), Grunnleggende landma
Êling. Universitetsforlaget.
Weibel, R., Heller, M., 1993. Digital terrain modelling. In: Maguire,
D.J., Goodchild, M.F., Rhind, D.W. (Eds.), Geographical
Information Systems. Longman Scienti®c & Technical, London.
Winter, S.M., 1985. Toddler play behaviours and equipment
choices in an outdoor playground. In: Frost, J.L., Sunderlin,
S. (Eds.), When Children Play. Proceedings of the International
Conference on Play and Play Environments. Association for
Childhood Education International.
Worboys, M.F., 1995. GIS: a Computing Perspective. Taylor &
Francis, London.
Ingunn Fjùrtoft is Assistant Professor, Department of Teacher
Education, Telemark College, Norway; Cand. real (M.Sc.) in
Biology, University of Trondheim, Norway; Cand. mag. (B.A.) in
Sport Sciences and Physical Education, Norwegian College of
Sports, Oslo; Ph.D. student at the Norwegian College of Sports,
Oslo; areas of professional interest: early childhood, Children and
environment, playgrounds and natural playscapes.
Jostein Sageie is Chief Engineer, Institute of Informatics
and Mathematical Studies, Telemark College, Norway; Senior
Consultant, Geographical Information Systems; Project leader `Co-
operation Land Reform Project' in Lithuania; GIS/Mapping
Consultant for Ministry of Planning and International Co-operation
(MOPIC) Palestine; GIS/GPS Consultant for `Norwegian
Archadian Survey Project' in Greece; areas of professional
interest: use and implementation of Geographical Information
Systems.
I. Fjùrtoft, J. Sageie / Landscape and Urban Planning 48 (2000) 83±97 97
... In the past few decades, there has been increasing research interest in understanding how children's outdoor environments affect their everyday experiences (e.g. Fjørtoft and Sageie 2000;Chatterjee 2005; Aziz and Said 2016; Loebach and Gilliland 2019). The properties and attributes of the outdoor environment were recognized as the values that set the stage for children's learning, health, well-being, risk-taking, attitudes and behaviour. ...
... Concomitantly, children actualize more affordances when their parents more easily granted permission to explore the neighbourhood (Chaudhury et al. 2019). They tend to affiliate with the outdoor environment for a long time when playing in nature (Fjørtoft and Sageie 2000;Zamani 2017). Said (2012) and Hussein (2015) developed a more in-depth understanding, arguing that natural attributes trigger children's sensory affordances, affecting their mood and health. ...
... There is a paucity of research exploring affordances in the natural environment and the dynamics of nature within a particular time frame, such as the seasonal tropical fruits season (Said 2012) and seasonal weather patterns (Fjørtoft and Sageie 2000;Samborski 2010). Moreover, self-initiated (Loebach and Gilliland 2019), self-directed (Kyttä et al. 2018), unstructured play (Koller and Farley 2019), and direct and spontaneous contact with nature (Aziz and Said 2016), have decreased over the years. ...
Article
In children's geographies of the rural milieu, little is known about the geography of the outdoor environment in coastal areas, especially in rural fishing villages. This study explores affordance levels of the outdoor environment in a rural coastal area, with specific reference to young children's play behaviour. This study used an ethnographic approach together with direct participant observations, auto photographs, go-along interviews, and focus group interviews, to elicit the numbers of affordances experienced by 45 respondents, aged 3-13, in Bum Bum Island, Sabah, Malaysia. Hot Spot (Getis-Ord Gi*) spatial statistical and content analyses revealed that perceived affordances are the highest levels of affordances accumulated, followed by utilized and shaped affordances. The most frequent hot spots for the perceived, utilized and shaped affordances occurred in the transitional zone compared to the land and sea zones. This zone was the nucleus for actions of daily play-based experiences for the children in the coastal community based on the natural phenomena of the tides.
... People of varying ages have different reasons for visiting parks during their leisure time, reasons often based on their background (Kaźmierczak, 2013;Peters, Elands, & Buijs, 2010), needs (Chen, 2017;Fjørtoft & Sageie, 2000), and the people with whom they go to parks (Staats & Hartig, 2004). To better understand the varying motives for people's use of parks, several studies have focused on the people and their preferences (Heft, 2010;Kaplan, Kaplan, & Brown, 1989) using Kaplan's (1987) model of environmental preferences (Hadavi, Kaplan, & Hunter, 2015;Kaplan et al., 1989;Kaplan, 1979;Müderrisoglu & Gültekin, 2015). ...
... Wastelands or vacant areas, stoops, streets and yards can serve as common activity settings, although are not necessarily preferred (Min & Lee, 2006). Nicholson (1971) suggests that young park users love to interact with elements, such as shapes, materials, and smells with which they can explore, discover and experiment (cited from Fjørtoft & Sageie, 2000). ...
... Environments with natural features that fulfil the needs of youth include open grass fields, sloping hills and playgrounds with plants that can afford motor development (Chawla, 2015;Fjørtoft & Sageie, 2000;Han & Kim, 2018;Islam, Moore, & Cosco, 2016;Kyttä et al., 2018). Some of these areas are designated for youth to play in, while others are not specifically designed for youth but are areas in which they see possible activities or affordances (Gibson, 1979) for play. ...
Article
People of varying ages have different reasons for visiting parks during their leisure time, and while previous studies often focused on youth activities during their play within parks, less attention has been given to which park settings are preferred by the youth themselves for socialising, and with whom they want to socialise. This study explored youth social engagement within neighbourhood parks as behaviour settings, considering the environments as theorised entities that embody predetermined uses by suggesting and or enabling specific activities. Data was collected using an online visual data collection method called a photo-choice tool with a total of 192 youth, aged 9–17 from the Moreton Bay Region of Queensland, Australia, who completed the tool while visiting a neighbourhood park. Using theoretical saturation during the analysis, the data was grouped into three main themes; environmental attributes (park settings), social attributes (people with whom youth preferred to go), and behavioural attributes (motives for the preferred activities). Findings show that the presence of others, including friends, siblings and parents, motivate youth in both age categories (9–12 years and 13–17 years) to visit parks, and this presence of people also affects their preferences for park activities. The results of this research demonstrate that accompanying friends is one of the most important reasons youth visit a park, and that social activities and social interactions among youth are not only affected by the affordances of the park settings, but also by the social affordances that youth present to each other.
... Urban forests have increased dramatically recently in China and received a great deal of attention [1][2][3]. Urban forests offer a wide range of benefits, such as the regulation of air, water, soil, and climate [4], reducing mental stress and depression [5,6], promoting psychological restoration [7,8], enhancing mood and self-esteem [9], and even fostering creativity [10]. Although a growing body of research has highlighted the socio-ecological advantages of urban forests, limited knowledge exists regarding how people perceive various design interventions for urban forests. ...
Article
Full-text available
Individuals’ preferences for urban forest scenes are an essential factor in the design process. This study explores the connection between landscape design intensity, visual preferences, and eye fixations in urban forest scenes. Five pictures representing different urban forest scenes (plaza, lawn, garden path, pond, and rockery) were selected as stimuli, representing the original landscape design intensity. Three additional levels of design intensity (low, moderate, and high) were created by modifying the landscape elements of the original picture. A group of 50 participants was randomly assigned to observe the four levels of design intensity pictures within each type of landscape using eye-tracking technology. They also rated their preferences for each scene. In total, 250 participants took part in the study, with five groups observing five types of urban forest scenes. The results indicate that landscape design intensity has a positive impact on visual preferences, with moderate design intensity showing the strongest effect. However, the influence of design intensity and preferences also depends on the specific landscape scene. The fixation data did not show a significant relationship with design intensity but were associated with the type of landscape scene. In conclusion, this study suggests that moderate design intensity is recommended for urban forest design. However, it also highlights the importance of considering the specific landscape scene type. The research provides valuable insights into urban forest design and contributes to the understanding of eye-tracking technology in landscape perception studies.
... Structured games are often reflected in the teaching process, which is more prominent in the teaching form (Singh, 2021). Children's cognitive, social, and motor development is affected differently by play in various situations (Fjørtoft and Sageie, 2000;Ashley, 2011). According to Bartlett (1999), play is influenced by the local environment, including how children interact with one another in that context. ...
Article
Full-text available
Children’s access to outdoor activities is restricted due to the continuous spread of urbanisation in public spaces. Therefore, figuring out how to improve outdoor physical space within the existing environment is becoming increasingly important. This study conducted a systematic review of 55 studies using PRISMA method to examine the link between the physical settings in neighbourhoods and children’s outdoor play. Through Scopus databases to provide a complete list of journal articles based on stringent inclusion and exclusion standards. Identify the gaps by categorising the significant themes and aspects influencing children’s play area requirements. Studies show that the physical environment of a neighbourhood is affected by several social factors besides the needs and preferences of children’s games. It was also found that the most commonly used research methods in children’s play spaces were survey and interview, and the Affordance Theory was widely used as a research framework. This research provides new design possibilities for children’s play areas in residential neighbourhoods. These guidelines emphasise games with varied spatial qualities, the integration of spatial functions, the additional support requirements of parents for children’s play, and the flexibility of children to utilise outside space. Consequently, this analysis identifies knowledge gaps and suggests research directions for future studies.
... The feeling of choice when taking risks offers pleasure and promotes social inclusion through the development of relatedness (Gull et al., 2018;Siegel & Fryer, 2017). Furthermore, PA in a natural setting offers satisfaction, fosters euphoria and harmony in the human body, and develops creativity as well as communicative and collaborative skills (Crain, 1997;Fjørtoft, & Sageie, 2000;Moore & Wong, 1997). Compared to children in the classroom or traditional schoolyards, those involved in outdoor activities are less anxious, more cooperative, more creative, more cheerful, and more focused on their tasks (Dennis et al., 2014). ...
Article
Full-text available
Physical activity levels are alarmingly low and new strategies must be adopted to prevent this trend. This study focuses on examining the impact of an outdoor adventure education programme on adolescents. The participants were 75 adolescent students aged 12–14. The programme lasted two school years. Students participated in extracurricular schoolyard outdoor activities and outdoor adventure activities conducted during the two one-day and one four-day trips in both school years. A qualitative methodology was used with data collected using semi-structured interviews. At the end of each school year, 12 students were interviewed. Data analysis revealed that the programme promoted basic psychological needs. More specifically, adolescents reported that schoolyard activities and outdoor adventure activities promoted their satisfaction with autonomy, competence and relatedness. Interestingly, the programme gave girls the autonomy to choose activities that promoted their competence and interaction with peers in a better way than a typical PE lesson. Modifying existing curricula with innovative practices or planning new ones could improve the general quality of life.
... In natural setting, studies conducted through children's observations, play behavior has been linked to more cooperative and creative social play (Chawla 2015), as well as to more diverse type of play behavior (Luchs and Fikus 2013;Söderström et al. 2004). Studies conducted observing children in different school settings, or before and after green setting were introduced to schools, indicate a more imaginative, constructive, and sensory play, with children engaging in more socially cooperative play behaviors in natural areas than with asphalt or built play equipment (Blizard and Schuster 2004;Cloward Drown and Christensen 2014;Cosco 2007;Fjørtoft and Sagaie 2000;Grahn et al. 1997;Herrington and Studtmann 1998;Kirkby 1989;Kuh, Ponte, and Chau 2013;Moore and Wong 1997;Samborski 2010;Stanley 2011). ...
Article
Studies regarding the experience of children in schools’ outdoor environments report several benefits of nature on children’s cognitive, emotional, and physical development; thus, there is a need to systematically assess the characteristics of children’s place experience considering their own activities and social interactions. The present study investigated the perceptions of the physical environment and children-environment interactions during recess-time in natural (vs. built) schoolyards. In a between-subjects quasi-experiment in two Italian Primary schools, we collected and analyzed 298 children’s drawings and written descriptions regarding the activities they engage during recess time, developing a detailed coding system. Results showed how during the green break (vs. grey) children significantly engaged in more exploratory behaviors, rough and tumble behaviors, and active conversation, while in the grey break their play was more structured and left less space for free activities. In drawings, the natural environment (vs. built) is pictured as significantly more green, open, and rich in biodiversity and details, compared to the built environment that is depicted as grey, closed, and more crowded. Implications are discussed in both theoretical and practical terms on how natural schoolyards can afford different place experiences and play and non-play behaviors in children, contributing to improve their well-being during school-time.
... Some others have summarized the landscape and space elements suitable for elderly-oriented sports environment in urban parks, and analyzed the influence of these elements in terms of different use purposes through clustering analysis [11] . Among studies on environmental characteristics conducive to the growth of children [12] [13] and adolescents [14] , researchers have explored their special needs and preferences of activity types, and targeted environmental design indicators, as well as the correlations between these indicators. By now, researchers have developed mature evaluation tools and theoretical systems, such as QUality INdex of Parks for Youth (QUINPY) [15] and the Affordance Theory applied to study children's perception of environment [16] [17] . ...
Article
Full-text available
GRAPHICAL ABSTRACT HIGHLIGHTS · Establishes an influencing factor model of users' perception of the inclusiveness of urban parks ·Clarifies the path and influencing factors to users' evaluation on the inclusiveness of urban parks ·Clarifies the difference of influence degree of each influencing factor on different groups ·Improves the connotation of inclusive design of urban parks based on the concept of design exclusion RECEIVED DATE
Article
Nature has been a plentiful source of materials, replenishment, inspiration, and creativity. Nature collage, as a crafting technique, is a fun and educational activity for children to explore nature and engage their creativity. However, the raw material collection is limited to static things such as leaves, ignoring inspiration from nature sounds and dynamic elements such as babbling creeks. Using a mobile application, we hope to encourage children's creativity by renewing collage materials collection and careful observation in nature. To explore this, we conducted a formative study with children (N=20) and a design workshop with experts (N=6) to formulate NaCanva, an AI-assisted multi-modal collage creation system for children. Drawing on the interactivity between children and nature, NaCanva enables the multi-modal material collection, including images, sound, and videos, which differs our system from traditional collages. We validated this system with a between-subject user study (N=30), and the results suggested that NaCanva unleashes children's creativity in nature collage creation by enhancing children's multidimensional observation and engagement in nature.
Article
Çocukların oyunlarında risk almaları ve oyun alanlarında riskli ekipmanların bulunmasının gelişimsel faydaları giderek daha fazla araştırmaya konu olmaktadır. Özellikle şehirleşmeyle birlikte çocukların oyun tercihleri de değişiklik göstermektedir. Bu araştırma Türkiye'de bulunan bir büyükşehir belediyesine ait açık hava oyun parklarının riskli oyun olanaklarının gözlemler ve görüşmelerle derinlemesine incelenmesini amaçlamaktadır. Araştırma kapsamında açık hava oyun parklarının fiziksel özellikleri ve çocukların bu parklardaki oyun davranışları gözlemlenmiş; ayrıca yerel yönetimlerden bir yetkili ve ailelerle yapılan görüşmelerle riskli oyunların potansiyel ve uygulamadaki destekleyicileri derinlemesine ortaya koyulmuştur. Araştırma sonunda, oyun park alanlarında yüksek hız ve büyük yükseklik kategorilerindeki unsurların sıklıkla gözlemlendiği fakat riskli oyunlarda kullanılabilecek tehlikeli unsur ve aletlerin neredeyse hiç gözlemlenemediği ortaya çıkmıştır. Oyun park alanlarında gözlemlenen ekipmanlara paralel olarak çocukların riskli oyunlarında da en sık yüksek hızda koşma ve yüksekliklerde sallanma/atlama gibi davranışların ortaya çıktığı bulunmuştur. Aileler ve yerel yetkili ile yapılan görüşmeler sonucunda; ekipmanlara karar verme süreçlerinin ve ailelerin riskli oyunlara yönelik görüşlerinin çocukların riskli oyunlarında rolü olduğunu söylemek mümkündür.
Article
Urban children prefer playgrounds near home spaces due to physical and parental limitations on outdoor play. To increase the play value and use of playgrounds, it is essential to take a comprehensive design approach that considers the playground’s design and surrounding environment. Using the play value calculation tool designed by Woolley and Lowe, and making site location analyses, this study aims to evaluate the relationship between physical and environmental characteristics of playgrounds with land use decisions through an analysis of the numbers of users and play value of playgrounds in neighbourhood parks in Balçova, İzmir (Turkey).
Chapter
A laboratory experiment is conducted to investigate how two individual cognitive style factors, field dependence and need-for-cognition, relate to decision-making performance for a spatial task. The intent of the investigation is to establish a methodology for measuring cognitive fit for spatial tasks. The experiment assesses the performance of 142 subjects on a site location task where the problem complexity and availability of a geographic information system are manipulated on two levels. Significant relationships are found for both field dependence and need-for-cognition with the two dependent performance variables, solution time and percent error.
Article
Digital terrain models (DTMs) are a major constituent of geographical information processing. DTMs help to model, analyse and display phenomena related to topography or similar surfaces. This chapter is an attempt at a comprehensive review of relevant techniques and applications of DTMs. The individual elements of digital terrain modelling - techniques for the generation, manipulation, interpretation (analysis) and visualization of DTMs - are outlined. Application domains that use DTMs and their specific functional requirements are identified. Due to the complexity of the topic this chapter should be considered primarily as an annotated guide to the rich literature on digital terrain modelling. -Authors
Chapter
In the urban environment, the creation of childhood places cannot be left to chance or the vagaries of pressure groups; they must be deliberately fostered by planning, design, and management to satisfy basic human needs. Our purpose therefore is to present existing empirical findings, within a behavior-environment ecological framework, to support more rational decision-making.