ArticlePDF Available

Cyber-Flirting: An Examination of Men's and Women's Flirting Behaviour Both Offline and on the Internet

Authors:

Abstract

While flirting is a relatively underresearched area within psychology, even less is known about how people cyber-flirt. This paper explores how often individuals flirt offline compared to online. Moreover, it attempts to examine how men and women flirt within these different spaces. Five thousand, six hundred and ninety-seven individuals, of which 3554 (62%) were women and 2143 (38%) were men, completed a survey about their flirting behaviour both in face-to-face interactions and in chatrooms. The first hypothesis, which stated that the body would be used to flirt with as frequently online as offline, was partly supported. However, it was found that individuals downplayed the importance of physical attractiveness online. Women flirted by displaying nonverbal signals (offline) or substitutes for nonverbal cues (online), to a greater extent than men. In chatrooms men were more likely than women to initiate contact. It is concluded that cyber-flirting is more than simply a meeting of minds and that future research needs to consider the role of the body in online interactions.
Cyber-flirting
1
Whitty, M.T. (2004). Cyber-flirting: An examination of men's and
women's flirting behaviour both offline and on the Internet. Behaviour
Change, 21(2), 115-126.
Running head: Cyber-Flirting
Cyber-flirting: An Examination of Men’s and Women’s Flirting Behaviour both Offline
and on the Internet
Dr. Monica T. Whitty
Cyber-flirting
2
ABSTRACT
While flirting is a relatively under-researched area within psychology, we know even less
about how people cyber-flirt. This paper explores how often individuals flirt offline
compared to online. Moreover, it attempts to examine how men and women flirt within
these different spaces. Five thousand, six hundred and ninety seven individuals, of which
3554 (62%) were women and 2143 (38%) were men, completed a survey about their
flirting behaviour both in face-to-face interactions and in Chat Rooms. The first
hypothesis, which stated that the body would be used to flirt with as frequently online as
offline, was partly supported. However, it was found that individuals downplayed the
importance of physical attractiveness online. Women flirted by displaying non-verbal
signals (offline) or substitutes for non-verbal cues (online), to a greater extent than men.
In Chat Rooms men were more likely than women to initiate contact. It was concluded
that cyber-flirting is more than simply a meeting of minds and that future research needs
to consider the role of the body in online interactions.
KEYWORDS: Cyber-flirting, flirting, Internet relationships, sex differences, attraction
Cyber-flirting
3
Cyber-flirting: An Examination of Men’s and Women’s Flirting Behaviour both Offline
and on the Internet
The distinctiveness of the flirt lies in the fact that she awakens delight and desire
by means of unique antithesis and synthesis: through the alternation or
simultaneity of accommodation and denial…
-- [Simmel, 1984, p. 134]
Given the burgeoning number of reports of relationships that initiate online (e.g.,
McCown, Fischer, Page, & Homant, 2001; Parks & Roberts, 1998; Whitty & Gavin,
2001) there is now an additional pressing need to understand exactly how these
relationships begin. Some research has considered how romantic relationships progress
online. For example, Whitty and Gavin (2001) contended that relationships often do not
remain online but tend to progress from chat to email to phone to face-to-face. McKenna,
Green and Gleason (2002) concluded from their research that the absence of gating
features and the ability to self-disclose more in an anonymous environment were
important reasons why relationships on the Internet develop quickly and are often close
and intimate. However, while their research does in part support their claims, these
researchers did not explore in detail the types of conversations that take place online. For
instance, were some of these conversations about appearance? Whitty has argued that
while physical bodies are not present online, the body still matters (Whitty, 2003, Whitty
& Carr, 2003). For example, one can still flirt online by describing what one’s body
looks, feels, and smells like.
Cyber-flirting
4
Given the strong emphasis by theorists, such as McKenna et al. (2002), on the
absence of the body online and the arguments made for attraction on the Internet to be as
a result of a meeting of minds, one aim of this study was to examine whether the role of
the body has been overlooked. Moreover, this study aimed to explore whether men and
women flirt online in traditionally defined ways. While it is recognised here that there is a
great range of flirting behaviours that social scientists ought to consider, this study
limited its focus to the following flirtatious behaviours: non-verbal behaviours, such as
smiling, eye gaze and touch, substitutes for non-verbal behaviours, such as emoticons
(smiles, winks), acronyms (LOL – laugh out loud), descriptions of physical
attractiveness, descriptions of socio-economic status, and initiating contact.
Traditional flirting versus cyber-flirting
Previously, it has been argued that discussions around the absence of the body on
the Internet are counter-productive, and instead social scientists need to shift their focus
to how the body is portrayed online (Whitty, 2003; Whitty & Carr, 2003). Givens (1978)
believed that face-to-face flirting behaviour mostly consists of non-verbal signals,
especially in the early stages of relationships. Unlike the spoken word, body language can
signal attraction without being too obvious. This ambiguity protects people from any
humiliation if the person to whom they are signalling attraction does not share these
sentiments. Some basic codes that are important to consider in flirting include kinetics,
oculesics, physical appearance, olfactics, vocalics, proxemics and haptics (defined
below). Whilst individuals might be skilled at displaying these flirting signals in face-to-
face encounters, the question is how are these traditional offline cues replicated online (if
Cyber-flirting
5
at all)? This paper now turns to consider these basic flirting codes in greater detail and
speculates on what the online equivalents might be.
Traditional displays of kinetic gestures include hair tossing, licking lips, mirroring
behaviour, smiling, laughing or giggling. Oculesics, or eye movements, include pupil
dilation, demure glances, short darting glances, eyebrow flashes often accompanied by a
smile and eye contact. Olfactic forms of flirting are exemplified by the wearing of
perfume and after-shave. Flirtatious speech is typically more animated speech with
moderate amounts of laughter, fewer silences and pauses and increased warmth and
interest. Proxemics is the amount of personal distance kept between individuals.
Individuals who lean toward one another and who are at the same body angle are
perceived as being more seductive than those individuals who lean away from each other.
Haptics, or the use of touch, is a common form of communication, particularly in
flirtatious behaviour. Another sign of seductive behaviour is the use of unnecessary
clothing adjustment.
For flirting to occur in cyberspace the body needs to be represented through text
(Whitty, 2003, Whitty & Carr, 2003). For example, rather than making an effort to look
good, as one would traditionally do for an offline date, individuals can create a first
impression by describing via the text how attractive they appear. However, unlike
traditional interactions, this is not restricted to the appearance of one’s actual body. As
argued in previous papers, cyberspace allows one, through text, to create new attractive
bodies (Whitty, 2003, Whitty & Carr, 2003). Indeed, we can devise an entirely new
attractive being, one that has a good job, earns huge sums of money, and is well educated.
Even if photos are exchanged online, these are not necessary photos of the actual
Cyber-flirting
6
individual, or alternatively, people are able to carefully select images of themselves that
put themselves in the best light (possibly by selecting pictures of when they were much
younger).
Demure glances and eyebrow flashes are not as easily replicated online. However,
the are some alternatives to these non-verbal gestures. For example, emoticons, which are
drawings made from grammatical symbols, might be a useful alternative. We can use
facial expressions such as smiley faces, winks and kisses as a substitute for body
language. Moreover, rather than use audible laughing and giggling, individuals can use
acronyms, such as LOL (laugh out loud or lots of laughs). Seductive ‘screen names’ are
another device people can add to their repertoire of online flirting behaviours. Although
one cannot physically touch online, one can describe touching another individual, what
that touch feels like and in turn how they visualise being touched by the person they are
interacting with (see Whitty, 2003, Whitty & Carr, 2003 for a more detailed discussion of
cyber-flirting).
Some offline flirting behaviours are not so easy to translate online. For example,
it is difficult to find substitutes for olfactics, vocalics and proxemics. Online participants
do not know what the person they are chatting with smells like, nor are they allured by
their sexy deep voice. The subtleties of voice, such as pitch and tone, are not evident
online. Pauses in conversation might be attributed to a poor Internet connection, or bad
typing skills, as opposed to a lack of interest. Individuals also cannot indicate attraction
online by leaning closer to another or by mirroring their body movements.
Cyber-flirting
7
Gender differences
In addition to considering how the body might be represented in flirtatious
behaviour online, this study examined whether similar gender differences characteristic
of offline flirting were evident in online flirting. Evolutionary psychologists contend that
in considering courting behaviours we need to examine the resources that men and
women contribute to mating and potential offspring (e.g., Buss, 1988; Trost & Alberts,
1998). They suggest that women contribute their physical bodies, which should indicate
good health, youthfulness and fertility. In contrast, some of the important defining
characteristics for men include physical dominance, and an ability to produce resources
(e.g., social status, ambition, and high income). Indeed research has found evidence to
support these assumptions. Men, more than women, rate physical attractiveness to be an
important quality in a partner (Kenrick, Sadalla, Groth, & Trost, 1990; Travis, 1977).
Women, more than men, rate traits reflecting dominance and social status as an important
characteristic in selecting a potential partner (Kenrick, Groth, Trost, & Sadalla, 1993;
Kenrick et al., 1990).
If men and women place more importance on some attractive qualities than
others, we would, in turn, expect men and women to flirt in different ways in order to
accentuate these characteristics. Moore (1985) contends that women are not passive in
the courting process, but rather, in the main, control much of the flirting process. Her
research has observed women in places, such as singles bars. She identified 52 different
non-verbal displays by women, which she argued were courtship signals that served to
attract and elicit the approach of men. These included gestures such as facial and head
Cyber-flirting
8
patterns, smiling, laughing, touch, leaning and primping. In addition to Moore’s (1985)
research, others have argued that women posses a larger repertoire of flirtation strategies
used to signal interest in men (e.g., Muehlenhard, Koralewski, Andrews, & Burdick,
1986). Hall (1984), for instance, has purported that women gate at interaction partners
more, and use touch and body movements more in interpersonal interactions. McCormick
and Jones (1989) observed 70 couples and found that women were more active
participants in flirtation and were often the initiators of the flirtation. Trost and Alberts
(1998) reported that, in contrast to women, men were more likely to flirt by signalling
status and dominance, which is often achieved by flashing money, exaggerating their
income, wearing expensive clothes, bragging about their superior intelligence, and
exaggerating their level of sexual popularity.
These gender differences are not simply confined to face-to-face encounters.
Research conducted in the 1970s found that women in personal ads were more likely to
offer attractiveness and seek financial security, while men were more likely to offer
financial security and seek attractiveness (Harrison & Saeed, 1977). Smith, Waldorf and
Trembath (1990) analysed personal ads from six issues of ‘On the Scene’ magazine from
January 1989 to June 1989. They were interested in what attractive qualities individuals
were seeking in a partner. Not surprisingly, these researchers found that physical
attractiveness was the highest ranking quality desired by men and in fact appeared more
than twice as often in men’s ads than it did in women’s ads. Women, in contrast, were
more likely to hope for a man who is understanding, emotionally healthy and is
financially stable. Koestner and Wheeler (1988) examined what attractive features men
and women were more likely to emphasise about themselves in lonely hearts columns.
Cyber-flirting
9
Again, these researchers found that men were more likely than women were to emphasise
their own educational and occupational status.
Transcending gender roles on the Internet?
To date, little is known about gender differences in courting behaviour and
attraction online. Some theorists have suggested that cyberspace is a place where people
are liberated to be whoever they want to be (e.g., Turkle, 1995). Applying Turkle’s
understandings of cyberspace, this is possibly a place where individuals can transcend
gender roles when it comes to flirtatious behaviour. In contrast to Turkle’s view, some
research suggests that the trends that exist in offline relationships are replicated online.
For example, it has been found that in Chat Rooms men, more than women, lie or
exaggerate details about their education, occupation and income (Whitty, 2002). Scharlott
and Christ’s (1995) work also sheds some light on gender differences and attraction in an
online dating site. These researchers surveyed people in 1990 using an Internet dating site
called ‘Matchmaker’. At the time, photos and videos were not a feature of the service,
however, participants were able to rate their attractiveness on a scale from very good
looks to below average. These individuals also included other details about themselves
including income. Scharlott and Christ (1995) found that “many heterosexual users of
Matchmaker largely conform to the gender-specific roles traditionally ascribed to
‘available’ men and women” (p. 201). In their study men were more likely to initiate
contact and women were more likely to take on a more passive role. Moreover, the
women who rated their own appearance as above average or very good received more
Cyber-flirting
10
messages from men than the women who rated themselves as average. Interesting, the
same result did not occur for men.
For the purposes of this study, chatrooms were chosen, as text was the primary
mode of communication in these spaces at the time of the study; unlike, for example,
online dating sites which typically have photographs displayed. Drawing on the research
described in this paper, the following hypotheses were generated:
H1: That the body (or depictions of it) will be used to flirt with as frequently in Chat
Rooms as it is face-to-face situations.
H2: Women will flirt more frequently than men both in Chat Rooms and in face-to-
face situations by employing non-verbal signals (or substitutes online for these
signals), such as smiling and eye-gaze, laughing (or indicating laughter online)
around someone they are attracted to, using touch (or indicating the use of touch
online), and by emphasising physical attractiveness.
H3: Men will flirt more frequently than women both in Chat Rooms and in face-to-
face situations by describing their social economic status, and by initiating contact
with someone they find attractive.
METHOD
Participants
Participants who had used Chat Rooms were invited to participate in the study.
Five thousand, six hundred and ninety seven individuals completed the survey, of which
3554 (62%) were women and 2143 (38%) were men. Although a limitation of the survey,
in order to eliminate bias, only heterosexuals were invited to participate in the study.
Ages ranged from 18 to 70 years with a mean of 23 years (SD = 8.28) years. Seventy
Cyber-flirting
11
percent of the sample had a high school education, 14% had completed a diploma or a
certificate, 13% a degree, and 3% post-graduate qualifications. Fifty-five percent of the
sample were single, 29% had a boyfriend or girlfriend, 11% were married or cohabiting,
4% were separated or divorced, and 1% did not answer. Seventy four percent had formed
a romantic relationship with someone they had met offline, while 23% had formed a
romantic relationship with someone they had met online. Thirty five percent had met up
with someone face-to-face whom they had first encountered online. Fifty-nine percent of
the sample stated they flirted more frequently offline, 22% online, 13% the same, and 6%
did not flirt. The sample consisted of individuals from a range of countries, with 69%
from America and Canada completing the survey, 16% from Australia and New Zealand,
6% from the UK, 2% from Europe, 2% from Asia, 1% from South America, 0.5% from
the Middle East and 0.2% from Africa (the remainder of the participants did not state
their country of residence).
Materials
For this study a survey was constructed which focused on ways people flirt in
face-to-face situations and how they flirt in Chat Rooms. Although a plethora of flirting
behaviours could have been selected for this study, given that the aims were to examine
whether the body can be reconstructed online to flirt with and to examine whether men
and women flirt in traditionally defined ways, this study focussed on non-verbal
behaviours and substitutions of non-verbal behaviours online, descriptions of
attractiveness and socio-economic status and the initiation of contact with another.
Cyber-flirting
12
Past research on traditional flirting was considered in the construction of the
survey. In addition, substitute acts for flirting behaviour online were considered.
Participants were asked to rate on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = never, 7 = all the time) how
frequently they flirted by displaying particular behaviours to indicate attraction to
another. Participants were asked to consider flirtatious behaviour in face-to-face
situations and flirtatious behaviour in Chat Rooms. Some example of items included: “Do
you ever make an effort to look physically attractive so that you might attract
another/others?” and “Do you ever use emoticons, such as smiley faces :-) or a wink ;-) to
signal to someone online that you are attracted to them?” Participants were asked if they
flirted by describing income, and/or education, and/or occupation; making an effort to
look physically attractive; initiating conversation with someone they found attractive;
displaying non-verbal gestures, such as smiling or eye gaze; and laughing and touch.
Participants were asked if they flirted online by describing income, and/or education,
and/or occupation; describing oneself to appear physically attractive; initiating contact
with someone they met online; and by displaying non-verbal substitutes, such as
emoticons; and by using acronyms, such as LOL (laugh out loud, lots of laughs) with
someone they find attractive.
Procedure
For this study, it was deemed insufficient to exclusively recruit participants via a
web survey. Given that such a method might have solely attracted regular Internet users
and this study was interested in both offline and online flirting, it was decided to recruit
people both online and offline. Participants were recruited online by placing notices in a
Cyber-flirting
13
random selection of online newsgroups (these included religious, political, student and
relationships groups). With the increase in annoyance people are experiencing with spam,
it was elected to place notices, rather than target individuals randomly within these
groups as studies have done in the past (e.g., Parks & Floyd, 1996). In addition,
participants were targeted offline by inviting university students at the University of
Western Sydney, and by placing surveys in randomly selected cafes and libraries
throughout Sydney, Australia. Thirteen percent of the participants answered the survey
offline and 87% answered the survey online. To eliminate problems with multiple
submissions online IP addresses were logged. In both conditions participants were
ensured confidentiality and were able to withdraw consent up until the time of submitting
the survey. Participants were invited to complete the survey and place it in a provided
locked box. The survey ran continuously for 6 months.
RESULTS
An analysis was originally performed to determine if there was any significant
difference between participants who completed the survey online compared to those that
completed the survey offline as well as country of origin. Given there were no significant
differences this factor was not considered in the final analysis.
To test the first hypothesis paired t-tests were performed on the items that
assessed the extent to which the body was depicted in flirting behaviour. As shown in
Table 1, the first hypothesis was not supported for any of the items.
Table 1
Cyber-flirting
14
Paired t-tests for using the body to flirt with offline and online
Variable
Offline flirting
behaviours
M
(SD)
Online flirt
behaviours
M
(SD)
t
Appearing physically
attractive
4.90
(1.48)
2.28
(1.71)
96.22***
Non-verbal (e.g.,
smiles, emoticons)
4.59
(1.45)
3.50
(2.07)
37.06***
Laugh (or acronyms,
such as LOL)
4.57
(1.48)
3.45
(2.05)
37.80***
Touch (or descriptions
of touch)
3.76
(1.61)
2.21
(1.65)
55.76***
* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001
Given that the majority of the participants (59%) stated that they flirted more
frequently offline this it is perhaps not surprising. To explore this further, each of the
flirting behaviours were considered for whether participants claimed to flirt more
frequently offline, online, or both the same (note: those who claimed not to flirt were not
included in this analysis). A MANOVA was performed using the online and offline
flirting behaviours as the dependent variables, and place were individuals flirted more
frequently as the independent variable. Applying an α-level of 0.05, there was a
statistically significant difference on the combined dependent variables F(24, 10194) =
109.67, p < .001; Wilks’ Lambda = .63; partial eta squared = .21. The dependent
variables were then considered separately. Table 2 presents the Univariate F-tests, using
an α-level of .05. It is noteworthy to point out that according to Cohen (1988) these effect
sizes are mostly moderate.
Table 2
Frequency differences for online and offline flirting items.
Cyber-flirting
15
Variable
Flirt
more
offline
M
(SD)
Flirt
more
online
M
(SD)
Same
M
(SD)
F
η
2
Offline
Attractiveness
5.03
(1.41)
4.87
(1.46)
4.91
(1.51)
7.00**
.003
Offline Non-
Verbal
4.84
(1.32)
4.23
(1.40)
4.87
(1.43)
91.59***
.04
Offline Laugh
4.71
(1.38)
4.48
(1.46)
4.72
(1.50)
11.49***
.004
Offline Initiate
4.42
(1.27)
3.76
(1.30)
4.38
(1.31)
117.05***
.04
Offline Touch
4.02
(1.55)
3.28
(1.50)
4.03
(1.54)
106.19***
.04
Offline SES
2.05
(1.32)
2.13
(1.44)
2.14
(1.50)
2.55
.001
Online
Attractiveness
2.05
(1.57)
2.92
(1.91)
2.58
(1.82)
124.60***
.047
Online Non-
verbal
2.97
(1.95)
4.68
(1.76)
4.52
(1.88)
455.69***
.151
Online Laugh
2.97
(1.95)
4.54
(1.81)
4.36
(1.88)
375.56***
.128
Online Initiate
2.53
(1.72)
4.23
(1.62)
4.01
(1.80)
553.49***
.178
Online Touch
1.78
(1.35)
3.22
(1.85)
2.86
(1.80)
448.27***
.149
Online SES
1.97
(1.54)
2.71
(1.94)
2.42
(1.79)
98.28***
.037
* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001
Perhaps not surprisingly, Bonferroni post-hoc comparisons revealed that
individuals who flirted more frequently face-to-face were more likely than individuals
who flirted online, to flirt in face-to-face situations by emphasising attractiveness, using
non-verbal signals and laughter, initiating contact, and by using touch. Comparisons also
showed that individuals who flirted more frequently online emphasised attractiveness
more than individuals who flirted face-to-face or flirt as much online as they do offline.
Furthermore, individuals who flirted more frequently online, were more likely than
Cyber-flirting
16
individuals who flirted more frequently face-to-face to use substitutes for non-verbal
cues, initiate contact, use descriptions of touch, and emphasise socio-economic status.
Interestingly, an inspection of the means revealed, that emphasising the body
offline through attractiveness, non-verbally and through laughter appeared to be the more
favoured way to attract others for people who flirt more frequently offline. Nonetheless,
the body still appeared to play a significant part of cyber-flirting for people who flirt
more online, via non-verbal signals (such as emoticons) and by representing laughter.
However, the emphasis of physical attractiveness appears to be downplayed.
A MANOVA was performed to investigate, sex differences in flirting behaviour.
The dependent variables included items that measured online and offline flirtatious
behaviour. Applying an α-level of 0.05, there was a statistically significant difference on
the combined dependent variables for sex F(12, 5440) = 38.73, p < .001; Wilks’ Lambda
= .92; partial eta squared = .079. The dependent variables were then considered
separately. Table 3 presents the Univariate F-tests, an α-level of .05 was employed to
determine significance. The effect sizes are relatively small (Cohen, 1988).
Table 3
Sex differences for online and offline flirting items.
Variable
Men
Women
F
η
2
Offline
Attractiveness
4.62
(1.57)
5.08
(1.39)
125.21***
.022
Offline Non-
Verbal
4.50
(1.48)
4.65
(1.43)
14.90***
.003
Offline Laugh
4.34
(1.51)
4.72
(1.44)
85.54***
.015
Offline Initiate
4.20
(1.36)
4.18
(1.35)
0.349
.000
Offline Touch
3.51
(1.62)
3.93
(1.57)
88.83***
.016
Offline SES
2.11
2.02
5.37*
.001
Cyber-flirting
17
(1.41)
(1.32)
Online
Attractiveness
2.15
(1.67)
2.35
(1.73)
16.69***
.003
Online Non-
verbal
3.32
(2.02)
3.61
(2.09)
25.68***
.005
Online Laugh
3.25
(2.00)
3.57
(2.07)
32.41***
.006
Online Initiate
3.22
(1.86)
2.96
(1.87)
23.91***
.004
Online Touch
2.31
(1.67)
2.15
(1.64)
12.07**
.002
Online SES
2.17
(1.69)
2.17
(1.69)
.017
.000
* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001
Hypothesis 2 was partially supported by the results. Women did flirt more than
men in face-to-face situations by displaying non-verbal signals, with laughter, touch and
by making an effort with their appearance. Online, women flirted more than men using
substitutes for non-verbal cues (such as emoticons), using substitutes for laughter (such
as acronyms like LOL), and by describing themselves as physically attractive. However,
contrary to what was predicted, men scored higher on indicating touch in their flirting
online.
Hypothesis 3 was also partially supported. Men, more than women, flirted in face-
to-face situations by emphasising their socio-economic status. However, contrary to
expectations, men were not more likely to initiate contact offline. Online, however, men
were more likely than women to initiate contact but they were not as likely to emphasise
SES more than women.
DISCUSSION
Cyber-flirting
18
In spite of some researchers’ claims that the Internet is more of a meeting of
minds than of bodies, this study found some evidence that the body plays a significant
role in cyber-flirting in Chat Rooms. The data suggests that individuals are more likely to
use the body to flirt with offline. However, given that the majority of the sample stated
that they flirted more offline a second analysis was required. In this analysis people who
claimed to flirt more face-to-face were compared to people who claimed to flirt more
online and people who claimed to flirt online as much as they did offline. This
highlighted some significant differences. The results suggested that those who flirt more
online were able to translate the body through text. For example, kinetics (in the form of
acronyms, such as LOL), oculesics (in the form of emoticons, such as a wink),
speech/laughter and haptics (through descriptions in the text). Indeed, the representation
of non-verbal cues and laughter appeared to be a popular way for individuals to cyber-
flirt. This study goes some way in supporting previous theoretical arguments that the
body can be successfully translated online (Whitty, 2003, Whitty & Carr, 2003).
However, it is possibly the more Internet savvy that are better able to do this (future
research is required to test out this claim). Researchers have collectively argued that
using the body offline in isolation of verbal cues allows one to convey sexual interest
without the high risk of possible humiliation, shame or possible rejection (Feinberg,
1996; Koeppel, Montage, Miller, O’Hare and Cody, 1993, Moore, 1985). Although future
research needs to consider the role of verbal cues online in more detail, this study does
suggest that we cannot dismiss the importance of the body altogether when it comes to
initiating relationships in Chat Rooms.
Cyber-flirting
19
While this current study does provide some evidence that we must consider the
presence of the body more online, it must not be ignored that, in line with previous work,
such as McKenna et al.’s, 2002, who focused on presentation of self online, it was found
here that how one physically looks does not play as an important role online as it does
offline in the development of romantic relationships. Perhaps by overlooking how one
actually looks, individuals are able to maintain some anonymity. In turn, being
unidentifiable possibly creates more opportunities for individuals to feel free to open up
on the net (Whitty & Gavin, 2001).
In the main, men and women flirted in distinctively gender defined ways, both in
face-to-face encounters and in Chat Rooms. Although the small effect sizes suggest that
we need to treat these results with caution, as predicted, women were more likely than
men were to flirt online and offline by employing non-verbal signals, laughing and
emphasising physical attractiveness. The only item where this was not supported was
with descriptions of touch online, where men scored significantly higher than women did.
Perhaps this is because the use of touch in face-to-face situations is a more subtle
advance than the descriptions of touch online. Moreover, future research might
investigate what exactly these descriptions of touch are online (for example, are these
more explicitly sexual?).
Hypothesis 3 was only partly supported. Men were not more likely than women
were to initiate contact in face-to-face situations with women they were attracted to.
However, they were more likely than women were to emphasise SES. In contrast, online
men were more likely to make initial contact, but were not more likely to emphasise SES.
Perhaps men were more likely to make initial contact online, as the anonymity that the
Cyber-flirting
20
Internet provides allows men to feel less inhibited than they would in face-to-face
encounters.
The gender differences revealed in this research were interesting. Despite the
changes that the women’s movement has brought about and the increased likelihood that
women can fend for and support themselves and their children, when it comes to
initiating relationships women still pay more attention to their resources, such as their
physical bodies. This appears to be evident across all mediums, including face-to-face,
personal ads and as this study has found in Chat Rooms. Despite the opportunities, as
articulated so well by theorists such as Turkle (1995), that the Internet provides
individuals to play and experiment with identity, this study suggests that gender roles are
not easily transcended online. However, this study only partly supports an evolutionary
approach (see Buss, 1988), suggesting that other theories need to be developed to explain
these gender differences.
This study provides us with some important insights into how men and women
flirt online compared to face-to-face encounters. Nevertheless, there are some limitations
to this research that are necessary to point out, which future studies might want to
consider. One of these being that flirting is not necessarily an entirely conscious
behaviour. For example, people do not typically consciously dilate their pupils to indicate
sexual interest in others, nor do they always consciously flirt by displaying an eyebrow
flash, accompanied by a hair toss. This makes it difficult to measure flirtatious behaviour
with self-report questionnaires. Self reports on behaviour cannot guarantee that
individuals behave in the same way they report they do in surveys. Moore (1985)
attempted to overcome this limitation by using observational methods. However, her
Cyber-flirting
21
study also had its shortcomings, in that it is also difficult to ascertain for certain whether
the non-verbal signals she observed were truly flirtatious signals (conscious or
unconscious). Of course observing flirtatious behaviour online is fraught with difficulties.
Firstly, it is more difficult to determine if what is being said is flirtatious and secondly
there are a number of ethical issues to consider (see Whitty, 2004). Considering this in
another light, however, people could possibly be more aware of what they do to flirt
online as communication online is typically more thoughtful and strategic. Hence, self
reports might be the most appropriate tool to employ to measure cyber-flirting. Future
research might give further consideration to this question.
Another limitation of this study was the list of items chosen to consider flirtatious
behaviours. Future research, for instance, might consider a broader ranger of behaviours,
with more attention to verbal flirtation. Of course, it is also acknowledged here that more
research is required to confidently determine how the body is represented in online
flirting.
Rather than considering the Internet as one generic space, future studies might
continue to consider how men and women flirt in different spaces on the Internet. Spaces
such as MUDs and MOOs (multiple-user dungeon, or more commonly understood these
days to mean multi-user dimension or domains) which were originally a space where
interactive role-playing games could be played, very similar to Dungeons and Dragons,
might be considered further. In this space we find people experimenting with and playing
with multiple characters. We might predict that individuals playing in these spaces might
find it easier to flirt than other online places. We would also perhaps expect that online
dating sites, where the expectation is to find someone romantically to hook up with, are
Cyber-flirting
22
one of the more popular sites to flirt on in cyberspace. However, given the expectations
to put up photos of oneself, future research might find in these spaces online physical
appearance does play an important role. Spaces such as newsgroups (spaces on the
Internet devoted to the discussion of a specific topic) possibly create less opportunity for
individuals to cyber-flirt. The type of topic and whether people reveal who they really are
can alter the dynamics of such as space.
Conclusions
Overall, these results are important because they give a general overview of how
men and women flirt online compared to offline. Despite changes in our social
conditions, and the possibilities of experimenting with new roles online, both online and
offline, women tend to flirt more than men by emphasising physical attributes. This study
challenges the oft-touted claim by theorists that the Internet is a place where there is a
meeting of minds, in absence of the body (McRae, 1996). Instead, it is suggested here
that researchers need to focus more on how the body is reconstructed on the Internet. This
study suggests that in Chat Rooms physical appearance is downplayed, however, that the
body is nevertheless represented in emoticons and acronyms. In conclusion, these results
highlight that the empirical and theoretical examination of how we might conceptualise
flirting online is worthy of further investigation.
Cyber-flirting
23
REFERENCES
Buss, D. M. (1988). The evolution of human intrasexual competition: Tactics of
mate attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54 (4), 616-628.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Feinberg, L. S. (1996). Teasing: Innocent fun or sadistic malice? Far Hills, NJ,
US: New Horizon Press.
Givens, D. (1978). The nonverbal basis of attraction: Flirtation, courtship, and
seduction. Psychiatry, 41, 346-359.
Hall, J. A. (1984). Nonverbal sex differences: Communication accuracy and
expressive style. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Harrison, A. & Saeed, L. (1977). Let’s make a deal: An analysis of revelations
and stipulations in lonely hearts advertisements. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 38, 604-617.
Kenrick, D. T., Groth, G. E., Trost, M. R., & Sadalla, E. K. (1993). Integrating
evolutionary and social exchange perspectives on relationships: Effects of gender, self-
appraisal, and involvement level on mate selection criteria. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 64(6), 951-969.
Kenrick, D. T., Sadalla, E. K., Groth, G., & Trost, M. R. (1990). Evolution, traits,
and the stages of human courtship: Qualifying the parental investment model. Journal of
Personality, 58(1), 97-116.
Koeppel, L. B., Montagne Miller, Y., O'Hair, D., & Cody, M. J. (1993). Friendly?
Flirting? Wrong?, In P. J. Kalbfleisch (Ed). (1993). Interpersonal communication:
Cyber-flirting
24
Evolving interpersonal relationships. Communication. (pp. 19-45). Hillsdale, N. J.:
Erlbaum.
Koestner, R., & Wheeler, L. (1988). Self-presentation in personal advertisements:
The influence of implicit notions of attraction and role expectations. Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships, 5(2), 149-160.
McCormick, N. B., & Jones, A. J. (1989). Gender differences in nonverbal
flirtation. Journal of Sex Education and Therapy, 15(4), 271-282.
McCown, J. A., Fischer, D., Page, R., & Homant, M. (2001). Internet
relationships: People who meet people. CyberPsychology and Behavior, 4(5), 593-596.
McKenna, K. Y. A., Green, A. S., and Gleason, M. E. J. (2002). Relationship
formation on the Internet: What's the big attraction? Journal of Social Issues, 58, 659-
671.
McRae, S. (1996). Coming apart at the seams: Sex, text and the virtual body. In L.
Cherny & E. R. Weise (Eds.), Wired women: Gender and new realities in cyberspace (pp.
242-263). New York: Seal Press.
Moore, M. M. (1985). Nonverbal courtship patterns in women: Context and
consequences. Ethology and Sociobiology, 6(4), 237-247.
Muehlenhard, C. L., Koralewski, M. A., Andrews, S. L., & Burdick, C. A. (1986).
Verbal and nonverbal cues that convey interest in dating: Two studies. Behavior Therapy,
17(4), 404-419.
Parks, M. R., & Floyd, K. (1996). Making friends in cyberspace. Journal of
Communication, 46(1), 80-97.
Cyber-flirting
25
Parks, M. R., & Roberts, L. D. (1998). "Making MOOsic": The development of
Personal Relationships on-line and a comparison to their off-line counterparts. Journal of
Social and Personal Relationships, 15, 517-537.
Scharlott, B. W. & Christ, W. G. (1995). Overcoming relationship-initiation
barriers: The impact of a computer-dating system on sex role, shyness, and appearance
inhibitions. Computers in Human Behavior, 11 (2), 191-204.
Simmel, G. (1984). Georg Simmel: On women, sexuality, and love. Translated
and edited by Kurt H. Wolff. New York: The Free Press.
Smith, J. E., Waldorf, V. A. & Trembath, D. L. (1990). "Single white male
looking for thin, very attractive…" Sex Roles, 23 (11/12), 675-685.
Travis, C. (1977). Men and women report their views on masculinity. Psychology
Today, 10, 34-42.
Trost, M. R., & Alberts, J. K. (1998). An evolutionary view on understanding sex
effects in communicating attraction. In D. J. Canary (Ed.), Sex differences and
similarities in communication. Critical essays and empirical investigations of sex and
gender in interaction (pp. 233-255). Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc
Publishers..
Turkle, S. (1995). Life on the screen: Identity in the age of the Internet. London:
Weidenfeld & Nicolson.
Whitty, M. T. (2002). Liar, Liar! An examination of how open, supportive and
honest people are in Chat Rooms. Computers in Human Behavior, 18(4), 343-352.
Whitty, M. T. (2003). Cyber-flirting: Playing at love on the Internet. Theory and
Psychology, 13 (3), 339-357.
Cyber-flirting
26
Whitty, M.T. (2004). Peering into online bedroom windows: Considering the
ethical implications of investigating Internet relationships and sexuality. In E. Buchanan
(Ed.), Readings in Virtual Research Ethics: Issues and Controversies (pp. 203-218).
Hershey, USA: Idea Group Inc.
Whitty, M.T. & Carr, A.N. (2003). Cyberspace as potential space: Considering
the web as a playground to cyber-flirt. Human Relations, 56 (7), 861-891.
Whitty, M. T., & Gavin, J. K. (2001). Age/Sex/Location: Uncovering the social
cues in the development of online relationships. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 4(5), 623-
630.
... This allows one to save face when the feeling are unrequited and is marked by striving "not to define the situation, but rather to leave it open to possible, potential, futures" (Tavory 2009). Similarly, body language can signal attraction while providing ambiguity to protect one from humiliation if the other person does not share the feelings (Whitty 2004). People may use verbal ambiguity in sexual interactions through mundane conversation with sexual overtones, with the recipient being able to orient and respond to what is unsaid (Magnusson and Stevanovic 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
Digital technology has long provided new ways of initiating romantic relationships as people communicate through text messages, social media, and dating applications. Emojis have been widely adopted as a means of conveying nonverbal cues in digital communication. However, what role do platform‐provided social cues, such as emojis, play in fostering or impeding clear communication and shared romantic expectations from a flirtatious text message conversation? In this study, 713 college students were randomly assigned to read a Snapchat conversation with or without emojis and, they were subsequently asked to infer the characters' thoughts and feelings, clarity of the characters' intentions, and indicate their own discomfort with receiving a similar Snapchat message. The results showed that emojis increase the clarity of the main character's intentions. Moreover, the participants' cognitive efforts, the extent to which they were emotionally affected by the conversation, and the presence of emojis reduced comfort level with receiving a similar Snapchat message. These findings suggest that emojis provide clarity to romantic conversations, which can amplify the interpersonal discomfort of receiving text‐based sexual overtures.
... Li and Kenrick's research explored sex differences in preferences for shortterm mates, including motivations for infidelity in the context of evolutionary psychology. 17 (Monica T Whitty, 2004) Cyber flirting: An examination of men's and women's flirting behaviors both offline and on the internet. Comput Hum Behav. ...
Article
Full-text available
This research delves into the intricate dynamics of dating infidelity within the context of modern romantic relationships. By examining behaviors, motivations, consequences, challenges, and treatment interventions related to dating infidelity, this study offers a comprehensive exploration of this complex phenomenon. The findings reveal a spectrum of behaviors, ranging from emotional connections facilitated by technology to seeking sexual novelty and exploring relationships outside of primary partnerships. Motivational factors, including emotional fulfillment, sexual exploration, and dissatisfaction within the current relationship, emerge as key drivers of dating infidelity. The implications of infidelity extend beyond individuals, impacting relationship well-being and household welfare. Challenges such as communication breakdowns and unrealistic expectations hinder effective relationship dynamics. Treatment interventions, such as couples therapy, communication training, and strategies for rebuilding trust, provide a roadmap for addressing infidelity and promoting healing. This research enriches relationship science by offering insights into the evolving landscape of modern romantic connections and underscores the importance of open dialogue and communication to foster healthier and more resilient relationships.
... Flirting as a form of human interpersonal interaction often consists mainly of nonverbal signals (Moore 2010;Wade et al. 2016). Research indicates that men and women differ in how they employ verbal and nonverbal cues in flirtatious interactions (Whitty 2004), with women often more attuned to reading flirting cues (Albright and Carter 2019). Men often overperceive or misperceive a woman's sexual interest (France et al. 2009;Lindgren et al. 2008). ...
Article
Full-text available
Role‐taking is the process of mentally and affectively placing the self in the position of another. This study used a mixed‐methods approach to highlight how a traditional sexual script often imposes gendered expectations of role‐taking. To explore role‐taking within flirtatious exchanges, a sample of college students read a Snapchat conversation and then inferred what each character was thinking and feeling. Hooking up was the most common qualitative theme among the undergraduate participants; however, this theme was more prominent when considering the perspective of the male character, and this expectation was strengthened by the presence of emoji. The male character was expected to be excited or aroused, more focused on social cues—such as their roommate not being present—and to have the female character over to study as a way to pressure her for physical intimacy. The participants were more likely to think that the female character just wanted to study, was anxious or nervous, and was more aware of the male character's perspective. These findings highlight the complex nature of conveying sexual interest, which often forefronts desire and agency from a male perspective.
... This view stresses the different resources that men and women traditionally contribute to producing offspring (Buss, 1988). As a result, FOC and commitment readiness might show differential patterns with perceptions of good health, youthfulness, and fertility in women versus physical dominance, ambition, and socioeconomic status in men (see, e.g., Whitty, 2004). ...
Article
Full-text available
A sample of 36,592 online daters provided data on Commitment Readiness defined as “an individual’s desire and readiness to commit exclusively to one romantic partner” in relation to Age, Gender, and Parental Status (singles with and without children). Consistent with previous research, the women scored higher on Commitment Readiness than did the men. Furthermore, age and Commitment Readiness showed a strong inverted U-shaped relation, with younger and older respondents scoring lower on Commitment Readiness as compared to individuals aged 31-60 yrs. Interestingly, Commitment Readiness evidenced neither a significant effect of Parental Status nor an interaction of Gender by Parental Status. Besides the theoretical import of these results, our study illustrates the potential power of Internet research and provides a curious counterexample to criticisms of over-reliance on significance levels for correlational data. Keywords: commitment readiness, gender differences, invented syndromes, pop psychology
Chapter
This chapter presents the complex concept of love as interpersonal attraction, including componential and phenomenological dimensions. It defines attraction as a constellation of psychological phenomena, including attitudes, emotions, moods, and attachments. The appraisal of an attractive person and a relationship triggers a variety of cognitive, emotional, and behavioral processes involved in interpersonal attraction. The chapter also describes what motivates interpersonal attraction, as well as how men and women experience and express attraction to another person. It also presents varieties of interpersonal bonding that involve attraction and attachment between individuals that animals and humans experience, along with corresponding research data. The last section of this chapter provides a review of the methodologies and methods that researchers have proposed in their studies to describe, explore, and measure love as an interindividual attraction.
Chapter
How and where do individuals signal their desire to become romantically involved with another person, and who initiates this process? Although this is a theorized transitional stage in relationship-development models, answers to these questions do not currently exist because prior research has focused largely on the sexual-interest phase of human relationships (i.e., flirtation). Four independent samples of cisgender, heterosexual students at three universities (total N = 688) reported on who (man, woman, both) expressed romantic interest first, where it was expressed, and the cues (verbal, nonverbal) that were used. Men and women agreed on who signaled first, where, and the verbal and nonverbal cues that were used by each other. Our discussion focuses on how the expression of romantic interest is not only memorable to men and women, but also different from what has been documented in the literature about flirtation among cis-gender, heterosexual individuals.
Chapter
This chapter primarily covers the nonverbal expression of sexual interest in cisgender, heterosexual individuals, from their libidinal urges to their post-sex behaviors, with a major focus on flirtation. The focus on flirtation necessitates consideration of the theoretical frameworks and methodological issues that might elucidate, cloud, or distort a true understanding of this domain of scientific inquiry. In part 1, a new framework for defining and discussing gender differences in the human courtship sequence (e.g., expressions of sexual interest or flirting) is offered. In part 2, historical and contemporary research pertaining to how sexual interest is nonverbally expressed and gender differences in the same are discussed in the following areas: sex drive; flirtation; sexual arousal; consent to sex; sex activity; the orgasm gap; and post-copulation. Findings pertaining to non-heterosexual individuals are integrated within each of the covered areas, unless they were extensive enough to warrant separate treatment, which was the case with flirtation and sexual consent.
Chapter
The major limitations of the research findings included in this book are discussed. Suggestions for future research are discussed as well.
Article
Full-text available
Research typically examines the effectiveness of different hook-up strategies used by people to attract potential partners. And yet, only a limited number of studies have addressed self-perceptions regarding flirting skills and which variables contribute to those perceptions. To address this research gap, we developed the Perceived Flirting Self-Efficacy (PFSE) scale and assessed its psychometric properties and individual correlates in a sample of Colombian young adults (N = 857). Results showed the validity and reliability of this measure in assessing perceived self-efficacy for flirting in both social media (PFSE-SM) and face-to-face contexts (PFSE-F2F). Multiple regression analyses showed that higher PFSE-SM scores were associated with being younger, sociosexually unrestricted, and more connected to social networks. Higher PFSE-F2F scores were associated with being more extroverted, more open to experiences, sociosexually unrestricted, and more connected to social networks. Our findings highlight the importance of distinguishing flirting behaviors in different contexts, emphasizing the importance of future research to delve deeper into self-perceptions when examining flirting dynamics.
Article
Full-text available
While there exists some research on offline flirting, there is currently little in the way of conceptual theory or empirical research on flirting in cyberspace. This paper attempts to help redress this balance. The paper initially presents a summary of the behaviour of offline flirting and particularly identifies what constitutes offline flirting signals. Given this background context, suggestions are made as to how we might better conceptualize online flirting. The prevailing wisdom has been that we should focus on the absence of the body in cyberspace. This view is challenged here. Instead, it is argued that researchers should re-orient their focus to how the body is reconstructed online. Winnicott's notions of `potential space' and `transitional objects' are drawn upon in this paper to advance an argument that online flirting should be considered as a form of play. In making this argument, it is contended that online flirting has unique aspects in comparison to offline flirting. In particular, while realistic elements are present in online flirting, there is a blurring between what is reality and fantasy when one engages in flirtatious behaviour on the Internet.
Article
Full-text available
There is a class of nonverbal facial expressions and gestures, exhibited by human females, that are commonly labeled “flirting behaviors.” I observed more than 200 randomly selected adult female subjects in order to construct a catalog of these nonverbal solicitation behaviors. Pertinent behaviors were operationally defined through the use of consequential data; these behaviors elicited male attention. Fifty-two behaviors were described using this method. Validation of the catalog was provided through the use of contextual data. Observations were conducted on 40 randomly selected female subjects in one of four contexts: a singles' bar, a university snack bar, a university library, and at university Women's Center meetings. The results indicated that women in “mate relevant” contexts exhibited higher average frequencies of nonverbal displays directed at males. Additionally, women who signaled often were also those who were most often approached by a man: and this relationship was not context specific.I suggest that the observation of women in field situations may provide clues to criteria used by females in the initial selection of male partners. As much of the work surrounding human attraction has involved laboratory studies or data collected from couples in established relationships, the observation of nonverbal behavior in field settings may provide a fruitful avenue for the exploration of human female choice in the preliminary stages of male-female interaction.
Article
The importance of computer-mediated communication (CMC) for comparing the group that communicates face-to-face (FtF) in cyberspace is discussed. In online relationship, network convergence would imply not only that participants were introduced one another's online contacts, but also to people in their real-life social network. The growth of CMC poses new challenges for understanding of social relationships both in cyberspace and in general. The goal of such understanding is to provide an empirical reference point for evaluating conflicting visions of social life in cyberspace by exploring the prevalence and development of personal relationships.
Article
Heterosexual personal advertisements from two geographically separated, local, weekly newspapers were content-analyzed. Three significant patterns of findings emerged which shed light on gender differences in self-presentational style. First, women were found to be relatively more likely to offer instrumental or `male-valued' traits in their ads and to seek expressive or `female-valued' ones, while men showed the reverse pattern. This paradoxical finding was interpreted to reflect the influence of implicit notions of attraction and role expectations. Second, women were relatively more likely to offer weight and to seek height, while men were relatively more likely to offer height and to seek weight. This pattern was interpreted to reflect the influence of the `male-taller-norm' in mate selection as well as a societal bias toward thinness in women. Finally, as in previous studies of this sort, women were found to be relatively more likely to offer physical attractiveness and to seek professional status, while men were relatively more likely to offer professional status and to seek attractiveness. This pattern was interpreted to be consistent with traditional sex-role expectations wherein appearance is stressed for women and status for men. Overall, the findings show that advertisers exhibit an understanding of implicit theories of attraction: men and women tend to offer precisely those attributes which are sought by the opposite sex.
Article
This article compares traditional offline flirting with cyber-flirting. We begin by providing a definition of offline flirting, which we follow up with our own elaboration of cyber-flirting. The article then draws from psychoanalytic theory, in particular Winnicott's object-relations theory, to propose that cyber-flirting can be a form of play. While this is not an empirical study, we do attempt to present a theoretical framework for the conception of cyberspace. In presenting this framework, we draw from past qualitative and quantitative studies on Internet relationships. We emphasize the problems with past researchers' obsessive attention to the absence of the body online, and suggest that new theorizing on Internet relationships needs to consider how the body is re-constructed. We propose that cyberspace can be what Winnicott would describe as a `potential space' for play, and this particularly applies to online spaces such as MUDs, MOOs and chat rooms. In addition, we suggest that cyber-flirting may promote psychological growth, but it may also become a destructive and exploitative behaviour directed towards `others'. We conclude by pointing out the therapeutic implications of considering cyber-flirting as a form of play. It is intended that this article may assist our conceptualization of this under-researched area of cyber-interactions.
Article
Two studies identified 21 verbal and 15 nonverbal cues that convey a woman's interest in dating a man. Lists were compiled of 23 verbal and 18 nonverbal behaviors that seemed likely to be cues. For each behavior, two sets of male-female conversations were videotaped. For verbal behaviors, there were two conversations per set, one that used the behavior and one that did not. For nonverbal behaviors, there were three conversations per set, in which the woman used a high, medium, or low level of the cue. In a between-subjects design, 551 male and 225 female undergraduates watched the tapes and rated the probability that the woman would accept a date with the man. In addition, men rated the female model's attractiveness, and women rated the appropriateness of her behavior and the likelihood that they would behave similarly. A behavior was considered to be a cue if men thought the woman was more likely to accept a date with the man when she engaged in the behavior. The role that sending cues plays in women's dating initiation and the importance of empirically investigating what skills to teach in social skills training programs are discussed.