ArticlePDF Available

Longitudinal Study of Procrastination, Performance, Stress, and Health: The Costs and Benefits of Dawdling

Authors:

Abstract

Procrastination is variously described as harmful, innocuous, or even beneficial. Two longitudinal studies examined procrastination among students. Procrastinators reported lower stress and less illness than nonprocrastinators early in the semester, but they reported higher stress and more illness late in the term, and overall they were sicker. Procrastinators also received lower grades on all assignments. Procrastination thus appears to be a self-defeating behavior pattern marked by short-term benefits and long-term costs.
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE
Research Report
LONGITUDINAL STUDY
OF
PROCRASTINATION,
PERFORMANCE, STRESS, AND HEALTH:
The Costs
and
Benefits
of
Dawdling
Dianne M Tice
and
Roy F. Baumeister
Case Western Reserve Umverstty
AbstractProcrasttnatton ts variously descnbed a? harmful, tn-
nocuous, or even beneficial Two longitudinal studies examined pro-
crastination among students Procrasttnators reported lower stress
and less illness than nonprocrasttnators early in the semester, but they
reported higher stress and more illness late in the term, and overall
they were sicker Procrastinators also received lower grades on atl
assignment's Procrasttnatton thus appears to be a self-defeating be-
havior pattem marked by short-term benefits and long-term costs
Doing one's work and fulfilling other obligations in a timely fash-
ion seem like integral parts of rational, proper adult funcuoning Yet
a majonty of the population admits to procrastinating at least some-
times,
and substantial minonties admit to significant personal, occu-
pational, or financial difficulties resulting from their dilatory behavior
(Ferran, Johnson, & McCown, 1995)
Procrastinauon is often condemned, particularly by people who do
not think themselves guilty of it (Burka & Yuen, 1983, Ferran et dl,
1995) Cntics of procrastination depict it as a lazy self-indulgent
habit of putting things off for no reason They say it is self-defeating
m that It lowers the quality of performance, because one ends up with
less time to work (Baumeister & Scher, 1988, Ellis & Knaus, 1977)
Others depict it as a destructive strategy of self-handicappmg (Jones
& Berglas, 1978), such a,s when people postpone or withhold effort so
as to give themselves an excuse for anticipated poor performance
(Tice, 1991, Tice & Baumeister, 1990) People who finish their tasks
and assignments early may point self-nghteously to the stress suffered
by procrastinators at the last minute and say that putting things off is
bad for one's physical or mental health (see Boice, 1989, 1996, Roth-
blum, Solomon, & Murakami, 1986 Solomon & Rothblum, 1984)
On the other hand, some procrastinators defend their practice
They point out correctly that if one puts in the same amount of work
on the project, it does not matter whether this is done early or late
Some even say that procrastination improves perfonnance, because
the imminent deadline creates excitement and pressure that elicit peak
performance "I do my best work under pressure," in the standard
phrase (Ferran, 1992, Ferran et al, 1995, Uy, 1995) Even if it were
true that stress and illness are higher for people who leave things unul
the last minute—and research has not yet provided clear evidence that
in fact they both are higher—this might be offset by the enjoyment of
carefree times earlier (see Ainslie, 1992)
The present investigation involved a longitudinal study of the ef-
fects of procrastination on quality of performance, stress, and illness
Early in the semester, students were given an assignment with a
deadline Procrastinators were identified usmg Lay's (1986) scale
Students' well-being was assessed with self-reports of stress and ill-
Address correspondence
Case Western Reserve Unive
7123,
e-mail dxt2@po cwiu
o Dianne M Tice Department of Psychology,
sity 10900 Euclid Ave Cleveland OH 44106-
ness The validity of the scale was checked by ascertaining whethtr
students tumed in the assignment early, on time, or late Finally, task
performance was assessed by consulting the grades received Com-
peting predictions could be made
STRESS AND ILLNESS
Possible Costs
Procrastination has been linked to a vanety of negaUve mental
health vanables Solomon and Rothblum (1984) found that procras-
tination was significantly correlated with depression, irrational be-
liefs,
low self-esteem, anxiety, and poor study habits (Unfortunately
the scale these authors used to measure procrastinauon operational-
lzed It as dilatory behavior accompanied by negative affect about the
dilatory behavior, leaving open the possibility that other people may
procrastinate memly without adverse effects, see Flett, Blankstein, &
Martin, 1995 ) Lay, Edwards, Parker and Endler (1989) found that
anxiety levels in procrastinators who have delayed studying are ex-
tremely high near the exam penod, and Fen-an et al (1995) cited
several similar findmgs from unpublished studies (see pp 29-30)
Researchers have frequently found a link between dejection and pro-
crastinauon, and Lay (1995) showed that dejecUon is an outcome of
procrastinauon (rather than a cause) Hett, Blankstein, and Martin
(1995) reported that scores on a procrastination scale were positively
correlated with measures of perceived stress, negauve life events, and
daily hassles Thus, a vanety of evidence suggests that procrastinauon
IS linked to negative mental health
Possible Benefits
Procrasunators might claim that focusing on the last-mmute efforts
and stresses is misleading Yes, procrastinators may suffer more than
other people at the last minute but that may conceal a patteni of sU-ess
suffered by nonprocrasunators who do their wonying and hard work-
ing earlier in the project penod In this view, procrastinators may
suffer late whereas others suffer early, but the total amount of suf-
fenng could be the same Indeed, it could even be that procrastinators
suffer less, because they compress the su«ss into a short penod
PERFORMANCE
In pnnciple, procrasunation would not necessanly have any effect
on task performance Whether a task is done far ahead of the deadline
or only slightly ahead of it does not necessanly make any difference
in the quality of the work Thus, there is a reasonable theoreucal basis
for the null hypothesis prediction that procrasunation would not affect
quality of performance Sull, both procrasunators and their cnucs
Copynght © 1997 Amencan Psychological SocietyVOL 8, NO 6, NOVEMBER 1997
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE
Dianne M Tice and Roy F
ave pfx>posed possible r
xteds why task perfonnance may be af-
Possible Costs
On the negative side, it seems plausible that procrastination could
I
esult in less effort on the task If the person underesUmates how much
time a task will take—a problem that is endemic to nearly all planning
(Buehler, Gnffin, & Ross, 1994) and perhaps especially to procrasu-
nators (Aitken, 1982, cited in Ferran et al, 1995, p 44, McCown,
1986)—the late starter may be unable to find the additional time
required for success
Even if the estimate of time is accurate, the late starter may per-
form worse because unforeseen delays or obstacles anse Task-related
setbacks (e g , computer difficulues) or extraneous interferences (e g ,
personal problems) may temporanly impair one's capacity to work,
and if the project has been put off until the last minute, the result may
be failure In contrast, if most of the work is already completed before
the delay, or if the delay occurs when there is still plenty of ume to
finish, sausfactory completion of the task may sull be possible
Moreover, performance may be worse under stress If the person
performs the task with the deadline approaching and finds greater
stress at that point, then he or she may suffer vanous negauve effects
of sU-ess or pressure (eg, Amabile, DeJong, & Lepper, 1976,
Baumeister, 1984, Glass, Singer, & Fnedman, 1%9)
Possible Benefits
The negauve effects of stress on task performance are not uniform,
and It IS possible that some people may not expenence them Indeed,
: forms of stress can improve performance (e g , Hanson, 1986)
People who are not harmed by stress would have less reason to per-
form a task far ahead of the deadline, and self-selected procrasunators
might well be such people
If one IS not adversely affected by su«ss and pressure, then m some
ways It makes sense to postpone the task unul near the deadline
Sometimes addiuonal, useful infonnation is made available only near
the deadline Indeed, if one assumes that a student is leammg new
matenal all semester long, then he or she should be able to wnte a
better paper at the end of the term than at the beginning
Another possible benefit of waiUng is that efficiency may increase
Some people may find that in the absence of extemal constraints, they
waste ume explonng tangenual ideas and possibiliues, and so they
perform effectively and efficiently only under the discipline imposed
by the deadline Others may find that without extemal constraints,
they lack motivation to perfomi well, after all, a deadline is an im-
portant fonn of extnnsic motivaUon, and m the relative absence of
mtnnsic motivation, a deadline may be the main or sole moUvator (see
Amabile et al, 1976) The procrastinators' claim that they do their
best work under pressure thus could have some jusuficauon
ParOcipants were 44 students taking a health psychology course
They volunteered
At the start of the semester, the due date for the tenn paper was
announced, and students were also told that if they could not meet the
deadline they could have an automatic extension to a specific later
date Four weeks into the fall semester, participants filled out Uy's
(1986) General Procrasunauon Scale
FOT
the next 30 days, they com-
pleted daily symptom checklists and weekly measures of stress and
work requirements
At the end of the semester, the date that each student handed m the
required paper was recorded (specifically, whether the paper was
tumed in early, on Ume, dunng the automauc extension of the dead-
line, or late) When students tumed in their papers for the course, they
were also asked to fill out a quesuonnaire reporting how relieved they
felt about having completed the work
The lnsuiictor for the class did not have access to the students'
self-report measures, so grading was blind to procrastinauon status In
addition, participants were repeatedly assured that the instructor
would not see the self-report measures This confidenUality helped
ensure that the self-reports would not be contaminated by students'
wishes to communicate anything (e g , excuses for poor poformance)
to the mstmctor Only after the semester was ended did the students
who chose to allow their matenals to be used in this study provide
their names linked to their subject numbers so that grades could be
matched to personality and health reports All students were fully
debnefed
Results and Discussion
Procrastination
behavior
Scores on the General Procrasunauon Scale were correlated with
the date the paper was handed m, r = 45 Procrastinators turned m
their papers significantly later than nonprocrasunators (Unless oth-
erwise noted, all correlaUons are significant at p < 05 or better, with
42 degrees of freedom For ease of discussion, high scorers on the
procrasunauon scale are refeaed to as procrasunators, and low scorers
are referred to as nonprocrasunators ) Of the 7 students tuming in their
papers late (l e, after both the deadline and the extension), only 1
student scored below the median on the procrasunation measure, and
more than half scored more than one standard deviation above the
mean procrasunauon score, confirming the validity of Lay's measure
of procraiUnation
Grades
Procrasunators received significantly lower grades than nonpro-
crasunators both on the term paper, r = - 29, and on the two exams,
r = -64
Health
Procrasunauon scores were correlated with stress, r = - 29, and
symptom reporting, r = - 36 The negauve conelations mean that
procrasunators expenenced significanUy less stress and fewer symp-
toms than nonprocrastmators Procrasunators also reported signifi-
cantly more relief after tunung in their papers than nonprocrastinators
Taken together, the pattem of results provides mixed evidence
about the costs and benefits of procrastinauon Procrasunators re-
ceived poorer grades but reported better health than nonprocrastma-
tors Unfortunately, an alternative explanation for the health benefits
of procrasunauon is possible given the Uining of the daU collection
The health measures were completed m the early part of the semester.
VOL 8, NO 6, NOVEMBER 1997
PSYCHOUXJICAL SCIENCE
Procrastination, Performance, Stress,
and
Health
whereas
any
adverse effects
of
procrasunauon
on
sUiess
and
health
would presumably anse late
m the
semester Study
2
was conducted
to mvesUgate this possibility
STUDY
2
Study
2
was designed
to
replicate
the
finding that procrastinators
expenence less sU^ss and fewer symptoms
of
physical illness early
in
the semester
and to
determine whether this outcome reverses
and
procrasunators suffer poorer health
as
semester deadlines approach
We predicted that the conflation between procrasunauon
and
illness
would
be
negaUve early
in the
semester (replicaung Study
1) but
posiuve
at the end of
the term
Method
Participants were
60
sUidents taking
a
health psychology course
They volunteered Two failed
to
complete the matenals,
and
another
took
the
class
but
declined
to
allow
his or her
data
to be
used
for
research,
the
data
for
these
3
students were dropped
The procedure
for
Study
2
was similar to the procedure
for
Study
1 except that students also filled out reports
of
any visits to health-care
professionals and
a
number
of
addiuonal quesuonnaires were admin-
istered in the last week of
class
Also, to provide converging evidence,
we used the McCown and Johnson (1989, cited m Fen-an
et
al, 1995)
measure
of
procrastinauon
m
addiUon
to
Lay's measure
The
final
quesuonnaires were similar
to the
quesuonnaires completed
in the
first month
of
class Students reported the number
of
symptoms they
had expenenced
in the
past week,
the
amount
of
sU-ess they
had
expenenced
m the
week,
and the
number
of
visits they
had
made
to
the health-care center
m the
past month
For
health-care visits,
we
excluded rouUne visits such
as for
birth control
or
allergy shots
Results
and
Discussion
All the findings for Lay's scale in Study
1
were replicated in Study
2 First, scores
on
this procrasunation scale were con-elated with
be-
havioral procrasunation (tuming
in the
paper late),
r = 37
(Unless
otherwise noted,
all
correlations
are
significant
at p < 05 or
better,
with 56 degrees
of
freedom
) Of
the
6
students tuming
in
their papers
late (after both the deadline and the extension), only
1
student scored
below
the
median
on the
procrasunauon measure,
and two
thirds
scored more than
one
standard deviauon above
the
mean procrasti-
nauon score, confirming
the
validity
of
the scale Second, procrasti-
nauon scores were negauvely correlated with early symptom reports,
r
= -
45,
and
su«ss raUngs,
r = -
31
Thus, early
in the
semester,
procrasunators expenenced significantly less sU-ess
and
fewer symp-
toms
of
physical illness than nonprocrastinators There
was no
rela-
Uonship between procrasunauon and health-care visits dunng the first
month
of
the semester,
r = (X)
Thus, procrastmaUon seems innocu-
ous
or
even beneficial
to
health early
in the
semester
Third, procrastination scores were negatively correlated with
grades on the assigned paper,
r = -
26, and with grades on the exams,
r
= -
66 Thus, procrasunators received significantly lower grades
than nonprocrasunators
on all
tasks
in
both sUidies
The mam contnbuUon
of
Study
2
concerned health outcomes late
in
the
semester (which
had not
been assessed
in
Study
1) As pre-
dicted,
the
seemingly beneficial relaUonship between procrasunation
and health
was
reversed
at the end of
the semester Procrastmat(
^
reported more symptoms,
r = 65,
more sU^ss,
r = 68, and mo
visits
to
health-care professionals,
r =
37. than nonprocrasUnatoi
Procrasunators
may
enjoy
a
healthy, su^ss-fi-ee life when deadlin
are
far off,
but they suffer more than other people when deadlines a
imminent (see
Fig 1)
It
IS of
some interest
to ask
whether
the
late-semester costs
<,
procrasunation outweigh the early-semester benefits The present dai
i
do
not
offer
a
complete answer because health
was not
measured
conUnually over the enure semester, and
it is
not possible to esumate
at what point the shift fTom benefit
to
cost may have occurred Sull
It seems reasonable simply to add our data, weighung them so that the
assessments
of
30 days
of
early-semester health
and
1 week
of
late-
semester health would
be
equally represented Combinmg the data in
that
way
yields
the
conclusion that procrastinators suffered signifi-
cantly more symptoms,
r = 46, and
marginally significantly more
SU-ess,
r =
25, than nonprocrastinators They also visited health-care
professionals
for
illness more often,
r = 27 In
sum, combining
all
data in Study 2 leads to the conclusion that procrastinators were sicker
than nonprocrastinators
Analyses were also computed using McCown and Johnson's Adult
Inventory
of
ProcrastinaUon (AIP) instead
of
Lay's General Procras-
tinauon Scale The two scales were highly correlated with each other,
r
= 86, and
results using
the AJP
were similar
to
those
for
Lay's
scale
The AIP was
negauvely correlated with symptoms
and
stress
early
in
the semester, positively correlated with symptoms, stress, and
clinic visits
at
the end
of
the semester, and posiUvely correlated with
total symptoms
and
clinic visits summed across
all
measurements
It
correlated negatively with exam grades
and
positively with date
of
handing
in
the term paper Thus,
it
too associated trait procrasunauon
with better health early but poorer health later and overall, with poorer
performance, and with lateness The only result not replicated signifi-
cantly was the negative correlauon between procrastination scores and
Fig
1
Number
of
symptoms (per week) reported
by
proerasunators
and nonprocrasunators
in
Smdy
2
Participants were categonzed
as
procrastinators
and
nonprocrasunators based
on a
median split
of
Lay's General Procrasunauon Scale
The
numbers
in the
figure
rep-
resent the mean number
of
symptoms reported each week by procras-
tinators and nonprocrasunators The mean score on the scale was 42
7
the median was 45,
and
the range
was 18-^3
VOL 8, NO 6, NOVEMBER 1997
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE
Dianne M Ticc and Roy F Baumeister
; rades on the term paper AIP s<
Ith term paper grades;s did not correlate significantly
GENERAL DISCUSSION
The results of the present mvesugauon shed light on the benefits
iind costs of procrastinauon As noted at the outset, there are plausible
tlieoreucal bases for a broad range of compeUng predicuons about the
effects of procrasunauon The results do not fully support any one
\ lew. and so a proper evaluation of procrasunauon may need to com-
promise between its stemest cnucs and its most opUmisUc apologists
The main results can be summanzed as follows
First. It appears that procrasUnaUon does bnng short-term benefits
to health Procrasunators do appear to benefit from the carefree, ca-
sual situauon they create for themselves early m the project phase
Nonprocrastmators get nght to work on the project and apparently
begin to suffer from the suess and health problems nght away, too
There are thus at least two significant benefits of procrasunauon.
which are that suess is lowered and illness is reduced by putting off
the task As long as the deadline remains remote, procrasunators are
better off
Second, however, the stress and health benefits of procrasUnation
are reversed as Ume goes by Toward the end of the project penod.
procrasunators reported greater suess and more illness than nonpro-
crasunators Thus, although procrasunation may produce initial ben-
efits.
It produces significant costs later on. as the deadline approaches
Third, the cumulauve effect of procrasunation on suess and health,
summed across early and late measures, is negauve Total suess and
illness are higher for procrastinators than for nonprocrasunators Or.
to put It another way. the early benefits are outweighed by the later
costs Procrasunauon does not simply shift the same amount of sUess
and illness from early to late in the project penod. rather, it apparently
increases the amount of sUess and illness Further work to corroborate
this finding is needed, however
Fourth, procrastinators end up producing infenor work The pres-
ent studies found no support for the claim that procrastinators do
better work because of moU vauonal or other consequences of deadline
pressure Rather, the present results are consistent with the view that
postponing work on a project may lead to compromises and sacnfices
in quality Procrasunation is not a neuual or innocuous form of time
management, let alone a helpful or beneficial one (as some people
claim)
A potential altemative explanation for procrastinators' lower
grades is that procrasunators are less intelligent or less talented stu-
dents than others Several pnor smdies have refuted that suggesUon.
however, by showing no relauon between procrasunauon and intelli-
gence (Ferran. 1991. Taylor. 1979). and occasional findmgs have
even linked procrasunauon to higher scholastic apuuide scores (Ait-
ken.
1982. cued in Ferran et al. 1995. p 44) Hence, it seems most
likely that the procrasunation itself is to blame for the poor perfor-
It IS worth emphasizing that the present findings are based on
self-selecuon into procrasUnator and nonprocrasUnator groups Al-
though self-selecuon weakens the causal inferences that could be
made had there been expenmental randomizauon. it increases some of
the theoreucal and pracucal importance of the results Thus, if the
present suidy had shown that health and performance were impaired
among people who had been randomly assigned to procrasunate. other
VOL 8. NO 6. NOVEMBER 1997
procrasunators might object that they would not suffer the same fate
Some procrasunators do in fact claim that they, unlike other pec^le.
benefit by doing theu best work under last-minute pressure The pres-
ent findings refute such a claim, however. Even people who freely
choose to procrasunate and believe procrasunauon to be beneficial
end up doing worse and being sicker than others
Limitations of this work must be acknowledged Without random
assignment and expenmental control, we cannot assert that procras-
unation causes the sUess and health effects Our results arc essenually
correlauonal The possibility that procrasunauon causes sUvss that in
tum causes illness is perhaps the most plausible account of our find-
ings,
but the data do not provide evidence regarding those possible
causal relauonships Furthermore, although high scores on the pro-
crasunauon scales predicted tuming the paper in late, we can only
assume (as opposed to directly venfymg) that the self-idenufied pro-
crasunators actually did procrasunate on their assignments Apart
from the two procrasunation scales themselves, there is no way to
differenUate among people who might have planned all along to do
the work al the last minute, people who ended up working at the last
minute because they just did not get around to working on their
assignments (although they meant to), and people who may have
ended up working at the last minute for other reasons (such as unex-
pected cnses) All we can say is that self-idenufied procrasunators
tended to work at the last minute (more than other students) and to
suffer vanous consequences A final limitauon is that the present
studies used samples of university students UniversiUes might con-
ceivably cluster their deadlines more than other msututions (e g . at
the end of the semester), thereby making procrasunation more costly
than would be the case if deadlines were diffused
IMPLICATIONS
The present results suggest that procrastination should be consid-
ered as one category of self-defeating behavior because it apparently
leads to suess. illness, and infenor perfonnance It corresponds to the
pattem of short-term gams and long-tenn costs, which is a common
feature of self-defeaung behaviors (Baumeister. 1997. Baumeister &
Scher. 1988. Loewenstein & Eister. 1992. Platt. 1973)
Choosing short-term benefits over greater long-temi ones is also a
hallmark of poor self-regulation, a finding first idenUfied by early
studies of delay of grauficauon (Mischel. 1974. 1996) This pattem
also extends to alcohol and dmg abuse, violence, and other impulsive
acts (see Baumeister. Heatherton. & Tice, 1994. for review) In view
of the present findings, claims that procrasunauon is innocuous or
beneficial appear to be raUonalizaUons for self-indulgent behavior
The present evidence suggests that procrasunators enjoy themselves
rather than working at assigned tasks, until the nsing pressure of
imminent deadlines forces them to get to work In this view, procras-
unauon may denve from a lack of self-regulauon and hence a depen-
dency on extemally imposed forces to moUvate work
An altemative view is that procrasunators sincerely but mistakenly
believe that they can improve performance by such postponement
According to this view, a procrasUnator who has both the ume and the
inchnauon to work on the task far ahead of the deadline might sull put
It off. because of a sincere belief that he or she will perform better by
waiung unul later One might even admire the procrasUnator for the
willpower shown, while feelmg sony for the person because of the
false assumpuon behind that exercise Sull. there is httle evidence to
457
PSYCHO1X)GICAL
SCIENCE
ProCTasUnaUon, Performance, Stress, and Health
support
this view Correlational findings firom questionnaire research
point
toward
a
deficit
m
self-control
as the
explanation Flett, Hewitt,
and
Marun (199S) found that procrastinators scored
low on a me:
of
self-control,
and
Shouwenburg (1995) found that procrasunators
reported
poor work discipline Ferran
et al
(1995,
p 44)
proposed
that
low
Conscienuousness
is the
main
Big
Five charactensUc
of
procrastinators
CONCLUSION
Part
of
the appeal
of
procrasunation
may be
that
it
confers genuine
benefits
m the
short run Procrasunators
may
find that they feel better
and
are
healthier when
the
deadline
is far off and
they postpone
the
task
These benefits
are
eventually more than offset
by the
costs,
however,
because
the
stress and illness suffered
by
procrasunators late
in
the
task exceed
and
outweigh
the
initial benefits Furthermore,
procrasunauon
appears
to
result
in
work
of
infenor quality
Thus,
despite
its
apologists
and its
short-term benefits, procrasti-
nation
cannot
be
regarded
as
either adapuve
or
innocuous Procrasu-
nators
end up
suffenng more and performing worse than other people
Acknowledgments—Findings
from this drucle were presented
at the an-
nual
convention
of
the
Amencan Psychological Society
in
San Francisco,
June
1996
REFERENCES
Ainslie
G
(1992)
Picotconomics
The
straiegic mieracnon
of
successive motnanonal
suites within
the person.
Cunbndge England Cambndge Umversity Press
Amabile
TM
DeJong,
W
&
Lepper
MR
(1976)
Effect,
of
entemally
imposed
deadlines
on
subsequent intrinsic motivation Joumat
of
PersonaUty
and
Social
Psychology
34
92-98
Baumeister
RF
(1984) Choking under pressure Self consciousness
and
paradoxical
effects
of
incentives
on
skillful perfonnance Joumal
of
PersonaUty
and
Social
Psychology
46
610-620
Baumeister
RF
(1997) Esteem
threat,
self-regulatory breakdown
and
emouonal distress
as
factors
in
self-defeaung behavior
R^vwH
o/G(rnfra//'n'c/ioiox>
/
145-174
Baumeister
RF
Heathenon
TF
&
Tice
DM
(,l<m)
Losing control Hoy^andwhy
people
fad at
self
regulation. San
Diego Academic Press
Baumeister
RF
&
Scher
SJ
(1988) Self-defeaung behavior pattems among nonnal
individuals Review
and
analysis
of
common self-destnicuve tendencies Psycho
logical
Bulletin, 104
3-22
Boice
R
(1989) Procrastmation busyness
and
bingeing Behavior Research
and
Therapy
27
605-611
Boice
R
(1996) Binge wnting
and
procrasttnatton/blockmg
amongst
new
professors
Manuscnpl submitted
for
publication
Buehler
R, Gnffin. D &
Ross
M
(1994) Explonng
the
'planning fallacy
Why
people underestimate their task completion Umes Joumal
of
Personality
and
Sociat
Psychology
67
366-381
BuriuuJB
&
Yuen
LM
(1983) Procrastination
Why you do it and
what
lo do
abt>ut
It
Reading
PA
Addison
Wesley
Ellis
A
& Knaus. WJ
(1977)
Overcoming
proc:
Rauonal
Living
Ferran
JR
(1991) Compulsive pixxrastination Some self-reported personality char
tensucs
Psychological
Reports
68
455-458
Fenvi
J R
(1992) Psychometnc validation
of two
adult measures
of procrasunau.
Arousal
and
avoidance measures Joumal
of
Psychopathology
&
Behavioral
^
sessment
14
97-100
Fenan
JR
Johnson
JL
&
McCown
WG (Edi)
(1995)
Procrastination
and lu
avoidance Theory
research, and
treatment
New
York Plenum Press
Flett.
GL
Blank.'itein
KR
&
Martm
TR
(1995)
Procrasunation
negauve
sel
evaluadon
and
stress
in
depression
and
anxiety
A
review
and
preliminary
modi.
In J R
Ferrsn
J L
Johnson
& W G
McCown
(Eds )
Procrastination
and tas
avoidance Theory research
and
treatment
(pp
137-167)
New
York Plenum
Press
Flett.
GL
Hewitt
PL
&
MarUn
TR
(1995)
Dimensions
of
perfectiomsm
and pro
crasunauon
In J
R
Ferran
J L
Johnson
& W
G
McCown
(Eds )
Procrastmation
and
task avoulance Theory
research, and
treatment
(pp
113-136)
New
York
Plenum Press
Glass
DC
Singer
JE
&
Fnedman
LN
(1%9)
Psychic cost
of
adapUUon
to an
environmental stressor Joumal
of
PersonaUty
and
Social Psychology
12
200-210
Hanson
PG
(1986)
The joy of
stress
Fairway
KS
Andrews
McMeel
&
Parker
Jones
EE
&
Berglas
SC
(1978) Control
of
atuibuuons about
the
self through self
handicapping strategies
The
appeal
of
alcohol
and the
role
of
underachievemeni
PersonaUty
and
Social Psychology Bulletm
4
200-206
Lay
CH
(1986)
At
last
my
research article
on
procrastination Joumal
of
Research
m
PersonaUty
20
474-495
Lay
CH
(1995) Trait procrasunation agitauon dejection
and
self-discrepancy
In J R
Fenan
JL
Johnson
&WG
McCown
(Eds )
Procrastination
and
task
avoidance
Theory research
and
treatment
(pp
97-112)
New
York Plenum Press
Lay
CH
Edwards
JM
Parker
JDA
&
Endler
NS
(1989) An assessment of
appraisal anxiety coping and procrasunation dunng an examination penod
Eu
ropean Joumal
of
Personality
3
195-208
Loewenstem
G
&
Eister
J
(Eds)
(1992) Choiceo^ertime
New
York Russell Sage
McCown
W
(1986) Behavior
of
chronic college-student procrasunators
An
expenmen
tal
study Social Science
and
Behavioral Documents
17 133
Mischel
W
(1974) Processes
m
delay
of
graUficaUon
In
L
Berkowitz
(Ed )
Advances
tn
expenmental social psychology
(Vol
7 pp
249-292)
San
Diego Academic
Mischel
W
(1996) From good intenUons
to
willpower
In P
Gollwitrer
& J
Bargh
(Eds) The
psychology
of
action
(pp
197-218)
New
York Gmlfoid Press
Platt.
J
(1973) Social traps
Amencan
Psychologist
28
641-651
Rothblum
ED
Solomon
LJ
&
Murakami.
J
(1986) Affective cogniUve
and
behav
loral differences between high
and
low
procrastmators Joumal
of
Counseling
Psychology
33
387-394
Shouwenburg
HC
(1995) Academic
procrastination
Theoreucal notions measurement
and
research
In J
R
Fenan
J
L
Johnson
& W G
McCown
(Eds
)
Procrastina
tion
and
task avoidance Theory research
and
treatment
(pp 71-%) New
York
Solomon
LJ & Rothblum. E D
(1984) Academic procrasunauon Frequency
and cog
nitive behavioral correlates Joumal
of
Counseling Psychology
31
503-509
'lor
R
(1979) Procrasunauon
The
personality
and
situationat conelates
of
procras
40(4)
DM
(1991) Esteem protection
or
enhancement'
Self handicapping
moU'
attnbuUons differ
by
trait self esteem Joumat
of
Personality
and
Sociat
P
ogy 60
711-725
Tice
DM
&
Baumeister
RF
(1990) Self esteem self-handicapping
an
presentation
The
sd^tegy
of
inadequate pracUce Joumal
of
Perwnality
5,
464
(RECEIVED
8/12/96.
ACCEPTED
12/1/%)
VOL
8. NO 6,
NOVEMBER
1997
... This behavior affects a significant proportion of students, with estimates suggesting that up to 95% of college students procrastinate at least occasionally on academic tasks (Solomon & Rothblum, 1984). The implications of academic procrastination are profound, ranging from diminished academic performance to adverse psychological outcomes, such as heightened anxiety, guilt, and lowered self-esteem (Tice & Baumeister, 1997). ...
... Research consistently highlights that procrastination is linked to maladaptive academic outcomes. Procrastinators tend to receive lower grades, experience greater academic stress, and report higher levels of emotional discomfort (Ying & Lv, 2012;Tice & Baumeister, 1997). Furthermore, they often find themselves trapped in a cycle of last-minute efforts and compromised performance, despite initial intentions to succeed. ...
... Furthermore, they often find themselves trapped in a cycle of last-minute efforts and compromised performance, despite initial intentions to succeed. While some proponents argue that procrastination can serve as a motivational tool under the pressure of looming deadlines, the majority of empirical evidence refutes this claim, indicating that procrastination typically results in inferior work quality and heightened psychological stress (Tice & Baumeister, 1997). ...
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this study is to examine the relationship between the academic procrastination levels and academic achievement of associate degree students. The study employed a correlational survey model. The sample consisted of 391 associate degree students studying at public universities in Konya, including 164 female and 227 male students. The "Student Information Form" and the "Academic Procrastination Scale" were used as data collection tools. The data were analyzed using the SPSS software, and Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated along with a simple linear regression analysis. The results of the study indicate a low but significant relationship between the academic procrastination levels and academic achievement scores of associate degree students. Furthermore, students' academic procrastination levels were found to significantly predict their academic achievement scores. Although academic procrastination is generally considered a negative trait, the results of this study suggest that procrastination can have positive effects under certain conditions. This supports the hypothesis that students might consciously use procrastination as a strategic tool to enhance performance.
... Thus, procrastination may be an indicator of anxiety, as noted in other studies. [16] Likewise, procrastinating students reported higher levels of tension and anxiety as deadlines approached than those who started tasks earlier, according to Tice and Baumeister (1997). [24] To further investigate the link between anxiety and procrastination, we categorized procrastination into task avoidance, time management, and regret, and analyzed their correlations with both anxiety and procrastination. ...
... [16] Likewise, procrastinating students reported higher levels of tension and anxiety as deadlines approached than those who started tasks earlier, according to Tice and Baumeister (1997). [24] To further investigate the link between anxiety and procrastination, we categorized procrastination into task avoidance, time management, and regret, and analyzed their correlations with both anxiety and procrastination. The analysis revealed moderate-to-high positive correlations between procrastination and anxiety, as well as statistically significant correlations between procrastination and anxiety. ...
Article
Full-text available
Abstract: Background: Procrastination, the unnecessary delay of tasks, is common among college students, especially in academic settings. Academic procrastination often leads to last-minute rushes and increased anxiety. The relationship between procrastination and anxiety is intricate, complicating the determination of which triggers the other. Aim: To examine how anxiety and procrastination interact among medical students, particularly focusing on their reciprocal influence. Materials and Methods: This descriptive cross-sectional study included 311 MBBS students, recruited through simple convenient sampling. Procrastination was assessed with the Irrational Procrastination Scale (IPS) and anxiety with the Generalized Anxiety Disorder 7-item (GAD-7) scale. Data analysis was conducted using SPSS version 27, with Chi-square tests, Pearson correlation, and linear regression. Statistical significance was set at P < 0.05. Results: Pearson correlation showed a significant, weak positive correlation between anxiety and procrastination (r = 0.346, P < .001). Subcategories of the IPS correlated with anxiety from negligible to high. Chi‑square analysis found a significant association of anxiety with procrastination (P < 0.001) and gender (P = 0.038), but no significant link of anxiety or procrastination with the year of study was observed. Linear regression revealed that anxiety significantly predicted procrastination (B = 0.354, P < 0.05) and vice versa (B = 0.339, P < 0.05), explaining 12% of the variance. Conclusion: The study found a weak yet significant positive correlation between anxiety and procrastination, indicating that the relationship might be bidirectional. However, the cross-sectional design and reliance on self-reported data highlight the need for further longitudinal research to better understand these dynamics.
... Procrastination, which is a common phenomenon, comes with important costs. Procrastination has been associated with various negative health and well-being outcomes including; psychological distress [10], poorer mental health and less mental help-seeking behaviours [11], experiencing guilt and anxiety [5], lower self-esteem [12], depression and rumination [13,14], elevated stress and poorer physical health [15], delaying treatment and more visits to health-care professionals due to reported illnesses and symptoms in the long run [16,17]. In academic settings, procrastination has also been linked to poorer academic performance and lower grades [8,12,16,[18][19][20]. ...
... Procrastination has been associated with various negative health and well-being outcomes including; psychological distress [10], poorer mental health and less mental help-seeking behaviours [11], experiencing guilt and anxiety [5], lower self-esteem [12], depression and rumination [13,14], elevated stress and poorer physical health [15], delaying treatment and more visits to health-care professionals due to reported illnesses and symptoms in the long run [16,17]. In academic settings, procrastination has also been linked to poorer academic performance and lower grades [8,12,16,[18][19][20]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Given the widespread occurrence of procrastination and its adverse association with well-being, investigating the individual variables that influence procrastination is a crucial matter. Previous research has identified dispositional mindfulness to be negatively associated with procrastination, but the underlying mechanisms driving this relationship remain unclear. In this study, the aim was to investigate whether the inverse relationship between dispositional mindfulness and procrastination could be explained by the mediating roles of trait anxiety and maladaptive perfectionism. In a cross-sectional survey, 126 participants (aged 18–33) completed the 15-Item Five-Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire, the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory-Trait Form, the Almost Perfect Scale-Revised, and the Pure Procrastination Scale. A parallel mediation model was tested to investigate the mediating role of self-reported maladaptive perfectionism and trait anxiety to explain the relationship between dispositional mindfulness and procrastination with a bootstrapped multivariate technique. The results revealed that maladaptive perfectionism significantly mediated the relationship between mindfulness and procrastination -indicating that dispositional mindfulness has a significant indirect effect on procrastination via decreased levels of maladaptive perfectionism. It was determined that 15% of the variance in procrastination was significantly explained by this model (R² = .15, β = −.39, B = −.83, 95% CI = [−1.18, −.48], p < .001]. As the study highlights the importance of maladaptive perfectionism to explain the link between mindfulness and procrastination, we suggest that future research could investigate the influence of mindfulness on procrastination via mindfulness-based interventions, and include measurements of both trait anxiety and maladaptive perfectionism across longitudinal or experimental designs to unpack causality with respect to our pattern of observed findings.
... Mathematics homework procrastination also profoundly impacts students' academic and homework emotions, possibly in a bidirectional manner. For instance, procrastination may lead to heightened math anxiety, which, in turn, increases discomfort, particularly before exams, further aggravating procrastination in mathematics study and homework completion (Ferrari, 2000;Lay & Schouwenburg, 1993;Tice & Baumeister, 1997;Yang et al., 2024). Consequently, mathematics homework procrastination may even negatively affect students' physical and mental health, as students prone to procrastinating math homework often experience intense aversion toward it, potentially leading to a dislike of mathematics itself-an outcome detrimental to both current and future learning in mathematics (Ferrari et al., 2007;Xu, 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
Mathematics homework is a crucial learning activity for students, and procrastination in mathematics homework not only results in a decline in the quality of homework completion but also reduces mathematics achievement. However, there is still a lack of empirical research on what factors cause students to procrastinate on their mathematics homework. Drawing on Self-Determination Theory, this study seeks to explore factors that may be significantly associated with student procrastination in completing mathematics homework. A quantitative research design was adopted, selecting 771 middle school students from different districts of Tianjin, China as research subjects. Using Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM) techniques, the study constructed structural equations to investigate the pathways through which student competence, autonomy, relatedness, and homework emotion influence mathematics homework procrastination. The research indicates that homework emotion has the greatest impact on students' mathematics homework procrastination, especially the negative emotions of anxiety and boredom, which are most evident in causing procrastination. Autonomy follows, showing a significant negative correlation with mathematics homework procrastination. Parental and teacher support for autonomy are factors of relatedness that affect students' mathematics homework procrastination. Parental and teacher autonomy support directly applied to mathematics homework often exacerbates students' procrastination in completing math assignments. This study systematically reveals the factors influencing students' mathematics homework procrastination and provides guidance for students, parents, and teachers to collaboratively reduce it in practice.
... Conversely, low achievement motivation can increase procrastination and reduce the ability to be responsible for a possessed task [38]- [40]. Even students who procrastinate in academics possess the tendency to obtain low grades for each subject and on final examinations [41]- [43]. According to Khattab [44], many children still possess low achievements in learning. ...
Article
Full-text available
span>This research involves the development of items on an achievement motivation scale that is used in improving the achievement motivation of senior high school students. No research on instrument development has been carried out on measuring the level of achievement motivation of senior high school students in Indonesia. Participants tested the development of 38 items and consisted of 1,909 respondents as students from senior high schools in the City of Surabaya. The utilized analysis technique was the Rasch model. Results of applying the Rasch analysis indicated that achievement motivation scale was good, proper, and appropriate in items to the model. The accomplishment motivation scale is a valid and dependable instrument for precisely determining pupils’ levels of accomplishment motivation. In light of the achievement motivation scale results, this study explores the consequences and suggests directions for future research on the use of guidance and counseling.</span
... Academic procrastination refers to the tendency to delay academic tasks and activities without any reasonable justification, and is characterized by a pattern of behavior that involves frequent postponement in initiating and accomplishing such tasks (McCloskey, 2011). Academic procrastination had considerable impacts on different contexts of students' lives and based on research it is significantly had a positive association with anxiety and depression (Brader & Luke, 2013;Tice & Baumeister, 2018;Ulgener et al., 2020), It occurs in all ages and educational levels (McCloskey & Scielzo, 2015), and it has been shown that it is more prevalent among students than other groups (Balkis & Duru, 2009;Closson & Boutilier, 2017;Steel, 2007). The detrimental effects of academic procrastination have led to the development of various scales to assess this construct. ...
Article
Full-text available
Plain language summary Checking if the academic delay survey works for Iranian college students Putting off school work can be a real problem for students, sometimes causing poor grades and stress. To understand why students procrastinate, scientists have made special surveys. One of these, the Academic Procrastination Scale (APS), has been useful in many countries. Yet, to be certain it works for students everywhere, we need to test it in different places, like Iran. Our research did just that. We looked closely at the APS by asking Iranian university students to fill it out. We examined every part of the survey through two studies. One study was like an exploration, checking if the questions covered different aspects of procrastination. Another confirmed what we found, ensuring that our results were trustworthy. We also used a method called ‘Item Response Theory’ to see if the survey’s questions were good at picking up on different levels of procrastination, making sure none were too easy or too hard, and that they measured what we wanted to measure. And it all checked out—the survey did its job well. To top it off, we checked if the APS results matched another well-known procrastination survey, the Tuckman Procrastination Scale. They did! This link-up means that the APS can be trusted to measure procrastination in Iranian students in a way that matches what we already know from other research. Why is this important? When we know a survey works, we use it to find students who need help managing their time and avoiding the last-minute rush. This isn’t just about grades; it’s about helping students feel less stressed and more in control. With reliable surveys, we can make a real difference.
... Dalam situasi berkendara, stres dan kecemasan ini dapat bermanifestasi dalam bentuk perilaku agresif, seperti mengemudi dengan sembrono atau menunjukkan ketidaksabaran terhadap pengendara lain (Sirois, 2014). Prokrastinasi sering kali menyebabkan akumulasi tugas yang belum selesai, yang pada gilirannya meningkatkan tingkat stres dan kecemasan individu (Ferrari, 2010;Tice & Baumeister, 1997). Ferrari et.al (1995) menyimpulkan bahwa pengertian prokrastinasi dapat dipandang dari berbagai sudut pandang yaitu 1).prokrastinasi adalah setiap perbuatan untuk menunda mengerjakan tugas tanpa mempermasalahkan tujuan dan alasan penundaan 2). ...
Article
Full-text available
The increasing number of motor vehicles, which has led to a rise in traffic congestion and accidents on the roads, has become a common phenomenon in major cities in Indonesia. This research aims to investigate the relationship between procrastination, known as "the culture of being late," and aggressive driving in the Jabodetabek area. Procrastination, or the habit of delaying, has become a common phenomenon in Indonesian society and is often associated with aggressive behavior on the roads. This research uses the General Procrastination Scale (GPS) and the Aggressive Driving Scale to measure the levels of procrastination and aggressive driving among a sample of adult drivers aged 17-35 years. The analysis results indicate a significant positive relationship between procrastination and aggressive driving, particularly in the Jabodetabek area, with a correlation coefficient (r) of 0.833, where individuals with a high tendency to procrastinate tend to be more aggressive while driving. This finding provides insights into how procrastination behavior can contribute to risky driving behavior, highlighting the importance of time management and self-control in the context of road safety.
Article
The main purpose of the current research is to find the moderated mediation of gender and religiosity in the procrastination and stress of researchers. The study type is quantitative and survey research design is used in the current study. All public-sector universities in province Punjab are the population of the current research. The sample of current study is six public sector general universities of province Punjab. The sampling technique used in the current study is multistage sampling. Total data form from 303 respondents is collected using three adapted instruments, i.e., Religiosity Questionnaire Akhter (2020), “Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale” (DASS-21) Lovibond and Lovibond, (1995) and Tuckman Procrastination Scale (TPS) Tuckman (1991) in the study. A Google Form is used for the process of online data collection. The collected data is analyzed using analysis techniques of regression, mediation, moderation and moderated mediation in SPPS V.23. The findings presented that the moderated mediation of gender and religiosity in procrastination and stress is insignificant. It is recommended that universities need to arrange workshops and seminars to aware the researchers about the negative consequences of procrastination and stress and to realize the coping nature of religiosity for balancing research work and other daily routines. It is also recommended that future research other than general public universitates’ should undertake to increase the generalizability of the findings.
Article
Full-text available
Three conceptual models of self-defeating behavior can be distinguished on the basis of intentionality (desiring and foreseeing harm). In primary self-destruction, the person foresees and desires harm to self; in tradeoffs, the harm is foreseen but not desired; and in counterproductive strategies, the harm is neither foreseen nor desired. We review 12 categories of self-defeating behavior patterns from the research literature in social psychology. No clear evidence of primary self-destruction is found. Several tradeoff patterns have been shown: Typically, the individual favors short-term benefits despite long-term costs and risks, especially under the influence of aversive emotional states and high self-awareness. Counterproductive strategies have also been found, usually based on misjudging self or misjudging contingencies. It is concluded that normal people do harm themselves and defeat their projects by means of poor judgments, by maladaptive responses, through unforeseen consequences of nonoptimal methods, and by disregarding costs and risks in favor of immediate pleasure or relief; however, there is no clear evidence of intentional, deliberate self-destructiveness among normal (nonclinical) individuals.
Article
Full-text available
In Sample 1, 46 procrastinators compared with 52 nonprocrastinators claimed lower self-esteem, greater public self-consciousness and social anxiety, and a stronger tendency toward self-handicapping. In Sample 2, 48 procrastinators compared with 54 nonprocrastinators reported a weaker tendency toward seeking self-identity information but a stronger tendency toward a diffuse-identity style, yet there were no significant differences in verbal and abstract thinking abilities. Further research must provide evidence for persistent procrastination as a personality disorder that includes anxiety, avoidance, and a fear of evaluation of ability.
Book
Procrastination means putting off a difficult, delayable, important task in favor of something easier, quicker, and less anxiety-provoking. It also means delaying vital actions until the performance and result are less than they would have been if done in a timely manner. Similarly, blocking means that we stumble, delay, and panic in response to a demanding responsibility. Blocking typically occurs when we face public scrutiny (as in writing). In this revisionist and sometimes irreverent book, the author takes academic and professional psychologists to task for neglecting a pair of related problems that are often derided but that can be profoundly debilitating for individuals and economically devastating for schools, businesses, and communities.
Chapter
It has been estimated that over 70% of college students engage in procrastination (Ellis & Knaus, 1977). Solomon and Rothblum (1984) concluded that academic procrastination may depend on the task: Among university students in an introductory psychology course, 46% of students reported procrastination when writing a term paper, 30% when reading weekly assignments, 28% when studying for exams, 23% on attendance tasks, and 11% on administrative tasks. In addition, they examined possible causes of academic procrastination, as perceived by students. Results indicated that fear of failure and task aversiveness were the primary motives reported. These motives have been replicated with Dutch university students as well (Schouwenburg, 1992a).
Chapter
The defining characteristic of trait procrastinators is the extension of temporal sequences between their intentions and their corresponding goal-directed behavior. Procrastinators are often acutely aware of these intention-behavior gaps. For example, student procrastinators will recall numerous occasions on which their study behavior failed to match their temporal intentions. Trait procrastinators may also anticipate such discrepancies in their future behavior. These self-cognitions should have implications for the everyday affective experiences of such individuals. This Chapter addresses these implications, drawing from Higgins’s (1987) self-discrepancy theory—a theoretical account that may be most useful in understanding and explaining the behavior of procrastinators.
Article
Patterns of human self-defeating or self-destructive behavior are examined in relation to several hypothesized causes. Threatened egotism appears to be a major, pervasive cause: Self-defeating responses are especially common when people feel that others may perceive them less favorably than the people desire. Self-regulation failure is also a common element in most self-defeating behavior. Emotional distress is often a precipitating factor. Several causal processes, including foolish risk taking and escapist responses, link emotional distress to self-defeat.
Chapter
This chapter provides an overview of research on choice preferences for delayed, larger versus immediate, smaller gratifications. In spite of the widespread recognition of the important role of delay of gratification in human affairs, previous experimental research on the topic has been limited. At the empirical level, extensive experimental work has been done on delay of reward in animals. Surprisingly, although voluntary delay behavior has been assumed to be a critical component of such concepts as “ego strength,” “impulse control,” and “internalization,” prior to the present research program relatively little systematic attention had been devoted to it in empirical work on human social behavior. The chapter presents, in greater detail, selected studies that focus on the role of cognitive processes during self-imposed delay. Many theorists have paid tribute abstractly to the importance of cognition for the phenomena of personality in general and for self-regulatory processes in particular. These tributes have been accompanied by some correlational research that explores, for example, the links between intelligence, self-control, cognitive styles, and other dispositional. The chapter offers a further theoretical analysis of the determinants of delay behavior.
Chapter
Procrastination is defined typically as an irrational tendency to delay tasks that should be completed (Lay, 1986). Procrastination is believed to be associated with several cognitive, behavioral, and affective correlates and is regarded as a “dysfunction of important human abilities” in routine tasks and critical life tasks (Milgram, Sroloff, & Rosenbaum, 1988, p. 210). The extent of the dysfunction is reflected by estimates indicating that at least 25% of students suffer from severe levels of procrastination (see Hill, Hill, Chabot, & Barrall, 1978; McCown, Johnson, & Petzel, 1989b). Given the potential importance of the procrastination construct, it is perhaps not surprising that it is a topic that has been discussed at length by clinicians and by counselors (e.g., Burka & Yuen, 1983; Ellis & Knaus, 1977; Rarer, 1983).
Article
Four studies sought to differentiate between self-enhancement and self-protection as motivations self-handicapping. High-self-esteem participants self-handicapped to enhance success, whereas low-self-esteem participants self-handicapped to protect against the esteem-threatening implications of failure. This was supported with 2 different forms of self-handicapping and corroborated by attributional statements regarding the implications of handicaps for performance outcomes. Thus, different people use the same (self-handicapping) strategy for different reasons. Also, patterns of social motivation vary with level of trait self-esteem.