ArticlePDF Available

Caloric restriction improves memory in elderly humans

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Animal studies suggest that diets low in calories and rich in unsaturated fatty acids (UFA) are beneficial for cognitive function in age. Here, we tested in a prospective interventional design whether the same effects can be induced in humans. Fifty healthy, normal- to overweight elderly subjects (29 females, mean age 60.5 years, mean body mass index 28 kg/m(2)) were stratified into 3 groups: (i) caloric restriction (30% reduction), (ii) relative increased intake of UFAs (20% increase, unchanged total fat), and (iii) control. Before and after 3 months of intervention, memory performance was assessed under standardized conditions. We found a significant increase in verbal memory scores after caloric restriction (mean increase 20%; P < 0.001), which was correlated with decreases in fasting plasma levels of insulin and high sensitive C-reactive protein, most pronounced in subjects with best adherence to the diet (all r values < -0.8; all P values <0.05). Levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor remained unchanged. No significant memory changes were observed in the other 2 groups. This interventional trial demonstrates beneficial effects of caloric restriction on memory performance in healthy elderly subjects. Mechanisms underlying this improvement might include higher synaptic plasticity and stimulation of neurofacilitatory pathways in the brain because of improved insulin sensitivity and reduced inflammatory activity. Our study may help to generate novel prevention strategies to maintain cognitive functions into old age.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Caloric restriction improves memory in
elderly humans
A. V. Witte
a
, M. Fobker
b
, R. Gellner
c
, S. Knecht
a
, and A. Flo
¨el
a,d,1
Departments of aNeurology and cInternal Medicine, bCenter for Laboratory Medicine, and dInterdisciplinary Center of Clinical Research, University of
Mu¨ nster, Albert-Schweitzer-Strasse 33, 48149 Mu¨ nster, Germany
Edited by Fred Gage, The Salk Institute, San Diego, CA, and approved December 19, 2008 (received for review September 4, 2008)
Animal studies suggest that diets low in calories and rich in
unsaturated fatty acids (UFA) are beneficial for cognitive function
in age. Here, we tested in a prospective interventional design
whether the same effects can be induced in humans. Fifty healthy,
normal- to overweight elderly subjects (29 females, mean age 60.5
years, mean body mass index 28 kg/m
2
) were stratified into 3
groups: (i) caloric restriction (30% reduction), (ii) relative increased
intake of UFAs (20% increase, unchanged total fat), and (iii)
control. Before and after 3 months of intervention, memory per-
formance was assessed under standardized conditions. We found
a significant increase in verbal memory scores after caloric restric-
tion (mean increase 20%; P<0.001), which was correlated with
decreases in fasting plasma levels of insulin and high sensitive
C-reactive protein, most pronounced in subjects with best adher-
ence to the diet (all rvalues <0.8; all Pvalues <0.05). Levels of
brain-derived neurotrophic factor remained unchanged. No signif-
icant memory changes were observed in the other 2 groups. This
interventional trial demonstrates beneficial effects of caloric re-
striction on memory performance in healthy elderly subjects.
Mechanisms underlying this improvement might include higher
synaptic plasticity and stimulation of neurofacilitatory pathways in
the brain because of improved insulin sensitivity and reduced
inflammatory activity. Our study may help to generate novel
prevention strategies to maintain cognitive functions into old age.
aging cognition diet unsaturated fatty acids
Because of the constant growth of the elderly population in
today’s societies worldwide (1), the search for new preven-
tion and treatment strategies to maintain higher brain functions
throughout life is of major economic and medical importance
(see for example ref. 2). In the last 3 decades, numerous studies
suggested that modifiable lifestyle factors including a low-calorie
diet (caloric restriction, CR), and specific micro- and macronu-
trients like unsaturated fatty acids (UFA), might exert beneficial
effects on the aging brain (3–7). In animal models of aging and
neurodegenerative diseases, CR protected hippocampal, striatal,
and cortical neurons, and ameliorated functional decline (8–18).
In longitudinal observations in humans, it was found that a CR
diet, as consumed by residents of the city of Okinawa, Japan,
contributed to healthy aging and longevity (19). Conversely,
obesity as a result of high energy intake has been shown to
increase the risk of age-related cognitive decline (20).
A diet rich in mono- and polyUFA has been demonstrated
to enhance cognitive performance in rats (21). It has been
further proposed by epidemiological studies in humans that
UFA, provided e.g., by olive oil and sea-fish in the traditional
mediterranean diet, exert a risk-lowering effect for AD and
cognitive impairment (22–27). Recently, 2 interventional stud-
ies reported a significant cognitive improvement in patients
suffering from mild cognitive impairment (MCI) after intake
of omega-3 polyUFA supplements vs. placebo (28, 29). How-
ever, inconclusive or negative findings from animal and ob-
servational studies have also been reported for CR (e.g., 30, 31,
32) and UFAs (33, 34).
Taken together, potential benefits of specific ‘‘brain-healthy
diets’’ have been proposed, but have not been confirmed un-
equivocally by animal experiments and human epidemiological
studies. Evidence drawn from prospective interventional trials in
humans is still missing (CR) or scarce (UFA, 28, 29). Therefore,
the aim of the present study was to elucidate cognitive effects of
a diet low in calories or high in UFAs in healthy elderly
individuals (for a f lowchart, see Fig. 1). Because memory
impairment is an early indication of AD and its precursor, MCI
(35), we considered the ability to remember and learn new
contents as our primary outcome measure, in accordance with
previous studies on lifestyle interventions (36, 37). Moreover, we
tried to identify potential mechanisms underlying the positive
effects of these dietary interventions. Metabolic factors like
insulin-resistance or low-grade inflammation might contribute
to age-related cognitive impairments (38, 39), and improvement
of metabolic state should result in acute improvement of cog-
nition, in addition to long-term deceleration of cognitive decline.
Therefore, we assessed peripheral blood levels for insulin,
glucose, and markers of inflammation. Neuronal function may
also be enhanced via neurotrophic factors (4), which are sug-
gested to be activated by moderate stressors like CR via adaptive
cellular stress response pathways (5). This possibility was tested
by assessing neurotrophic levels in peripheral blood.
Results
Dietary Compliance. Details of physiological measures and serum
levels at baseline and after intervention are shown in Table S1.
As intended by the intervention, there was a significant weight
loss (F
(2, 46)
7.25, P0.002; t
(18)
3.24, P0.005; Fig. 2;
Table S1) and body mass index (BMI) reduction (F
(2, 46)
7.24,
P0.002; t
(18)
3.33, P0.004; Fig. 2, Table S1) in the CR
group (group 1). In addition to significant weight loss and
reduction of fasting insulin levels, CR subjects’ postintervention
questionnaire on adherence to dietary guidelines demonstrated
that they followed the instructions (16 of 18 answered ‘‘definitely
yes,’’ or ‘‘predominantly yes’’). The remaining 2 subjects of the
CR group (n 18, 1 subject did not complete the questionnaire)
answered they changed their dietary habits ‘‘at least half of the
time. Asked whether they changed their physical activity during
the study, 18 of 18 answered with ’’no.‘‘ No significant changes
emerged for body fat and waist-to-hip ratio in this group, nor for
parameters of lipid metabolism (P0.05; Table S1). For the
UFA enhancement group (group 2) and for the control group no
Author contributions: S.K. and A.F. designed research; A.V.W., M.F., and A.F. performed
research; A.V.W., M.F., R.G., and A.F. analyzed data; and A.V.W., R.G., S.K., and A.F. wrote
the paper.
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.
1To whom correspondence should be addressed at: Department of Neurology, University
of Mu¨ nster, Albert-Schweitzer-Strasse 33, 48129 Mu¨ nster, Germany. E-mail: floeel@
uni-muenster.de.
This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/cgi/content/full/
0808587106/DCSupplemental.
© 2009 by The National Academy of Sciences of the USA
www.pnas.orgcgidoi10.1073pnas.0808587106 PNAS
January 27, 2009
vol. 106
no. 4
1255–1260
NEUROSCIENCE
significant changes emerged for weight, BMI, body fat, and waist
to-hip ratio, nor for serum levels of triglycerides, cholesterol,
HDL, LDL, or their ratio (all P0.05; Fig. 2; Table S1).
Dietary intake at baseline and after the intervention (self-
reported) is shown in Table S2. Dietary records revealed that all
groups increased the proportional intake of UFAs significantly
(F
(1,46)
92.45, P0.001; t
(48)
⫽⫺10.06, P0.001; Table S2),
yet with highest increase in the UFA enhancement group
(18%, close to the aim of 20%). The UFA-to-saturated fatty
acids ratio was significantly improved only in the UFA enhance-
ment group (F
(2, 46)
3.32, P0.045; t
(19)
⫽⫺4.58, P0.001;
Fig. 2; Table S2). Considering marine sources of omega-3 UFAs,
intake of eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA) and docosahexaeinoic
acid (DHA) was relatively low at baseline (mean 0.2 g/day; max.
1.6 g/day) and did not increase over the intervention (P0.05).
Intervention Effects. ANOVA
RM
showed a significant TIME x
GROUP interaction on memory scores (F
(2, 46)
5.42, P
0.008). Subsequent t tests revealed that this was due to significant
differences in memory scores in the CR group before and after
intervention, with higher scores after the intervention (t
(18)
4.73, P0.0002; Fig. 3). Likewise, there was a significant
difference for memory scores uncorrected for false-positive
misidentifications (t
(18)
⫽⫺2.85, P0.011) and for ‘‘number of
false-positives’’ (t
(18)
2.62, P0.018). In summary, subjects
remembered more words and made fewer mistakes after caloric
restriction. No differences were shown for memory scores in the
UFA enhancement group (all Pvalues 0.31) or in the control
group (all Pvalues 0.62).
In the CR group, inverse associations emerged between
changes in several laboratory parameters [insulin, fasting glu-
cose, hs-CRP, and tumor necrosis factor-alpha (TNF-
)] and
changes in memory score:
Increases in memory score were correlated with decreases in
insulin levels (r⫽⫺0.45, P0.06). Focusing the analysis on
those individuals with best adherence to the intervention (de-
fined as weight loss 1 SD of mean weight loss of the control
group, resulting in a weight loss of 2 kg, n9), a highly
significant inverse association emerged (Bonferroni corrected,
r⫽⫺0.78, P0.014; Fig. 4). In addition, there was a trend for
Fig. 1. Flow-chart of the study. 50 healthy elderly subjects were initially
included in the study and performed baseline measurements of physiological
parameters, fasting serum levels, and memory tests (session I). Based on age,
sex, and BMI, subjects were stratified into 3 groups to follow either a specific
diet, namely caloric restriction (n20, group 1) or unsaturated fatty acids
(UFA) enhancement (n20, group 2), or not to change previous eating habits
(control, n10). Dietary instructions were provided by clinical dieticians. One
women from group 1 was not available for posttesting. After a period of 3
months, participants again underwent measurements of physiological param-
eters, fasting serum levels, and memory tests (session II). At baseline, after 6
and after 12 weeks, subjects additionally completed nutrition diaries over 7
consecutive days.
Fig. 2. Percentage changes in weight (black bars), BMI (gray bars), and
changes in unsaturated fatty acids (UFA)-to-saturated fatty acids (SFA) ratio
(striped bars) after caloric restriction (group 1), UFA enhancement (group 2),
and control condition. Note that caloric restriction led to a significant decrease
in weight, BMI, and UFA enhancement to a significant increase in the UFA-
to-SFA-ratio. Error bars indicate standard error. ***,P0.001; **,P0.01
according to ANOVARM posthoc testings.
Fig. 3. Percentage memory scores normalized to baseline values before and
after caloric restriction (dashed line), unsaturated fatty acid (UFA) enhance-
ment (dotted line), and control (solid line). Note that after caloric restriction,
a highly significant improvement in memory scores can be seen. Baseline
memory scores were not significantly different. Dots give means, bars indicate
standard error. ***,P0.001.
Fig. 4. Inverse correlation (Spearman, r⫽⫺0.81, P0.014) between
changes in insulin levels and memory score improvements after caloric restric-
tion in those subjects with best adherence to the diet (n9). Line indicates
regression fit.
1256
www.pnas.orgcgidoi10.1073pnas.0808587106 Witte et al.
a significantly reduced mean value of fasting glucose serum
levels in the CR group after the intervention (t
(18)
1.82, P
0.086), yet ANOVA
RM
failed to reach significance (F
(2, 46)
1.98; P0.15).
Furthermore, increases in memory score were correlated with
decreases in hs-CRP levels (trend; r⫽⫺0,41, P0.083). Again,
if including only those subjects with best adherence to the diet,
a highly significant inverse association emerged (Bonferroni
corrected, r⫽⫺0.83, P0.005; Fig. 5). A weak correlation was
also found for increases in memory score and decreases in
TNF-
in the CR group (r⫽⫺0.39, P0.102), more obvious
in those subjects with best adherence to the diet (r⫽⫺0.59, P
0.094).
For serum levels of BDNF, IGF-1, and IL-1
, no significant
correlations with memor y scores emerged (P0.05), nor for
these or any of the other parameters in the UFA enhancement
group or in the control group (P0.05). Likewise, no significant
effects for GROUP TIME was detected by ANOVA
RM
.
Discussion
In this prospective interventional study in healthy normal to
overweight elderly individuals, we found a significant improve-
ment in memory performance after a caloric restricted diet (CR)
over a period of 3 months. Memory improvement was correlated
with decreases in fasting insulin and hs-CRP, most pronounced
in those individuals with best adherence to the CR diet. In
contrast, no significant changes in memory performance
emerged after a diet rich in UFA or after control conditions.
Caloric Restriction. The findings of this interventional trial in
humans support experimental animal studies (4) and epidemi-
ological observations in humans (19, 40) that have suggested
beneficial effects of CR on the aging brain. For example, CR has
been demonstrated to enhance spatial memory performance in
rats (17), and even CR over a period of 4 months sufficed to
reduce age-related impairments in motor- and learning tasks in
mice (9). Moreover, Fontan-Lozano and colleagues (41) re-
ported that a CR diet, using an intermittent fasting regime
enhanced learning and consolidation processes in mice, probably
via higher expression of an NMDA-receptor subunit in the
hippocampus. Interestingly, adult-onset short-term CR over 7
weeks in rats attenuated the effects of excitotoxic insults in
hippocampal slices compared with ad libitum control diet (8).
These results concur with the current study, because we found
that even moderate CR over a period of 3 months improves
cognition in healthy elderly subjects.
Unsaturated Fatty Acids. Considering UFA, observational studies
in elderly cohorts (23, 25, 42) and small clinical trials in patients
suffering from MCI or AD (43, 44) suggested that a diet high
in mono- or omega-3 UFA might postpone cognitive decline.
In rats, it has been demonstrated that a diet rich in mono- and
di-UFA enhanced spatial memory performance (21). Con-
versely, higher intake of omega 3-fatty acids did not improve
cognitive performance in epidemiological studies (for exam-
ple, see ref. 33). The latter results are consistent with our study
that failed to detect a beneficial effect of a 3-month dietary
intervention high in UFA on cognitive performance. Several
possible explanations may account for these negative findings:
First, our results could be due to low adherence to the UFA
diet, or to insufficiently high UFA dosage in the dietary
protocol, rather than to a lack of positive effects of UFA on
cognition per se. Because our dietary protocol promoted a
self-prepared diet arranged independently at home, the
present study does not allow us to distinguish between these
possibilities. According to dietar y records, however, there was
a significant increase in the UFA-to-saturated fatty acids ratio
in the UFA enhancement group, which nearly met the protocol
aim of 20% UFA increase. However, self-reported dietary
information is prone to errors (45).
Second, the amount of marine omega-3 UFA (mainly EPA,
DHA) did not increase over the intervention period according
to dietary records, mainly because there was no significant
increase in fatty seafish meals. Because these sources of omega-3
fatty acids are suggested to be most effective in delaying cog-
nitive decline (23, 28, 29), a low EPA/DHA-intake might have
contributed to our negative findings, yet the evidence is still
unequivocal (see e.g., 46 for positive results on olive oil, however,
this might also depend on other, non-UFA ingredients in olive
oil). Therefore, future studies have to further evaluate the effects
of different UFAs on cognitive functions on the aging brain in
health and disease.
Mechanisms of Diet-Induced Cognitive Changes.
Insulin.
In the
present study, we found a decrease of fasting peripheral insulin
in the CR group, in accordance with studies in healthy rats (47)
and monkeys (48–50) after CR, and with clinical data in obese
patients (e.g., 51). Reducing peripheral insulin levels should
result in increased insulin sensitivity and central insulin levels
(3), because higher levels of peripheral insulin lead to a down-
regulation of insulin transport at the bloodbrain-barrier and thus
to central hypoinsulinemia (52, 53). Importantly, improved
insulin signaling in the brain has been suggested to have neu-
roprotective effects (38, 52, 54,), whereas increased peripheral
circulating insulin may promote the development of cognitive
impairments and AD (52, 54, 55).
Furthermore, the observed correlation between decrease in
peripheral insulin and increase in memory points to a possible
role of insulin in mediating the beneficial effects of CR on
memory functions (for review, see ref. 7). Levels of insulin,
insulin receptors, and insulin-regulated pathways in the brain are
involved in glutamate- and GABA-mediated synaptic plasticity
and in gene expressions required for long-term memory consol-
idation (38, 56). For example in the hippocampus, insulin has
been shown to induce NMDA receptor phosphorylation (57),
and to increase channel activities of NMDA receptors (58),
which play an important role in learning and memory formation
(for example, see ref. 59). Thus, it has been convincingly
demonstrated that insulin signaling exerts neuroprotective and
neuromodulatory effects in the brain, although the molecular
machinery linking insulin and cognitive improvement, for ex-
ample the exact role of kinase molecules in learning and
memory, needs to be further elucidated (38).
In summary, the present study lends experimental support to
a model derived from animal studies in which reduced fasting
insulin levels due to CR led to lower insulin resistance, higher
insulin sensitivity, subsequently to improved insulin signaling in
Fig. 5. Inverse correlation (Spearman, r⫽⫺0.83, P0.005) between
changes in high sensitive C-reactive protein (hs-CRP) and memory score im-
provements after caloric restriction in those subjects with best adherence to
the diet (n9). Line indicates regression fit.
Witte et al. PNAS
January 27, 2009
vol. 106
no. 4
1257
NEUROSCIENCE
the brain and to increased synaptogenesis and neuronal survival
(3). Higher insulin sensitivity due to CR in our subjects, with
subsequently improved insulin signaling in the brain, would be a
plausible explanation for the observed memory improvements in
the current study.
Neurotrophic factors.
Neuronal function may also be enhanced via
neurotrophic factors (5, 38), which are suggested to be activated
by moderate stressors like exercise and CR via adaptive cellular
stress response pathways (e.g., heat shock protein 70; for details,
see refs. 4 and 60). Neurotrophic factors, such as IGF-1 and
BDNF, are widely known to be involved in neuronal growth and
neurogenesis and might also protect mature neurons from
degeneration (61). IGF-1 is also a ligand for insulin receptors
(62), thus activating insulin pathways in the brain. Both IGF-1-
(63) and BDNF-levels (64) have been suggested to be enhanced
after CR in rodents. Our results did not show a significant
difference in either IGF-1 or BDNF induced by dietary inter-
ventions. One explanation might be that we could only assess
peripheral levels of IGF-1 and BDNF. Even though both pe-
ripheral IGF-1 (65) and BDNF (66) have been shown to pass the
blood–brain barrier, these measures may not be a perfect
reflection of brain concentrations. In addition, other neurotro-
phic molecules such as glia-derived neurotrophic factor (GDNF)
and nerve growth factor (NGF) might have changed in adapta-
tion to CR (3), which were not assessed in the current study. To
clarify these issues, measurements of these factors could be
additionally assessed in future studies.
Inflammation.
CR has been shown to exert anti-inflammatory
effects (4), including down-regulation of hs-CRP levels in ro-
dents (67) and TNF-
in humans (68, 69), in line with the present
data. With regard to cognition, several studies have proposed
that ‘‘inflammatory activity,’’ as indicated by serum markers of
inflammatory responses, is negatively correlated with neuropsy-
chological performance and cognitive decline (70, 71). For
example, an observational study by Teunissen and colleagues
(72) found significant inverse correlations of serum levels of
CRP and haptoglobin with performance in a verbal learning task
(congruent to the memor y test used in the current study) in
healthy elderly individuals. The current study is the first to
confirm these findings, and to extend the proposed association
for TNF-
in an interventional design. However, anti-
inflammatory pathways linking CR and memory remain to be
further elucidated (4), e.g., by including a larger number of
subjects with elevated levels of inf lammatory markers at base-
line, and testing an extended range of markers. Interestingly,
TNF-
has been demonstrated to promote insulin resistance in
experimental animal studies (73, 74). Therefore, a reduction of
TNF-
by CR might additionally contribute to maintain cogni-
tive functions via improved insulin signaling (3).
Limitations. Several limitations should be considered when in-
terpreting our findings. First, dietary habits were self-reported
only and thus prone to over- or underestimation (45). However,
in the CR group, weight loss and BMI reduction demonstrated
adherence to the intended dietary regime. Second, individuals in
the control group did not receive the same amount of attention
by dietary counsellors, and interaction with group members, as
participants in the CR group. Better memory per formance may
thus be due to a Hawthorne (75) effect or an effect of ‘‘enhanced
environmental enrichment’’ by social interaction in the CR
group. However, the finding that individuals in the UFA en-
hancement group, receiving a similar amount of attention and
social interaction, did not show memory improvements, renders
this explanation highly unlikely.
Conclusion
To our knowledge, the current results provide first experimental
evidence in humans that caloric restriction improves memory in
the elderly. Our findings further point to increased insulin
sensitivity and reduced inf lammatory activity as mediating
mechanisms, leading to higher synaptic plasticity and stimulation
of neuroprotective pathways in the brain. Future studies incor-
porating measurements of additional neurotrophic and inflam-
matory markers, and brain imaging to assess structural changes
(for example, see ref. 36), should provide further insights into
potential mediators of improved cognition by changes in dietar y
habits.
The present findings may help to develop new prevention and
treatment strategies for maintaining cognitive health into old
age (3).
Materials and Methods
Subjects. Fifty healthy elderly subjects (age: 60.5 years 7.6 SD, BMI: 28 kg/m2
3.7 SD; 29 females) were recruited via newspaper advertisement. Inclusion
criteria were age between 50 and 80 years, a BMI 21 to exclude potential
underweight after intervention, and postmenopausal status for women. At
screening visit, participants underwent a routine medical and neurological
examination. Exclusion criteria were severe cardiac and pulmonary disease,
diabetes or other metabolic disorder, psychiatric disorders, memory impair-
ment based on a score of 26 on the MiniMental State Examination (MMSE;
76), and drug abuse, including alcohol dependence and heavy smoking.
Psychiatric comorbidity was additionally monitored using the Beck’s Depres-
sion Inventory (BDI, German version; 77) and Spielberger’s State Trait Angst
Inventar (STAI 1 and 2, German version; 78). One woman was not available for
postassessment, leaving 49 participants for final analyses. Based on age, sex,
and BMI, subjects were stratified into 3 groups: (i) Caloric restriction, (ii)
increase of the amount of UFA (‘‘UFA enhancement’’), and (iii) control group;
for details on groups see below. Demographic variables at baseline are given
in Table S3. The study was conducted at the Department of Neurology at the
University of Mu¨ nster, Germany. All subjects provided written informed con-
sent and received reimbursement for participation. The research protocol was
approved by the Ethics Committee of the University Hospital of Muenster,
Germany.
Caloric Restriction. According to previous recommendations based on studies
of rodents and rhesus monkeys (49, 60), participants (n19) were instructed
to reduce caloric intake aiming at a 30% reduction relative to previous habits,
over a period of 3 months. The intended individual caloric intake was calcu-
lated a priori based on individual dietary records, because the aim of the
caloric restriction intervention was to reduce each subject’s individual caloric
intake by 30%, compared with pretrial levels. To avoid cognitive changes due
to malnutrition (79), minimal intake was set to 1,200 kcal per day.
Unsaturated Fatty Acids Enhancement. According to previous recommenda-
tions based on studies of rodents (21), participants (n20) were instructed to
enhance intake of UFAs aiming at a 20% increase compared with previous
habits, over a period of 3 months. They were instructed to keep the amount
of total fat intake unchanged.
All subjects assigned to 1 of the 2 dietary interventions were trained on how
to follow their respective diet by experienced clinical dieticians blinded to the
underlying study hypothesis. Therefore, after completing baseline measure-
ments, participants attended a 2-h tutorial (maximum 12 persons each).
Additionally, they received dietary instructions in written forms. Group 1
additionally underwent 1-h individual schooling at baseline and a second 1-h
tutorial carried out by the clinical dieticians after a period of 6 weeks.
Moreover, subjects of the 2 intervention groups received supplementary
dietary counseling via telephone if needed, so that any problems in adhering
to the intervention could rapidly be addressed during the entire intervention
period. To provide optimal supervision, dieticians obtained information
about individual nutritional intake from the nutrition diaries and personal
interviews. Adherence to the intervention was monitored by measures of
weight, BMI, waist-to-hip ratio, amount of body fat, and fasting serum levels
of triglycerides, cholesterol, and hs-CRP, because these parameters have been
shown to decrease after caloric restriction and/or after a dietary enhancement
of UFA (80–83). In addition, information on adherence to the diet was
collected by a postintervention Questionnaire, and self-reported nutritional
records were collected in the course of the intervention period.
Control. Participants (n10) were instructed not to change previous eating
habits over a period of 3 months. No specific dietary counselling was admin-
istered to avoid self-chosen/self-administered changes in dietary patterns that
1258
www.pnas.orgcgidoi10.1073pnas.0808587106 Witte et al.
have been reported after any dietary counselling (for example, ref. 84). For
simplicity this condition is subsequently referred to as control intervention.
Nutritional records. Supply of nutrients, caloric-, and UFA-intake were docu-
mented by nutritional records at the beginning of the study (record 1), after
6 weeks intervention (record 2), and after 12 weeks intervention (record 3).
Each record encompassed 7 days of protocol. For nutritional records, all
subjects had to plot, on a daily basis, all food and drink intake in an in-house
developed standard nutrition diary (University of Mu¨ nster, Department of
Internal Medicine) similar to records used in other studies (see e.g., 45). The
diary contained numerous nutritional items presented in standard servings,
and additional free lines to describe foods not listed in the diary. Subjects had
to mark the respective items, with the possibility to adjust for individual
servings. Nutrients, amount of calories, and amount of UFAs where quantified
using the software EBISpro (Erhardt).
Physiological Parameters and Blood Sampling. Before and after 3 months of
intervention (sessions I and II; see Fig. 1), the following variables were assessed:
Weight (in kilograms; measured), height (in meters; self-reported), waist-to-
hip ratio (in centimeters/centimeters, measured), body fat (percentage, mea-
sured), diastolic and systolic blood pressure, heart rate, fasting serum levels of
triglyceride, total-, HDL- and LDL-cholesterol, insulin, glucose, insulin-like
growth factor 1 (IGF-1), brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), cat-
echolamines (85), markers of inflammation [i.e., high sensitive-C-reactive
protein (hs-CRP) and tumor necrosis factor-alpha (TNF-
)], and routine pa-
rameters (sodium, potassium, calcium, phosphate, protein, creatinine, urea;
data not shown), for details, see SI Text.
Neuropsychological Testing. Before and after 3 months of intervention, sub-
jects were tested on memory performance using the German version of the
Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Task (AVLT) (84). The test was performed by a
trained clinical neuropsychologist. Participants were asked to learn as many
words as possible out of a list of 15 words. As primary outcome measure
‘‘memory score,’’ we considered the total number of retrieved words after 30
min (delayed memory), adjusted for false positive misidentifications (86, 87),
in line with previous studies (36, 37, 88), and with signal detection theory (89).
Additionally, analysis was conducted for total number of retrieved words
without adjustment for false-positives, and for total numbers of false-positive
errors. Two different but congruent versions were presented at the 2 test
sessions to avoid test-retest effects.
To assess potential differences in attention or working memory due to the
intervention, subjects completed trail making tests (TMT) A and B (90), and
forward/backward digit span (WMS-R, 91), before and after the intervention.
No significant differences emerged between pre and post intervention test
sessions (all P0.5).
Statistical Analysis. Before data analysis, normal or near-normal distribution
and homogeneity of variances were tested by the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test
and the Levene’s Test.
To monitor dietary compliance, individual physiological parameters and
serum levels and dietary intake before and after the intervention were
compared by a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVARM) with TIME
(‘‘baseline,’’ ‘‘after intervention period’’), and between factor GROUP (‘‘ca-
loric restriction,’’ ‘‘UFA enhancement,’’ ‘‘control’’), followed by post hoc
paired ttests (2-tailed) as appropriate.
To assess intervention effects, an ANOVARM on the outcome variable
‘‘memory scores’’ was conducted, with TIME and between factor GROUP.
Depending on significance, post hoc paired ttests (two-tailed) were per-
formed as appropriate.
Correlations between changes in memory score and changes in physiolog-
ical parameters and serum levels from baseline to post intervention assess-
ment were assessed using Spearman’s correlation coefficient.
Significance was set at P0.05, all data are presented as mean with
standard error of the mean, unless indicated otherwise.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS. We thank N. Ro¨ sser and C. Willemer for help with
subject recruitment. This work was supported by Deutsche Forschungsge-
meinschaft Grant Fl 379 –4/1 (to A. F.), Interdisziplina¨ res Zentrum fu¨ r Klinische
Forschung Grant Floe/3/004/08 (to A.F.), Bundesministerium fu¨ r Forschung
und Bildung Grant 01GW0520 (to A.F. and S.K.), and Innovative Medizinische
Forschung Mu¨ nster Grant FL110605 (to A. F.).
1. Pressley JC, Trott C, Tang M, Durkin M, Stern Y (2003) Dementia in community-dwelling
elderly patients: A comparison of survey data, medicare claims, cognitive screening,
reported symptoms, and activity limitations. J Clin Epidemiol 56:896–905.
2. Dwyer J (2006) Starting down the right path: Nutrition connections with chronic
diseases of later life. Am J Clin Nutr 83:415S–420S.
3. Martin B, Mattson MP, Maudsley S (2006) Caloric restriction and intermittent fasting:
Two potential diets for successful brain aging. Ageing Res Rev 5:332–353.
4. Mattson MP, Duan W, Wan R, Guo Z (2004) Prophylactic activation of neuroprotective
stress response pathways by dietary and behavioral manipulations. NeuroRx 1:111–
116.
5. Stranahan A, Mattson M (2008) Impact of Energy Intake and Expenditure on Neuronal
Plasticity Neuromol Med, 10.1007/s12017-008-8043-0.
6. Parrott MD, Greenwood CE (2007) Dietary influences on cognitive function with aging:
From high-fat diets to healthful eating. Ann NY Acad Sci 1114:389–397.
7. Gomez-Pinilla F (2008) The influences of diet and exercise on mental health through
hormesis. Ageing Res Rev 7:49– 62.
8. Youssef FF, Ramchandani J, Manswell S, McRae A (2008) Adult-onset calorie restriction
attenuates kainic acid excitotoxicity in the rat hippocampal slice. Neurosci Lett
431:118–122.
9. Ingram DK, Weindruch R, Spangler EL, Freeman JR, Walford RL (1987) Dietary restric-
tion benefits learning and motor performance of aged mice. J Gerontol 42:78– 81.
10. Duan W, Lee J, Guo Z, Mattson MP (2001) Dietary restriction stimulates BDNF produc-
tion in the brain and thereby protects neurons against excitotoxic injury. JMol
Neurosci 16:1–12.
11. Patel NV, et al. (2005) Caloric restriction attenuates Abeta-deposition in Alzheimer
transgenic models. Neurobiol Aging 26:995–1000.
12. Wang J, et al. (2005) Caloric restriction attenuates beta-amyloid neuropathology in a
mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. FASEB J 19:659– 661.
13. Duan W, Mattson MP (1999) Dietary restriction and 2-deoxyglucose administration
improve behavioral outcome and reduce degeneration of dopaminergic neurons in
models of Parkinson’s disease. J Neurosci Res 57:195–206.
14. Bruce-Keller AJ, Umberger G, McFall R, Mattson MP (1999) Food restriction reduces
brain damage and improves behavioral outcome following excitotoxic and metabolic
insults. Ann Neurol 45:8–15.
15. Zhu H, Guo Q, Mattson MP (1999) Dietary restriction protects hippocampal neurons
against the death-promoting action of a presenilin-1 mutation. Brain Res 842:224–229.
16. Maswood N, et al. (2004) Caloric restriction increases neurotrophic factor levels and
attenuates neurochemical and behavioral deficits in a primate model of Parkinson’s
disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101:18171–18176.
17. Stewart J, Mitchell J, Kalant N (1989) The effects of life-long food restriction on spatial
memory in young and aged Fischer 344 rats measured in the 8-arm radial and the
Morris water mazes. Neurobiol Aging 10:669– 675.
18. Fontan-Lozano A, et al. (2007) Caloric restriction increases learning consolidation and
facilitates synaptic plasticity through mechanisms dependent on NR2B subunits of the
NMDA receptor. J Neurosci 27:10185–10195.
19. Willcox BJ, et al. (2007) Caloric restriction, the traditional Okinawan diet, and healthy
aging: The diet of the world’s longest-lived people and its potential impact on
morbidity and life span. Ann NY Acad Sci 1114:434– 455.
20. Knecht S, Ellger T, Levine JA (2008) Obesity in neurobiology. Prog Neurobiol 84:85–103.
21. Wong KL, Murakami K, Routtenberg A (1989) Dietary cis-fatty acids that increase
protein F1 phosphorylation enhance spatial memory. Brain Res 505:302–305.
22. Scarmeas N, Stern Y, Tang MX, Mayeux R, Luchsinger JA (2006) Mediterranean diet and
risk for Alzheimer’s disease. AnnNeurol 59:912–921.
23. van Gelder BM, Tijhuis M, Kalmijn S, Kromhout D (2007) Fish consumption, n-3 fatty
acids, and subsequent 5-y cognitive decline in elderly men: The Zutphen Elderly Study.
Am J Clin Nutr 85:1142–1147.
24. Issa AM, et al. (2006) The efficacy of omega-3 fatty acids on cognitive function in aging
and dementia: A systematic review. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 21:88–96.
25. Panza F, et al. (2004) Mediterranean diet and cognitive decline. Public Health Nutr
7:959–963.
26. Barberger-Gateau P, et al. (2002) Fish, meat, and risk of dementia: Cohort study. BMJ
325:932–933.
27. Morris MC, Evans DA, Tangney CC, Bienias JL, Wilson RS (2005) Fish consumption and
cognitive decline with age in a large community study. Arch Neurol 62:1849–1853.
28. Chiu C-C, et al. (2008) The effects of omega-3 fatty acids monotherapy in Alzheimer’s
disease and mild cognitive impairment: A preliminary randomized double-blind pla-
cebo-controlled study. Progr Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 32:1538–1544.
29. Freund-Levi Y, et al. (2006) Omega-3 fatty acid treatment in 174 patients with mild to
moderate Alzheimer disease: OmegAD study: A randomized double-blind trial. Arch
Neurol 63:1402–1408.
30. Bellush LL, Wright AM, Walker JP, Kopchick J, Colvin RA (1996) Caloric restriction and
spatial learning in old mice. Physiol Behav 60:541–547.
31. Yanai S, Okaichi Y, Okaichi H (2004) Long-term dietary restriction causes negative
effects on cognitive functions in rats. Neurobiol Aging 25:325–332.
32. Matochik JA, et al. (2004) Age-related decline in striatal volume in rhesus monkeys:
Assessment of long-term calorie restriction. Neurobiol Aging 25:193–200.
33. Larrieu S, Letenneur L, Helmer C, Dartigues JF, Barberger-Gateau P (2004) Nutritional
factors and risk of incident dementia in the PAQUID longitudinal cohort. J Nutr Health
Aging 8:150–154.
34. Arendash GW, et al. (2007) A diet high in omega-3 fatty acids does not improve or
protect cognitive performance in Alzheimer’s transgenic mice. Neuroscience 149:286–
302.
35. Blacker D, et al. (2007) Neuropsychological measures in normal individuals that predict
subsequent cognitive decline. Arch Neurol 64:862–871.
Witte et al. PNAS
January 27, 2009
vol. 106
no. 4
1259
NEUROSCIENCE
36. Pereira AC, et al. (2007) An in vivo correlate of exercise-induced neurogenesis in the
adult dentate gyrus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:5638–5643.
37. Floel A, et al. (2008) Lifestyle and Memory in the Elderly. Neuroepidemiology 31:39–
47.
38. Zhao W-Q, Chen H, Quon MJ, Alkon DL (2004) Insulin and the insulin receptor in
experimental models of learning and memory. Eur J Pharmacol 490:71–81.
39. Stranahan AM, et al. (2008) Diet-induced insulin resistance impairs hippocampal
synaptic plasticity and cognition in middle-aged rats Hippocampus 18:1085–1088.
40. Beydoun MA, Beydoun HA, Wang Y (2008) Obesity and central obesity as risk factors
for incident dementia and its subtypes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Obesity
Rev 9:204–218.
41. Fontan-Lozano A, et al. (2007) Caloric restriction increases learning consolidation and
facilitates synaptic plasticity through mechanisms dependent on NR2B subunits of the
NMDA receptor. J Neurosci 27:10185–10195.
42. Solfrizzi V, et al. (2005) Dietary fatty acids intake: Possible role in cognitive decline and
dementia. Exp Gerontol 40:257–270.
43. Yehuda S, Rabinovtz S, Carasso RL, Mostofsky DI (1996) Essential fatty acids preparation
(SR-3) improves Alzheimer’s patients quality of life. Int J Neurosci 87:141–149.
44. Kotani S, et al. (2006) Dietary supplementation of arachidonic and docosahexaenoic
acids improves cognitive dysfunction. Neurosci Res 56:159–164.
45. Kroke A, et al. (1999) Validation of a self-administered food-frequency questionnaire
administered in the European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition
(EPIC) Study: Comparison of energy, protein, and macronutuient intakes estimated
with the doubly labeled water, urinary nitrogen, and repeated 24-h dietary recall
methods. Am J Clin Nutr 70:439– 447.
46. Solfrizzi V, et al. (1999) High monounsaturated fatty acids intake protects against
age-related cognitive decline. Neurology 52:1563–1569.
47. Masoro EJ, McCarter RJ, Katz MS, McMahan CA (1992) Dietary restriction alters
characteristics of glucose fuel use. J Gerontol 47:B202–208.
48. Kemnitz JW, et al. (1994) Dietary restriction increases insulin sensitivity and lowers
blood glucose in rhesus monkeys. Am J Physiol 266:E540–547.
49. Lane MA, et al. (1996) Calorie restriction lowers body temperature in rhesus monkeys,
consistent with a postulated anti-aging mechanism in rodents. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
93:4159– 4164.
50. Cefalu WT, et al. (1997) A study of caloric restriction and cardiovascular aging in
cynomolgus monkeys (Macaca fascicularis): A potential model for aging research. J
Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 52:B10–19.
51. Wycherley TP, Brinkworth GD, Noakes M, Buckley JD, Clifton PM (2008) Effect of caloric
restriction with and without exercise training on oxidative stress and endothelial
function in obese subjects with type 2 diabetes Diabetes Obesity Metabol 10:1062–
1073.
52. Craft S (2005) Insulin resistance syndrome and Alzheimer’s disease: Age- and obesity-
related effects on memory, amyloid, and inflammation. Neurobiol Aging 26:65–69.
53. Baura GD, et al. (1996) Insulin transport from plasma into the central nervous system
is inhibited by dexamethasone in dogs. Diabetes 45:86–90.
54. Cole GM, Frautschy SA (2007) The role of insulin and neurotrophic factor signaling in
brain aging and Alzheimer’s Disease. Exp Gerontol 42:10–21.
55. Taguchi A, White MF (2008) Insulin-like signaling, nutrient homeostasis, and life span.
Annu Rev Physiol 70:191–212.
56. McNay EC (2007) Insulin and ghrelin: Peripheral hormones modulating memory and
hippocampal function. Curr Opin Pharmacol 7:628– 632.
57. Christie JM, Wenthold RJ, Monaghan DT (1999) Insulin causes a transient tyrosine
phosphorylation of NR2A and NR2B NMDA receptor subunits in rat hippocampus.
J Neurochem 72:1523–1528.
58. Skeberdis VA, Lan J, Zheng X, Zukin RS, Bennett MV (2001) Insulin promotes rapid
delivery of N-methyl-D- aspartate receptors to the cell surface by exocytosis. Proc Natl
Acad Sci USA 98:3561–3566.
59. Nakazawa K, et al. (2002) Requirement for hippocampal CA3 NMDA receptors in
associative memory recall. Science 297:211–218.
60. Mattson MP (2000) Neuroprotective signaling and the aging brain: Take away my food
and let me run. Brain Res 886:47–53.
61. Connor B, Dragunow M (1998) The role of neuronal growth factors in neurodegen-
erative disorders of the human brain. Brain Res Rev 27:1–39.
62. Kitamura S, et al. (2003) Ghrelin concentration in cord and neonatal blood: Relation to
fetal growth and energy balance. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 88:5473–5477.
63. Niedernhofer LJ, et al. (2006) A new progeroid syndrome reveals that genotoxic stress
suppresses the somatotroph axis. Nature 444:1038–1043.
64. Lee J, Seroogy KB, Mattson MP (2002) Dietary restriction enhances neurogenesis and
up-regulates neurotrophin expression in the hippocampus of adult mice. J Neurochem
80:539–547.
65. Carro E, Torres-Aleman I (2006) Serum insulin-like growth factor I in brain function.
Keio J Med 55:59– 63.
66. Pan W, Banks WA, Fasold MB, Bluth J, Kastin AJ (1998) Transport of brain-derived
neurotrophic factor across the blood-brain barrier. Neuropharmacology 37:1553–
1561.
67. Kalani R, Judge S, Carter C, Pahor M, Leeuwenburgh C (2006) Effects of caloric
restriction and exercise on age-related, chronic inflammation assessed by C-reactive
protein and interleukin-6. J Gerontol Ser A 61:211–217.
68. Johnson JB, et al. (2007) Alternate day calorie restriction improves clinical findings and
reduces markers of oxidative stress and inflammation in overweight adults with
moderate asthma. Free Radical Biol Med 42:665–674.
69. Jung SH, et al. (2008) Effect of weight loss on some serum cytokines in human obesity:
Increase in IL-10 after weight loss. The J Nutr Biochem 19:371–375.
70. Dik MG, et al. (2007) Contribution of metabolic syndrome components to cognition in
older individuals. Diabetes Care 30:2655–2660.
71. Yaffe K, et al. (2003) Inflammatory markers and cognition in well-functioning African-
American and white elders. Neurology 61:76– 80.
72. Teunissen CE, et al. (2003) Inflammation markers in relation to cognition in a healthy
aging population. J Neuroimmunol 134:142–150.
73. Feinstein R, Kanety H, Papa MZ, Lunenfeld B, Karasik A (1993) Tumor necrosis factor-
alpha suppresses insulin-induced tyrosine phosphorylation of insulin receptor and its
substrates. J Biol Chem 268:26055–26058.
74. Stephens JM, Lee J, Pilch PF (1997) Tumor necrosis factor-alpha-induced insulin resis-
tance in 3T3–L1 adipocytes is accompanied by a loss of insulin receptor substrate-1 and
GLUT4 expression without a loss of insulin receptor-mediated signal transduction.
J Biol Chem 272:971–976.
75. Adair G (1984) The Hawthorne effect: A reconsideration of the methodological arti-
fact. J Appl Psychol 69:334–335.
76. Folstein M (1998) Mini-Mental and son. Int J Geriatric Psychiatry 13:290–294.
77. Hautzinger M (1991) The Beck Depression Inventory in clinical practice. [Der Beck-
Depressionsinventar (BDI) in der Klinik] Nervenarzt 62:689– 696.
78. Laux L, Glanzmann P, Schaffner P, Spielberger CD (1981) The State-Trait-Angst Inven-
tory. Theoretical Backgrounds and Manual. [Das State-Trait-Angst Inventar Theore-
tischer Hintergrund und Manual] (Beltz Test, Weinheim, Germany).
79. Goodwin JS, Goodwin JM, Garry PJ (1983) Association Between Nutritional-Status and
Cognitive-Functioning in A Healthy Elderly Population. J Am Med Assoc 249:2917–
2921.
80. Balk EM, et al. (2006) Effects of omega-3 fatty acids on serum markers of cardiovascular
disease risk: A systematic review. Atherosclerosis 189:19–30.
81. Harris WS (1997) n-3 fatty acids and serum lipoproteins: Human studies Am J Clin Nutr
65:1645S–1654S.
82. Walford RL, Mock D, MacCallum T, Laseter JL (1999) Physiologic changes in humans
subjected to severe, selective calorie restriction for two years in biosphere 2: Health,
aging, and toxicological perspectives. Toxicol Sci 52:61–65.
83. Heilbronn LK, et al. (2006) Effect of 6-month calorie restriction on biomarkers of
longevity, metabolic adaptation, and oxidative stress in overweight individuals—A
randomized controlled trial. J Am Med Assoc 295:1539–1548.
84. Brownell KD, Cohen LR (1995) Adherence to dietary regimens. 2: Components of
effective interventions Behav Med (Washington, DC) 20:155–164.
85. Maycock PF, Frayn KN (1987) Use of alumina columns to prepare plasma samples for
liquid-chromatographic determination of catecholamines. Clin Chem 33:286–287.
86. Helmstaedter C, Kurthen M (2001) Memory and epilepsy: Characteristics, course, and
influence of drugs and surgery. Curr Opin Neurol 14:211–216.
87. Strauss E, Sherman EM, Spreen O (2006) A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests.
Administration, Norms, and Commentary (Oxford Univ Press, Oxford, UK).
88. Knecht S, et al. (2008) High-normal blood pressure is associated with poor cognitive
performance. Hypertension 51:663–668.
89. Hochhaus L (1972) A table for the calculation of dand Beta. Psychol Bull 77:375–376.
90. Reitan RM, Herring S (1985) A short screening device for identification of cerebral
dysfunction in children. J Clin Psychol 41:643–650.
91. Markowitsch HJ, Harting C (1996) Interdependence of priming performance and
brain-damage. Int J Neurosci 85:291–300.
1260
www.pnas.orgcgidoi10.1073pnas.0808587106 Witte et al.
... Similarly, a two-year study in healthy elderly volunteers has found that long-term CR did not adversely affect cognition and was effective in improving memory, which might be related to lower protein intake, as protein restriction reduced protein oxidation in the brain and improved memory [15]. A threemonth CR has also been shown to improve memory in healthy older adults, where increased insulin sensitivity and decreased inflammatory activity may be mediators for increased synaptic plasticity in the brain and stimulation of neuroprotective pathways to maintain the cognitive health of older adults [16]. ...
... For normal adults, CR does not negatively affect their cognitive function, and even has a slight positive effect on generating [15]. In contrast, in older adults, CR can provide a more significant improvement in their cognitive performance, the mechanism of which may be related to higher synaptic plasticity and stimulation of neural facilitatory pathways in the brain [16]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose of Review The impact of dietary habits on cognitive function is increasingly gaining attention. The review is to discuss how caloric restriction (CR) and intermittent fasting (IF) can enhance cognitive function in healthy states through multiple pathways that interact with one another. Secondly, to explore the effects of CR and IF on cognitive function in conditions of neurodegenerative diseases, obesity diabetes and aging, as well as potential synergistic effects in combination with exercise to prevent cognitively related neurodegenerative diseases. Recent Findings With age, the human brain ages and develops corresponding neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer's disease, Parkinson's disease, and epilepsy, which in turn trigger cognitive impairment. Recent research indicates that the impact of diet and exercise on cognitive function is increasingly gaining attention. Summary The benefits of exercise for cognitive function and brain plasticity are numerous, and future research can examine the efficacy of particular dietary regimens during physical activity when combined with diet which can prevent cognitive decline.
... Aging, accompanied by a myriad of conditions, such as dementia, cancer, cardiovascular disease, hypertension, arthritis, cataracts, osteoporosis, diabetes, MASLD, and AD, poses a signi cant health challenge [22]. The advantages of calorie restriction (CR) and ketogenic diet in prolonging lifespan and postponing agerelated disease onset have been well documented across various species [23,24]. Circulating BHB is recognized as an anti-aging metabolite because its levels surge during CR and ketogenic diets [25]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background: Recently, cognitive impairment has emerged as a consequential aspect of metabolic dysfunction-associated steatotic liver disease (MASLD), prompting speculation that diminished β-Hydroxybutyrate (BHB) production might be intricately involved in its pathogenesis, along with that of cognitive decline. However, the intricate interplay between the circulating BHB levels and these conditions remains unclear. Methods: A BHB ELISA assay kit was used to quantify serum BHB concentrations in both healthy individuals and patients with MASLD or cognitive impairment. Pearson’s correlation analysis assessed associations between BHB levels and pertinent clinical parameters. The diagnostic utility of serum BHB levels in identifying MASLD and cognitive impairment was assessed using Receiver Operating Characteristic (ROC) curves. Results: The findings revealed a stark contrast: patients with MASLD and cognitive impairment exhibited notably lower serum BHB levels than healthy individuals. Furthermore, BHB levels were inversely associated with BMI (r = -0.407, P < 0.001), AST (r = -0.233, P = 0.044), TC (r = -0.266, P = 0.021), LDL-C (r = -0.236, P = 0.046), and HbA1c (r = -0.416, P < 0.001), while demonstrating a positively relationship with cognitive performance, as indicated by Mini-Mental State Examination (r = 0.434, P < 0.001) and Montreal Cognitive Assessment scores (r = 0.433, P < 0.001). Notably, ROC curve analysis underscored potential diagnostic significance of serum BHB in both MASLD and cognitive impairment. Intriguingly, supplementary research illuminated a negative correlation between serum BHB levels and age among healthy individuals (r = -0.775, P < 0.001). Conclusion: This work implies that a decrease on circulating BHB levels may lead to the occurrence of MASLD and cognitive impairment.
... All these processes contribute to the prevention of age-related diseases [106]. CR helped to improve memory in older individuals, and aging biomarkers were improved by FMD (food and mouth disease) cycles [107][108][109]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This review article investigates the intricate relationship between nutrigenomics and neurological disorders, highlighting how genetic variations affect an individual’s response to nutrients. The study delves into the role of diet-related oxidative stress and the gut-brain axis in the progression and management of brain disorders such as Parkinson’s disease, Alzheimer’s disease, epilepsy, stroke, migraines, and depression. The review encompasses various clinical trials and introduces new trends and techniques, including omics and artificial intelligence, in identifying and managing neurological disorders. The main findings emphasize that personalized diet recommendations, tailored to an individual’s genetic makeup, can significantly improve cognitive health and manage neurological conditions. The study concludes that further research in the field of nutrigenomics is essential to advancing personalized nutrition strategies for better neurological functioning, ultimately linking diet, genes, and brain health.
... p<0.001) was observed after 3 months of a calorie-restricted diet by 30% in healthy individuals, which was correlated with decreased insulin and CRP levels [45]. Similar results were obtained by Leclerc et al., who demonstrated greater improvement in working memory among healthy individuals following a 25% calorie-restricted diet for 2 years compared to the control group on an ad libitum diet [46]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction and purpose: In the face of an aging population, the increasing number of elderly individuals raises the incidence of age-related conditions, including Alzheimer's disease (AD), which is a leading cause of global disability and a significant burden on society. The lack of effective treatments for AD underscores the importance of prevention. Recent reports suggest that intermittent fasting (IF) may counteract the disease processes associated with AD and serve as a potential preventive strategy. This review aims to outline the impact of IF on the risk of developing Alzheimer's disease.Materials and methods: A literature search was conducted using the medical databases PubMed and Google Scholar. Articles were retrieved in English, employing the keywords: “Alzheimer’s disease”, “dementia”, “intermittent fasting”, “ketone bodies”, “cognition”.State of knowledge: IF is a dietary regimen involving cyclic restriction of food intake, practiced in many cultures and religions. The interest in IF has increased due to its numerous health benefits, and recent studies indicate its potential in delaying and preventing pathological processes associated with AD, such as β-amyloid accumulation, neuroinflammation, and vascular damage, making IF a potentially protective intervention against neurodegeneration.Summary: IF is a promising strategy for improving cognitive function and brain health. Due to the limited number of studies conducted on humans, further research is needed to confirm its effectiveness in preventing AD.
... CR slows the development of AD in multiple mouse models [4][5][6][7] , as well as in squirrel monkeys 8 . While the impact of CR on AD in humans is mostly unknown, short-term CR can improve memory 9 . As few humans are prepared to engage in long-term CR, there is substantial interest in understanding the mechanisms underlying CR to harness its benefits without the need to subject individuals to an abstemious dietary regimen. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Caloric restriction (CR) is a widely recognized geroprotective intervention that slows or prevents Alzheimer's disease (AD) in animal models. CR is typically implemented via feeding mice a single meal per day; as CR mice rapidly consume their food, they are subject to a prolonged fast between meals. While CR has been shown to improve metabolic and cognitive functions and suppress pathological markers in AD mouse models, the specific contributions of fasting versus calorie reduction remains unclear. Here, we investigated the contribution of fasting and energy restriction to the beneficial effects of CR on AD progression. To test this, we placed 6-month-old 3xTg mice on one of several diet regimens, allowing us to dissect the effects of calories and fasting on metabolism, AD pathology, and cognition. We find that energy restriction alone, without fasting, was sufficient to improve glucose tolerance and reduce adiposity in both sexes, and to reduce Aβ plaques and improve aspects of cognitive performance in females. However, we find that a prolonged fast between meals is necessary for many of the benefits of CR, including improved insulin sensitivity, reduced phosphorylation of tau, decreased neuroinflammation, inhibition of mTORC1 signaling, and activation of autophagy, as well as for the full cognitive benefits of CR. Finally, we find that fasting is essential for the benefits of CR on survival in male 3xTg mice. Overall, our results demonstrate that fasting is required for the full benefits of a CR diet on the development and progression of AD in 3xTg mice, and suggest that both when and how much we eat influences the development and progress of AD.
Article
Background Waist Hip Ratio (WHR), a measure of abdominal obesity is being co-related with memory consolidation based on its relation with stress and sleep. The objective of the present cross-sectional study was to evaluate the association between WHR and memory consolidation in adolescent medical undergraduates. Methodology An analytical cross-sectional study was conducted in a South Indian medical college. After obtaining ethics clearance, the study was conducted on 42 males and 63 females of 17–19 years of age. WHR was measured according to WHO guidelines using Gulick 2 plus inch tape. Sleep-dependent memory consolidation test was conducted in evening 3:00 pm followed by next morning 9:00 am. WHR between genders was compared using Student t test. The study population was divided into obese and non obese individuals, based on the calculated WHR. Then, the mean memory consolidation test scores were compared using Mann Whitney U test. The correlation between WHR and memory consolidation test scores was evaluated using Pearson’s correlation test. Results & Discussion The average WHR of males (0.845 ± 0.058) was significantly higher than females (0.778 ± 0.051) ( P <0.001). The morning session memory consolidation test scores were higher in the normal group when compared to the substantially increased risk for obesity groups, although not statistically significant. WHR showed significant negative correlation with sleep dependent memory experiment scores. Conclusion The relationship between WHR and memory consolidation in the present study highlights the need for an awareness to adopt life style modifications which might not only benefit the general health status but also might have an impact on their memory consolidation and academic performance.
Article
Full-text available
Tumor necrosis factor-alpha (TNF) has recently been shown to induce insulin resistance. We have examined the possible effect of TNF on the early events in insulin transmembrane signaling. Incubation of the insulin-sensitive rat hepatoma Fao cells with 5 nM TNF for 1 h led to a 65% decrease in insulin-induced tyrosine phosphorylation of both the insulin receptor beta-subunit and IPS-1, its major cytosolic substrate. TNF-induced impairment of tyrosine phosphorylation was maximal at 0.5 nM and was not accompanied by any reduction in insulin binding. Sixteen hours of TNF incubation led to further impairment in insulin-induced tyrosine phosphorylation of these proteins. Our findings suggest that TNF may exert its anti-insulin effect by interrupting the early insulin-stimulated tyrosine phosphorylation events, which are crucial to insulin transmembrane signature.
Article
Full-text available
In a number of previous reports we showed the salutary effects on rats of SR-3, a compound comprising a 1:4 ratio of n-3 and n-6 fatty acids. Improvements were noted in learning tasks, thermoregulation, recovery from neurotoxins, and seizure protection. Because we were impressed that these effects are related to changes in membrane fluidity and neuronal functioning and because Alzheimer's Disease is also associated with lipid defects, we undertook a short term (4 week) double blind study with 100 Alzheimer patients (60 received SR-3 and 40 in a placebo control). The results indicated improvements in mood, cooperation, appetite, sleep, ability to navigate in the home, and short term memory. Overall improvement was reported for 49 patients, and in no case did a guardian report adverse effects to the compound. While not uniform or permanent, and while no mode of action for SR-3 can be precisely identified at this time, the promising results in quality of life for the patient and caregiver warrant further clinical trials and continued basic research into the neuropsychological substrate of the disease and its response to SR-3.
Article
Full-text available
The methodology of signal detection theory is a preferred technique for assessing an S's ability to discriminate the occurrence of discrete binary events. A compact table is presented which will permit precise calculation of signal detection parameters, d' (discrimination index) and BETA (response bias), for hit and false alarm rate combinations in the range from .01-.99. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Parkinson's disease (PD) is an age-related disorder characterized by progressive degeneration of dopaminergic neurons in the substantia nigra (SN) and corresponding motor deficits. Oxidative stress and mitochondrial dysfunction are implicated in the neurodegenerative process in PD. Although dietary restriction (DR) extends lifespan and reduces levels of cellular oxidative stress in several different organ systems, the impact of DR on age-related neurodegenerative disorders is unknown. We report that DR in adult mice results in resistance of dopaminergic neurons in the SN to the toxicity of 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine (MPTP). MPTP-induced loss of dopaminergic neurons and deficits in motor function were ameliorated in DR rats. To mimic the beneficial effect of DR on dopaminergic neurons, we administered 2-deoxy-D-glucose (2-DG; a nonmetabolizable analogue of glucose) to mice fed ad libitum. Mice receiving 2-DG exhibited reduced damage to dopaminergic neurons in the SN and improved behavioral outcome following MPTP treatment. The 2-DG treatment suppressed oxidative stress, preserved mitochondrial function, and attenuated cell death in cultured dopaminergic cells exposed to the complex I inhibitor rotenone or Fe²⁺. 2-DG and DR induced expression of the stress proteins heat-shock protein 70 and glucose-regulated protein 78 in dopaminergic cells, suggesting involvement of these cytoprotective proteins in the neuroprotective actions of 2-DG and DR. The striking beneficial effects of DR and 2-DG in models of PD, when considered in light of recent epidemiological data, suggest that DR may prove beneficial in reducing the incidence of PD in humans. J. Neurosci. Res. 57:195–206, 1999. © 1999 Wiley-Liss, Inc.
Article
Biosphere 2 is a closed ecological space of 7-million cubic feet near Tucson, AZ, containing 7 biomes: rain forest, Savannah, ocean, marsh, desert, agricultural station, and habitat for humans and domestic animals. Sealed inside, 4 men and 4 women maintained themselves and the various systems for 2 years. All organic material, all water, and nearly all air was recycled, and virtually all food was grown inside. On the low calorie but nutrient-dense diet available, the men sustained 18% and the women 10% weight loss, mostly within the first 6 to 9 months. The nature of the diet duplicated rodent diets that had been shown to enhance health, lower disease incidence, and retard aging. Using blood specimens frozen at different points during and after the 2 years, determinations were made of a number of biochemical parameters judged to be pertinent based on past studies of rodents and monkeys on similar diets. These included blood lipids, glucose, insulin, glycosylated hemoglobin, renin, and others. The results clearly suggest that humans react to such a nutritional regime similarly to other vertebrates. In addition to these studies, and because this was a tightly closed, isolated environment, the levels of insecticides or pollutants or their derivatives were determined in the sera of 2 crew members. It was found that levels of the lipophilic toxicant DDE and the “total PCB” load increased with the loss of body fat during the first 12‐18 months inside Biosphere 2, then decreased.
Article
We evaluated the association between nutritional status and cognitive functioning in 260 noninstitutionalized men and women older than 60 years who had no known physical illnesses and were receiving no medications. Nutritional status was evaluated by three-day food records and also by biochemical determination of blood levels of specific nutrients. Cognitive status was evaluated by the Halstead-Reitan Categories Test (a nonverbal test of abstract thinking ability) and by the Wechsler Memory Test. Subjects with low blood levels of vitamins C or B12 scored worse on both tests. Subjects with low levels of riboflavin or folic acid scored worse on the categories test. These differences remained significant after controlling for age, gender, level of income, and amount of education. "Subclinical" malnutrition may play a small role in the depression of cognitive function detectable in some elderly individuals, or depressed cognitive function may result in reduced nutrient intake. (JAMA 1983;249:2917-2921)
Article
Dietary influences on Alzheimer disease (AD) are gaining recognition. Because many aging processes are attenuated in laboratory mammals by caloric restriction (CR), we examined the effects of short-term CR in two AD-transgenic mice, APPswe/ind (J20) and APPswe+PS1M146L (APP+PS1). CR substantially decreased the accumulation of Aβ-plaques in both lines: by 40% in APPswe/ind (CR, 6 weeks), and by 55% in APP+PS1 (CR, 14 weeks). CR also decreased astrocytic activation (GFAP immunoreactivity). These influences of CR on AD-transgenic mice are consistent with epidemiological reports that show that high caloric diets associate with the risk of AD, and suggest that dietary interventions in adult life might slow disease progression.
Article
Parkinson's disease (PD) is an age-related disorder characterized by progressive degeneration of dopaminergic neurons in the substantia nigra (SN) and corresponding motor deficits. Oxidative stress and mitochondrial dysfunction are implicated in the neurodegenerative process in PD. Although dietary restriction (DR) extends lifespan and reduces levels of cellular oxidative stress in several different organ systems, the impact of DR on age-related neurodegenerative disorders is unknown. We report that DR in adult mice results in resistance of dopaminergic neurons in the SN to the toxicity of 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine (MPTP). MPTP-induced loss of dopaminergic neurons and deficits in motor function were ameliorated in DR rats. To mimic the beneficial effect of DR on dopaminergic neurons, we administered 2-deoxy-D-glucose (2-DG; a nonmetabolizable analogue of glucose) to mice fed ad libitum. Mice receiving 2-DG exhibited reduced damage to dopaminergic neurons in the SN and improved behavioral outcome following MPTP treatment. The 2-DG treatment suppressed oxidative stress, preserved mitochondrial function, and attenuated cell death in cultured dopaminergic cells exposed to the complex I inhibitor rotenone or Fe2+. 2-DG and DR induced expression of the stress proteins heat-shock protein 70 and glucose-regulated protein 78 in dopaminergic cells, suggesting involvement of these cytoprotective proteins in the neuroprotective actions of 2-DG and DR. The striking beneficial effects of DR and 2-DG in models of PD, when considered in light of recent epidemiological data, suggest that DR may prove beneficial in reducing the incidence of PD in humans. J. Neurosci. Res. 57:195–206, 1999. © 1999 Wiley-Liss, Inc.