ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

The purpose of this study was to examine whether mental fatigue influences the perceived effort required to complete fairly light and hard effort self-paced exercise challenges. 12 partici- pants completed 2 trials in a randomised cross- over design. Each participant was required to complete a time-matched pre-exercise task: 1) a continuous cognitive activity test (EXP con- dition; n=12), or 2) a time-matched passive neutral observation task (CON condition; n = 12). Following the pre-exercise task, participants performed 2 consecutive bouts of self-paced cycling exercise again in randomized order at fairly light (RPE 11) and hard (RPE 15) effort. Physiological, psychological and EEG indices were measured throughout both conditions. EXP participants reported significantly greater sen- sations of fatigue (p < 0.01) and demonstrated greater EEG beta-band activation compared with CON (p<0.01) prior to exercise. Power out- puts from the exercise bouts were significantly reduced for EXP in both self-paced: RPE 11 (83 ± 7 vs. 99 ± 7 W; p = 0.005) and RPE 15 (132 ± 9 vs. 143 ± 8 W; p = 0.028) trials. This study demon- strates that individuals with higher self-reported sensations of fatigue and elevations of EEG beta activity in the prefrontal cortex of the brain prior to exercise produce less work during self-paced exercise trials than in a control condition, prob- ably due to an altered perception of effort.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Personal pdf le for
www.thieme.de
For personal use only.
No commercial use, no depositing in repositories.
With compliments of Georg Thieme Verlag
Publisher and Copyright
© 2013 by
Georg Thieme Verlag KG
Rüdigerstraße 14
70469 Stuttgart
ISSN
Reprint with the
permission by
the publisher only
J. Brownsberger, A. Edwards, R. Crowther, D. Cottrell
Impact of Mental Fatigue on
Self-paced Exercise
Int J Sports Med
DOI 10.1055/s-0033-1343402
0172-4622
Physiology & Biochemistry
Brownsberger J et al. Mental Fatigue and E ort Perception … Int J Sports Med
accepted after revision
March 04 , 2013
Bibliography
DOI http://dx.doi.org/
10.1055/s-0033-1343402
Published online: 2013
Int J Sports Med
© Georg Thieme
Verlag KG Stuttgart · New York
ISSN 0172-4622
Correspondence
Dr. Andrew Edwards
Institute of Sport & Exercise
Sciences
James Cook University
McGregor Road
4870 Cairns
Australia
Tel.: + 61/7/4042 1653
Fax: + 61/4/4942 1590
andrew.edwards@jcu.edu.au
Key words
mental fatigue
e ort perception
brain regulation
pacing
EEG
Impact of Mental Fatigue on Self-paced Exercise
[ 3 ] rather than the brain [ 25 ] . However, recent
studies [ 16 , 26 , 28 ] have suggested that the brain
regulates exercise performance based on a erent
information from peripheral physiologic recep-
tors that give rise to sensations (e. g. nausea and
thirst among others). This model is supported by
the observation that the brain does not fully
recruit all motor units in the target musculature
even when exercise is performed maximally [ 28 ] ,
suggesting that the brain operates to regulate
physical performance in a process informed by
a erent-e erent neuromuscular signalling.
Although a recent study demonstrated that sen-
sations of mental fatigue impeded maximal
endurance performance [ 21 ] , the mechanisms
responsible for this e ect have not yet been fully
elucidated. It has been suggested that a state of
mental tiredness increases the perceived
demands of a physical task [ 20 ] , which could
explain why individuals tend to perceive physical
tasks to require greater e ort later in the day [ 2 ] ,
why athletes often attribute poor performance to
mental tiredness [ 4 ] and why military pilots are
reported to experience greater risk when men-
tally fatigued [ 14 ] . Similarly, the challenge of
Introduction
Mental fatigue has recently been demonstrated
to negatively in uence endurance performance
[ 21 ] and it has been suggested that this nding
may be due to an alteration in the perception of
e ort when individuals are mentally tired [ 20 ] .
However, no studies have yet examined whether
or not mental fatigue in uences the voluntary
(self-paced or self-regulated) e ort applied to
exercise challenges, an observation which could
explain the previously observed impairment to
endurance performance [ 21 ] .
It is well known that mental fatigue represents
sensations of tiredness experienced during or
after prolonged periods of cognitive activity
[ 5 , 18 ] . However, as sensations of tiredness often
in uence decision making, attention, motivation
and the voluntary willingness to resist fatigue
[ 5 ] , it is surprising that studies have rarely con-
sidered the impact of mental fatigue on physical
performance. It is possible the lack of research in
this area is attributable to most researchers sup-
posing that fatigue is a physiological phenome-
non caused by de ciencies within local muscles
Authors J. Brownsberger
1 , A. Edwards
1 , R. Crowther
1 , D. Cottrell
2
A liations
1 Institute of Sport & Exercise Science, James Cook University, Cairns, Australia
2 Psychology, James Cook University, Cairns, Australia
Abstract
The purpose of this study was to examine
whether mental fatigue in uences the perceived
e ort required to complete fairly light and hard
e ort self-paced exercise challenges. 12 partici-
pants completed 2 trials in a randomised cross-
over design. Each participant was required to
complete a time-matched pre-exercise task:
1) a continuous cognitive activity test (EXP con-
dition; n = 12), or 2) a time-matched passive
neutral observation task (CON condition; n = 12).
Following the pre-exercise task, participants
performed 2 consecutive bouts of self-paced
cycling exercise again in randomized order at
fairly light (RPE 11) and hard (RPE 15) e ort.
Physiological, psychological and EEG indices
were measured throughout both conditions. EXP
participants reported signi cantly greater sen-
sations of fatigue ( p < 0.01) and demonstrated
greater EEG beta-band activation compared
with CON ( p < 0.01) prior to exercise. Power out-
puts from the exercise bouts were signi cantly
reduced for EXP in both self-paced: RPE 11
(83 ± 7 vs. 99 ± 7 W; p = 0.005) and RPE 15 (132 ± 9
vs. 143 ± 8 W; p = 0.028) trials. This study demon-
strates that individuals with higher self-reported
sensations of fatigue and elevations of EEG beta
activity in the prefrontal cortex of the brain prior
to exercise produce less work during self-paced
exercise trials than in a control condition, prob-
ably due to an altered perception of e ort.
Physiology & Biochemistry
Brownsberger J et al. Mental Fatigue and E ort Perception … Int J Sports Med
exercise appears to be perceived with greater severity when
individuals experience sensations consistent with mental
fatigue [ 10 , 12 ] . It is consequently plausible that to avoid over-
exertion, a central neural mechanism evokes a voluntary reduc-
tion in self-selected power outputs as a coping mechanism when
individuals are free to self-regulate exercise intensity [ 12 , 26 ] . As
such, reduced power output due to an altered perception of
e ort could explain why mental fatigue has been reported to
impede maximal endurance performance [ 21 ] .
The combined use of electroencephalogram (EEG) and routine
laboratory performance monitoring systems are rarely utilised
in exercise trials, usually due to the inherent di culties of sig-
nal-to-noise issues in EEG measurement during exercise [ 8 ] .
However, due to recent advances in EEG technologies, it is now
possible that EEG data might reveal systematic di erences in
electro cortical activity between experimental exercise condi-
tions [ 27 ] such as those with or without prior mental fatigue
[ 5 , 20 ] . However, to our knowledge, no studies have yet meas-
ured both physical performance and EEG outputs in the context
of mental fatigue.
Mental tiredness is often self-reported by athletes during train-
ing and by professionals working extended shift hours [ 2 , 30 ] . To
replicate these sensations experimentally, a protocol of sus-
tained vigilance and concentration for 1–3 h is usually employed
[ 5 ] . Such prolonged cognitive activity requires heightened con-
centration and, consequently, is likely to produce elevated beta
wave (13–30 Hz) electrocortical activity in the frontal region of
the brain. Elevated beta wave activity also results from sustained
emotional, attentional and cognitive processes [ 5 , 23 , 24 ] and
this is likely to be responsible for evoking subsequent sensations
of mental fatigue.
The aims of this investigation were to examine 1) whether or not
a condition of elevated mental fatigue in uences the power out-
put in fairly light and hard e ort self-paced exercise bouts and
2) to examine selected neural indices (alpha and beta waves) of
electro cortical activity in the pertinent brain region most in u-
ential in decision-making (i. e., prefrontal cortex: EEG electrode
location F3) [ 23 , 24 ] . A reduction in power output during self-
paced exercise and di erences in electro cortical neural activa-
tion as a consequence of mental fatigue could provide useful
mechanistic insights to previous observations of impaired
endurance performance [ 21 ] .
Materials and Methods
Participants
12 eligible participants [8 male and 4 female; mean ± SD, age
24 ± 5 yr, height 171 ± 9 cm, weight 71 ± 15 kg, peak oxygen
uptake (V
˙O
2 peak) 56 ± 6 ml · kg 1 · min 1 ) were given written
instructions describing all procedures related to the study and
each provided informed consent prior to participation. The
study complies with the established ethical standards for sports
medicine [ 15 ] and was approved by the Research and Ethics
Committee of the University.
All participants were regular exercisers, free of medications (e. g.
antidepressants, antipsychotics) and free of known disease (e. g.
epilepsy, clinical depression) that may in uence exercise toler-
ance and/or psychological responses [ 21 ] . Each participant com-
pleted all testing sessions over a 4-week period with a 48-h
minimum recovery period between visits to the laboratory.
Experimental design
All participants performed an initial incremental test on a labo-
ratory cycle ergometer (Velotron Dyna t Pro, RacerMate Inc.,
USA) to assess V
˙O
2 peak with expired air analysis (ML206 Gas
Analyser, AD Instruments, USA). The data acquisition system uti-
lised was a PowerLab 8/35 (AD Instruments, USA). 2 familiariza-
tion sessions were subsequently completed for initial trials of
the cognitively demanding mental fatigue protocol and also to
assess participants’ aptitude to self-pace an exercise bout [ 11 ]
while cycling at a self-regulated pace corresponding to descrip-
tors from Borg’s 6-20 Rating of Perceived Exertion (RPE) scale
[ 6 , 7 , 9 ] . Coe cient of variation analysis for test-retest reliability
of power output (W) across 3 familiarisation bouts (RPE 7
CV = 8 %; RPE 13, CV = 5 %; RPE 18, C V = 4 %) demonstrated the
ability of participants to consistently replicate similar perform-
ance outcomes when placed in the same condition [ 7 ] .
Following completion of the familiarization sessions, all (n = 12)
participants completed the 2 trials (EXP and CON conditions) in
an individually randomized order. 48 h prior to each session,
participants were given written instructions to drink 3.5 ml of
water per kg of body weight, sleep for at least 7 h, refrain from
the consumption of alcohol and avoid any vigorous exercise the
day before each visit [ 11 , 17 ] . All testing was conducted in the
morning 2 h after breakfast, for which participants were
instructed to maintain habitual practice, and assessments were
subsequently made of capillary blood glucose concentration to
ensure that glucose was in homeostasis at the start of each trial.
The 2 trials required the participants to complete a 90-min pre-
exercise routine of either 1) a continuous cognitive activity
(CCA) task designed to induce mental fatigue (EXP condition) or
2) a passive neutral observation (PNO) procedure (CON condi-
tion) (20), prior to a 3-min warm up and 2 consecutive self-
paced 10-min bouts of cycling exercise. The two 10-min cycling
bouts were completed at self-selected intensities representative
of fairly light e ort (RPE 11 – Fairly Light) and hard e ort (RPE
15 – Hard). Participants were able to self-adjust power output
throughout each exercise bout to sustain the required RPE. An
overview of the main trials is presented in
Table 1 . RPE inten-
sities for the 2 trials were deliberately di erent from those used
in the familiarization sessions, as the purpose of familiarisation
was to introduce the novel concept of pacing exercise bouts in
accordance with transient sensations of e ort and not exercising
at a pre-assigned power output. The participants were not
instructed as to the speci c purpose or expected outcome of the
experiment and were encouraged to consider performance for
each component of the protocol as it arose.
Table 1 Each condition was time matched and consisted of pre-exercise
and exercise phases. Both conditions were followed by two 10 min self-paced
exercise bouts of RPE of 11 (Fairly Light) and RPE 15 (Hard). The 2 exercise
bouts were performed in a randomized order. All participants (N = 12) per-
formed both conditions.
90 min
(Experimental
(EXP) or Control
(CON) conditions)
3 min
Warm up
10 min
Bout 1
(RPE 11 or 15)
10 min
Bout 2
(RPE 11 or 15)
PRE-EXERCISE
PHASE EXERCISE PHASE
Physiology & Biochemistry
Brownsberger J et al. Mental Fatigue and E ort Perception … Int J Sports Med
Experimental and control conditions
The CCA protocol was designed to induce mental fatigue in
accordance with earlier experiments [ 5 , 21 ] . Brie y, the partici-
pants were seated in a dimly lit, sound-attenuated room in a
thermo neutral environment (between 20–22 °C, 55 % RH),
0.70 m from a 17 inch PC monitor. In this task, sequences of let-
ters were presented on the computer monitor and participants
were requested to respond to selected letters on a computer key-
board and maintain task vigilance for 90 min. To further promote
the retention of task vigilance across the 90-min period, a $100
cash prize was advertised for the most vigilant participant.
For the PNO protocol in the CON condition, participants watched
2 documentary programmes “World Class Trains – The Al Anda-
lus Express” and “World Class Trains – The Venice Simplon Ori-
ent Express”; Pegasus-Eagle Rock Entertainment, 2004) as a
continuous 90-min sequence under the same viewing condi-
tions as the EXP condition [ 21 ] .
Performance outcomes
Mean power output was recorded from a cycle ergometer for all
RPE exercise bouts in all 5 sessions, and data were analysed
using Velotron coaching software. Heart rates were also recorded
throughout all testing sessions (Polar RS400, New York, USA).
Electroencephalogram recordings and analysis
Prior to each condition, an EEG-cap (Electro-cap International,
Inc., USA) with 16 Ag-AgCl electrodes arranged in the interna-
tional 10-20 system was tted to the participants’ head. In
accordance with similar experiments examining fatigue and
decision making [ 23 , 24 ] , electrode sites in the left hemisphere
were selected for detailed analysis in the main area of interest
(frontal region of the cortex: electrode F3) and, for comparison
of localised cortical activation, an electrode from the parietal
region of the left hemisphere cortex (P3) was also analysed [ 8 ] .
The EEG-cap was permeable to air in order to prevent an increase
in heat during cycling. Electrodes were lled with Signa-gel
TM
(Parker Laboratories Inc., USA) for optimal signal transduction. A
Biosemi-Active-Two ampli er (Biosemi Inc., Amsterdam, Neth-
erlands) was used with high and low pass lters across a fre-
quency range from 0.16 to 100 Hz. All EEG recordings were
sampled in 30 s epochs [ 23 ] . EEG was processed and analysed
using BESA software (version 5.2 MEGIS software GmbH, Ger-
many) and artefacts due to cardio rhythms, body movement and
eye blinking were eliminated. A power spectrum was calculated
from the EEG signal using Fast Fourier Transformation and
exported in alpha (α) band (8–13 Hz) and beta (β) bands (13–
30 Hz) for analysis [ 27 ] .
EEG outputs sampled across EXP and CON trials were subjected
to detailed analysis at di erent intervals for comparative pur-
poses between conditions and across time: 1) at 5-min (settling
period of pre-exercise phase), 2) 45-min (at the mid-point of the
pre-exercise phase), 3) 85-min (at the end-point of pre-exercise
phase), 4) during the mid-point of the warm up and 5) the mid-
point of the exercise bouts.
Psychological scales
Visual analogue scales (VAS) have previously been demonstrated
to be a valid and reliable means of conveniently assessing a vari-
ety of psychological constructs [ 20 ] . The practicality and rapid-
ity of VAS measurements were an important consideration in
this experiment so as to not interrupt the sequential, continuous
protocol. Therefore participants were asked to indicate by plac-
ing a mark on a 10 cm line their mood (from negative to posi-
tive), their perceived level of fatigue (from not at all to completely
exhausted) and their motivation to exercise (from very low to
very high) in accordance with other studies [ 1 , 27 ] . All VAS
assessments were made immediately prior to the start of the
protocol, at the conclusion of the 90-min pre-exercise phase and
immediately at the end of the protocol.
Blood sampling
Prior to EXP and CON trials, participants gave a 0.6 μl sample of
whole fresh blood from a ngertip for assessment of glucose
concentration (ACCU-CHECK Aviva Blood Glucose Meter System,
Roche Diagnostics, Germany). All trials commenced 2 h after
breakfast to ensure blood glucose levels were within normal
range. This was to ensure similar homeostatic regulation and
postprandial rates of food digestion between conditions to mini-
mise glucose-induced mood disturbance.
Statistical analysis
Data from EXP and CON were analysed using repeated measures
of ANOVA (time × condition) with Greenhouse-Geisser correc-
tion when the assumption of sphericity was violated (SPSS ver-
sion 19.0). Post-hoc Tukey tests of honest signi cant di erence
were used to examine di erences when indicated by ANOVA.
Statistical signi cance was accepted at p < 0.05. 12 par ticipants
were recruited and all completed the EXP and CON conditions of
the experiment. Sample size analysis was calculated on the
dependent variable of power output (W) in response to the
cycling protocol. This was based on a statistical power (1-beta)
of 90 % and alpha of 0.05. A sample size of 8 was required to gen-
erate a large e ect ( > 0.8) according to Cohen’s d e ect size for
the target population. All data are presented as means ± SD
unless otherwise stated. Diagrams are shown as ± SE.
Results
ANOVA indicated that there was a signi cant main e ect
( F (1,11) = 25.2, p < 0.001, η
p 2 = 0.70) of the fatigue manipulations
(EXP vs. CON). The interaction between the fatigue manipula-
tion and the exercise bouts was not signi cant ( F (1,11) = 1.62,
p = 0.23, η
p 2 = 0.13). Post hoc tests indicated that the EXP condi-
tion produced signi cantly lower self-selected power outputs
for both the RPE 11 (EXP: 83 ± 7 W vs. CON: 99 ± 7 W; p = 0.005)
and RPE 15 (EXP: 132 ± 9 W vs. CON: 143 ± 8 W; p = 0.028) exer-
cise bouts. There was no evidence of learning e ects. Individual
responses to the exercise bouts are shown in
Fig. 1 .
Mean heart rates were similar between conditions in the 90-min
pre-exercise phase for EXP and CON (40.9 % and 41.2 % of maxi-
mum heart rate respectively) (
Table 2 ). In response to the 2
exercise bouts, a small but signi cant elevation of mean heart
rates was observed for CON compared to EXP for the RPE 11 bout
(4.3 % di erence between conditions; p < 0.05).
All participants’ maintained blood glucose within the normal
homeostatic range at the commencement of the trials (mean:
5.7 ± 0.46 mmol · L 1 ). There was no di erence between trials
( p = 0.91; C V = 1.2 %).
Analysis of EEG α-band cortical activity in the prefrontal cortex
sampling site (electrode F3) for both the 90-min pre-exercise
and exercise phases (
Fig. 2a ) did not identify signi cant dif-
ferences between the EXP and CON conditions ( F (1,11) = 4.31,
p = 0.06, η
p 2 = 0.28). There was however a signi cant main e ect
Physiology & Biochemistry
Brownsberger J et al. Mental Fatigue and E ort Perception … Int J Sports Med
of testing phase ( F (5,55) = 26.6, p < 0.001, η
p 2 = 0.71) and the inter-
action approached signi cance ( F (5,55) = 3.23, p = 0.058, η
p 2 = 0.23).
Post hoc analysis indicated that both EXP (RPE 11: p = 0.001; RPE
15: p < 0.001) and CON conditions showed signi cant (RPE 11:
p = 0.002; RPE 15: p < 0.001) and similar increases in α-band
activity at the onset of exercise compared to the start of the pro-
tocol. There was also a trend for reduced α-power in EXP
( p = 0.09) at the mid-point of the 90-min mental fatigue pre-
exercise task.
In contrast, a separate repeated measures ANOVA revealed a sig-
ni cant main e ect of fatigue manipulation ( F (1,11) = 90.8,
p < 0.001, η
p 2 = 0.89), a testing phase main e ect ( F (5,55) = 15.6,
p < 0.001, η
p 2 = 0.59) and a fatigue manipulation x testing phase
interaction ( F (5,55) = 4.92, p = 0.005, η
p 2 = 0.31) for electro cortical
activity of the β-band of the prefrontal lobe (electrode F3). Post
hoc comparisons indicated no signi cant di erence in β-band
activity between the EXP and the CON condition in the pre-exer-
cise phase (5 min: p = 0.76). However, the β-band activity was
elevated in the EXP at the mid-point (45-min; p = 0.005), at the
end of pre-exercise phase (90-min: p = 0.004) and during the
warm-up phase prior to exercise ( p = 0.00002) (
Fig. 2b ). Dur-
ing both subsequent exercise bouts, the di erence in β-band
activity between the EXP and CON condition decreased to non-
signi cant levels (RPE 11: p = 0.08 RPE 15: p = 0.06).
The ANOVA of the parietal lobe sampling site (electrode P3) did
not demonstrate any signi cant main e ects or interactions for
α-band (
Fig. 3a ). In the β-band (
Fig. 3b ) there was a signi -
cant main e ect of testing phase ( F (5,55) = 4.77, p = 0.007,
η
p 2 = 0.30).
Reported motivation to exercise and mood were not signi -
cantly a ected by the mentally demanding task compared to the
control task. There was, however, a signi cant change in reported
mental fatigue. Participants reported signi cantly greater men-
tal fatigue ( p = 0.001) in the EXP condition at the conclusion of
the 90-min pre-exercise phase compared to CON. Immediately
at the conclusion of the 2 exercise bouts, self-reported fatigue
was similar in the EXP and CON conditions (
Fig. 4 ) .
Discussion
The main nding of this study was that completion of a 90-min
pre-exercise task of continuous cognitive activity (mental
fatigue task) led to the production of less power output during
self-paced exercise than in a control condition. The participants
also reported sensations of mental tiredness and exhibited
greater β-band activation of the frontal lobe (electrode F3) com-
pared to the CON condition prior to exercise. These results sug-
gest the voluntary regulation of e ort during exercise may not
only be mediated by a erent physiological information, but also
by central factors such as negative cognitive associations to sen-
sations of mental fatigue immediately prior to exercise [ 12 , 20 ] .
This nding supports previous observations [ 20 , 21 ] that exer-
cise performance is impaired when experiencing greater mental
fatigue and suggests this may be attributable to an altered per-
ception of task di culty. As mental tiredness is often self-
reported by athletes during training and by professionals
working extended shift hours [ 2 , 30 ] , it is important to recognise
that mental fatigue potentially impedes self-paced exercise per-
formances. Subsequent intervention studies might examine
procedures to alleviate mental fatigue and the consequent e ect
on exercise, while also considering experimental designs includ-
ing additional placebo/sham conditions to eliminate the possi-
bility that expectancy is a factor in the observations of this
study.
The sustained elevation of β-band electro cortical activity for
EXP prior to exercise compared to CON (electrode F3) indicates
the 90-min pre-exercise procedure was successful in eliciting
greater attention, information processing and cognitive engage-
ment [ 8 , 18 , 19 , 24 ] . This elevation of cortical excitation appears
to have been localised to the prefrontal region of the cortex
where factors such as attention and decision-making are known
to in uence EEG activity [ 5 ] . The same level of excitation was
not observed from the parietal region (electrode P3), which was
largely una ected by testing phase (
Fig. 3a, b ). However, this
is commonly the case in such comparisons. The only signi cant
di erence between cortical activation from electrodes F3 and P3
was in β-band activity during the 90-min pre-exercise phase.
This is likely to be a consequence of localised (F3) excitation of
Fig. 1 Individual power outputs for the experimental (EXP) and control
(CON) conditions in response to the 2 bouts of 10-min cycling, self-paced
at individuals’ perceptions of e ort corresponding to RPE 11 (fairly light)
and RPE 15 (hard) descriptors (n = 12). The order of the 2 exercise bouts
was randomised for each condition and for each individual.
165
155
145
135
125
115
105
95
85
75
0EXP CON
RPE 11
Power output (W)
RPE 15
EXP CON
Table 2 Mean heart rate responses to the 90-min pre-exercise phase and the 2 subsequent self-paced exercise bouts.
Participants (N = 12)
Heart rate responses across pre-exercise and exercise bouts
90-min pre-exercise 10-min RPE 11 bout 10-min RPE-15 bout
Mean HR %HR max Mean HR %HR max Mean HR %HR max
EXP condition 80.3 ± 7.5 40.9 118.3 ± 18.7* 60.4 138.9 ± 16.1 70.9
CON condition 80.8 ± 8.1 41.2 126.8 ± 19.8* 64.7 141.8 ± 15.8 72.3
*signi cant di erence between conditions; p < 0.05
Physiology & Biochemistry
Brownsberger J et al. Mental Fatigue and E ort Perception … Int J Sports Med
the prefrontal cortex during the mental fatigue task and pro-
vides evidence that the protocol elicited greater regional brain
activation in an area likely to be implicated in decision-making
regarding power output during self-paced exercise. Clearly, as no
other studies have yet examined EEG in response to self-pacing
and self-selected work rates, this observation should be consid-
ered as a preliminary nding in need of further substantiation.
As the 90-min pre-exercise phase of the protocol for the CON
condition required only passive observation, it is unsurprising
there was no evidence of shift from α (passive/resting) to β
(active) cortical energy during that 90-min period. This is con-
sistent with aims of the passive task of the CON condition requir-
ing minimal attention and it therefore appears to have acted as
an appropriate control condition [ 19 , 22 ] . It seems plausible that
when participants undertook the EXP condition, the sustained
e ort of maintaining signi cant attention for 90-min evoked an
increased use of mental ‘energy’ that then induced mental
fatigue. Self-reported sensations of mental fatigue (
Table 3 )
were reported in the EXP condition prior to exercise, and the
combination of these factors with sustained greater β-band acti-
vation could explain the subsequent reduction in self-paced
exercise outputs compared to the CON condition. As such, the
mental consequences of the 90-min mental fatigue task may
have carried forward sensations of tiredness into the self-paced
exercise bouts, thereby in uencing the magnitude of the e orts
generated.
In the CON condition, an increase of β-band electro cortical acti-
vation in the prefrontal cortex was observed only after the
90-min of passive television observation (
Fig. 2b ). This e ect
is probably attributable to the greater mental engagement in the
self-paced exercise tasks compared to the observation task,
along with the physiological up-regulation of metabolism [ 13 ] .
The role of catecholamines for example, in the ght-or- ight
response is known to up-regulate preparedness for physical
challenges [ 13 , 29 ] . An abrupt increase of generalised EEG activ-
ity would therefore be anticipated at the onset of exercise
[ 8 , 24 , 27 ] .
Despite the observation of signi cant di erences in β-band EEG
outputs (F3) during the mental fatigue task in this study, it is
important to note that EEG recordings in response to exercise
are often problematic and prone to interference from external
noise [ 8 ] . Standard EEG artefact elimination control measures
were applied in this study [ 27 ] , and so while signi cant pre-
exercise di erences were noted, other more reliable brain indi-
ces such as fMRI may reveal systematic di erences during
exercise that were not identi ed in this study [ 5 ] . Due to the
limitations of EEG it is therefore not possible to draw a de nitive
conclusion as to whether or not di erences exist in regional
brain activation during exercise when mentally fatigued. Per-
formance and self-pacing are compromised, but the mechanism
for this e ect requires further elucidation.
As the performance outcome (e. g. power output) for both exer-
cise bouts was impeded in the EXP condition (RPE 11 bout;
p = 0.005 and RPE 15 bout; p = 0.028) compared to CON (
Fig. 1 ),
it is likely that the 90 min of attention requiring tasks prior to
exercise carried residual e ects of mental fatigue into the subse-
quent self-paced exercise bouts. This observation indicates that
the brain not only considers a erent peripheral physiologic
Fig. 2 a , b Log transformed α- and β-band power
(μV
2 ) for position F3 in both the 90-min pre-exer-
cise and exercise phases of the protocol. EXP = ex-
perimental condition (n = 12) and CON = control
condition (n = 12). *signi cant between condition
e ect ( p < 0.05), **signi cant between condi-
tion e ect ( p < 0.01). # signi cant time e ect for
intra-condition time e ect ( p < 0.05), ## signi cant
time e ect intra-condition time e ect ( p < 0.01).
Diagrams shown as ± SE.
1.8
1.7
1.6
1.5
Alpha power (log µV
2
)
1.4
1.3
1.2
1.1
1
0Start Mid End Warm up RPE11
EXP
#
##
CON
RPE15
Exercise phase90-min pre-exercise phase
0.8
0.7
Beta power (log µV
2
)
0.6
0.5
0Start Mid End Warm up RPE11
EXP
#
**
**
*
CON
RPE15
Exercise phase90-min pre-exercise phase
a
b
Physiology & Biochemistry
Brownsberger J et al. Mental Fatigue and E ort Perception … Int J Sports Med
information such as muscle soreness, changes in pH or respira-
tory discomfort [ 25 ] when determining appropriate e ort in
self-paced exercise, but also considers psychological factors such
as perceived tiredness [ 11 ] . This supports the previously pro-
posed concept that alterations to the perception of e ort when
mentally fatigued in uences performance attenuation and the
e ect of brain regulation over exercise performance [ 21 , 25 ] .
Heart rates between conditions were similar in the pre-exercise
phase of the experiment (EXP: 80.3 ± 7.5 vs. CON: 80.8 ± 8.1 b/
min), indicating that underlying physiological stress was similar
for that period (~40 % of maximal heart rate) (
Table 2 ). Conse-
quently, sensations of fatigue derived from this pre-exercise
period would not have had a physical cause. This strongly sug-
gests that any subsequent performance impediment in the self-
paced tasks were the result of mental and not physical factors
[ 20 ] . Analysis of mean heart rates for both subsequent exercise
bouts indicated they were all performed within the normal
range observed for the speci ed RPE descriptors (RPE 11:
60–65 % of maximum heart rate) and RPE 15: 70–75 % of maxi-
mum heart rate) [ 25 ] . Yet the CON condition produced slightly
elevated mean heart rates and signi cantly so for the lower
intensity exercise bout (
Table 2 ; p < 0.05). This observation
supports the nding of greater power outputs for CON across
both RPE 11 and RPE 15 bouts (
Fig. 1 ), which demonstrates
that heart rate responds to changes in physical work, such that
the greater muscular work (power output) achieved in the CON
condition evoked a subsequent requirement for greater cardio-
vascular support [ 25 ] .
Task motivation was una ected by the 90-min mental fatigue
condition in this experiment. This indicates that the monetary
incentive (prize) o ered for the best performance in the mental
fatigue task was e ective in eliciting sustained motivation on
the attention-demanding activity and that participants did not
disengage from it. Mental disengagement from the experiment
could have indicated that boredom [ 5 ] rather than mental
fatigue was the most plausible explanation for the observed
e ect on self-paced exercise. As motivation was well maintained
it seems that this was unlikely in our experiment.
Fig. 3 a , b Log transformed α- and β-band power
(μV
2 ) for position P3 in both the 90-min pre-exer-
cise and exercise phases of the protocol. EXP = ex-
perimental condition (n = 12) and CON = control
condition (n = 12). Diagrams shown as ± SE.
1.8
1.7
1.6
1.5
1.4
Alpha power (log µV2)Beta power (log µV2)
1.3
1.2
1.1
0.9
0.8
0.7
0.6
0.5
0Start
0Start Mid
90-min pre-exercise phase
End Warm up RPE15
EXP CON
RPE11
Exercise phase
Mid
90-min pre-exercise phase
End Warm up RPE15
EXP CON
RPE11
Exercise phase
1
a
b
Fig. 4 Mean rated a fatigue, b mood and c moti-
vation to exercise at pre-test, mid-test (following
the 90 min pre-exercise phase) and post exercise.
Diagrams shown as ± SE.
10
Rated Fatigue (max 10)
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1
0Pre-test Mid-test Post-
exercise
Pre-test Mid-test Post-
exercise
Pre-test Mid-test Post-
exercise
10
Rated Mood (max 10)
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1
0
10
Rated Motivation (max 10)
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1
0
EXP CON
ab c
Physiology & Biochemistry
Brownsberger J et al. Mental Fatigue and E ort Perception … Int J Sports Med
At the conclusion of the 2 exercise bouts, it was evident that
fatigue had improved in the EXP trial relative to mid-test
(
Table 3 ). Improvements to mood states and other positive
associations with fatigue sensations are often reported after
submaximal exercise [ 3 ] . To our knowledge, however, no studies
have examined mood state or fatigue levels after an exercise
bout speci cally examining responses from individuals who are
experiencing sensations of mental fatigue. Nevertheless, despite
the post-exercise improvement in psychological state, power
outputs for EXP had already been impeded. The psychological
state of the individual prior to exercise is therefore an important
consideration for subsequent successful performance [ 21 ] .
In conclusion, this study demonstrates mental fatigue negatively
in uences the performance of self-paced exercise at 2 di erent
intensities of e ort. This is probably due to an altered perception
of e ort mediated by a sustained period (90 min) of pre-exercise
mental activity [ 5 , 20 ] . Signi cant localised β-band activity from
the prefrontal cortex accompanied sensations of mental fatigue
prior to exercise performance. These factors may have contrib-
uted to the impaired performances in the 2 self-paced exercise
bouts. However, the mechanisms of mental fatigue remain
equivocal. Nevertheless, the ndings of this study support previ-
ous observations that mental fatigue negatively in uences
endurance performance [ 20 , 21 ] , and it seems likely that this is,
at least in part, due to an altered perception of e ort.
The practical signi cance of the observations from this study
indicate that mental fatigue experienced prior to the completion
of self-paced physical tasks is likely to compromise performance.
This was evident by a 19 % di erence in performance in the fairly
light e ort bout (RPE 11) and an 8 % di erence between condi-
tions in the hard e ort bout (RPE 15), both of which were statis-
tically signi cant observations, although the e ect was more
consistent across individuals in the lighter e ort trial (
Fig. 1 ).
Nevertheless, these observations may lead other researchers to
further explore this novel concept and could have important
implications for the completion of tasks where safety is an issue,
or in any circumstances (e. g. racing) where performance impair-
ment is meaningful. Further work is required to ascertain the
signi cance of these ndings to di erent population groups and
in response to other modes of exercise.
Disclosure statements: 1. All authors contributed equally to
planning, data collection, analysis and assembly of this manu-
script. The corresponding author is the team leader and project
supervisor. 2. This project was not subject to a grant and was
nancially supported by discretionary budget of the Institute of
Sport & Exercise Science for incidental consumables and minor
costs. 3. The authors of this manuscript have no competing
interests.
References
1 Ahearn E P . The use of visual analog scales in mood disorders: A critical
review . J Psychiatr Res 1998 ; 31 : 569 – 579
2 Åkerstedt T , Kecklund G , Johansson S . Shift work and mortality . Chrono-
biol Int 2004 ; 21 : 1055 – 1061
3 Bassett D J , Howley E T . Maximal oxygen uptake: “classical” versus
“contemporary” viewpoints . Med Sci Sports Exerc 1997 ; 29 : 591 – 603
4 Berger B G , Motl R W . Exercise and mood: A selective review and syn-
thesis of research employing the pro le of mood states . J Appl Psychol
2000 ; 12 : 69 – 92
5 Boksem MA S , Meijman T F , Lorist M M . E ects of mental fatigue on
attention: An ERP study . Cognit Brain Res 2005 ; 25 : 107 – 116
6 Borg G A . Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion . Med Sci Sports
Exerc 1982 ; 14 : 377 – 381
7 Chen M , Fan X , Moe S . Criterion-related validity of the Borg ratings
of perceived exertion scale in healthy individuals: a meta-analysis . J
Sports Sci 2002 ; 20 : 873 – 899
8 Crabbe J B , Dishman R K . Brain electrocortical activity during and after
exercise: a quantitative synthesis . Psychophysiol 2004 ; 41 : 563 – 574
9 Dunbar C C , Robertson R J , Baun R , Blandin M F , Metz K , Burdett R , Goss
F L . The validity of regulating exercise intensity by ratings of perceived
exertion . Med Sci Sports Exerc 1992 ; 24 : 94 – 99
10 Edwards A M , Bentley M B , Mann M E , Seaholme T S . Self-pacing in inter-
val training: a teleoanticipatory approach . Psychophysiol 2011 ; 48 :
136 – 141
11 Edwards A M , Claxton D B , Challis N V , Chapman J H , Fysh M L . O x y g e n
uptake kinetics determined by PRBS techniques di erentiate elite
endurance runners from elite sprinters . Int J Sports Med 1999 ; 20 : 1 – 6
12 Edwards A M , Polman RC J . Pacing in sport and exercise: a psycho-
physiological perspective . New York : Nova Scienti c Publishers , 2012 ;
49 – 70
13 French D N , Kraemer W J , Volek J S , Spiering B A , Judelson D A , Ho man J R ,
Maresh C M . Anticipatory responses of catecholamines on muscle force
production . J Appl Physiol 2007 ; 102 : 94 – 102
14 Goode J H . Are pilots at risk of accidents due to fatigue? J Safety Res
2003 ; 34 : 309 – 313
15 Harriss D J , Atkinson G . Update – Ethical standards in sport and exercise
science research . Int J Sports Med 2011 ; 32 : 819 – 821
16 Lambert E V , St Clair Gibson A , Noakes T D . Complex systems model of
fatigue: integrative homoeostatic control of peripheral physiological
systems during exercise in humans . Br J Sports Med 2005 ; 39 : 52 – 62
17 Lander P J , Edwards A M , Butterly R J . Self-paced exercise is less physi-
cally challenging than enforced constant pace exercise of the same
intensity: in uence of complex central metabolic control . Br J Sports
Med 2009 ; 43 : 789 – 795
18 Lorist M M , Bezdan E , Caat M , Span M M , Roerdin JBTM , Maurits N M .
The in uence of mental fatigue and motivation on neural network
dynamics; an EEG coherence study . Brain Res 2009 ; 1270 : 95 – 106
19 Lorist M M , Klein M , Nieuwenhuis S , de Jong R , Mulder G , Meijman T F .
Mental fatigue and task control: Planning and preparation . Psycho-
physiology 2000 ; 37 : 614 – 625
20 Marcora S , Staiano W . The limit to exercise tolerance in humans: mind
over muscle? Eur J Appl Physiol 2010 ; 109 : 763 – 770
21 Marcora S M , Staiano W , Manning V . Mental fatigue impairs physical
performance in humans . J Appl Physiol 2009 ; 106 : 857 – 864
22 Miller R . Theory of the normal waking EEG: From single neurones
to waveforms in the alpha, beta and gamma frequency ranges . Int J
Psychophysiol 2007 ; 64 : 18 – 23
23 Moraes H , Ferreira C , Deslandes A , Cagy M , Pompeu F , Ribeiro P , Piedade
R . Beta and alpha electroencephalographic activity changes after acute
exercise . Arqde Neuro-Psiquiat 2007 ; 65 : 637 – 641
24 Nielsen B , Hyldig T , Bidstrup F , González-Alonso J , Christo ersen GR J .
Brain activity and fatigue during prolonged exercise in the heat .
P ügers Arch Eur J Physiol 2001 ; 442 : 41 – 48
Self-perceptions
0–10 VAS scales
Test occasions for EXP and CON conditions
Pre-test Mid-test (90-min) Post-exercise
EXP CON EXP CON EXP CON
fatigue (n = 12) 2.1 ± 2.2 2.2 ± 2.1 6.5 ± 1.6 ##** 4.2 ± 2.3 4.8 ± 2.4 # 4.5 ± 2.4
mood (n = 12) 7.2 ± 1.5 7.3 ± 1.2 5.6 ± 1.9 6.4 ± 1.8 7.1 ± 1.8 7.4 ± 1.2
motivation (n = 12) 6.0 ± 2.8 5.9 ± 2.4 5.4 ± 2.2 4.8 ± 2.4 6.5 ± 2.4 7.0 ± 1.9
# = signi cant time e ect from pre-test; p < 0.05, ## signi cant time e ect from pre-test; p < 0.01, * signi cant di erence between
conditions; p < 0.01
Table 3 Psychological indices
of perceived fatigue and mood
at baseline (pre-test), mid-test
(immediately following the
90-min pre-exercise phase), and
post-exercise intervals.
Physiology & Biochemistry
Brownsberger J et al. Mental Fatigue and E ort Perception … Int J Sports Med
28 Swart J , Lamberts R P , Lambert M I , Clairgibson A , Lambert E V , Skowno J ,
Noakes T D . Exercising with reserve: evidence that the central nervous
system regulates prolonged exercise performance . Br J Sports Med
2009 ; 43 : 782 – 788
29 Watt M J , Stellingwer T , Heigenhauser GJ F , Spriet L L . E ects of plasma
adrenaline on hormone-sensitive lipase at rest and during moder-
ate exercise in human skeletal muscle . J Physiol 2003 ; 550 : 325 – 332
30 Yeung R R . The acute e ects of exercise on mood state . J Psychosom
Res 1996 ; 40 : 123 – 141
25 Noakes T D . Time to move beyond a brainless exercise physiology: the
evidence for complex regulation of human exercise performance . Appl
Physiol Nutr Metab 2011 ; 36 : 23 – 35
26 Noakes T D , Snow R J , Febbraio M A . Linear relationship between the
perception of e ort and the duration of constant load exercise that
remains . J Appl Physiol 2004 ; 96 : 1571 – 1573
27 Schnieder S , Askew C D , Diehl J , Mierau A , Kleinert J , Abel T , Carnahan
H . EEG activity and mood in health orientated runners after di erent
exercise intensities . Physiol Behav 2009 ; 96 : 709 – 716
... CTR: looking at a blank screen. In relation to the sex of the participants, there are no studies including only women, and only in some of them included women as a part of the sample (Brown & Bray, 2019;Brownsberger et al., 2013;Filipas et al., 2018;Lam et al., 2021;Lopes et al., 2020;MacMahon et al., 2014MacMahon et al., , 2019Marcora et al., 2009;Martin et al., 2019;Penna et al., 2018). In these studies, only two of them have female participation exceeding male participation. ...
... To determine the influence of mental fatigue on performance, all studies employed tests performed at maximal intensities or at various intensities corresponding to maximum oxygen consumption or a specific pedaling power output. Specifically, the studies included time-trial tests over a specific distance in swimming (Filho, Penna, Wanner, et al., 2018;Penna et al., 2021), rowing, running (Lam et al., 2021;MacMahon et al., 2014;Pageaux et al., 2014), or cycling (Brownsberger et al., 2013;Filipas et al., 2019;Martin, Staiano, Menaspà, et al., 2016;Pires et al., 2018;Silva-Cavalcante et al., 2018); or tests analyzing time to exhaustion using constant-load tests (Barzegarpoor et al., 2020;Lopes et al., 2020;Marcora et al., 2009;Martin et al., 2019) or incremental-load tests (MacMahon et al., 2019;Salam et al., 2018). Intermittent performance tests have also been considered (e.g., Yo-Yo IR1) (Filipas et al., 2021;Greco et al., 2017;Lam et al., 2021;Penna et al., 2018;Smith et al., 2016;Veness et al., 2017;Weerakkody et al., 2021) since they also met the inclusion criteria. ...
... No differences were observed between the control and experimental conditions in professional cyclists, but significant differences were found in recreational cyclists (Martin, Staiano, Menaspà, et al., 2016). However, with regard to perceptual variables, the subjective perception of effort was higher in conditions of mental fatigue in only four out of 15 studies (Brownsberger et al., 2013;Pageaux et al., 2014;Pires et al., 2018;& Staiano et al., 2019). (Filipas et al., 2018(Filipas et al., , 2019Lam et al., 2021;MacMahon et al., 2014;Martin, Staiano, Menaspa, et al., 2016;Pageaux et al., 2014;Penna et al., 2018;& Silva-Cavalcante et al., 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Mental fatigue affects not only cognitive performance, but also physical performance. The main objective of this study was to review the existing literature on the influence of mental fatigue on the ability to withstand sustained physical efforts (endurance tasks). For this purpose, we performed a systematic literature review in the online databases Web Of Science, PubMed and Scopus with the following search terms and Boolean conjunctions: mental fatigue or cognitive fatigue and exercise or endurance and time to exhaustion or time trial or running or cycling. A total of 28 articles, comprising 29 interventions, were included with a high methodological quality (range: 6-8 points out of 10 possible) measured using the PEDro scale. A total of 86% of the included interventions show that mental fatigue reduces performance in the endurance tests compared to a control condition without mental fatigue. In addition, mental fatigue increases the subjective perception of effort, without altering heart rate or blood lactate concentration. Therefore, the results show the importance of avoiding any activity that involves high cognitive demands prior to training or competition events, due to the detrimental effects on endurance capacity.
... This is in line with the results of other studies that also used a Stroop-task performed for 30 min and more as an intervention task [58,[86][87][88]. In this context, Brownsberger et al. [89] and Tanaka et al. [90] reported an increased activity of the prefrontal cortex measured with EEG (β-waves) during and after a cognitive demanding task like the Stroop-task and the 2-back-task. The higher activity in the prefrontal cortex was associated with improved attention, information processing, and cognitive engagement suggesting a compensatory mechanisms to maintain cognitive performance despite the presence of perceived cognitive fatigue [91]. ...
... The reading control task, on the other hand, led to a decrease in word list generation task performance in the older adults. This could also be related to the already discussed increased activation of the prefrontal cortex during the cognitively demanding Stroop intervention task [89,90], which might have positively influenced cognitive task performance under single task conditions. During the dual-task conditions, no change in cognitive interference task performance after the Stroop intervention task as well as after the reading control task was found for any of the three interference tasks (word list generation task, arithmetic task, Strooptask) in both age groups. ...
Article
Full-text available
During their daily lives humans are often confronted with sustained cognitive activities (SCA) leading to state fatigue, a psychobiological state characterized by a decrease in cognitive and/or motor performance and/or an increase in perception of fatigue. It was recently shown that performing SCA can impair overground dual-task gait performance in older adults, but it is currently unknown whether there is a task- and/or age-specific modulation in gait performance during treadmill walking. Therefore, the effect of a SCA on single- and dual-task treadmill walking performance was investigated in young and old adults. Using a crossover design, spatio-temporal gait parameters of 24 young and 23 older healthy participants were measured using motion capturing during single- and dual-task (including three cognitive interference tasks: word list generation, arithmetic, and Stroop-task) treadmill walking before and after SCA (30 min Stroop-task) and a control task (reading). Moreover, cognitive fatigue, wakefulness, mood, and arousal were assessed. Although the SCA induced age-specific perceptual responses, no difference was found for cognitive performance during the Stroop-task. The cognitive interference task performance (word list generation, arithmetic, and Stroop-task) during walking on the treadmill did not decrease after the SCA. Single- and dual-task gait performance (e. g., step width and step length) specifically changed after the SCA and after the reading control task in both groups. Data indicate that perceived cognitive fatigue has an impact on single- and dual-task treadmill walking performance, with task- and age-specific differences. Although no general age-specific changes in single- and dual task gait performance following SCA were identified, perceived cognitive fatigue should be considered as an intrinsic risk factor for falls.
... We searched the WOSCC to identify all studies on the objective assessment of cognitive fatigue. Since most researchers considered that "cognitive fatigue, " "mental fatigue, " and "brain fatigue" are conceptually similar, union logic was applied to the 3 keywords by inserting the conjunction "OR" (Van Cutsem et al., 2017;Marcora et al., 2009;Brownsberger et al., 2013;Pageaux et al., 2015;Smith et al., 2015;Bijleveld, 2023;Sengupta et al., 2017;Sievertsen et al., 2016). Likewise, 15 synonyms screened from research papers were linked by the logic Flow-chart of the study. ...
... Indeed, most of the literature suggests that cognitive, mental, and brain fatigue refer to the same meaning. However, more authors considered that "mental fatigue" is more appropriate as it includes emotion and motivation rather than just cognition (Marcora et al., 2009;Brownsberger et al., 2013;Pageaux et al., 2015;Smith et al., 2015;Bijleveld, 2023;Sengupta et al., 2017;Sievertsen et al., 2016). Surprisingly, timeline visualization of keywords network has shown that the term "cognitive fatigue" has occurred more frequently in recent years, implying that more researchers are concentrating on the effect of high-workload tasks on one's cognition ( Figure 11). ...
Article
Full-text available
Aim The objective of this study was to gain insight into the nature of cognitive fatigue and to identify future trends of objective assessment techniques in this field. Methods One thousand and eighty-five articles were retrieved from the Web of Science Core Collection database. R version 4.3.1, VOSviewer 1.6.20, CiteSpace 6.2.R4, and Microsoft Excel 2019 were used to perform the analysis. Results A total of 704 institutes from 56 countries participated in the relevant research, while the People’s Republic of China contributed 126 articles and was the leading country. The most productive institute was the University of Gothenburg. Johansson Birgitta from the University of Gothenburg has posted the most articles (n = 13). The PLOS ONE published most papers (n = 38). The Neurosciences covered the most citations (n = 1,094). A total of 3,116 keywords were extracted and those with high frequency were mental fatigue, performance, quality-of-life, etc. Keywords mapping analysis indicated that cognitive fatigue caused by continuous work and traumatic brain injury, as well as its rehabilitation, have become the current research trend. The most co-cited literature was published in Sports Medicine. The strongest citation burst was related to electroencephalogram (EEG) event-related potential and spectral power analysis. Conclusion Publication information of related literature on the objective assessment of cognitive fatigue from 2007 to 2024 was summarized, including country and institute of origin, authors, and published journal, offering the current hotspots and novel directions in this field.
... With further regard to research involving fMRI, researchers have also used EEG to examine the neural manifestations of mental fatigue. While a broad range of spectral bands (delta, theta, lower and higher alpha, and beta) have been explored across most neural regions [49][50][51], a starting point for this discussion is the recent review by Tran et al. [52]. This well cited network meta-analysis collated the findings of 21 studies that employed a mental fatigue intervention. ...
Article
Mental fatigue is referred to as a psychophysiological or neurobiological state caused by prolonged periods of demanding cognitive activity. Sports and exercise science research have investigated the effects of experimentally induced mental fatigue on cognitive performance, with mixed results. It has been suggested that negative effects of mental fatigue on cognition performance in laboratory studies could translate to impaired sport performance. However, it remains unclear if impairments in sport performance are due to mental fatigue and how mental fatigue may differ from physical fatigue. Fatigue is well understood as a complex multifactorial construct involving interactions between physiological and neuropsychological responses across brain regions. It may be prudent for researchers to return to the origins of fatigue and cognition before attempting to connect mental fatigue and sport cognition. This article reviews the concept of mental fatigue, its mechanisms and neuroanatomical basis, models of cognition relevant to sports science, how mental fatigue may influence cognition, and suggests future research directions. Mental fatigue as a construct separate from fatigue could be an oversight that has hindered the development of our understanding of mental fatigue. Future sports science research could work to enhance our knowledge of our definitions of fatigue.
... In addition other experimental approaches aiming to manipulate higher-order cognitive factors during exercise, such as shifting attentional focus, listening to music or developing mental fatigue, have been shown to impact effort perception without necessarily altering cardiovascular responses (Brownsberger et al., 2013;Lohse & Sherwood, 2011;Potteiger et al., 2000). Differences in findings between these approaches and the current study, or work using hypnosis (Williamson et al., 2001), may reflect differences in the nature of the higher-order manipulation (e.g. ...
Article
Full-text available
In this study we have used a highly immersive virtual reality (VR) cycling environment where incongruence between virtual hill gradient (created by visual gradient and bike tilt angle) and actual workload (pedalling resistance) can experimentally manipulate perception of exercise effort. This therefore may provide a method to examine the role of effort perception in cardiorespiratory control during exercise. Twelve healthy untrained participants (7 men, age 26 ± 5 years) were studied during five visits. On visit 1 participants underwent cardiopulmonary exercise testing, and during subsequent visits (2–4) participants performed repeated hill climbs at different gradients (of 3%, 6% and 9% in counterbalanced order) with the actual workload ‘congruent’ with virtual hill gradient. On visit 5 participants completed three incongruent trials with virtual hill gradients of 3%, 6% and 9% but a fixed workload equal to that for the 6% climb (iVR3%, iVR6% and iVR9% trials). Despite no difference in power output, there was a significantly elevated rating of perceived exertion (RPE) and mean arterial blood pressure in iVR9% compared to iVR3% and iVR6%, although this effect decayed over time. There was no effect on any respiratory variable, and no significant reduction in RPE or cardiovascular responses was observed during the iVR3% trial. These data suggest that perception of effort and cardiovascular responses to exercise can be manipulated experimentally via virtual hill gradient (using visual and/or vestibular cues) in a VR environment. This work supports those previously showing the existence of a control mechanism which integrates perception of effort and the cardiovascular response to exercise in humans. image Key points We aimed to assess whether using a highly immersive virtual reality (VR) cycling environment to create incongruence between perceived effort (virtual hill gradient) and actual effort (pedal resistance) can manipulate cardiorespiratory responses to exercise. At an equivalent power output cycling up a steeper virtual hill produced greater ratings of perceived exertion (RPEs) and blood pressure responses compared to a virtual hill congruent to power output. This work suggests the existence of a control mechanism which integrates perception of exercise effort and the cardiovascular response to exercise, which can be experimentally manipulated by VR.
... An ideal VR gymnasium should enhance the users' willingness to continue exercising. However, fatigue is a major threat that causes people to give up exercising [3]. Intense exercise in a gymnasium can lead to hyperarousal, causing people to perceive that time passes more slowly [4]., ...
Article
Full-text available
This study explored the relationship between surrounding avatars and time perception in a virtual reality (VR) gymnasium. Previous research has highlighted that motion speed and exercise intensity significantly influence time perception. In VR, time perception is shaped by various factors, such as an avatar’s embodiment at different levels. However, the specific effects of the surrounding avatar on time perception in a VR gymnasium context remain unclear. Thus, this study focuses on two key attributes of the surrounding avatar: (1) motion speeds and (2) exercise intensity. Participants in a VR gymnasium either rode a stationary bike or sat on one while observing avatars performing exercises in the virtual environment(VE). They were then asked to estimate the duration judgment and the feeling of the passage of time for each task. The results revealed that when the surrounding avatars exercised at a faster motion speed, participants perceived the duration of time as longer and felt that time passed more quickly. Additionally, high-intensity exercise led participants to perceive the passage of time as faster.
... Although exercise promotes health improvements, recent studies show that acute strenuous exercise, such as marathon running, impairs mood [1][2][3]. Several non-exclusive mechanisms are associated with worsening mood after strenuous exercise, including changes in serotonin and cortisol concentrations, dehydration, hypoglycemia, and neurophysiological changes [1,4]. In addition, elevation of pro-inflammatory cytokines, such as IL-6 and TNF-α, may also be associated with worsening mood after strenuous exercise [1]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: The gut–brain axis may mediate mood changes due to strenuous exercise. Therefore, probiotic supplementation may mitigate mood worsening. Purpose: The present study aims to evaluate the effect of probiotic supplementation on mood and immunometabolic parameters after a marathon. Materials and methods: Fourteen marathon runners were selected and divided into placebo and probiotic groups that were supplemented for 30 days. Before and after the marathon, mood (POMS) was assessed, and blood was collected for analysis of immunometabolic parameters. Statistical analysis was performed, and p < 0.05 was considered to determine statistically differences. Results: Tension decreased after the marathon in both groups. Vigor decreased only in the placebo group. Fatigue increased after the marathon in both groups. TMD increased after the marathon in placebo. The IL2/IL-4 ratio decreased in the probiotic group after the marathon compared to before and increased compared to the placebo group. The IL-10 increased after the marathon in placebo. TNF-α increased after the marathon in probiotics. The TNF-α/IL-10 ratio decreased after the marathon in both groups. LPS decreased in the probiotic group after the marathon compared to before and in the placebo group. Conclusions: Thirty days of probiotic supplementation attenuated the impact of marathons on mood worsening. The decrease in LPS in the probiotic group mediated the change in the pro/anti-inflammatory balance, indicating an immunometabolic mechanism by which the gut–brain axis impacts mood after strenuous exercise.
Thesis
Full-text available
Studies suggest that prior cognitive effort (PCE) can negatively affect the physical, behavioral, and perceptual-cognitive performance of athletes. However, little is known about how different forms of PCE influence perceptual, cognitive, and physiological responses, or whether they equally impact sports performance. This study compared the effects of three types of PCE (Modified Stroop Test - MST, social media use - SM, and watching a documentary - CON) on the neurophysiological response, visuomotor performance, and physical performance of 15 taekwondo (TKD) athletes. Levels of mental fatigue and satisfaction were measured at regular intervals, along with electroencephalography (EEG) at rest and during tasks, focusing on theta, alpha 1, alpha 2, and beta bands, as well as event-related potentials (ERPs; N200 and P300). After cognitive manipulations, athletes performed visuomotor tests and a TKD-specific physical task. Results showed that MST increased mental fatigue (p < 0.001) and reduced enjoyment (p < 0.001), impaired response time (p = 0.04) and accuracy (p < 0.001) in the Stroop test, and worsened the number of kicks in the TKD task (p = 0.03). EEG revealed spectral power changes only in the parietal region, with a reduction in theta frequency after all conditions (ps = 0.04) and increased theta, alpha 1, and alpha 2 values in MST at the 15th minute of cognitive manipulation (ps < 0.01), followed by a decrease, with no significant difference for the other conditions in subsequent minutes. N200 amplitude was higher in CON compared to MST (p = 0.02) at the Fz electrode. There were no differences in visuomotor performance between conditions (ps > 0.05). It was concluded that PCE induced by cognitive tests (MST) negatively affected perceived fatigue, neuroelectric responses, and physical performance, while social media use did not have the same effects.
Article
Full-text available
Mental fatigue is a transient psychophysiological state characterized by impaired cognition and behavior across a range of dynamic contexts. Despite increasing interest in this phenomenon, its (neuro)physiologic representations remain unclear. This systematic review aimed to quantify the range of (neuro)physiologic outcomes and methodologies used to investigate mental fatigue in laboratory-based settings. Across the 72 studies meeting our inclusion criteria, we identified 30 unique physiologic, four visual outcomes, and the application of several neuroimaging techniques investigating neuronal function. Mental fatigue increased heart rate, systolic and diastolic blood pressure, mean arterial pressure, low frequency, and root mean square of successive differences (RMSSD), and reduced standard deviation of normal-to-normal intervals (SDNN) (all P ≤ 0.04) when compared with controls. Applying electroencephalography to investigate delta, theta, and alpha bandwidths may provide useful insights into this phenomenon, and functional near-infra-red spectroscopy to right-lateralized frontoparietal regions would be helpful to investigate cortical activity change in response to mental fatigue. More data are needed across a range of methodological contexts in order to further determine the (neuro)physiological manifestations of mental fatigue. However, this review provides direction to researchers and will assist them in navigating and considering the range of options available. Supplementary Information The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.3758/s13428-025-02620-7.
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this study was to examine the role of cardiac interoception on self‐regulated (Experiment 1) and externally prescribed (Experiment 2) exercises. Cardiac interoception was assessed using heartbeat tracking and discrimination tasks in both experiments. Based on heartbeat discrimination performance, participants were partitioned into groups demonstrating GOOD and POOR cardiac interoceptive accuracy. In Experiment 1, 20 participants completed two self‐regulated 20‐min cycling tasks at two intensities (light rated physical exertion [RPE on Borg Scale = 10] vs. hard‐to‐very hard, RPE = 16). During self‐regulated exercise, the POOR cardiac interoception group showed lower differences in their exercise work rates and physiological responses between light and hard‐to‐very hard intensity exercises. These differences were partly attributable to a higher work rate over the first 5 min of light intensity exercise and a higher initial rate of work in the first min of hard‐to‐very hard intensity exercise. In Experiment 2, 15 participants completed an externally prescribed, constant‐load cycling task performed at 80% of the peak power output, to task failure. During externally prescribed exercise, GOOD and POOR groups did not differ in their time‐to‐task failure nor in their physiological and perceptual responses to the exercise. Together these findings demonstrate that individual differences in interoceptive accuracy influence the regulation of self‐paced exercise but do not affect externally prescribed exercise tolerance under constant load.
Article
Full-text available
This paper highlights the use of the Profile of Mood States (POMS) in physical activity research by reviewing and synthesizing literature generated in exercise settings. The results of many studies using the POMS have supported the relationships between exercise and acute mood changes in normal populations and between exercise and chronic mood changes in clinical populations. Based on the multitude of studies utilizing the POMS, Berger and colleagues developed a preliminary taxonomy containing enjoyment, mode, and practice guidelines to help maximize the mood benefits associated with exercise. The POMS also has been employed to identify underlying mechanisms that may promote mood alteration. Unfortunately, there is no conclusive evidence that identifies a single mechanism or group of mechanisms as consistently influencing the exercise-mood relationship. Although knowledge of the relationship between exercise and mood alteration is substantial, much remains to be studied. Promising avenues for future investigation of exercise include mood changes in specific populations, environmental influences on mood alteration, and personal characteristics impacting mood alteration.
Article
Full-text available
In 1923, Nobel Laureate A.V. Hill proposed that maximal exercise performance is limited by the development of anaerobiosis in the exercising skeletal muscles. Variants of this theory have dominated teaching in the exercise sciences ever since, but 90years later there is little biological evidence to support Hills belief, and much that disproves it. The cardinal weakness of the Hill model is that it allows no role for the brain in the regulation of exercise performance. As a result, it is unable to explain at least 6 common phenomena, including (i) differential pacing strategies for different exercise durations (ii) the end spurt (iii) the presence of fatigue even though homeostasis is maintained; (iv) fewer than 100% of the muscle fibers have been recruited in the exercising limbs (v) the evidence that a range of interventions that act exclusively on the brain can modify exercise performance and (vi) the finding that the rating of perceived exertion is a function of the relative exercise duration rather than the exercise intensity. Here I argue that the central governor model (CGM) is better able to explain these phenomena. In the CGM, exercise is seen as a behaviour that is regulated by complex systems in the central nervous system specifically to ensure that exercise terminates before there is a catastrophic biological failure. The complexity of this regulation cannot be appreciated if the body is studied as a collection of disconnected components, as is the usual approach in the modern exercise sciences.
Article
Full-text available
In exercise physiology, it has been traditionally assumed that high-intensity aerobic exercise stops at the point commonly called exhaustion because fatigued subjects are no longer able to generate the power output required by the task despite their maximal voluntary effort. We tested the validity of this assumption by measuring maximal voluntary cycling power before (mean +/- SD, 1,075 +/- 214 W) and immediately after (731 +/- 206 W) (P < 0.001) exhaustive cycling exercise at 242 +/- 24 W (80% of peak aerobic power measured during a preliminary incremental exercise test) in ten fit male human subjects. Perceived exertion during exhaustive cycling exercise was strongly correlated (r = -0.82, P = 0.003) with time to exhaustion (10.5 +/- 2.1 min). These results challenge the long-standing assumption that muscle fatigue causes exhaustion during high-intensity aerobic exercise, and suggest that exercise tolerance in highly motivated subjects is ultimately limited by perception of effort.
Article
Full-text available
To examine whether self-pacing reduces the physiological challenge of performing 5000 m rowing ergometry exercise in comparison with a matched-intensity exercise condition in which a constant effort pacing strategy is enforced. Nine healthy well-trained male participants volunteered to participate in three 5000 m rowing conditions (two submaximal and one maximal conditions) in an individualised order. In the submaximal conditions, participants were required to (1) perform 5000 m at a constant rating of perceived exertion (RPE 15-Hard) (SubRPE) or (2) perform 5000 m at an enforced constant pace equivalent to the mean power output (PO) of the SubRPE condition (SubEXT). A maximal condition (MaxTT) was included to disguise the purpose of the study and to facilitate an element of randomisation in the test sequence. Dynamic intratest responses were assessed every 30 s: PO, VO2, iEMG, core (Tc) and skin temperatures (Tsk). There was no difference between performance times of the two submaximal trials. The mean PO represented 83.83 (SD 8.88)% (SubRPE) and 83.40 (8.84)% (SubEXT) of the mean MaxTT power output. Tc (SubRPE:38.46 (0.23) degrees C, SubEXT:38.72 (0.36) degrees C; p<0.01), post-test BLa (SubRPE:5.24 (2.18), SubEXT:6.19 (2.51) mmol/l; p<0.05) and iEMG (p<0.05) were significantly elevated in SubEXT compared with SubRPE. There were no differences in the dynamics of HR or VO2 between SubEXT and SubRPE. The intratest stroke-to-stroke variability of power output was significantly greater in the SubRPE condition compared with SubEXT (p<0.01). Enforced constant paced exercise presents a significantly greater physiological challenge than self-paced exercise. The ability to dynamically self-pace effort via manipulations of power output during exercise is an important behavioural response to homeostatic challenges and thus forms an integral part of a complex central regulatory process.
Book
Pacing in sport and exercise: a psychophysiological perspective is the first book which comprehensively examines the way humans pace exercise and sporting activities. This concept represents an exciting opportunity to combine the latest physiological and psychological theories with practical sport and exercise applications. The book will be an essential read for sport and exercise science academics, students, coaches, athletes and general exercise enthusiasts. Pacing is the strategy by which effort is managed across an exercise bout in relation to a specific goal and in the knowledge of the likely demands of the task. It is widely recognised that individuals distribute effort across a task and this is in evidence during all non-reflex exercise situations where individuals are able to receive and act upon neural feedback from peripheral physiologic systems. As such, pacing across a wide range of sport and exercise contexts is investigated and explained. Sporting activities as diverse as sprinting, soccer, triathlon, rowing and cycling are discussed. In addition, training methodologies to improve the ability to self-pace exercise are reviewed and a self-regulatory system to optimize training across all exercise types is proposed. Templates for consolidating training sessions, design and monitoring strategies are included. Pacing in relation to specific population groups such as children, overweight/obese individuals and those with chronic or acute illnesses is also discussed. Finally, it is hoped this new book will stimulate further research, growth and awareness of this important topic.
Article
An increasing number of studies within the recent years connected physical exercise with changes in brain cortical activity. Most of this data (1) refers to aerobic exercise and (2) does not correlate to psychological parameters although it is well known that exercise has a positive effect on mood. In times where health activities play a major role it is increasingly necessary to connect somato-physiological and somato-psychological components of physical activity.This study aimed to find changes in EEG activity and mood after low, preferred and high intensity running. EEG and actual state of mood were recorded before and after exercise. Results showed an effect for the preferred and high intensity velocity in both, EEG and mood. As only the higher frequency areas > 18 Hz showed persisting decreases post-exercise we concluded that this might be a sign of outlasting effects of exercise on brain cortical activity which may have influences on general well-being. We could also show that there is a clear relationship between EEG activity and mood reflecting a basic principle of cortical excitation.
Article
The aim of the present study was to investigate whether the concurrent use of Rating of Perceived Exertion (RPE) and a new Perceived Readiness (PR) scale facilitates optimal interval training performance outcomes. Eleven competitive male runners completed outdoor interval track-running trials at a pre-set RPE. The PR scale was used to facilitate self-determined recovery, while minimum heart rate (HR) and work to rest ratio (WR) strategies were used as comparative conditions. Duplicate PR trial performances were similar but intercondition comparisons identified that the HR trial was significantly slower than both WR and PR conditions. There was no difference in performance between WR and PR, but recoveries for both PR trials were significantly shorter than for WR. Since the aim of interval training is to sustain performance with the shortest possible recovery time, the concurrent use of RPE and PR scales appears to be a useful psychophysiological technique to self- determine both work and rest in interval training.
Article
The purpose of the present study is to examine the effects of mental fatigue and motivation on neural network dynamics activated during task switching. Mental fatigue was induced by 2 h of continuous performance; after which subjects were motivated by using social comparison and monetary reward as motivating factors to perform well for an additional 20 min. EEG coherence was used as a measure of synchronization of brain activity. Electrodes of interest were identified using a data-driven pre-processing method (ten Caat, M., Lorist, M.M., Bezdan, E., Roerdink, J.B.T.M., Maurits, N.M., 2008a. High-density EEG coherence analysis using functional units applied to mental fatigue. J. Neurosci. Meth. 171, 271-278; ten Caat, M., Maurits, N.M. and Roerdink, J.B.T.M., 2008b. Data-driven visualization and group analysis of multichannel EEG coherence with functional units. IEEE T. Vis. Comp. Gr. 14, 756-771). Performance on repetition trials was faster and more accurate than on switch trials. EEG data revealed more pronounced, frequency specific fronto-parietal network activation in switch trials, while power density was higher in repetition trials. The effects of mental fatigue on power and coherence were widespread, and not limited to specific frequency bands. Moreover, these effects were independent of specific task manipulations. This increase in neuronal activity and stronger synchronization between neural networks did not result in more efficient performance; response speed decreased and the number of errors increased in fatigued subjects. A modulation of the dopamine system is proposed as a common mechanism underlying the observed the fatigue effects.