ArticlePDF Available

The anticipation of telephone calls: A survey in California

Authors:
  • Institute of Noetic Sciences

Abstract

A sample of 200 randomly selected people was surveyed in Santa Cruz County, California, to investigate the frequency and nature of anticipations of telephone calls. Of those surveyed, 78% said that they have had the experience of telephoning someone who said that they were just thinking about telephoning them; 47% said that they had had the experience of knowing who was calling them when the phone rang without any possible cue; and 68% said that they had thought about a person whom they have not seen for a while; who had then telephoned them that same day. A higher proportion of women than men gave positive answers to these questions. These results are in general agreement with 2 previous surveys in England, although there were several significant differences, which are discussed. These surveys reveal that seemingly telepathic experiences in connection with telephone calls are remarkably common. The authors suggest ways that this phenomenon can be investigated empirically.
Journal of Parapsychology 65, 145-156 (2001)
THE ANTICIPATION OF TELEPHONE CALLS: A SURVEY IN CALIFORNIA
By David Jay Brown and Rupert Sheldrake
ABSTRACT
200 randomly-selected people were surveyed in Santa Cruz County, California to investigate the frequency and nature of anticipations of
telephone calls. 78% of the people surveyed said that they have had the experience of telephoning someone who said that they were just
thinking about telephoning them. 47% of the respondents said that they had had the experience of knowing who was calling them when the
phone rang without any possible cue. 68% of those surveyed said that they had thought about a person that they haven't seen for a while,
who had then telephoned them that same day. A higher proportion of women than men gave positive answers to these questions. These
results are in general agreement with two previous surveys in England, although there were several significant differences, which we discuss.
These surveys reveal that seemingly telepathic experiences in connection with telephone calls are remarkably common. We suggest ways
that this phenomenon can be investigated empirically.
It is not uncommon for people to telephone friends or relations who say that they were just thinking of calling them. Likewise, many people
have had the experience of thinking of a particular person, with whom they have not spoken to for a while, who then calls them on the
telephone later the same day. Some domestic animals, notably dogs and cats, also seem to anticipate telephone calls from particular people,
their owners, while ignoring calls from other people (Sheldrake, 1999).
Some of these experiences might be explicable in terms of chance coincidence; but others might depend on telepathy, which is what many
people who have had these experiences believe. Surprisingly, this well-known phenomenon has been almost completely neglected by
psychical researchers and parapsychologists.
To find out how common this type of experience is we conducted a telephone survey of randomly selected households in Santa Cruz County,
California. Two previous telephone surveys in England, one in London and the other in Greater Manchester, indicated that this type of
experience occurs quite frequently (Sheldrake, 2000). In both surveys, more than half the respondents said they had felt that someone was
about to telephone them just before they did so. We wanted to find out if this phenomenon was similarly common in an American population.
We also asked respondents if they had had other kinds of seemingly telepathic experiences.
In a previous survey in Santa Cruz about perceptive pets, we found that significantly more pet owners said they had had a psychical
experience than people without pets (Brown & Sheldrake, 1998). Also in the English surveys, there was a tendency for more pet owners to
say they had anticipated telephone calls than people without pets (Sheldrake, 2000). To find out whether these observations were
repeatable, in this survey we also asked people whether or not they had a pet
We conclude by discussing ways in which seemingly telepathic telephone calls can be investigated empirically.
Method
Data were collected by means of telephone interviews conducted by David Brown and Sherry Hall (D.B. and S.H.), following a standard
questionnaire format. D.B. conducted 173 of these interviews and S.H. 27. The pattern of responses obtained by the two interviewers was
very similar, and there were no statistically significant differences between them.
The households surveyed were in and around Santa Cruz (population 52,700) in north-west California. Santa Cruz was chosen because it is
where D.B. lives, and calling within the local area helped to minimize the cost of the study. Also, we have already surveyed this area for
another study (Brown & Sheldrake, 1998) regarding the unexplained powers of animals, and we are building up a database from this region
about people's experiences of unexplained phenomena.
A random sample of households was selected from the Pacific Bell Santa Cruz County 1999 telephone directory (area code 831), using an
electronic random number generator to determine the page and column number, as well as its position on the page (Brown & Sheldrake,
1998).
D.B. and S.H. introduced themselves as follows: "Hello, my name is David Brown/Sherry Hall. I'm conducting a very brief telephone survey. I
was wondering if you had around two minutes to answer a few quick questions? I am not selling anything. The survey is about telephone
calls." Approximately 20% of the people reached by phone agreed to participate in the survey. This relatively low participation rate was
similar to that in a previous telephone survey in the same area (Brown & Sheldrake, 1998) and may reflect a reluctance of people there to be
bothered with such surveys. There was no mention of anything telepathic or psychic at this stage, so the respondents' agreement to take part
could not have been influenced by any bias for or against psychic phenomena.
When a cooperative subject was found, D.B. or S.H. then asked a series of questions and recorded the answers on a standard form as
follows:
1. Have you ever telephoned someone who said they were just thinking about telephoning you?
Yes/No/Not Sure
If yes: How often has this happened?
Telepathy _ California Survey
file:///C|/sheldrake/papers/Telepathy/california_survey.html (1 of 7) [04/01/2006 18:06:59]
Often/Sometimes/Once
2. Have you ever heard the telephone ring or picked up the telephone and known who was on the other end without any possible cue, before
they have spoken?
Yes /No/Not Sure
If yes: How often has this happened?
Often/Sometimes/Once
3. Has this ever happened when you were using a mobile phone?
Yes/No/Not Sure
4. Have you ever had a thought about a person you haven't seen for a while who has then telephoned you the same day?
Yes/No/Not Sure
If yes: How often has this happened?
Often/Sometimes/Once
5. Approximately how many telephone calls do you receive a day?
Less than 5/ 5 to 10/ More than 10
6. Apart from experiences like this with telephone calls, have you had any other experiences that seem to involve telepathy?
Yes/No/Not Sure
If yes: How often has this happened?
Often/Sometimes/Once
7. Do you have a pet?
Yes/No
The name, address and telephone number of each respondent were recorded, and their sex noted.
For the statistical comparisons, 2x2 contingency tables were used (Campbell, 1989).
Results
Two hundred people participated in this survey in Santa Cruz, 78 men and 122 women.
More than three quarters (78%) of the people surveyed said that they have had the experience of telephoning someone who said they were
just thinking about telephoning them (Table 1). This was the highest positive response in the survey. About a quarter of these people said
they had this experience often, but most said it happened "sometimes".
TABLE I
SURVEY RESULTS OF TELEPATHIC TELEPHONE CALLS IN SANTA CRUZ, CALIFORNIA
1. Have you ever telephoned someone who said they were just thinking about telephoning you?
Numbers (and percentages) of respondents
Yes No Not sure
All respondents 155 (78%) 31 (16%) 14 (7%)
Men 57 (73%) 15 (19%) 6 (8%)
Women 98 (80%) 16 (13%) 8 (7%)
Telepathy _ California Survey
file:///C|/sheldrake/papers/Telepathy/california_survey.html (2 of 7) [04/01/2006 18:06:59]
If yes, how often has this happened?
Often Sometimes Once
All respondents 35 (23%) 113 (73%) 7 (5%)
Men 8 (14%) 45 (79%) 4 (7%)
Women 27 (28%) 68 (70%) 3 (3%)
2. Have you ever heard the telephone ring and known who was on the other end without any possible cue, before they have
spoken?
Numbers (and percentages) of respondents
Yes No Not Sure
All respondents 93 (47%) 80 (40%) 27 (14%)
Men 34 (44%) 35 (45%) 9 (12%)
Women 59 (48%) 45 (37%) 18 (15%)
If yes, how often has this happened?
Often Sometimes Once
All respondents 35 (37%) 56 (60%) 2 (2%)
Men 14 (40%) 18 (53%) 2 (2%)
Women 21 (36%) 38 (64%) 0 (0%)
Note There were 200 respondents in all, 78 men and 122 women
3. Has this ever happened when you were using a mobile phone?
Numbers (and percentages) of respondents
Yes No Not sure
All respondents 15 (8%) 176 (88%) 9 (5%)
Men 7 (9%) 67 (86%) 4 (5%)
Women 8 (7%) 109 (89%) 5 (4%)
Telepathy _ California Survey
file:///C|/sheldrake/papers/Telepathy/california_survey.html (3 of 7) [04/01/2006 18:06:59]
4. Have you ever thought about a person you haven't seen for a while, who has then telephoned the same day?
Numbers (and percentages) of respondents
Yes No Not sure
All responents 125 (63%) 43 (22%) 32 (16%)
Men 46 (59%) 21 (27%) 11 (14%)
Women 79 (65%) 22 (18%) 21 (17%)
If yes, how often has this happened?
Often Sometimes Once
All respondents 19 (15%) 97 (78%) 9 (7%)
Men 4 (9%) 40 (89%) 2 (4%)
Women 15 (19%) 57 (72%) 7 (9%)
5. Approximately how many telephone calls do you receive a day?
Numbers (and percentages) of respondents
<5 5-10 >10
All respondents 98 (49%) 64 (32%) 38 (19%)
Men 37 (47%) 20 (26%) 21 (27%)
Women 61 (50%) 44 (36%) 17 (14%)
6. Apart from experiences like this with telephone calls, have you had any other experiences that seem to involve telepathy?
Numbers (and percentages) of respondents
All respondents 90 (45%) 71 (36%) 39 (20%)
Men 31 (40%) 31 (40%) 16 (20%)
Women 59 (48%) 40 (33%) 23 (19%)
If yes, how often has this happened?
Often Sometimes Once
All respondents 20 (22%) 67 (74%) 3 (3%)
Telepathy _ California Survey
file:///C|/sheldrake/papers/Telepathy/california_survey.html (4 of 7) [04/01/2006 18:06:59]
Men 5 (16%) 25 (81%) 1 (3%)
Women 15 (25%) 42 (71%) 2 (3%)
When asked about their own responses, 47% of the respondents said they had known who was calling them when the telephone rang
without any possible clue, and 37% of these people said this happened often. This question focused attention on the respondents' intuitions
about who was calling them while the telephone was actually ringing, or while they were picking it up to answer it. But when asked if they had
thought about a person they had not seen for a while who then telephoned the same day, 63% gave a positive answer. Most of these people
said it happened only sometimes, but 15% said it happened often (Table 1).
A minority of the respondents (8%) said they had known who was calling them when the phone rang when they were using a mobile phone.
However, many of the people surveyed did not have mobile phones.
On average, 49% of the respondents said they received fewer than 5 telephone calls a day, 32% received between 5 and 10 telephone calls
a day, and 19% received more than 10 (Table 1). People who received more than 10 calls a day gave a slightly higher proportion of positive
answers to questions 2 and 4 than those who received fewer calls, but these differences were not significant statistically.
In addition to their intuitions about telephone calls, 45% of the respondents said that they had had other experiences that seemed to involve
telepathy, and just under a quarter of these people (22%) said that their seemingly telepathic experiences happened often.
Most people (68%) who said they had had other seemingly telepathic experiences gave a positive answer to question 2, in striking contrast to
those who said they had not had other seemingly telepathic experiences (27%) or were not sure (33%). This difference in the positive
response to question 2 between those who said they had had other telepathic experiences and those who said they had not or were not sure
was very significant statistically ( p =5x10 -6 ). This is not surprising, and simply means that most people who seemingly experienced
telepathy in connection with telephone calls also did so in other contexts, and most of those who were not seemingly telepathic in relation to
calls were not telepathic in other contexts. What is more surprising is that about a third of those who experienced telepathy in other contexts
did not seem to do so in relation to the telephone calls. Conversely more than a quarter of the people who said they had had no other kinds
of telepathic experience were seemingly telepathic in relation to telephone calls.
Similarly there were more positive answers to question 4 ("Have you ever had a thought about a person you haven't seen for a while who has
then telephoned you the same day?") from those who said they had had other kinds of telepathic experiences (77%) than from those who
said they had not (41%) or were not sure (69%). The difference between those who said they had had other telepathic experiences and those
who said they had not or were not sure was significant statistically ( p =.0002).
Differences between women and men
A higher proportion of women than men said that they had known who was calling them when the telephone rang (48% as opposed to 44%),
that they had thought about a person they had not seen for a while who then telephoned the same day (65% as opposed to 59%), that they
had called someone who said they were just thinking about calling them (80% as opposed to 73%), and that they had had other experiences
that seemed to involve telepathy (48% as opposed to 40%). A higher proportion of women than men also said they had had such
experiences often, except in the case of Question 2 (Table 1). However, none of these differences between women and men were
statistically significant at the p =.05 level.
About half the men and women surveyed received less than 5 calls per day, but a higher proportion of men than women received more than
10 calls per day (Table 1, Question 5).
Pet ownership
Sixty nine per cent of the respondents owned pets. There was little or no difference between the responses of pet owners and people without
pets to the various questions. To question 1, 77% of the pet owners and 79% of the non-pet-owners, said that they had had the experience of
telephoning someone who said he or she was just thinking about telephoning them. 47% of the pet owners and 45% of the non-pet-owners
said that they knew who was calling them when the phone rang without any possible cue. The largest difference between people with and
without pets was in the response to Question 4. More pet owners (65%) than non-pet owners (57%) said that they had thought about a
person that they haven't seen for a while, who had then telephoned them that same day. However, this difference was not statistically
significant at the p =.05 level.
The proportion of people who said that they had had other seemingly telepathic experiences was 45% both for pet owners and for people
without pets.
Discussion
Comparison with other surveys
These results are directly comparable with those of a recent survey in Bury, Greater Manchester, England (Sheldrake, 2000). People in Bury
were asked the same questions as people in Santa Cruz (except for the question about mobile phones, not asked in Bury). The surveys in
both places followed the same procedures and the same number of people was surveyed. The main difference between the surveys
themselves was that a higher proportion of people contacted at random agreed to take part in the survey in Bury, 70% as opposed to 20% in
Santa Cruz.
For some questions, the responses were very similar in both places: for Question 2, 47% in Bury and 49% in Santa Cruz answered yes, and
to Question 5, 47% in Bury and 45% in Santa Cruz answered yes. But there were also some significant differences. Santa Cruz had a higher
percentage of people who said that said that they had telephoned someone who said they were just thinking about telephoning them: 78% in
Telepathy _ California Survey
file:///C|/sheldrake/papers/Telepathy/california_survey.html (5 of 7) [04/01/2006 18:06:59]
Santa Cruz, compared with 65% in Bury (p<0.02). The biggest difference was in response to Question 3, concerning thinking about someone
who later called: 63% said yes in Santa Cruz as opposed to 45% in Bury (p<0.00005). We do not know the reasons for these significantly
higher percentages of positive responses in Santa Cruz. They may reflect both the cultural differences between Santa Cruz, with its liberal
Californian culture, and the more conservative culture of Bury, and also demographic differences: Santa Cruz as a beach and college town
has an unusually youthful population.
In the survey in London, people were asked only one question about telephone calls: "Have you ever felt that someone was going to
telephone you just before they did?" to which 51% said yes.
In London, significantly more women than men gave a positive response to this question. In Bury more women than men gave positive
responses to the other questions about telephone calls and about other kinds of telepathic experience, but as in Santa Cruz these differences
were not large enough to reach statistical significance at the p =0.05 level.
In both London and Bury, pet owners gave a higher percentage of positive answers than people without pets to all the questions about the
anticipation of telephone calls and other telepathic experiences, but these differences were not statistically significant. In Santa Cruz, there
was little difference between people with and without pets. This finding contrasts with a previous survey we carried out in Santa Cruz in which
significantly more pet owners than people without pets said they had had psychic experiences themselves (Brown & Sheldrake, 1998). We
do not know why these surveys have given such different results in this respect, and this question could only be resolved by more extensive
surveys involving larger samples.
In spite of these various differences, the English surveys and the present one agreed in showing that the anticipation of telephone calls from
particular people is common, and in all three surveys there was a tendency for women to give more positive answers than men.
In this survey 45% of the respondents said they had had experiences that seemed to involve telepathy apart from any seemingly telepathic
experiences with telephone calls. For comparison, in a review of national surveys, Haraldsson (1983) quoted a figure of 58% for the USA,
and an average of 32% in Western Europe claiming personal experience of telepathy. In a nationwide Gallup survey of American adults
conducted in 1990, a smaller proportion, 25% said they had had telepathic experiences, but in the same survey 75% said they had had at
least one kind of paranormal experience (Gallup & Newport, 1991). Palmer (1979) reported that 38% of the citizens he surveyed in Virginia
and 39% of the students said they had had an experience of "waking ESP". And in our previous survey in Santa Cruz (Brown & Sheldrake,
1998), 56% of the respondents said they had had a psychic experience at some point in their lives. Thus the figures in the present survey are
in same general range as those in previous reports.
Does the anticipation of telephone calls involve telepathy?
If telepathy really exists, then telephone calls would provide a good way of revealing it. Before calling someone on the telephone, it is
necessary to think about them and then dial their number. The caller's intention is focused on the individual being called, and this
concentration of intention might create favorable conditions for telepathy. Telepathy could also work the other way round. If A thinks about B,
by telepathy B might start thinking about A and then call A, who might then say, "I was just thinking about you."
However, although most people say they have known before answering the phone who was calling them, or have received calls from people
they had just been thinking about, this does not necessarily imply a form of telepathic communication, for at least four reasons.
First, some people may merely pretend to have been thinking about someone before they call, for example because they feel guilty about not
having called them when they should have done. Second, some people may have given misleadingly positive responses to our questions,
perhaps out of a desire to please the interviewers. Third, consciously or unconsciously people may have had reason to expect calls from
particular people for reasons that have nothing to do with telepathy, for example because they usually call on a particular day, or at a
particular time. Fourth, people may frequently think about people who do not subsequently call, and simply forget all the occasions they were
wrong; the seemingly telepathic anticipation of telephone calls may be an illusion that arises through a combination of chance coincidence
and selective memory.
These are all reasonable possibilities, but at present there is no evidence either for or against them. They are, however, not quite as plausible
as they might at first seem.
Although some people may have given misleadingly positive responses in our survey, some may also have given misleadingly negative
responses out of shyness or an unwillingness to admit to a stranger on the telephone that they had had experiences of a potentially
controversial nature. In informal settings, the percentage of people who claim to have had seemingly telepathic anticipations of telephone
calls is often as high as 80 to 90% (Sheldrake, 2000). So these formal surveys may well have given an underestimate rather than an
overestimate of the frequency of the phenomenon.
Moreover, many people may be capable of distinguishing between calls that are expected for normal reasons and ones that seem to imply
telepathy. And many may be quite capable of recognizing and remembering that they are sometimes wrong. People may not be as prone to
illusion and self-deception as skeptics commonly suppose (Blackmore, 1997).
It cannot be assumed that people are wrong when they say they have had telepathic experiences, just as it cannot be assumed that they are
right. The question can only be decided by empirical investigation.
The empirical investigation of seemingly telepathic telephone calls
There are at least two ways in which this phenomenon can be investigated empirically.
First, people who say they often anticipate calls can be encouraged to keep log books near to the telephone in which they record any
intuitions they may have about the identity of the caller before they pick up the receiver. (Of course, any caller ID system should be switched
off for the purpose of this research.) After the call, they note down the date and time, record who the caller was, indicate whether their guess
was right or wrong, and say whether the call was expected or not. From such log books it can be worked out how often people are right or
wrong, if the calls from particular people show any routine pattern, and if the success rate is higher with some people than others. Expected
Telepathy _ California Survey
file:///C|/sheldrake/papers/Telepathy/california_survey.html (6 of 7) [04/01/2006 18:06:59]
calls would be excluded from this analysis. These log books would provide valuable data on the natural history of the phenomenon and also
enable the selective memory hypothesis to be tested.
Second, experiments can be carried out in which callers are selected from a pool of potential callers at random, so that there is no normal
means by which the subject could guess who is calling. For this purpose the subject needs to nominate a number of potential callers (say
four), and times have to be chosen for experiments when the subject is by the telephone, not expecting other calls, and the potential callers
are available to take part. Then, in a given experiment, one caller is selected at random from the pool of four, and the times at which they call
are also selected at random. When the phone rings during the experimental period, the subject writes down before answering who she thinks
the caller is. The calls are also tape-recorded to provide an objective record. By chance, 25% of the subjects' guesses will be correct. If
scores are significantly above chance, this would imply a telepathic influence from the caller.
Telepathy in connection with telephone calls is either a widespread illusion, or one of the commonest forms of telepathic experience in the
modern world. Simple empirical investigations could enable this question to be resolved, and open up a fertile new field for parapsychological
research.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank all the people who took part in this survey, as well as Sherry Hall for her help in conducting it. This work was supported by the
Institute of Noetic Sciences, Sausalito, CA, the Lifebridge Foundation, New York and the Bial Foundation, Portugal.
REFERENCES
BLACKMORE, S.J. (1997). Probability misjudgment and belief in the paranormal: A newspaper survey. British Journal of Psychology, 88,
683-689.
BROWN, D.J., & SHELDRAKE, R. (1998). Perceptive pets: A survey in northwest California. Journal of the Society for Psychical Research,
62, 396-406.
CAMPBELL, R.C. (1989). Statistics for biologists. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
GALLUP, G.H., & NEWPORT, F. (1983). Belief in paranormal phenomena among adult Americans. Skeptical Inquirer, 15 (2), 137-146.
HARALDSSON, E. (1985). Representative national surveys of psychic phenomena. Journal of the Society for Psychical Research , 53,
145-158.
PALMER, J. (1979). A community mail survey of psychic experiences. Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research, 73, 221-251.
SHELDRAKE, R. (2000). Telepathic telephone calls: two surveys. Journal of the Society for Psychical Research, 64, 224-232.
SHELDRAKE, R. (1999). Dogs that know when their owners are coming home, and other unexplained powers of animals. New York: Crown.
P.O. Box 1082
Ben Lomond
CA 95005, USA
20 Willow Road
London NW3 1TJ
England
Telepathy _ California Survey
file:///C|/sheldrake/papers/Telepathy/california_survey.html (7 of 7) [04/01/2006 18:06:59]
... Approximately simultaneous changes in mental charge states may result in what are often perceived as synchronicities or "entanglement." For instance, when a pair of individuals simultaneously think of each other (in what might be revealed in the form of an aptly timed email or phone call (Brown & Sheldrake, 2001), coherent internal biological and electrochemical processes may determine the timing. The partners or "halves" of the pair may experience a simultaneity in their thoughts and actions, but do not need to be present in the same physical location for there to be effects of a significant coherence between them. ...
... The idea of a conscious or empathetic field has been proposed by many others before in a variety of ways (Mansfield, 1996;Brown & Sheldrake, 2001;Tubert-Oklander, 2007;McTaggart, 2007). But a conscious field is likely not to be a "quantum field" that takes advantage of mysterious acausal "entangled" mechanisms or an actual manifestation of the brain outside the body, except perhaps as it exists in culture and other impacts on the surrounding environment. ...
Article
Full-text available
Transpersonal psychology involves an approach to behavior and the self that transcends ordinary states of mind as well as extends to the larger environment as a whole. Treating the individual, their mind, and behavior in relation to others and the larger natural system utilizing properly developed and practically applicable concepts from physics, chemistry, and biology may provide a successful interpretation that may be more powerful than the standard views of psychology. For instance, basic concepts such as charge pair attraction-repulsion, bonding, and synchronous behavior may be transformed into highly effective and even "spiritual" concepts that can add sophistication to human action, thought, and emotion in a way that is both more naturalistic and transcendent than current definitions in psychology.
... There is no analogue for such ability amongst the known bodily communication channels. However, there is good evidence that in everyday life average people have a weak version of this capacity, for instance being able to detect if someone is staring at them (Sheldrake, 2005) or thinking about them (Sheldrake, 2000;Sheldrake & Brown, 2001). These data are controversial exactly because proponents of the mainstream view accept that the known bodily sensori-kinetic channels cannot mediate such perceptions (Kurtz, 1985). ...
... Under ordinary circumstances, reflecting good soul-body integration (eupraxia), people typically have weak (but not insignificant) psychonetic abilities, enabling them for instance to detect when they are being stared at, to know when a remote friend is thinking about them, to sense that a remote loved one is in crisis, to engender useful "co-incidences," and to facilitate or hinder the smooth working of equipment (Gurney, Myers, & Podmore, 1886;Sheldrake, 1999Sheldrake, , 2000Sheldrake, , 2002Sheldrake, , 2005Sheldrake & Brown, 2001;Walach & Schmidt, 2005, pp. 58-60). ...
Article
In this paper I support the view that NDEs provide empirical sup-port for mind-body substance dualism and argue that a systems-theoretical an-alysis of the evidence is required to obtain valid insights into the nature of the mind as a substantial object existing in addition to the body. Without such an approach, systems phenomena such as property emergence and property mask-ing could lead to mischaracterization of both the nature of the mind itself and the ways in which the mind and body work together holistically. Applying a systems-theoretical perspective, I show that some psychic abilities are emergent capacities of the mind-body system, that ordinary faculties such as emotional perceptiveness can be understood within the same framework as extraordinary faculties such as telepathy, and that NDE evidence favors a naturalistic form of Substance Dualism.
... Many people say that they have had telepathic experiences in connection with telephone calls, emails, SMS messages and instant messaging systems (Brown & Sheldrake, 2001;Sheldrake, 2000Sheldrake, , 2003. Typically, people say they started thinking about somebody for no apparent reason who shortly afterwards called them on the telephone, sent an email, or text message, or they felt who was calling when the phone rang before looking at the caller ID or answering the phone. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: We bring together results from 15 published papers describing 26 telecommunication telepathy experiments published between 2003 and 2024 in a meta-analysis to explore the patterns in these results and their overall significance. Methods: The basic experimental design in these experiments involved four potential callers in remote locations. For each trial one of these callers was chosen at random and asked to call the participant, who was on a telephone without a caller ID. The participant then named the caller before answering the phone. Similar experiments were carried out with email and SMS messages. By random guessing, the hit rate would be around 25%. We collected relevant studies from reference lists and online searches and used a random-effects model in the meta-analysis. Results: Overall, hit rates were very significant above chance level (p = 1x10-7). By contrast, in tests carried out under precognitive conditions, the hit rates were at chance. There was no significant difference between the results of Sheldrake and his colleagues, who carried out most of the studies, and independent replications. Selected participants had significantly higher hit rates than unselected participants, and hit rates were significantly higher when callers and participants shared an emotional bond. The effect sizes in telecommunication telepathy are higher than those in ganzfeld and dream telepathy tests. Conclusion: Research on telecommunication telepathy could become an increasingly fruitful area for psi research, especially in conjunction with automated intuition training apps.
... One of the most common kinds of apparent telepathy occurs in connection with telephone calls (Sheldrake, 2000(Sheldrake, , 2003Brown & Sheldrake, 2001). Most people claim to have had experiences in which they think of someone for no apparent reason, then that person calls; or they know who is calling when the phone rings before picking it up; or they call someone who says "I was just thinking about you!" ...
Article
It is not always easy to draw a dividing line between true scientific theo- ries, erroneous but verifiable scientific theories and pseudo-scientific ones that are impos- sible to verify. As an example, this paper presents researches on various phenomena of extra-sensory perception, precognition, clairvoyance, distance communication, premonition for receiving SMS messages, phone calls or e-mails, which are classified in the domain of telepathy research. They were conducted within different paradigms, with diverse re- search methodologies and approaches, from anecdotal experiences, case studies and intui- tive reasoning, to careful recording of data and their statistical processing. In recent years, researches have been conducted using state-of-the-art “brain-to-brain” technology, which is used to register brain activities of the research participants. Telepathy has not remained uninteresting even to some modern physicists who find in Einstein’s theory of relativity and the laws of quantum physics the possibility of postulating a new quantum model of telepathy. This new, unconventional approach to the research of telepathy is conditioned by the paradigm shift in the research of this phenomenon and significant shifts from the pseudo-scientific to the scientific approach. This paper aims to point out the necessity of setting a clear demarcation line between scientific and pseudo-scientific research and building the ability of critical thinking, which is a condition for differentiating science from pseudoscience. Using a specific example of the phenomenon of telepathy, we underlined the importance of research in the development of scientific knowledge, but the kind of research that meets the reproducibility and verifiability criteria that clearly differentiate science from pseudoscience.
... The study of telepathy is perhaps not so unwisely eschewed by a skeptical scientific establishment. At present, it remains difficult to be able to refer to a consistently reproducible experimental example of a pure form of it, although there have been some suggestive hints of it reported recently (e.g., Brown and Sheldrake, 2001). ...
Article
Full-text available
A novel approach is introduced to explain the appearance of telepathy-like effects as the consequence of disturbances in the normal equilibration or 'equilibrated non-equilibrium' of ordinary human experience. A new electromagnetic version of Freud's psychoanalytic model is proposed, which begins by treating the mind as a charged object that interacts with others and the environment in a generally balanced pairwise fashion. If the brain is assigned a net charge for a given state of mind, there may be considered to be a balancing of superego, ego, and id level interactions. A temporary altered state of consciousness may result from an over or undercharged experience (from a social perturbation or unconscious wish) that would otherwise be processed during normal habituated experience as part of interdependent behavior during conscious awareness or sleep. Deviations from the normal "equilibration" of such balanced charge states that are not immediately cathected or decathected during personal interactions may result in the deceptive perception of telepathy or a neurosis-like symptom, perhaps in the form of a remembered dream. However, a genuine telepathy-like phenomenon may be postulated to exist due to synchronistic effects when considering the unique aspects of synchronous human behavior and possible subconscious information acquisition through inference. Electrochemical charges and energies may be assigned to the thoughts, emotions and actions of the human body and these may often be psychologically and socially coordinated and coherent with others: two people may change state at the same time. A macroscopically relevant coherence may result in approximately simultaneous thoughts and actions, and even a form of knowledge, between separated people.
... Tales experiencias son el tipo más común de telepatía en el mundo moderno (Sheldrake, 2000, 2003, Brown & Sheldrake, 2001. Sorprendentemente, los investigadores psíquicos y parapsicólogos parecen haber ignorado este fenómeno. ...
Article
Full-text available
_Revista Argentina de Psicología Paranormal_ RESUMEN Muchas personas afirman saber quien llama antes de atender el teléfono, o haber pensado en alguien sin razón aparente, y la persona luego llama. Llevamos cabo una serie de experimentos para testear si la gente podía o no decirnos realmente quien estaba llamando por teléfono. Cada participante tuvo cuatro posibles "llamadores" potenciales, y cuando el teléfono sonaba se les invitaba a decir quien estaba llamando antes que la otra persona hablara. La probabilidad estadística de éxito se calculó en un 25% de un total de 571 ensayos no videograbados, involucrando a 63 participantes. El resultado global fue del 40% con un 95% de confiabilidad dentro de los límites entre 36 a 45%. El efecto fue altamente significativo (p= 4x10 -16). Investigamos subsiguientes pruebas con cuatro participantes bajo condiciones más rigurosas, de las cuales fueron videograbadas las sesiones experimentales, y estas videocintas evaluadas en forma independientemente por un sujeto a "ciegas" de los detalles experimentales. De un total de 271 ensayos videograbados, el rango de éxito fue de 45% (p= 1x10-12). El nivel de confiabilidad fue de un 95% dentro de un rango de éxito de entre el 39% al 51%. Los participantes tuvieron mucho más éxito con llamadas de familiares que con llamadas de extraños y esta diferencia fue estadísticamente significativa. No hubo efecto de declinación con la distancia, aún cuando algunos llamadores se encontraban a 18.000 km. de distancia. Estos efectos parecen ser inexplicables en términos de habiliades o fraude y produjo una fuerte evidencia de la realidad de la telepatía telefónica. Abstract. Many people claim to have known who was calling before they picked up the telephone, or to have thought about someone for no apparent reason, and that person then called. We carried out a series of experiments to test whether or not people really could tell who was telephoning. Each participant had four potential callers, and when the telephone rang had to guess who was calling before the other person spoke. By chance the success rate would have been 25%. In a total of 571 non-videotaped trials, involving 63 participants, the overall success rate was 40%, with 95% confidence limits from 36 to 45%. This effect was highly significant statistically (p= 4x10-16). We then carried out further trials with four participants under more rigorous conditions in which they were videotaped throughout the experimental sessions, and the videotapes were evaluated independently by a person blind to the experimental details. In a total of 271 videotaped trials the success rate was 45% (p= 1x10-12). The 95% confidence limits of this success rate were from 39% to 51%. Participants were much more successful with familiar callers than unfamiliar callers, and this difference was highly significant statistically. There was no decline with distance, even when callers were 18.000 km. away. These effects do not seem to be explicable in terms of artefacts or cheating and provide strong evidence for the reality of telephone telepathy.
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated possible telepathic communication in connection with e-mails. On each trial, there were four potential e-mailers, one of whom was selected at random by the experimenter. One minute before a prearranged time at which the e-mail was to be sent, the participant guessed who would send it. 50 participants (29 women and 21 men) were recruited through an employment web site. Of 552 trials, 235 (43%) guesses were hits, significantly above the chance expectation of 25%. Further tests with 5 participants (4 women, 1 man, ages 16 to 29) were videotaped continuously. On the filmed trials, the 64 hits of 137 (47%) were significantly above chance.
Article
Some people claim to occasionally know who is calling them without using traditional means. Controlled experiments testing these claims report mixed results. We conducted a cross-sectional study of triads examining the accuracy of knowing who was calling using two randomly selected designs: 1) a web server randomly chose the caller before the callee's guess (telepathic/pre-selected trials), and 2) a web server randomly chose the caller after the callee's guess (precognitive/post-selected trials). We also performed exploratory multilevel mixed-effects logistic regressions on the relationship of genetic relationships, emotional closeness, communication frequency, and physical distance data with accuracy. A total of 177 participants completed at least one trial (105 “completers” completed all 12 trials). Accuracy was significantly above chance for the 210 completers telepathic/pre-selected trials (50.0% where the chance expectation was 33.3%, p<.001) but not the 630 completers precognitive/post-selected trials (31.9% where the chance expectation was 33.3%, p = .61). We discuss how these results favor the psi hypothesis, although conventional explanations cannot be completely excluded. Genetic relatedness significantly predicted accuracy in the regression model (Wald χ2 = 53.0, P < .001) for all trials. Compared to 0% genetic relatedness, the odds of accurately identifying the caller was 2.88 times (188%) higher for 25% genetic relatedness (Grandparent/Grandchild or Aunt/Uncle or Niece/Nephew or Half Sibling; β = 1.06, z = 2.10, P = .04), but the other genetic relatedness levels were not significant. In addition, communication frequency was significant (β = 0.006, z = 2.19, P = .03) but physical distance (β = 0.0002, z = 1.56, P = .12) and emotional closeness (β = 0.005, z = 1.87, P = .06) were not for all trials. To facilitate study recruitment and completion, unavoidable changes to the protocol were made during the study due to persistent recruitment difficulties, including changing inclusion/exclusion criteria, increasing total call attempts to participants, adjusting trial type randomization schema to ensure trial type balance, and participant compensation. Thus, future research will be needed to continue to improve the methodology and examine the mechanism by which people claim to know who is calling, as well as factors that may moderate the effects.
Book
Full-text available
Las alucinaciones son percepciones que ocurren en ausencia de estímulos sensoriales correspondientes, pero desde el punto de vista subjetivo del individuo que las experimenta son indistinguibles de la percepción normal. Algo es percibido, pero objetivamente no hay nada para percibir. La alucinación presenta tres características; en primer lugar, existe la convicción de que el fenómeno tiene su origen fuera de uno mismo, hay una falta de control por parte del individuo que in¬tenta distinguir entre las alucinaciones y, por ejemplo, la imaginación; y en tercer lugar, existe una imposibilidad, o por lo menos una dificultad, de alterar o disminuir la experiencia en forma voluntaria. En buena medida, lo disfuncional de una experiencia alucinatoria dependerá de la respuesta social. Definitivamente, existe diferencia entre una persona que experimenta un delirio místico y una que tiene una experiencia mística. Es diagnóstico psicológico de una persona que dice ver a Jesucristo o a la Virgen María en un santuario mariano no recibe el mismo juicio de integridad mental que una persona que está en un hospital neuropsiquiátrico. Además, individuos que mantienen conversaciones con espíritus son menos propensos a padecer empeoramiento funcional, en tanto y en cuanto, estén rodeados por personas que acepten su experiencia, que si están rodeados por individuos que menosprecian o rechazan sus experiencias. En conclusión, el autor sostiene que las alucinaciones también pueden ser observadas en personas que no presentan ningún tipo de trastorno psicopatológico. Esta situación lleva a proponer una continuidad del fenómeno alucinatorio entre la normalidad y la patología. Más aún, el modelo de continuidad de la experiencia alucinatoria que se desarrolla en esta obra se basa principalmente en estudios epidemiológicos, y como varía la distribución de estos síntomas en la población general según cómo se mida el fenómeno y explorar otras experiencias perceptuales anómalas. Finalmente, se examina si resulta necesario una nueva palabra para suplir el termino “alucinación” a causa de la clara connotación patológica de la experiencia.
Article
Full-text available
A telephone survey of 200 households was carried out in North-West California to find out how many pet owners claim to have observed seemingly psychic abilities in their animals. 132 of the households surveyed had pets. 45% of dog owners claimed their animal knew in advance when a member of the household was on the way home, compared with 31% of cat owners, and around 20% of these animals were said to react more than 10 minutes in advance. 65% of dog owners and 37% of cat owners said their pets knew that they were going out before they showed any physical signs of doing so. 46% of dog owners and 41% of cat owners said that their pet responded to their thoughts or silent commands, and 42% of dog owners and 34% of cat owners said that their pet was sometimes telepathic with them. 49% of pet owners and 31% of non-pet owners said that some of the animals that they had known in the past were telepathic. Significantly more pet owners claimed to have had psychic experiences themselves than non-pet owners, and a significantly higher proportion of 'psychic' pet owners claimed that their pets exhibited psychic powers than 'non-psychic' owners. These findings are in general agreement with a previous survey in England. Some implications of these results are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Many people claim to have thought about a particular person who then calls them on the telephone. Through informal surveys I have found that seemingly telepathic telephone calls are common. Two telephone surveys were carried out in London and Bury to investigate the frequency of these experiences in a random sample of the population. In both surveys, half the respondents said they had felt that someone was about to telephone them just before they did. In Bury, 45 per cent of the respondents said they had thought about a person they had not seen for a while who then telephoned the same day, and two thirds of the Bury respondents said they had telephoned people who said they were just thinking about telephoning them. In Bury, 37 per cent of respondents who said they had know in advance who was calling without any possible clue said this happened often. In both surveys significantly more women than men gave positive responses, and in both surveys more pet owners than people without pets gave positive responses. In London significantly more people claimed to have anticipated telephone calls that to have had psychic experiences. Telepathic telephone calls may be one of the commonest kinds of psychic experience in the modern world, and I suggest ways in which they can be investigated empirically.
Article
In March, 1974, a 46-item questionnaire was mailed to a randomly selected sample consisting of 300 students from the University of Virginia and 700 other adult residents of Charlottesville and surrounding suburbs. Respondents were asked to report the incidence and detailed characteristics of various psychic and psi-related experiences. Information concerning attitudes and the personal impact of such experiences was solicited, along with demographic data. Usable questionnaires were obtained from 89% of the student sample and 51% of the town sample. Claims of such experiences were widespread: over half of the respondents claimed at least 1 extrasensory perception (ESP) experience, for example. There was a tendency for persons who claimed these experiences to claim a large number of them. Variables related to naturally-occurring altered states (e.g., vividness and frequency of dream recall) tended to be strong predictors of such experiences, while demographic variables generally were poor predictors. Many respondents indicated that experiences of this kind had affected their attitudes toward life and/or life-styles. (10 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Reviews results of national surveys conducted from 1974–1984 in Iceland, the UK, Sweden, the US, and Western Europe, which show widespread belief in the existence of psychic phenomena, although they also reflect national differences. Findings reveal that the majority of the Ss believed in some psychic phenomena and that at least 6 in every 10 Ss interpreted some of their personal experiences as paranormal in nature. In some countries the role of education was positively related to reports of psychic experience. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Probability misjudgment and belief in the paranormal: A newspaper survey
BLACKMORE, S.J. (1997). Probability misjudgment and belief in the paranormal: A newspaper survey. British Journal of Psychology, 88, 683-689.
Representative national surveys of psychic phenomena
HARALDSSON, E. (1985). Representative national surveys of psychic phenomena. Journal of the Society for Psychical Research, 53, 145-158.
Dogs that know when their owners are coming home, and other unexplained powers of animals
  • R Sheldrake
SHELDRAKE, R. (1999). Dogs that know when their owners are coming home, and other unexplained powers of animals. New York: Crown.