Available via license: CC BY-NC 3.0
Content may be subject to copyright.
Evolutionary Psychology
human-nature.com/ep – 2006. 4: 85-94
¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯
Original Article
The Intensity of Human Body Odors and the MHC: Should We Expect A
Link?
Claus Wedekind, Department of Ecology and Evolution, Biophore, University of Lausanne, 1015
Lausanne, Switzerland; Program for Evolutionary Dynamics, and Department of Organismic and
Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA; Zoological Institute,
University of Bern, 3032 Hinterkappelen, Switzerland. Email: claus.wedekind@unil.ch.
Thomas Seebeck, Institute of Cell Biology, Uni
v
ersity of Bern, 3012 Bern, Switzerland.
Florence Bettens, Institute of Immunology, University Hos
p
ital Bern, 3010 Bern, Switzerland.
Alexander J. Paepke, Zoological Institute, University o
f
Bern, 3032 Hinterkappelen, Switzerland.
Abstract: It is now well established that genes within the m
ajor histocompatibility
complex (MHC) somehow affect the production of body odors in several vertebrates,
including humans. Here we discuss whether variation in the intensity of body odors
may be influenced by the MHC. In order to examine this question, we have to control
for MHC-linked odor perception on the smeller’s side. Such a control is necessary
because the perception of pleasantness and intensity seem to be confounded, and the
causalities are still unsolved. It has previously been found that intense odors are
scored as less pleasant if the signaler and the receiver are of MHC-dissimilar type, but
not if they are of MHC similar type. We argue, and first data suggest, that an effect of
the degree of MHC-heterozygosity and odor intensity is likely (MHC-homozygotes
may normally smell more intense), while there is currently no strong argument for
other possible links between the MHC and body odor intensity.
Keywords: Odor intensity, MHC homo- and heterozygosity, 'good genes'
hypothesis,
'compatible genes' hypothesis, T-shirt experiment.
¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯
Introduction
The physiology and the biological significance of human body odors is
multifaceted and not yet well understood yet (Beauchamp and Yamazaki, 2005;
Grammer et al., 2005; McClintock et al., 2005; Yamazaki and Beauchamp, 2005;
Ziegler et al., 2005). The reason for this lack of knowledge seems obvious: odors are
often short-lived and difficult to store, hard to describe, and still very difficult to
analyse chemically, at least in the quantities that can be relevant in social contexts.
The Intensity of Human Body Odors and the MHC: Should We Expect A Link?
Despite these analytical problems and the shortage of hard data, humans seem to vary
in the intensity of their odors, even if we correct for behavioral and environmental
factors. Among humans, there is variation in the size and the number of glands on the
skin (Stoddart, 1990), variation in gland activity (Kreyden et al., 2002), and obvious
variation in the number and density of those body hairs that amplify odors through
their support of microorganisms and their role in odor diffusion. However, our
personal perception can easily mislead us when it comes to the generality of odor
quality and intensity.
Whether or not an odor is perceived as intense depends not only on the kind
and the amount of volatile molecules that are emitted (the strength of the signal), but
also on factors that influence the perception of the odor (the sensitivity of the
receiver). With regard to the latter, there are many factors that may play a role. For
example, it seems that women are usually more sensitive to body odors than men
(Dalton et al., 2002), and that the sensitivity for odor components can be trained in
women during their reproductive age (Dalton et al., 2002). Also, the perception of
body odors varies within the menstrual cycle (Doty et al., 1981; Gangestad and
Thornhill, 1998; Rikowski and Grammer, 1999), changes with pregnancy (Gilbert
and Wysocki, 1991), generally decreases with age (Wedekind and Füri, 1997) and
appears to be influenced by the contraceptive pill (Wedekind et al., 1995; Thorne et
al., 2002).
The perception of body odors is also linked to genes of the MHC (Yamazaki
et al., 1976; Penn and Potts, 1999; Ziegler et al., 2005). These genes can be quite
important in explaining odor preferences. In T-shirt experiments, when the influence
of many potentially confounding variables is reduced, up to 23 % of the variance in
pleasantness could be explained by the degree of similarity at the loci of the MHC
between T-shirt wearer and smeller (Wedekind & Füri 1997). In mice, the estimates
are even higher and reach up to 50% (Ziegler et al., 2005). The intensity of one and
the same body odor can be perceived very differently by different individuals. In
Wedekind & Füri (1997), when 121 students rated the same six odors for intensity,
pleasantness and sexiness, all the odors received nearly all possible scores from very
weak to very intense. In one case (Wedekind et al. 1995), a women even reported
nausea for several hours after smelling the experimental T-shirts. She specifically
linked her nausea to one odor which she rated as very intense and very unpleasant,
while other smellers scored this particular odor as less intense and quite pleasant. In
this context, it may be interesting that this woman was using the contraceptive pill at
the time of the experiment. Pregnancy, which is partly simulated by the pill, is often
linked to nausea and vomiting as a response to odors (Heinrichs, 2002).
Wedekind et al. (1995) found that the perception of pleasantness and of
intensity of an odor are correlated if smeller and odor source have dissimilar MHC
types, but not if they are of similar MHC type. Since non-related individuals are
normally dissimilar at the MHC , we would expect that, as a general tendency,
weaker body odors are normally rated as more pleasant than more intense ones. This
expectation was confirmed by Wedekind & Füri (1997). However, the causality
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 4. 2006.
- 86 -
The Intensity of Human Body Odors and the MHC: Should We Expect A Link?
remains unclear. Is an odor perceived as unpleasant because it is intense, or as intense
because it is unpleasant?
Not controlling for MHC-linked odor perception can introduce disturbing
biases in an analysis of odor intensity. Vice versa, a lack of control for odor intensity
can confound or weaken an analysis of MHC-dependent preferences (Wedekind and
Seebeck, 1996; Wedekind, 2002; Roberts et al., 2005). It is therefore necessary to
disentangle possible MHC effects on odor intensity from MHC-dependent odor
perception. In a previous study we used an experimental method that allows to test
for, and to control for, MHC-dependent variation in odor perception within a
population (Wedekind et al., 1995). Here we use the data of Wedekind et al. (1995)
to analyse, on the one hand, intensity ratings of receivers that are either dissimilar or
similar to the signaller’s MHC, i.e. we control for the effects of the MHC by
analysing possible effects within the two experimental groups. On the other hand, we
calculate MHC-neutral odor scores that can be used as estimates of the average
intensity of body odors. We use these neutral scores to ask whether the intensity of a
man’s odor is in any way connected to his MHC genotype. We test three questions: 1)
Is the degree of MHC heterozygosity linked to odor intensity? 2) Are the odors of
carriers of most common MHC antigens generally perceived as differently intense
than other body odors? 3) Are men with more intense odors also more similar to each
other in their MHC genotypes than expected by chance? Effect size approximations
are used to quantify the observed trends in the data and to estimate the significance of
each possible link between MHC and odors intensity. We use our analyses to discuss
several hypotheses on the biology of MHC-linked odor signalling in humans.
Methods
The 38 men whose odors are studied here were on average 24.5 years old (SD
= 2.2) and students of the University of Bern at the time of the experiments. They all
appeared to be of Caucasian origin, spoke the Swiss German dialect without any
obvious accents, and had forenames and surnames that are common in the German
speaking part of Switzerland. They were typed for their MHC (HLA-A, -B, and –DR)
and asked to wear T-shirts during two nights. The odors of these worn T-shirts were
subsequently evaluated by women who had an MHC type that was either similar (on
average 2.7 dissimilar antigens) or dissimilar (on average 5.9 dissimilar antigens) to
the T-shirt wearer’s MHC. See Wedekind et al. (1995) for a detailed description of
the methods and for an analysis of the general effects of the MHC on these odors and
on female preferences for them.
We use three procedures to control for MHC-linked odor preferences. First,
there is evidence for an interaction between MHC-linked preferences and the
contraceptive pill (Wedekind et al., 1995). Since the exact nature of this interaction is
not fully understood yet, we only used evaluations of women who did not use the
contraceptive pill during the time of the experiments (we could therefore only use a
subset of the data of Wedekind et al. (1995) in the present analyses). Second, each
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 4. 2006.
- 87 -
The Intensity of Human Body Odors and the MHC: Should We Expect A Link?
odor was evaluated by on average 4.7 women (range 2 to 12) and at least once each
by a woman with similar or dissimilar MHC type, respectively. We tested for possible
links between odor intensity and the MHC within each to the two experimental
groups (“similar” and “dissimilar”). Third, we averaged the ratings of all MHC-
similar and MHC-dissimilar women and used the mean of these two average ratings
to obtain scores that includes the ratings of many women but are neutral with respect
to the degree of MHC similarity between the man and the smelling women.
We used parametric test statistics when graphical inspections of the data
suggested that the respective model assumptions were met. In order to test whether
the men’s MHC type is correlated to the intensity of their body odors, we used the
following test procedures: (i) We tested whether the degree of MHC heterozygosity is
linked to odor intensity in two-sample t-tests; (ii) we used again two-sample t-tests to
test whether the presence or absence of the most common MHC antigens in the study
population was linked to the average odor intensity; (iii) we grouped the men into two
groups of N=19 each according to their odor intensity (“strong” versus “weak”). We
then tested whether these two groups also clustered with respect to the average degree
of similarity on the MHC. This was done by comparing the mean number of
dissimilar antigens of all pairwise comparisons within each group to a null
expectancy. This null expectancy was derived from 1000 random permutations of the
above procedure (randomization test). Power analyses for two-sample t-tests were
done online on www.stat.uiowa.edu/~rlenth/Power/, assuming equal variances and
equal sample sizes.
Results
Of the 38 men tested, 17 (45%) were typed as homozygous for at least one
MHC antigen. On average, their odors were not significantly more intense than those
of MHC heterozygotes (Table 1). The estimated effect size (in average standard
deviations) was d = 0.552. A power analysis revealed that if we missed an existing
effect (type II error), one would have an 80% change of finding it at p = 0.05 if the
sample size were about tripled (Table 1). However, three men were homozygous for
two of the three loci, and their average body odor intensity was even slightly lower
than the intensity of the other homozygotes.
Since we have two average scorings for each odor, one by MHC-similar and
one by MHC-dissimilar smeller, we could also test for the effect of the signallers’
MHC homozygosity within each group of smellers. We found that the intensity scores
of MHC-similar smellers were not significantly influenced by homozygosities on the
signaller’s side, while MHC homozygotes smelled significantly more intense to
MHC-dissimilar smellers than MHC heterozygotes (Figure 1). These intensity scores
did not seem to be significantly influenced by the perception of pleasantness (within
each experimental group,|t| always < 0.82, p always > 0.42). However, when all mean
scores per signaller were correlated, pleasant odors tended to be less intense ones
(Spearman rank order correlation coefficient r
s
= -0.46, p = 0.003).
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 4. 2006.
- 88 -
The Intensity of Human Body Odors and the MHC: Should We Expect A Link?
Figure 1: The average odor intensity of T-shirts worn by men who are either
heterozygous on all the MHC loci analysed, or who are homozygous on at least one
of these loci. The figure gives the mean (+SE) scorings of MHC similar (open bars)
and MHC-dissimilar smellers (closed bars) for the same 38 odors each. The p-values
are two-tailed.
MHC-dissimilar
smellers
p = 0.02
MHC-similar
smellers
p = 0.77
Odor intensity
4
5
6
7
8
No Yes No Yes
Homozygous on MHC loci
The most common MHC antigen in the population is by far HLA-A2, with a
prevalence of 50% in the present sample, and around 50% in comparable samples
(Grundschober et al. 1994, Wedekind & Füri 1997, Milinski and Wedekind 2001).
We found no significant effect of this antigen (Table 1) or any of the eight other most
common antigens that Milinski & Wedekind (2001) listed (|t| always < 1.33, p always
> 0.19). The group of homozygotes were not dominated by men who possessed HLA-
A2 (only 37% of them are homozygous on any MHC locus, χ
2
= 0.47, p = 0.49).
Therefore, the second analysis in Table 1 is not confounded with the first. The
estimated effect size for HLA-A2 as grouping factor was d = 0.339. We found
analogous non-significant results if we tested for a possible effect of HLA-A2 within
the scorings of the MHC-similar and MHC-dissimilar smellers, respectively (|t|
always < 0.8, p always > 0.42).
Men with more intense odors do not have significantly more similarities in
their MHC genotypes than expected by chance (Table 1). The one-tailed
randomization tests did not reveal any tendencies. It turned out that exactly half of all
men with HLA-A2 were each in the group “intense” and the group “weak” (χ
2
= 0.0,
p = 1.0). Hence, the results of the third test in Table 1 can be seen as statistically
independent of the second test.
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 4. 2006.
- 89 -
The Intensity of Human Body Odors and the MHC: Should We Expect A Link?
Table 1: Tests for possible MHC effects on the intensity of male body odors. MHC-
linked perception is controlled for by using weighted mean scorings of MHC-similar
and MHC-dissimilar smellers.
Question
Analysis*
Test**
N
min
***
Conclusion if
tendency indicated
real effect
(i) Effect of MHC-
homozygosity?
(17 of 38 men are
homozygote)
µ
1
= 5.21 (1.87)
µ
2
= 6.17 (1.59)
t = 1.67
P
2
= 0.10
106
MHC homozygotes
would smell more
intense than others.
(ii) Effects of most
common MHC antigen?
(18 of 38 men have
HLA-A2)
µ
1
= 5.35 (1.82)
µ
2
= 5.96 (1.76)
t = 1.04
P
2
= 0.30
276
Men with the most
common MHC antigen
would smell more
intense than others.
(iii) Average similarities
in MHC-types
- among 19 men with
relatively intense odors?
- among 19 men with
relatively weak odors?
Randomization test
µ
obs
= 4.62, µ
exp
= 4.61
µ
obs
= 4.78, µ
exp
= 4.61
P
1
= 0.51
P
1
= 0.97
No tendency towards
any clustering of MHC-
type with odor intensity
* Mean (SD)
** P
2
= two-tailed, P
1
= one-tailed
*** The number of male odors that would be needed to demonstrate an effect (if it exists) at α = 0.05
with a probability of 80%.
Discussion
MHC-linked odor and mate preferences are typically seen as today’s best
example of sexual selection for compatible genes, i.e. for a choice of genetic
complementarity (reviews in Mays and Hill, 2004; Neff and Pitcher, 2005). In such
compatible-genes sexual selection models, the intensity of a signal is not necessarily
expected to be linked to its information content (Trivers, 1972; Wedekind, 1994;
Tregenza and Wedell, 2000). Alternatively, the so called “good-genes” models of
sexual selection (Zahavi, 1975; Grafen, 1990; Møller and Alatalo, 1999) predict a
correlation between signal intensity and health and vigor. The argument here is that
health and vigor is expected to depend on overall genetic quality, and only
individuals in good health and vigor can afford the costs of the signal.
We may often assume that the intensity of a signal is positively correlated
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 4. 2006.
- 90 -
The Intensity of Human Body Odors and the MHC: Should We Expect A Link?
with health and vigor, because stronger signals are normally more expensive to
produce or maintain. In the case of body odors, however, it is not immediately clear
whether an odor that is perceived as intense (i) is an indicator of better overall health
and vigor (Zahavi, 1975), or (ii) is perceived as intense because intensity rating is
confounded with pleasantness rating, and unpleasant odors tend to be rated as intense
regardless of the amount of volatiles that are involved. In the second case, high-
quality odor components, i.e. components that are more expensive to the signaller and
more attractive to the perceiver, may cause an odor to be rated as more pleasant and
hence less intense. At the moment, we have to leave it open which scenario is more
likely for human body odors. Nevertheless, the two types of sexual selection models
mentioned above still make clear predictions for the case of body odors. If MHC-
linked sexual selection is indeed about finding genetically compatible mates, we
would not expect a link between odor intensity and the specificity of MHC antigens.
If, however, some MHC-antigens or antigen-combinations are better adapted to
current challenges than others, body odors pleasantness and/or intensities may be
correlated to the MHC because they reveal an individual’s health and vigor (as seems
to be the case for spurs in pheasants (von Schantz et al., 1996)). As one possibility,
we may then predict a link between odor intensity and the specificity of MHC
antigens. Our effect-size estimations and the power analyses suggest that such a link
does not exist or is very weak in the case of human body odors. This observation for
itself contradicts the good-genes sexual selection hypothesis and is in agreement with
the compatible-genes sexual selection hypothesis (Neff & Pitcher 2005).
Brown (1997) suggested that the overall degree of heterozygosity or the
degree of heterozygosity on certain key loci may be positively linked with an
individual’s health and vigor, and that sexual signals may therefore reveal
heterozygosity at these loci. In the case of the MHC, it seems that these loci are still
under selection in many human populations, because MHC heterozygotes are
normally more frequent than expected under Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (Hedrick
and Thomson, 1983; Black and Hedrick, 1997). However, it is not entirely clear
whether this is due to natural or sexual selection, or both (Apanius et al., 1997;
Prugnolle et al., 2005). If natural selection is responsible for these findings, MHC
heterozygotes are generally in better health and vigor than people with homozygous
MHC loci. Brown’s (1997) hypothesis would then predict a link between MHC
heterozygosity and characteristics that are used for mate selection. Indeed, Robert et
al. (2005) recently found a link between MHC heterozygosity and facial
attractiveness in humans. In their experiments, MHC heterozygosities were scored as
more attractive, and when the scores were specifically asked for, faces of MHC
heterozygous subjects were perceived as “healthier” than faces of people with
homozygosities at the MHC locus.
In the case of body odors the direction of a possible correlation between MHC
heterozygosity and signal intensity is not immediately obvious. Our first results and
the effect size estimates suggest that there could be a difference in odor intensity
between MHC heterozygotes and homozygotes, with homozygotes smelling more
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 4. 2006.
- 91 -
The Intensity of Human Body Odors and the MHC: Should We Expect A Link?
intensely, especially for MHC-dissimilar smellers (i.e. for most individuals in an
outbred and MHC-diverse population). However, we do not know whether MHC
heterozygosity and health and vigor are correlated in our study population.
Alternatively to Brown’s (1997) hypothesis, it is possible that there is no link
between MHC heterozygosity and the signaller’s health and vigor, and that the
differences between MHC homozygotes and heterozygotes are explained by some yet
unknown constraints in the physiology of odor production.
In conclusion, if we control for MHC-linked perception we find few
indications for a possible link between body odor intensity and MHC specificity. It
appears that MHC homozygotes produce body odors that are on average perceived as
more intense than those of heterozygotes. If such a link can be verified in future
studies, it may indicate a statistical link between MHC homozygosity and general
health and vigor or, alternatively, it may contribute to a better understanding of the
physiology of MHC-linked odor production.
Acknowledgements
We thank the participants for making this study possible, E. Frei, S. Füri, K.
Viragh, B. Streb and M. Perlen for technical assistance, and C. Dulac, S. Edwards, M.
Walker, and an anonymous reviewer for discussion and/or helpful comments on the
manuscript. The study was supported by the Roche Foundation, the Swiss National
Science Foundation, and a Sarah and Daniel Hrdy Visiting Fellowship to CW.
Received 20 September, 2005; Revision received 22 March, 2006; Accepted 22
March, 2006.
References
Apanius, V., Penn, D., Slev, P. R., Ruff, L. R. and Potts, W. K. (1997). The nature of
selection on the major histocompatibility complex. Critical Reviews in
Immunology, 17: 179-224.
Beauchamp, G. K. and Yamazaki, K. (2005). Individual differences and the chemical
senses. Chemical Senses, 30: I6-I9.
Black, F. L. and Hedrick, P. W. (1997). Strong balancing selection at HLA loci:
Evidence from segregation in South Amerindian families. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences USA, 94: 12452-12456.
Brown, J. L. (1997). A theory of mate choice based on heterozygosity. Behavioral
Ecology, 8: 60-65.
Dalton, P., Doolittle, N. and Breslin, P. A. S. (2002). Gender-specific induction of
enhanced sensitivity to odors. Nature Neuroscience, 5: 199-200.
Doty, R. L., Snyder, P. J., Huggins, G. R. and Lowry, L. D. (1981). Endocrine,
cardiovascular, and psychological correlates of olfactory sensitivity changes
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 4. 2006.
- 92 -
The Intensity of Human Body Odors and the MHC: Should We Expect A Link?
during the human menstrual cycle. Journal of Comparative Physiology and
Psychology, 95: 45-60.
Gangestad, S. W. and Thornhill, R. (1998). Menstrual cycle variation in women's
preference for the scent of symmetrical men. Proceedings of the Royal Society
of London Series B, 265: 927-933.
Gilbert, A. N. and Wysocki, C. J. (1991). Quantitative assessment of olfactory
experience during pregnancy. Psychosomatic Medicine, 53: 693-700.
Grafen, A. (1990). Biological signals as handicaps. Journal of Theoretical Biology,
144: 517-546.
Grammer, K., Fink, B. and Neave, N. (2005). Human pheromones and sexual
attraction. European Journal of Obstetrics Gynecology and Reproductive
Biology, 118: 135-142.
Grundschober, C., Sanchez-Mazas, A., Excoffier, L., Langaney, A., Jeannet, M.,
Tiercy, J. M. (1994) HLA-DPB1 DNA polymorphism in the Swiss
population: linkage disequilibrium with other HLA loci and population
genetic affinities. European Journal of Immunogenetics, 21: 143-157.
Hedrick, P. W. and Thomson, G. (1983). Evidence for balancing selection at HLA.
Genetics, 104: 449-456.
Heinrichs, L. (2002). Linking olfaction with nausea and vomiting of pregnancy,
recurrent abortion, hyperemesis gravidarum, and migraine headache.
American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 186: S215-S219.
Kreyden, O. P., Böni, R. and Burg, G. (eds.). (2002). Hyperhidrosis and botulinum
toxin in dermatology . Karger, Basel.
Mays, H. L. and Hill, G. E. (2004). Choosing mates: good genes versus genes that are
a good fit. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 19: 554-559.
McClintock, M. K., Bullivant, S., Jacob, S., Spencer, N., Zelano, B. and Ober, C.
(2005). Human body scents: Conscious perceptions and biological effects.
Chemical Senses, 30: I135-i137.
Milinski, M. and Wedekind, C. (2001). Evidence for MHC-correlated perfume
preferences in humans. Behavioral Ecology, 12: 140-149.
Møller, A. P. and Alatalo, R. V. (1999). Good-genes effects in sexual selection.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences,
266: 85-91.
Neff, B. D. and Pitcher, T. E. (2005). Genetic quality and sexual selection: an
integrated framework for good genes and compatible genes. Molecular
Ecology, 14: 19-38.
Penn, D. J. and Potts, W. K. (1999). The evolution of mating preferences and major
histocompatibility complex genes. American Naturalist, 153: 145-164.
Prugnolle, F., Manica, A., Charpentier, M., Guegan, J. F., Guernier, V. and Balloux,
F. (2005). Pathogen-driven selection and worldwide HLA class I diversity.
Current Biology, 15: 1022-1027.
Rikowski, A. and Grammer, K. (1999). Human body odour, symmetry and
attractiveness. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B, 266: 869-
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 4. 2006.
- 93 -
The Intensity of Human Body Odors and the MHC: Should We Expect A Link?
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 1474-7049 – Volume 4. 2006.
- 94 -
874.
Roberts, S. C., Little, A. C., Gosling, L. M., Perrett, D. I.,
Carter, V., Jones, B. C.,
Penton-Voak, I. and Petrie, M. (2005). MHC-heterozygosity and human facial
attractiveness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26: 213-226.
Stoddart, D. M. (1990). The scented ape. The biology and culture of human odour.
Cam
bridge University Press.
Thorne, F., Neave, N., Scholey, A., Moss, M. and Fink, B. (2002). Effects of putative
ma
le pheromones on female ratings of male attractiveness: Influence of oral
contraceptives and the menstrual cycle. Neuroendocrinology Letters, 23: 291-
297.
Tregenza, T. and Wedell, N. (2000). Genetic compatibility, ma
te choice and patterns
of parentage: Invited review. Molecular Ecology, 9: 1013-1027.
Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. - In: Campbell, B.
(ed.) Sexual selection and the descent of m
an. Aldine Press, pp. 136-179.
von Schantz, T., Wittzell, H., Goransson, G., Grahn, M. and Persson, K. (1996).
MHC genotype and m
ale ornamentation: Genetic evidence for the Hamilton-
Zuk model. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B, 263: 265-
271.
Wedekind, C. (1994). Handicaps not obligatory in sexual selection for resistance
genes. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 170: 57-62.
Wedekind, C. (2002). The MHC and body odors: arbitrary effects caused by shifts of
mean pleasantness.
Nature Genetics, 31: 237-237.
Wedekind, C. and Füri, S. (1997). Body odour preferences in men and wom
en: do
they aim for specific MHC combinations or simply heterozygosity?
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B, 264: 1471-1479.
Wedekind, C. and Seebeck, T. (1996). MHC and ma
te selection in humans? Reply.
Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 11: 24-25.
Wedekind, C., Seebeck, T., Bettens, F. and Paepke, A. J. (1995). MHC-dependent
mate preferences in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London
Series B, 260: 245-249.
Yamazaki, K. and Beauchamp, G. K. (2005). Chemosensory recognition of olfactory
individuality. Chemical Senses, 30: I142-i143.
Yamazaki, K., Boyse, E. A., Mike, V., Thaler, H. T., Mathieson, B. J., Abbott, J.,
Boyse, J., Zayas, Z. A. and Thomas
, L. (1976). Control of mating preference
in mice by genes in the major histocompatibility complex. Journal of
Experimental Medicine, 144: 1324-1335.
Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection - a selection for a handicap. Journal of theoretical
Biology, 53: 205-214.
Ziegler, A., Kentenich, H. and Uchanska-Ziegler, B. (2005). Fema
le choice and the
MHC. Trends in Immunology, 26: 496-502.