ArticlePDF Available
SOCIAL reasoning embodies expla-
nations for why events happen —
where issues such as what hap-
pened, who is responsible and who is
innocent or guilty are debated. A
common cultural reaction to incidents of
sexual violence is, as the extract on the
right illustrates, that negative and
derogatory judgements are often
directed at the victim in addition to, or
even instead of, the perpetrator of the
incident.
Angus Diggle, a lawyer, had been
found guilty of attempted rape of a col-
league. In journalist Anne Robinson’s
explanation for the incident, it is the per-
petrator who is defined as the innocent
victim of a feckless ‘sexually provoca-
tive’ woman. It is the perpetrator’s
behaviour which is normalised, exoner-
ating him from responsibility for the act,
and the victim who is positioned as
deviant and blameworthy.
Secondary
victimisation
Blaming a victim for being raped1is a
form of ‘secondary victimisation’, where
victims can be made to feel guilt or
shame because they are further
victimised by negative feedback about
their conduct. Secondary victimisation
can have a number of profound conse-
quences.
Davis and Breslau (1994) argue that
victim blaming is related to the onset of
prolonged negative psychological
effects. For example, 57 to 80 per cent
of self-defined victims of rape have
been shown to meet the criteria for life-
time post-traumatic stress disorder,
experiencing symptoms such as depres-
sion and persistent re-visualisation of the
crime.
Secondary victimisation has also been
linked to the under- reporting of rape to
the police, close friends and relatives
(Renner et al., 1988; Koss & Harvey, 1991)
and may seriously limit the perceived
accessibility of institutional support for
rape survivors. For example, Lees (1993,
1997) discusses how rape trials are tanta-
mount to ‘judicial rape’ — ‘a spectacle of
degradation visited upon the victim
rather than the offender’ (1997, p.73).
Rape myths
Feminist standpoint research has been
crucial to the development of the litera-
ture on rape, and has attracted
substantial attention from the discipline
of psychology (Ward, 1995). Feminist
scholars have argued that the tendency
to attribute responsibility to the victims
and exonerate the perpetrators of rape is
underpinned by a number of myths
about rape, namely ‘prejudicial, stereo-
typed or false beliefs about rape, rape
victims and rapists’ (Burt, 1980, p.217).
There are a number of rape myths
which operate within a range of profes-
sional and everyday contexts. We list five
of the most common here:
1. Women precipitate rape by their
behaviour or appearance.
2. Rape is not damaging because, after
all, it is only sex.
3. Real rape victims have signs of injury
to prove it because you can’t be raped
against your will.
4. Women often lie about rape because
they are malicious and deceitful.
5. Real rapists are psychopathic individ-
uals.
Rape myths are embedded within and
reinforce culturally dominant, stereotyp-
ical assumptions about femininity,
masculinity and the nature of normative
heterosexuality (Brownmiller, 1975;
Nicolson, 1994; Ussher, 1997). Russell
(1982) has argued that these myths are
widely and uncritically accepted, creat-
ing a ‘rape-supportive’ culture which is
hostile to women in general and to rape
survivors in particular.
Many rape myths suggest that
women should take responsibility for the
control of male sexual behaviour. They
Talking about rape
The Psychologist December 1998 583
Perpetuating rape-supportive culture
Talking about rape
Kathy Doherty and
Irina Anderson
examine social
reasoning about rape
and some of its
negative
consequences for the
victims.
‘Mr Diggle, given the circumstances,
behaved as you would imagine any
half-drunk, virile man would. If any
damage has been done to the reputa-
tion of the legal profession, it is by
the stupid, unnamed woman who
apparently continues to earn her
living as a lawyer yet clearly pos-
sesses not an ounce of common
sense.’
Anne Robinson, Daily Mirror, 15
February 1995
Kathy Doherty Irina Anderson
also trivialise the severity of a rape expe-
rience through construing it as a
potentially pleasurable sexual act rather
than as an act of violence and oppression.
For many people, rape myths provide
a ‘commonsense’ resource for making
sense of rape incidents. However, there
now exists a volume of research which
suggests that these beliefs are essentially
mythical. For example, Bonney (1985)
indicates that 44 per cent of rape victims
present no signs of physical injury what-
soever, because in many cases sexual
coercion is achieved through verbal or
psychological means.
There is also little evidence to support
the view that women commonly lie
about rape. Studies have estimated that
false reports of rape account for between
only 1 and 4 per cent of alleged rapes —
not significantly different from other vio-
lent crimes against the person such as
robbery (Ward, 1995).
Contrary to the view that rapists are
necessarily psychopathic strangers, there
is some evidence to suggest that rapists
are likely to be ‘regular men’. For
example, the majority of victims are at
least acquainted with the offender and
some will know him very well (Parrott,
1985; London Rape Crisis Centre, 1982).
Also, Tieger (1981) found that 36 per cent
of male undergraduates agreed that they
would rape a woman if they ’could be
certain they would not be caught’.
Social definition
Although rape in theory is generally
regarded as a legally and morally unjus-
tifiable act, from the moment that a rape
survivor makes a public declaration that
she was raped, her claim will be scruti-
nised and debated.
The social environments in which
rape victims are the focus of attention are
characterised by a competition for the
appropriate meaning of the encounter
and disputes about the legitimacy of the
claim to rape victimhood. Aclaim of rape
may be challenged by arguing that the
event should be understood as consen-
sual sex rather than rape or, if it is
accepted that forced intercourse did
occur, through suggesting that the victim
is still responsible because she acted stu-
pidly or recklessly (Burt & Estep, 1981).
To illustrate, Caroline Carey claimed
to be the survivor of a brutal rape in
which her assailant, a former friend,
‘raped me, beat me and then threatened
to kill me’ (The Times, 13 April 1997).
Dublin’s Central Criminal Court sup-
ported her definition of the event as rape
and her assailant, Liam Sheehy, was con-
victed and is now serving a seven-year
sentence. However, members of their
local community continued to defend
Sheehy by arguing that the event should
be understood as consensual sex.
Villagers speculated that she probably
consented to (or even initiated) sex, but
‘got more than she bargained for’ when
things ‘got a bit rough’. By offering this
version of events, Carey is positioned as
blameworthy, and the legitimacy of her
victimhood is undermined.
As we have argued, such examples of
secondary victimisation can have enor-
mously damaging consequences. It is
therefore imperative that psychologists
contribute to the minimisation of post-
rape trauma by understanding the social
processes which perpetuate our rape-
supportive culture.
Social psychology
investigates
There are now two extensive literatures
within social psychology which focus on
reasoning about rape, both of which
can be located squarely within the
perceptual–cognitive paradigm.
Firstly, rape perception research
attempts to understand the process of
rape victim blaming within an attribu-
tion theory framework, by examining
individual interpretations of the causes
of rape incidents (Pollard, 1992; Krahé,
1991). Secondly, research grounded in
attitude theory tries to describe and mea-
sure attitudes towards rape and rape
victims, often in relation to a variety of
demographic or psychological variables
(Lonsway & Fitzgerald, 1994).
Although positivist research within
this tradition has been successful in
adding rhetorical weight to the concept
of ‘rape-supportive culture’ (Ward,
1995), we would like to argue that it
cannot provide an understanding of the
complex social processes which perpetu-
ate rape-supportive culture in everyday
talk. Social cognition research is limited
in this respect because it offers an eco-
logically invalid, oversimplistic view of
the way that blame and accountability
are managed in conversation (Edwards
& Potter, 1992; Beattie & Doherty, 1995;
Anderson & Doherty, 1997).
This oversimplification arises firstly
because such research conceptualises
explanation as individual and private,
ignoring joint sense-making practices in
public conversations (Antaki, 1994).
Secondly, the methodologies used serve
to separate explanation from its usual
venue of spontaneous argument and
conversation. For example, the non-
negotiable, straightforward attitude
statements on questionnaires requiring a
measurable, constrained response (e.g.
level of agreement) bear little resem-
blance to the flexible, active and subtle
way in which evaluative claims are made
in the dynamics of ordinary discourse
(Potter & Wetherell, 1988; Wetherell &
Potter, 1988, 1992).
Discursive
approach
We would like to argue that a discursive
psychological approach (DP) (Potter &
Wetherell, 1987; Edwards & Potter, 1992;
Edwards, 1997; Potter, 1996) to the analy-
sis of talk may provide a fruitful
alternative framework. Within this per-
spective, analysis is guided by
theoretical concerns which emphasise
the constructive and active nature of
talk. For example, one of the most impor-
tant characteristics of any stretch of talk
is its ‘could have been otherwise’ quality
(Edwards, 1997).
Descriptions of events have consider-
able actual and potential variability. So
claims about ‘what happened’ contain a
silent endorsement of particular
concepts at the expense of alternatives
(Billig, 1987). In this sense, talk does
not simply reflect the world, it consti-
tutes it.
DP argues that this flexibility of
description should be crucially under-
stood as meshed with, and oriented to,
the accomplishment of social activities
such as evaluation, explanation and
persuasion.2As the Caroline Carey
example illustrates, competing versions
of reality make possible a range of
inferences about the legitimacy of
victimhood.
Furthermore, DP draws our attention
to the subtle way in which issues of
blame and accountability are managed
in conversation. Edwards and Potter
(1992) argue that potentially contentious
blamings are produced indirectly, in
a way that effectively denies any
undesirable motivation on the speaker’s
part. This is done by presenting versions
that are apparently neutral and factual —
merely ‘commonsense’ descriptions of
the world ‘as it is’.
In the following analysis of fragments
from three conversations concerning a
hypothetical rape incident, we hope to
illustrate the way in which rape myths
that distribute blame towards the victim
are embedded implicitly in a joint
version of the rape event. These conver-
sations were generated by giving men
and women, in pairs, a rape description
that we view as a provisional version of
reality which the participants argue
with.
Although there is an acceptance in
these discussions that the victim was
raped, we hope to go some way towards
unpacking the way in which blame is
still allocated to the victim in a manner
that seems ‘reasonable’ and convincing.
In this way, we hope to gain an
insight into the subtle ways in which
rape-supportive culture is perpetuated
in talk.
Talking about rape
584 December 1998 The Psychologist
Conversations
Conversation 1 (C1)
Vernon(1): well yeah so here we’re saying
that she should have probably been aware
of this and not [been so foolish] as to take
a short cut but even so
Sally(2): maybe … she … yeah it does
seem that way doesn’t it, they should
be able to I think
Vernon(3): yeah but even so … people should
be able to to walk about in freedom but
Sally(4): but the trouble is you can’t
nowadays can you … I mean it’s too
dangerous
Vernon(5): well…
Sally(6): I mean people are forever telling
you on television you know whatever
you don’t
Vernon(7): yeah I think people are becoming
more and more aware of it
Conversation 2 (C2)
John(1): you would have thought she would
have been aware of the danger wouldn’t
you
Debbie(2): yeah it does seem like … you
know it was a silly thing to do really
John(3): yeah it does seem a bit strange I
mean … I agree with you … I mean but
yeah like I said
Debbie(4): but you can’t can you that’s
the trouble … people should be able
to walk about without being attacked
but given that it’s a violent … it’s a
violent society that we live in … er …
people have to be aware of the dan-
gers I mean it’s no good just walking
about with her head in the clouds is it
Conversation 3 (C3)
Alison(1): yeah but I dunno it reflects that
women always have to be careful I mean I
know blokes get beaten up and stuff but
you know you can still walk home and
stuff like that
Frank(2): oh well yeah I know it’s not fair
but it’s life innit
Alison(3): hmmm … I mean so that implies
that this poor woman has to spend her
whole life looking over her shoulder and
[inaudible]
Frank(4): [no not at all] but if you walk
back take the lit way where it’s like
well lit up instead of taking this huge
short cut across the badly lit field or
whatever
Discourse
analysed
What follows is by no means a complete
analysis of the texts presented.
Nevertheless, we hope to provide a
flavour of the way in which victim blam-
ing is indirectly accomplished and to
highlight some of the features which
make it seem reasonable and difficult to
undermine.
We are not suggesting that rape-
supportive culture is perpetuated by a
few ‘bad’ or prejudiced individuals. Our
focus is on the effects and consequences
of discursive resources and practices
which are culturally available and insti-
tutionally sanctioned.
In each of the conversations, the
speakers jointly develop a version of the
circumstances of the rape based on a
construction of what the world is like
‘nowadays’ (C1(4)). The definition which
is offered characterises society as a ‘dan-
gerous’ and ‘violent’ place where people
run the risk of being ‘attacked’ (C1(4);
C2(4)). It is also suggested that this char-
acterisation is increasingly taking hold
(C1(7)) through continual reinforcement
by ‘people … on television’ (C1(6)).
Feminist scholars have argued, how-
ever, that the specific nature and
consequences of perceived threats in the
public sphere are different for men and
women. Griffin (1971) suggests that the
‘unnamed fear’ which relentlessly fig-
ures as a ‘daily part of every woman’s
consciousness’ (p.27) is specifically the
fear of sexual violence. The fear of rape
exerts a powerful means of social control
over all women, often leading to the
adoption of behaviours such as not
going out alone (Riger & Gordon, 1979).
The participants in conversation 3
allude to this explicitly. They acknowl-
edge that although ‘blokes get beaten up
and stuff’ this is not the constant concern
that rape is for women, who ‘always
have to be careful’. The risk of violence
does not necessarily restrict men’s
behaviour in a general way; ‘you can still
walk home and stuff...’ (C3(1)).
The dangers to women are located by
speakers to be in predictable places such
as clearly demarcated, dark, unpopu-
lated ‘short cuts’ (C1(1); C3(4)). The
image of the ‘classic stranger rape’ is
reproduced in C3(4). Here the source of
fear — the rapist — lies in wait in the
shadows waiting to pounce on potential
victims, presumably in this case from
behind a large tree or hedge in ‘the field’.
The function of this is to suggest that
‘safe’ routes do exist and that they are
easily identifiable. This implication is
supported through the construction of
an unproblematic ‘lit way where it’s …
well lit up’ in extreme contrast to the
construal of the short cut as ‘huge ...
across the badly lit field’.
In these texts, we therefore see the
implicit reproduction of two common
rape myths. Firstly, that women are most
at risk from rape perpetrated by psycho-
pathic rapists who lurk in dark,
unpopulated public places. Secondly,
that rape is easily avoidable — women
will be safe if only they avoid ‘danger-
ous’ places.
It is against this backdrop of a partic-
ular construction of the circumstances of
the rape that the victim’s identity and
character are established. The victim is
variably categorised as ‘foolish’ (C1(1)),
‘silly’ (C2(2)) and naive, through the
suggestion that she walks about ‘with
her head in the clouds’ (C2(4)).
An important insight here is that we
select categories in talk to accomplish
social actions (Wowk, 1984; Edwards,
1991). Here, the selection of categories
works to allocate blame towards the
victim and away from the perpetrator.
The key point is that these derogatory
categorisations of the victim only appear
sensible and convincing because of the
implication that she consciously selected
an ‘obviously dangerous’ route home
when ‘safer’ alternatives were easily
available. It is important to note once
again that the victim could have been
categorised in an infinite number of dif-
ferent ways (e.g. perhaps as ‘rational’ for
taking the short cut because it’s quicker
— a common feature in our conversa-
tional data on male rape, not reported
here). This would make very different
implications available regarding her
blameworthiness.
Overall, we see that in offering a ver-
sion of accountability for the rape, the
participants focus on the behaviour and
personal characteristics of the victim and
not the perpetrator. The status of the
rape survivor as a legitimate victim is
undermined by the implication that she
precipitated her attack through her own
stupidity and recklessness. It is implied
that the victim only has herself to blame
because she placed herself in the path of
a rapist and, by her mere presence,
incited his ‘uncontrollable’ sexual urges.
There are several other features of the
content and organisation of the talk
which make the implicit attribution of
responsibility towards the victim seem
convincing. We focus on just one here.
In each of the conversations a particu-
lar kind of content, ‘pragmatic realism’
(Wetherell & Potter, 1988, 1992), is repro-
duced (e.g. C1(4); C2(4)). This
emphasises the constraints which exist in
practice. It functions to position speakers
as sensible because they are ‘realistic’
enough to recognise the constraints ‘out
there’ beyond the control of ordinary
folk which ‘inevitably’ inhibit our free-
dom. Also, by expressing regret about
these constraints, speakers appear rea-
sonable — e.g. ‘I know it’s not fair but
it’s life innit’ (C3(2)). This is a particu-
larly robust piece of rhetoric, as it
operates as a closed argument against
the possibility of social change.
Analysis can lead
to change
We have argued that it is necessary to
understand the social processes that lead
to the secondary victimisation of rape
survivors. This is imperative not only
because of the negative interpersonal
and institutional effects discussed above,
but also because of the material conse-
quences that flow from an emphasis on
Talking about rape
The Psychologist December 1998 585
the victim’s character and behaviour in
explanations for rape.
If victims are the focus of explanation,
then they also become the targets for
intervention. This then results in a preoc-
cupation with regulating the behaviour
of ‘foolish’ or ‘wayward’ women accord-
ing to the dictates of femininity (e.g. stay
at home, be passive, be modest), while
the seriousness of rape as a violent and
oppressive crime is undermined, and
rapists continue to go unpunished. In
1985, 24.4 per cent of the recorded rapes
in the UK resulted in a conviction. By
1994, the conviction rate had fallen to
just 8.4 per cent and many of these con-
victions were overturned on appeal
(Lees, 1996).
We argue that, to gain an insight into
the social mechanisms involved in the
perpetuation of rape-supportive culture,
it is necessary first to appreciate that the
negotiation of blame and responsibility
for rape incidents in open-ended
discourse is accomplished indirectly. We
feel that an ecologically valid approach
to research in this area must therefore
engage in a detailed exploration of the
way in which explanations for rape are
constructed and made to seem reason-
able.
We hope to have provided a flavour
of the type of analysis that such an enter-
prise might produce. We believe that this
work is an important step toward trans-
forming and undermining the damaging
discursive practices that shape the expe-
rience of many women in the aftermath
of rape.
‘What becomes clear is the impor-
tance of other people’s reactions towards
women who have been raped — how
much difference a clear, supportive,
positive response could make to a
woman’s self-image and the way she
views her experience.’ (Lees, 1993, p.25.)
Notes
1In this article, we discuss female rape only. See
Anderson and Doherty (1996a, b) for a discussion of
social reasoning about male rape.
2It might seem that our use of statistics and other
rhetorical devices in this article is inconsistent with
an otherwise postmodern, relativist epistemology.
However, we do so self-consciously to make our
argument convincing, and such reflexive practice is
consistent with a DP framework.
Acknowledgement
We would like to thank the members of the
Loughborough Discourse & Rhetoric Group, who
provided helpful comments on an earlier version of
this analysis.
References
Anderson, I. & Doherty, K. (1996a). The social con-
struction of rape in conversation. Paper delivered at
The British Psychological Society Women and
Psychology Conference, Bristol, July.
Anderson, I. & Doherty, K. (1996b). Constructions of
rape and consensual sex in conversation. Paper
delivered at The British Psychological Society
London Conference, London, December.
Anderson, I. & Doherty, K. (1997). Psychology, sex-
uality and power: Constructing sex and violence.
Feminism & Psychology, 7, 549–554.
Antaki, C. (1994). Explaining and Arguing. The Social
Organisation of Accounts. London: Sage.
Beattie, G. & Doherty, K. (1995). I saw what really
happened:The discursive construction of victims
and perpetrators in firsthand accounts of paramili-
tary violence in Northern Ireland. Journal of Language
& Social Psychology, 4, 408–433.
Billig, M. (1987). Arguing and Thinking: A Rhetorical
Approach to Social Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Bonney, R. (1985). Bureau of Crime Statistics and
Research Crimes: Sexual Assault Amendment Act.
Sydney: Attorney General’s Department.
Brownmiller, S. (1975). Against Our Will: Men,
Women and Rape. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Burt, M. (1980). Cultural myths and supports for
rape. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38,
217–230.
Burt, M.R. & Estep, R. (1981). Who is victim?
Definitional problems in sexual victimisation.
Victimology, 6, 15–28.
Davis, G. & Breslau, N. (1994). Post-traumatic stress
disorder in victims of civilian trauma and criminal
violence. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 17,
289–299.
Edwards, D. (1991). Categories are for talking: On
the cognitive and discursive bases of categorisation.
Theory & Psychology, 1, 515–542.
Edwards, D. (1997). Discourse and Cognition. London:
Sage.
Edwards, D. & Potter, J. (1992). Discursive
Psychology. London: Sage.
Griffin, S. (1971). Rape: The all-American crime.
Ramparts, September, pp.26–35.
Koss, M. & Harvey, M. (1991). The Rape Victim:
Clinical and Community Interventions. Newbury Park,
CA: Sage.
Krahé, B. (1991). Social psychological issues in the
study of rape. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds),
European Review of Social Psychology. Vol.2,
pp.279–309. Chicester: Wiley.
Lees, S. (1993). Judicial rape. Women’s Studies
International Forum, 16, 11–36.
Lees, S. (1996). Carnal Knowledge: Rape on Trial.
London: Penguin.
Lees, S. (1997). Ruling Passions: Sexual Violence,
Reputation and the Law. Buckingham: Open
University Press.
London Rape Crisis Centre (1982). Annual Report.
London: London Rape Crisis Centre.
Lonsway, K.A. & Fitzgerald, L.F. (1994). Rape
myths: In review. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18,
133–164.
Nicolson, P. (1994). Anatomy and destiny: Sexuality
and the female body. In P. Choi & P. Nicolson (Eds),
Female Sexuality: Psychology, Biology & Social Context.
London: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
Parrott, A. (1985). Comparison of acquaintance rape
in a college population. Cited in C. Ward (1995).
Attitudes Toward Rape: Feminist and Social
Psychological Perspectives. London: Sage.
Pollard, P. (1992). Judgements about victims and
attackers in depicted rapes: A review. British Journal
of Social Psychology, 31, 307–326.
Potter, J. (1996). Representing Reality: Discourse,
Rhetoric and Social Construction. London: Sage.
Potter, J. & Wetherell, M. (1987). Discourse and Social
Psychology: Beyond Attitudes and Behaviour. London:
Sage.
Potter, J. & Wetherell, M. (1988). Accomplishing atti-
tudes: Fact and evaluation in racist discourse. Text, 8,
51–68.
Renner, K.E., Wackett, C. & Ganderton, S. (1988).
The ‘social’ nature of sexual assault. Canadian
Psychology, 29, 163–173.
Riger, S. & Gordon, M. (1979). The structure of rape
prevention beliefs. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 5, 186–190.
Russell, D. (1982). Rape in Marriage. New York:
Macmillan.
Tieger, T. (1981). Self-rated likelihood of raping and
the social perception of rape. Journal of Research in
Personality, 15, 147–158.
Ussher, J. (1997). Fantasies of Femininity: Reframing the
Boundaries of Sex. London: Penguin.
Ward, C. (1995). Attitudes Toward Rape: Feminist and
Social Psychological Perspectives. London: Sage.
Wetherell, M. & Potter, J. (1988). Discourse analysis
and the identification of interpretative repertoires.
In C. Antaki (Ed.), Analysing Everyday Explanation: A
Casebook of Methods. London: Sage.
Wetherell, M. & Potter, J. (1992). Mapping the
Language of Racism. Discourse and the Legitimation of
Exploitation. Hemel Hempstead: Harvester
Wheatsheaf.
Wow k, M . (1984). Blame allocation, sex and gender
in a murder interrogation. Women’s Studies
International Forum, 7, 75–82.
Kathy Doherty is at the Department of
Communication Studies, School of Cultural
Studies, Sheffield Hallam University,
Collegiate Crescent, Sheffield S10 2BP.
E-mail: k.h.doherty@shu.ac.uk.
Irina Anderson is at the Department of
Psychology, University of East London,
Romford Road, London E15 4LZ. E-mail:
i.anderson@uel.ac.uk.
Talking about rape
586 December 1998 The Psychologist
... Bohner, Siebler & Schmelcher, 2006;Gerber & Cherneski, 2006;Hinck & Thomas, 1999;Murnen, Wright, & Kaluzny, 2002). Furthermore, rape myths promote self-blame by the victim, particularly in the case of acquaintance rape, an effect that may discourage women from reporting the crime, result in 'secondary victimisation' (Davis & Breslau, 1994) and deter victims from actively seeking recovery resources (Doherty & Anderson, 1998;Koss, Dinero, Seibel, & Cox, 1988). Therefore, as Davis and Lee (1996) have argued, current myths and stereotypes surrounding sexual assault reflect a society which exculpates perpetrators of assault and blames victims for failing to control men's sexuality, a set of circumstances which Doherty and Anderson (1998: 583) describe as enabling, or constituting, a 'rape-supportive culture'. ...
... Thus, building on the very small number of such studies in the literature (e.g. Anderson, 1999;Anderson et al., 2001;Doherty & Anderson, 1998;O'Byrne et al., 2006) here we examine how young men invoke the miscommunication, the victim precipitation and the social structural models in order to account for the phenomenon of rape. However, in addition to identifying the repertoires employed, we also explore the uses and functions to which they are put, and the consequences thrown up by their use ; see also Kurz, Donaghue, Rapley, & Walker, 2005). ...
... Here, the rhetorical effect of claiming insufficient knowledge of the subtle ways in which sexual refusals are normatively performed is to delete the accountability of men for rape. As such the data here suggest that, for these young men at least, the discursive resources of a rape-supportive culture (Doherty & Anderson, 1998) are readily available. Indeed, by maintaining the hegemonic repertoires of miscommunication and victim precipitation, the responsibility for rape that has long and widely been attributed to women is effectively sustained. ...
Article
Most psychological theories of rape tend to stress factors internal to both rapists and their victims in accounting for the phenomenon. Unlike such theories, social psychological and feminist accounts have drawn attention to social and cultural factors as productive of rape, and have criticized psychological accounts on the grounds that they often serve, paradoxically, to cement pre-existing ‘common-sense’. In this paper we examine the ways in which young Australian men draw upon widely culturally shared accounts, or interpretative repertoires, of rape to exculpate rapists. In particular, we discuss the reliance placed on a ‘lay’ version of Tannen's 1992 ‘miscommunication model’ of (acquaintance) rape and detail the use of this account—the claim that rape is a consequence of men's ‘not knowing’—as a device to accomplish exculpation. Implications of our methods for capturing young people's understanding of sexual coercion, rape and consent, and for the design of ‘rape prevention’ programmes, are discussed. Copyright © 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
... It is important to point out that sexual offences are often generalised as heterosexual offence [32] and therefore, there is very little known about male victims [33]. Recent figures indicate that around 12,000 males are victims of sexual assault or rape in the UK every year [34] and that nearly 1 in 4 men in the U.S. experienced some form of contact sexual violence in their lifetime. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Intense social and political changes in the past decades led to changes in crime rates and the way the public perceives crime. Although there is a concern about crime in general, there seem to be some types of offences that cause more distress to the general public than others, for example, sexual offences. They seem to receive significant amounts of public condemnation and represent the focus of many punitive policies. In order to understand criminality as an individual and a social problem, researchers began to study crime through the concept of criminal career. One of the ways of getting insight into this type of offenders is by exploring their criminal career referring to a longitudinal series of offences committed by a person which has a noticeable series of offences. The study of the criminal career of sexual offenders has the potential to bring new information to experts and legislators, contribute to a greater understanding of the continuity of transgression from adolescence to adult-hood and ensure better knowledge of the occurrence of sexual misconduct, how it begins, how it develops over time and how it ends.
... This is likely to be equally, if not more, distressing for gay and bisexual men who have yet to come to terms with their sexual orientation (Davies, 2002;Davies & Rutland, 2007). As already highlighted, a loss of masculine identity and/or change in one's subjective sexual Internalised homophobia, shame and self-blame following sexual assault can be compounded by victim blaming attitudes, disbelief, or being made to feel guilty for the assault (collectively known as secondary victimization; e.g., Doherty & Anderson, 1998) and homophobic reactions of those to whom the survivor discloses. Many men find it especially difficult to seek appropriate post-assault support such as counselling, psychotherapy or psychiatric care. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose The purpose of this paper is to provide a concise, selective review of the current status of service provision for male survivors of sexual assault in the UK. Design/methodology/approach This paper explored to what extent services are equipped to support the specific and complex needs of the male survivor and highlighted key implications for research, policy and practice. Findings The review outlined the historical backdrop to how and why services for male survivors have been consistently lacking, the current status of UK service provision is reported and suggestions for continued research in this developing area are made. Originality/value Despite a flurry of research in this area in recent years, UK service provision for male sexual assault survivors is inconsistent across locale. The value of the current paper is that it provides an outline of relevant work to date, and provides important suggestions for future directions.
... Women who have been raped often find criminal proceedings distressing. Sometimes, these criminal proceedings are referred to as secondary victimization (Bohmer & Blumberg, 1975;Doherty & Anderson, 1998;Latts & Geiselman, 1991). Why would an individual willfully and wittingly file a false allegation? ...
Article
Full-text available
The list of motives by Kanin (1994) is the most cited list of motives to file a false allegation of rape. Kanin posited that complainants file a false allegation out of revenge, to produce an alibi or to get sympathy. A new list of motives is proposed in which gain is the predominant factor. In the proposed list, complainants file a false allegation out of material gain, emotional gain, or a disturbed mental state. The list can be subdivided into eight different categories: material gain, alibi, revenge, sympathy, attention, a disturbed mental state, relabeling, or regret. To test the validity of the list, a sample of 57 proven false allegations were studied at and provided by the National Unit of the Dutch National Police (NU). The complete files were studied to ensure correct classification by the NU and to identify the motives of the complainants. The results support the overall validity of the list. Complainants were primarily motivated by emotional gain. Most false allegations were used to cover up other behavior such as adultery or skipping school. Some complainants, however, reported more than one motive. A large proportion, 20% of complainants, said that they did not know why they filed a false allegation. The results confirm the complexity of motivations for filing false allegations and the difficulties associated with archival studies. In conclusion, the list of Kanin is, based on the current results, valid but insufficient to explain all the different motives of complainants to file a false allegation.
... Where they would disagree is why people, men in particular, rape in the first place. For instance, a popular idea is that the cause of rape is a culture that endorses rape, also called a rape culture (Doherty & Anderson, 1998;Sanday, 1981Sanday, , 2003Sommers,1995). However, recent work in behavioral genetics suggests that people do not learn that rape is acceptable so much as they inherit genetic dispositions that facilitate rape (Barnes, TenEyck, Boutwell, & Beaver, 2013;Längström, Babchishin, Fazel, Lichtenstein, & Frisell, 2015) and sexual coercion is associated with more offspring in our closest living relatives, chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes; Feldblum et al., 2014). ...
Article
Full-text available
The Dark Triad traits have been repeatedly labeled as facilitating an exploitive mating strategy. However, various researchers have repeatedly conflated short-term mating or casual sex with an exploitive mating strategy. In this study using Mechanical Turk participants (N = 252; 142 men, 110 women), we provided a better test of just how sexually exploitive those high on the Dark Triad traits might be by examining how the traits related to rape-enabling attitudes. We examined how each trait may facilitate rape, whether these associations were robust to partialing the variance associated with the Big Five traits and similar in men and women, and showed that one reason why men may be more likely to rape than women is they are characterized by the Dark Triad traits more than women are. In so doing, we test the confluence model of rape that asserts that personality traits similar to the Dark Triad traits act as one pathway to rape.
... As extract 2 shows, in our data, this deletion of accountability was regularly accomplished via the discursive resources of a rape-supportive culture that ultimately attributes the responsibility for rape to women (Doherty and Anderson 1998). Key among these resources is the miscommunication model. ...
Article
Full-text available
This article describes a program of research that systematically examined the ways in which young heterosexual men draw on widely culturally shared accounts of rape to account for sexual coercion and sexual assault. In particular, the authors discuss the reliance placed on a lay version of Tannen’s (1992) miscommunication model of (acquaintance) rape—the claim that rape is a consequence of men’s not knowing—and young men’s use of this account as a device to accomplish exculpation. The authors discuss the implications of their methods for capturing young people’s understanding of sexual coercion, rape, and consent, as well as for designing rape prevention programs. KeywordsSexual coercion-Sexual assault-Sexual consent-Rape prevention-Interpersonal communication
Article
Prevailing theory stresses psychological factors in explaining why men rape. Recent UK government rape prevention campaigns employ a "miscommunication model", suggesting men understand neither consent, nor how women convey sexual refusal. In this paper we cast doubt on these claims.
Article
This article reviews gender inequities in the legal profession in the context of a study in which 278 Scottish students (123 men; 155 women) filled out a questionnaire about their most recent interview for a legal traineeship. There were no gender differences in responses with respect to the location, length, number and types of such meetings, though women were more likely to apply to medium-sized firms. There was no difference in topics discussed except that respondents who were mothers were more likely than those who were fathers to report discussion of domestic arrangements. Furthermore, extended comments suggested that women were more likely than men to be asked about sensitive issues but the numbers involved were very small. In general the study did not indicate explicit gender discrimination though it was pointed out that the inequitable gender balance on selection boards in favour of men may have disadvantaged women. These results are discussed within the context of other findings which reveal considerable gender inequity within the profession. It is suggested that further research is required to establish why the discriminatory effects suffered by women lawyers in Scotland do not emerge until later in their careers.
Article
This paper discusses the views that British men and women hold about gender roles. Drawing on a survey with over 4,000 university students and interviews with professional men and women, it is suggested that, while the majority of both genders are moving toward an egalitarian model of gender roles, men's views are more likely than women's to be constrained by an essentialist mode] of gender. The data presented indicate that men were more likely than were women to endorse traditional gender roles, to regard women as better equipped for child care than men, to believe that women's advances necessarily disadvantage men, and to believe that men's work opportunities have worsened in comparison to women's. Drawing on both the interviews and the survey, it is also argued that beliefs about gender roles tend to be mediated by individualist discourses, which exempt exceptional individuals from normative gender roles.
Article
Full-text available
This paper explores some of the distinguishing features of a discourse approach to the analysis of racism. A contrast is developed with attitude research in social psychology and we demonstrate how the concept of attitude inevitably neglects social and rhetorical context. In particular, attitude theory ignores the way respondents construct evaluations through their varying 'factual' accounts of attitudinal objects. The argument is illustrated through the detailed analysis of a section of interview transcript concerning 'Polynesian immigrants'. Overall, it is suggested that research concerning 'attitudes' to such contentious issues as race and gender has failed to appreciate the pressure on respondents to construct evaluative versions as out-there-in-the-world to avoid the blamewe attached when those versions are seen, not as feature of the world, but as a consequence of their own psychology or special interests.
Article
In this article, the authors demonstrate how discursive psychology may be applied to the analysis of eyewitness accounts of paramilitary-related violence in North Belfast, Ireland. Eyewitnesses are in a powerful position to construct reality for others in their community, but such accounts can easily be dismissed as partisan characterisations of events. The authors therefore demonstrate some of the ways in which these accounts are made difficult to undermine, how they are rhetorically designed to undermine alternative versions of events, and hour blamings and mitigations may be achieved through the construction of the social identities of the individuals in Me events as blameless victims or as ruthless perpetrators. They further consider how it is made discursively possible for ''blameless'' victims to exist alongside ''professional'' hitmen through the construction of assailants as professional but also personally flawed and this therefore compromises their claim to rational, political motivations for violence.
Article
Factor analyses of the responses of random samples of men and women from three cities indicate the presence of two relatively independent dimensions of rape prevention attitudes: a) beliefs about measures calling for restrictions in women's behavior, and b) beliefs about measures involving changes in the environment, or assertive actions by women. Sex and race differences in ratings of the effectiveness of the two types of prevention measures illustrate the usefulness of this two-dimensional approach for understanding rape prevention beliefs. The implications of these results for attribution theory and for anti-rape policies are discussed.
Article
The chapter presents a social psychological approach to the study of rape and sexual assault. Two issues are at the core of this approach: identifying the critical variables that affect attributions of responsibility to victims of rape, and exploring people's subjective definitions of rape, which may differ markedly from legal definitions. Following a review of the American evidence, a series of studies conducted in two European countries is presented to address these issues.
Article
This paper begins by drawing a distinction between cognitive and discursive approaches to linguistic categorization, and it is argued that cognitive approaches have ignored the prime importance of discourse. Rather than attempting to reject or refute the cognitive orientation in favour of a social alternative, it is argued that talk enlists cognition as a powerful element in the rhetoric of description and reality construction. Important features of categorization, such as prototype structures, in-definiteness of membership, indexicality of application and contrastive organization are shown to make sense as features designed for the situated rhetoric of talk, rather than for displaying a person's abstracted understanding of the world. It is argued that cognitive theories, while providing important insights into semantic organization, manage to sustain the explanatory primacy of perception and cognition only through the use of methods that systematically remove from view the flexibilities and action orientation of talk, while using imaginations of situated talk as a basis for semantic analysis.